You are on page 1of 11

International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

International Journal of Nursing Studies

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ns

Toward understanding nurses' decisions whether to miss care: A discrete


choice experiment
Nasra Abdelhadi ⁎, Anat Drach-Zahavy, Einav Srulovici
The Cheryl Spencer Department of Nursing, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Background: Studies of missed nursing care suggest that it results from ward-level, patient-related, and task-type
Received 10 September 2022 factors, while nurses' decision-making style was scarcely studied. Studying the effect of nurses' decision-
Received in revised form 17 January 2023 preference structures, namely a pattern of joint ward and patient factors, on missed care may also contribute
Accepted 19 January 2023 to understanding the phenomenon.
Objectives: To examine the relationships between decision-preference structures and missed care and the mod-
Keywords:
erating effects of decision-making styles and task type in these links.
Discrete choice experiment
Missed nursing care
Design: A discrete choice experiment with a between- and within-participants design.
Decision-making style Participants: A sample of 387 registered nurses working in acute medical surgical wards in Israel.
Task type Methods: Based on the protocol for discrete choice experiments, a survey was developed to assess the decision-
Situational factors preference structure, considering five factors: overload, presence of head nurse, clinical complexity, difficult pa-
Nurses tient, and presence of relatives. Participants were randomly assigned to four task-type conditions and completed
a survey regarding their task. Decision-making style was assessed using a validated questionnaire.
Results: Extensive workload (b = −0.46; p = 0.001), difficult patient (b = −0.20; p = 0.001), and patient clin-
ical complexity (b = −0.10; p = 0.006) were negatively linked to the probability of missed care. The interaction
between workload and task type (b = 0.252; p = 0.017) indicated that the probability of missed care under ex-
tensive compared with regular workload was lowest for developing a discharge plan and highest for providing
emotional support. The interaction of patient complexity and task type (b = 0.230; p = 0.013) indicated that
the probability of missed care in developing a discharge plan and medication administration was lower for pa-
tients having high compared with low clinical complexity. The interaction between difficult patient and task
type (b = −0.219; p = 0.044) indicated that the probability of missed care in emotional support, developing
a discharge plan, and patient's mobility was lower for difficult than for non-difficult patients. Finally, the interac-
tion between workload and decision-making style (b = − 0.48; p = 0.001) indicated that the probability of
missed care under heavy compared with regular workloads was lower for the dual-preference or the dominantly
intuitive styles.
Conclusions: This design enabled examining the prioritizing processes nurses use when deciding about whether
to miss care. The likelihood of missing more in structured tasks is lower under a heavy overload and when
patients appear difficult or clinically complex. Dual-preference styles or dominantly intuitive styles are more
suitable for the routine high workload.
© 2023 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

What is already known • Most previous studies on missed nursing care have relied heavily on
structural theories that posit that nurses miss care because of external
• Missed nursing care is a product of many factors including the organi- forces, particularly scarce resources.
zation, the patient, and the task.
What this paper adds

• A nurse's decision-making process plays a key role in limiting missed


⁎ Corresponding author at: The Cheryl Spencer Department of Nursing, University of
Haifa, 199 Aba Khoushy Ave., Mount Carmel, Haifa 3498838, Israel.
nursing care; the discrete-choice-experiment design facilitates under-
E-mail addresses: nasrahade@yahoo.com (N. Abdelhadi), anatdz@research.haifa.ac.il standing of nurses' decision-making processes by examining prefer-
(A. Drach-Zahavy), esrulovici@univ.haifa.ac.il (E. Srulovici). ence structures rather than singular attributes.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2023.104448
0020-7489/© 2023 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
2 N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448

• Structuring nursing tasks can help limit missed care, as more struc- former, also known as Type 1 thinking, describes processes that are
tured tasks are less likely to be missed; this is particularly true when fast and holistic, involve low cognitive control and low-conscious-
overload is heavy or patients are characterized by high clinical com- awareness, and thus enable high capacity. The latter—analytic
plexity or labeled “difficult.” decision-making, also known as Type 2 thinking—describes processes
• Nurses' decision-making personal style matters: while the dual- that are reflective and slow, involve high cognitive control and high con-
preference or dominantly intuitive decision-making styles are supe- scious awareness, and thus are resource-demanding (Croskerry, 2013).
rior for reducing missed care in extensive overloads, the dominantly The nursing profession has traditionally been seen as intuitive rather
analytical decision-making style is superior under regular overload. than analytic (Nibbelink and Brewer, 2018). Apparently, intuitive
decision-making processes are based on identification of patterns, ac-
quired through experience (Croskerry, 2013; Nibbelink and Brewer,
1. Introduction 2018). They serve nurses well in their daily routine of caring for pa-
tients, enabling them to complete most of their daily tasks (Nibbelink
1.1. Missed nursing care and Brewer, 2018). Yet intuitive decision-making processes are also
the primary source of errors, and perhaps of missed nursing care, par-
Missed nursing care—nursing tasks delayed, omitted, or incomplete tially due to experience-based preconceptions (Croskerry, 2013;
(Jones et al., 2020; Jones et al., 2015; Chaboyer et al., 2021)—is highly Nibbelink and Brewer, 2018).
prevalent. A majority of nursing personnel (55–98%) reported leaving As it became increasingly evident that neither intuitive nor analyti-
at least one task undone, even when viewed across countries, hospitals, cal decision-making adequately describes human decision-making, a
and wards (Jones et al., 2015; Chaboyer et al., 2021; Mandal et al., 2020). third approach to decision-making has been discussed in the nursing lit-
Because of the potential harm of missed nursing care for patients, erature: the cognitive continuum theory (Hammond, 1988; Cader et al.,
nurses, and organizations, it is a global concern (Jones et al., 2020; 2005; Hammond et al., 1987). This states that a decision is rarely either
Chaboyer et al., 2021; Mandal et al., 2020; Kalánková et al., 2019). For intuitive or analytic; effective decision-making requires that the two
patients, missed nursing care was associated with poorer quality of decision-making processes act in concert (Starcke and Brand, 2012).
care (Jones et al., 2020; Mandal et al., 2020), higher incidence of adverse Whereas intuitive decision-making functions as the default, analytic
events (Jones et al., 2015; Mandal et al., 2020; Mynaříková et al., 2020), decision-making supervises intuitive decision-making. Apparently, the
increased length of stay and hospital readmissions (Jones et al., 2020; analytic system “knows” the rational rules that the intuitive system is
Chaboyer et al., 2021), and lower levels of patient satisfaction (Jones prone to violate and thus can intervene to correct erroneous intuitive
et al., 2015; Mandal et al., 2020; Recio-Saucedo et al., 2018). It was judgments (Kahneman and Klein, 2009).
also associated with poorer nurse outcomes, including impaired job sat- Intuitive and analytical decision-making processes, and their combi-
isfaction (Jones et al., 2015; Mandal et al., 2020), increased turnover and nations, have been established as universal ways of making choices; yet
intentions to leave the job, and higher levels of moral distress, role con- nurses may vary in their personal decision-making styles, reflecting
flict, and job burnout (Ausserhofer et al., 2014; Papastavrou et al., 2014; their habits in making decisions, or their fairly stable way of interpreting
Uchmanowicz et al., 2020; Alsubhi et al., 2020; Clark and Lake, 2020). and responding to decisions (Driver, 1979; Harren, 1979). These differ-
Thus, missed nursing care has negative implications also for the organi- ences are thought to represent personal preferences and not differences
zation, for example, in terms of increased financial costs (Jones et al., in intuitive/analytical capacity or ability (Reber et al., 1991). Four main
2015; Alsubhi et al., 2020; Kalisch et al., 2009; Kalisch et al., 2013; See personal decision-making styles have been described: Dominantly intu-
et al., 2020). Therefore, it is important to understand how decisions itive, dominantly analytic, dual preference, and disengaged (Croskerry,
about which tasks to perform and which to miss are made by nurses 2014; Shiloh et al., 2002; Jokić and Purić, 2019; Bjørk and Hamilton,
at the patient's bedside. 2011; Fletcher et al., 2012; Epstein et al., 1996) (Table 1).
This study examines missed nursing care through a decision-making
lens. This perspective is important because traditional empirical research 2. Research model
on missed nursing care is grounded in structural theories, suggesting that
nurses respond primarily to economic and organizational factors (Jones 2.1. Nurses' preference structures
et al., 2020), thereby neglecting nurses' agency and the specific role nurses
play in deciding which tasks to complete and which to omit. The research model (Fig. 1) is informed by the missed nursing care
(Kalisch et al., 2009; Schubert et al., 2007) and cognitive continuum
1.2. Nurses' decision-making (Hammond et al., 1987) theories. The model begins by acknowledging
that nurses make decisions during complex decision-making encoun-
Clinical decision-making in nursing has been typically described by ters in which several attributes, sometimes at odds with one another,
two distinct processes: intuitive and analytic decision-making. The are considered in parallel. These complex circumstances are referred

Table 1
The personal decision-making styles.

Low intuitive decision-making style High intuitive decision-making style

Low analytic decision-making style Disengaged Dominantly intuitive

• present minimal involvement in their tasks and decision-making • low cognitive control and awareness;
processes • a medium degree of organization
• Typical use of quick and unconscious processes, such as
associative memory
High analytic decision-making style Dominantly analytic Dual preference style

• High cognitive control and awareness; • High ability to flexibly shift from an analytic to an intuitive
• Adopting a systematic, step-by-step procedure, generating tentative decision-making process, and vice versa.
hypotheses about the patient's condition, and weighing the decision • Apply logical reasoning and remain open-minded while
alternatives before also responding quickly to some situations intuitively
choosing the one that fits best in light of the evidence collected without erring.
N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448 3

Fig. 1. A double moderation research model.

to as preference structures combining different attributes in order to de- distressing and arouse negative emotions (Mamede et al., 2017;
scribe the subjective trade-offs between alternative contextual factors Schmidt et al., 2017). Apparently, nurses perceive patients on a con-
that affect the decision made (Nishimura and Ok, 2018). For example, tinuum between “likeable” and “difficult” (Mamede et al., 2017). It
whether to delay medication administration or to develop a discharge might be intuitively logical to suggest that nurses enjoy spending
plan for a patient may depend on work overload (Jones et al., 2015; more time with likable or grateful patients. Alternatively, nurses
Chaboyer et al., 2021) but also on the presence of the head nurse and/ may believe that more likeable patients will be more accepting of
or the patients' relatives, as well as on the perception of the patient as missed care. Indeed, research has shown that negative emotions
clinically complex or emotionally demanding (Abdelhadi et al., 2020). trigger avoidance behaviors (Dolan, 2002), and experiencing anger
Recent emerging empirical evidence indicated that factors in the during an encounter was found to decrease risk estimates, thereby
work context, or in relation to a specific patient, serve as red flags for leading individuals to ignore possible negative outcomes of their de-
nurses, encouraging them to shift from intuitive to analytic decision- cisions (Lerner et al., 2003; Lerner, 2001). Within healthcare set-
making, resulting in less missed nursing care (Abdelhadi et al., 2020). tings, treating “difficult patients” has been associated with
The specific encounter-level attributes encompassing nurses' decision impaired clinical decisions (Croskerry, 2013; Mamede et al., 2017),
preferences were chosen based on a designated qualitative study clinical mistakes (Schmidt et al., 2017), and neglect behaviors,
(Abdelhadi et al., 2020). The specific attributes are: namely not providing adequate care (Pickering et al., 2017).
Presence of the patient's relatives. Physicians spend more time com-
Workload. Scholars of missed nursing care agree that the most pow- municating with patients in visits where relatives are present than
erful factors related to this phenomenon are decreased resources in in visits where the patient is alone (Shields et al., 2005; Kang and
terms of staffing levels and diminished labor resources (Jones et al., Choy, 2015). Thus, we suggest that nurses will demonstrate less
2015; Mandal et al., 2020; Griffiths et al., 2018), inappropriate skill missed nursing care when patients are accompanied by relatives be-
mix (Griffiths et al., 2018), lack of material resources (Jones et al., cause nurses might perceive the relatives as potential monitors
2015), and patient acuity (Jones et al., 2015; Mandal et al., 2020). (Ferris and Rowland, 1983; Hills et al., 2019; Coats et al., 2018)
Patient's clinical complexity. The nurse's perception of a patient's who will call the nurse if missed care results in a complication, and
medical status might range from simple to complex, based on the therefore make every effort to complete their tasks.
number of chronic conditions the patient has, the medications the Presence of the head nurse. Missed nursing care may be reduced by
patient takes, or their prior usage of healthcare resources (Higashi head nurses monitoring nurses' care and ensuring that tasks are
et al., 2007; Ash et al., 2001; Charlson et al., 1987). Studies demon- completed (Shirey et al., 2008; Miltner et al., 2015). Nurses could
strated that a patient's clinical complexity was associated with de- also be more aware of action–outcome contingency if the head
creased missed nursing care (Jones et al., 2015). nurse is present (Hammond et al., 1987) and thus tend to complete
Difficult patients. These patients are defined as those whose behav- their tasks. Finally, head nurses may pitch in and provide care that
iors make the healthcare provider–patient encounter particularly would be otherwise missed (Drach-Zahavy and Dagan, 2002).
4 N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448

Although the above findings have merit, they are mainly based on Marks et al., 2008; Pacini and Epstein, 1999; Witteman et al., 2009)
nurses' narratives or correlational findings. This is where cognitive con- such as whether to miss care. However, we are interested here in the
tinuum theory (Hammond, 1988) can help in explaining how, why, and moderating role of decision-making personal style in the relationship
under what circumstances missed nursing care occurs. Accordingly, the between the nurse's preference structures and missed nursing care. Re-
model proposes two intervening mechanisms—the type of task and search on decision-making suggests that nurses should respond to com-
the nurse's decision-making style—that might moderate the relation- plex circumstances with either a more holistic style (i.e., intuitive style)
ships between patients' and wards' decision-making preferences and (Grant and Langan-Fox, 2014) or by remaining open-minded and
missed nursing care. smoothly juggling intuitive and analytical styles (i.e., dual decision-
making style) (Shiloh et al., 2002; Jokić and Purić, 2019; Fletcher et al.,
2.2. Moderating role of task type in the relationship between nurses' 2012; Epstein et al., 1996). As nurses apply these styles, they can more
decision-making preferences and missed nursing care wisely allocate their resources between the concomitant demands in
the preference structure, function more efficiently, and engage in
The model depicted in Fig. 1 also suggests that task type can moder- fewer missed nursing care incidents. By contrast, nurses characterized
ate the relationships between decision-preference structures and by a dominantly analytical style may find working in the overstretching
missed nursing care. The missed nursing care literature, in line with cog- demands characteristic of nurse-preference structures especially toxic.
nitive continuum theory, argues that certain tasks are more prone to be These nurses may prioritize one attribute over another, diverting high
missed than others (Jones et al., 2015). In line with this literature, main- awareness and reflective attention to one attribute while neglecting
taining patients' physical health is usually prioritized by nurses, the others (Croskerry, 2013), and therefore engaging in higher levels
whereas basic care, patient education, and emotional support are of missed nursing care overall. Last, nurses characterized by disengaged
more likely to be delayed (Mandal et al., 2020; Mynaříková et al., styles are inattentive to their decisions overall, resulting in the highest
2020; Griffiths et al., 2018; Bagnasco et al., 2020; Vincelette et al., level of missed nursing care.
2019), and that tasks that are more time-consuming, whose duration
is difficult to estimate, or whose requirements cannot be reduced by 3. Study aims and hypotheses
working around them are likely to be overlooked or delayed (Schubert
et al., 2013). Accordingly, cognitive continuum theory proposes a task The main aim of this study was to examine the relationships be-
continuum ranging from well-structured to ill-structured tasks. While tween decision-preference structures and missed care and the moderat-
well-structured tasks enable task decomposition into definable parts ing effects of decision-making styles and task type in these links (Fig. 1).
and have a clear criterion for success (Dane and Pratt, 2007; To this aim, the following hypotheses were derived:
Thompson et al., 2004), ill-structured tasks describe judgmental rather
than intellectual tasks having many feasible solutions (Thompson Hypothesis 1. Nurses' decision-making-preferences will be related to
et al., 2004). To grasp the range of well-structured and ill-structured the likelihood of completing or omitting care such that high patient clin-
tasks, we selected four tasks for the current study. Patient mobility ical complexity, presence of patients' relatives, and presence of the head
and medication administration represented well-structured tasks, as nurse will be negatively associated with the likelihood to miss care,
nurses know how to perform them by following a clear, step-by-step whereas perceiving a patient as difficult and having a heavy workload
procedure (Ausserhofer et al., 2014; Schutijser et al., 2019; Liu et al., will be positively associated with the likelihood to miss care.
2019). By contrast, discharge planning and emotional support represent
Hypothesis 2. Tasks categorized as well structured (i.e., patient mobil-
ill-structured tasks, as they are more emotional and holistic, making
ity and medication administration) will weaken the link between the
them harder to deconstruct, and have many options or solutions for per-
attributes and the likelihood to miss care compared with tasks catego-
forming them (Ausserhofer et al., 2014).
rized as ill structured.
Although direct effects of well-structured and ill-structured tasks on
missed nursing care have been reported (Jones et al., 2015; Zeleníková Hypothesis 3. Dual-preference or dominantly intuitive decision-
et al., 2020), the moderating role of task type on the relationship be- making styles will weaken the link between preference structures and
tween the encounter attributes of the patient and the ward and missed the likelihood to miss care compared with dominantly analytic or disen-
nursing care has yet to be examined. In accordance with cognitive con- gaged decision-making styles.
tinuum theory (Hammond et al., 1987), we propose that well-
structured tasks activate the analytic decision-making process, meaning
that information is processed slowly but systematically in a careful, 4. Methods
step-by-step manner, resulting in fewer instances of missed nursing
care irrespective of the patient and context attributes (Cader et al., 4.1. Design
2005; Hammond, 1996). Ill-structured tasks, by contrast, trigger the in-
tuitive decision-making process, indicating that information is proc- The study used a discrete choice experiment (please see Section 4.2
essed rapidly and holistically but is more sensitive to contextual and for a description of the discrete choice experiment), between- and
patient-related factors that might signal to the nurse that the patient's within-participant design. The within-participant component of the de-
quality of care may be at risk (Cader et al., 2005; Hammond, 1996). sign constituted the variability within nurses in their preferences across
Thus, the task and patient's and ward's encounter attributes may serve scenarios. Participants were presented with two decision-making pref-
as red flags to lead the nurse to attend to the patient and complete erences and asked to choose which one they prefer. Each alternative had
their care. Therefore, we suggest that the link between the attributes a different set of attributes and attribute levels, based on the results of a
and the likelihood to miss care will be stronger for ill as compared previous qualitative study (Abdelhadi et al., 2020) that sought to iden-
with well-structured tasks. tify the main attributes that guided nurses in choosing which care to
omit or postpone (Table 2). The between-participants component of
2.3. Decision-making style as moderating the relationship between nurses' the design included examining the variability in nurses responding to
decision-making preferences and missed nursing care the discrete choice experiment, given their decision-making style
(dominant analytic, dominant intuitive, dual preference, or disen-
The differences between nurses' four personal decision-making gaged). Additionally, to ease the burden on participating nurses, each
styles (i.e., dominantly analytic, dominantly intuitive, dual preference, nurse answered the discrete choice experiment survey for one of four
and disengaged) may predict decision outcomes (Shiloh et al., 2002; tasks (i.e., medication administration, patient mobility, developing
N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448 5

Table 2 focused on the nurse's likelihood of whether to provide or omit care in


Discrete choice experiment: Attributes and attribute levels. a particular encounter, and it was assumed that the nurse's decision crit-
Attribute Attribute level ically depended on how well these characteristics are concomitantly
Workload Regular workload (occupancy rate <100%)a
taken into account (Ryan, 2004; Tockhorn-Heidenreich et al., 2017; De
OR Bekker-Grob et al., 2012). Therefore, discrete choice experiments
Extensive workload (occupancy rate >100%) allow a more detailed investigation of the trade-offs between the differ-
Presence of head nurse Head nurse in the ward ent attributes in comparison to traditional regression analyses (De
OR
Bekker-Grob et al., 2012; Chudner et al., 2019; Mangham et al., 2009).
Head nurse is not in the ward
Presence of relatives Relatives are next to the patient Although discrete choice experiments were initially introduced to
OR healthcare economics and policymaking as a way to assess customer
No relatives next to the patient preferences (De Bekker-Grob et al., 2012; Chudner et al., 2019;
Difficult patient Patient stimulates negative emotions Mangham et al., 2009), they have been gradually extended to consider
OR
Patient does stimulate positive emotions
also the providers' preferences (Ryan, 2004).
Patient clinical complexity Patient has high levels of comorbidity Following the discrete choice experiment protocol (Ryan, 2004;
OR Louviere et al., 2010), the survey included eight scenarios, each present-
Patient has low levels of comorbidity ing two possible sets of options to omit care (option A or option
a
Levels of regular and extensive workload were determined based on the findings of a B) (Fig. 2). Each set of options represents a combination of the five attri-
qualitative study (Abdelhadi et al., 2020). butes that nurses consider vital in deciding whether to omit care
(Abdelhadi et al., 2020), and each attribute has two levels. In accordance
with the discrete choice experiment protocol recommendations, the at-
discharge plans, or providing patients emotional support); thus, only tributes and levels were chosen based on a qualitative study (Abdelhadi
differences between participants could be examined for task type. We et al., 2020), including (1) Patient clinical complexity: high versus low
followed all the recommended steps for discrete choice experiments, in- levels of patient comorbidity, (2) Difficult patient: patient stimulating
cluding selecting key attributes and assigning levels to them; develop- negative emotions versus positive emotions, (3) Presence of the patient's
ing an experimental design, scenarios, and surveys; administering relatives: yes or no, (4) Workload: regular (occupancy rate <100%) ver-
surveys to elicit preferences; and analyzing data (Lancsar et al., 2017). sus extensive workload (occupancy rate >100%), and (5) Presence of the
head nurse: yes or no (Table 2). Combining the five attributes and two
4.2. Discrete choice experiment levels yielded 32 (or 2 (Kalánková et al., 2019)) possible scenarios for
paired combinations of attributes. To avoid the high cognitive burden
Discrete choice experiments are an attribute-based measure of ben- that this full factorial design can cause, and in line with the experiment
efit, embedded in two complementary assumptions: (a) that healthcare protocol (Nittas et al., 2020; Hensher et al., 2005; Aizaki and Nishimura,
interventions, services, or policies are defined by the encounter charac- 2008), we developed an orthogonal fractional factorial design of eight
teristics (or attributes); and (b) the degree to which each of these char- scenarios using the Ngene software (ChoiceMetrics, 2014). The scenar-
acteristics is taken into account in a concomitant manner determines an ios' combinations were paired using a fold-over design (Burgess and
individual's preference. In this study, the discrete choice experiment Street, 2005).

In which case would you delay emotional support to the patient and help another patient in

the ward?

Prefer A Ward with Patient with low Head nurse is No relatives Patient
occupancy rate levels of not in the ward next to the stimulates
above 120% comorbidity patient negative
emotions

Prefer B Ward with Patient with high Head nurse is Relatives are Patient does not
occupancy rate levels in the ward next to the stimulate
below 80% comorbidity patient negative
emotions

In which case would you delay emotional support to the patient and help another patient in

the ward?

Prefer A Ward with Patient with high Head nurse is Relatives next Patient does not
occupancy rate levels of in the ward to the patient stimulate
above 120% comorbidity negative
emotions

Prefer B Ward with Patient with low Head nurse is No relatives are Patient stimulate
occupancy rate levels not in the ward next to the negative
below 80% comorbidity patient emotions

Fig. 2. Example of scenario combinations.


6 N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448

4.3. Sample dual preferences (high analytic/high intuitive), and disengaged (low
analytic/low intuitive).
The sample included 387 registered nurses from various acute-care
hospitals working in internal medicine or surgical hospital wards in 4.5. Ethical considerations
Israel and at least a 75% full-time equivalent. Nurses were randomly
assigned to the task-type condition: 99 completed the discrete choice Ethical approval was granted by the university's ethics committee
experiment for the medication administration task, 97 for the patient- (#142/18). Nurses were provided a full explanation of the study's
mobility task, 97 for developing-the-discharge-plan task, and 94 for aims, including their right to quit, and assured of confidentiality.
the emotional-support task. The sample size was adequate according
to Louviere et al. (2010) recommendation of 48 participants per task 4.6. Data analysis
(responding to the eight-scenario design explained above) to give a pre-
cise estimate for the main effect of each attribute level on missed nurs- Nurses' preference structures for attributes were analyzed with a
ing care given the five attributes and two levels in our study, at the conditional logistic model that included 3096 choice tasks. This model
conventional 5% significance level. estimates choice probabilities as a function of the attributes of the alter-
Data were collected via snowball sampling between July 2020 and natives under consideration (Craiu et al., 2016; Long, 2004); namely, it
January 2021 through social media networks (e.g., Facebook nursing so- compares the probability of selecting an alternative from among a set
cial groups). We chose this sampling method because we wanted to in- of attributes defining those alternatives (Hauber et al., 2016). To test a
clude as many participating nurses as possible, not limiting ourselves to moderation model, first, all attributes were entered into the model,
peripheral or central hospitals. Furthermore, we intentionally avoided and then the interaction terms Attributes × Task type and Attributes ×
recruiting participants through hospitals' officials in order to ensure Decision-making style were entered. All analyses were conducted in
the credibility and confidentiality of the responses. Those agreeing to SAS version 9.4, and the p-value was set at 0.05. Post-hoc analysis,
participate entered an online link to the Qualtrics software and were using Fisher's LSD, of the above interactions was conducted.
asked to complete the electronic questionnaire. After they completed
the electronic questionnaire, nurses were asked to share the link with 4.7. Model specification
colleagues who met the inclusion criteria. Nurses were offered a coffee
and pastry gift card for their participation. The analysis of the discrete choice experiments data assumes that a
nurse who chooses alternative A over alternative B perceives alternative
4.4. Measures A to provide greater utility (i.e., U) than alternative B (Hauber et al.,
2016). The utility function of the interactions according to the condi-
4.4.1. Missed nursing care tional logit model was specified as follows:
Missed nursing care was assessed via the survey. Nurses were asked
to answer, for each scenario, for which option (A or B) they would more U njt ¼ β1  workloadnjt þ β2  precence of head nursenjt þ β3
likely omit performing the specific task. Thus, they were forced to make  presence of relativesnjt þ β4  difficult patient njt þ β5
trade-offs between attributes and their levels to choose between the  clinical complexitynjt þ β6  workload  task typenjt þ β7
two alternative options for which they would omit or delay care (Fig. 2).  precence of head nurse  task typenjt þ β8
 presence of relatives  task typenjt þ β9  difficult patient
4.4.2. Task type  task typenjt þ β10  clinical complexity  task typenjt þ β11
Task type included four nursing activities: providing emotional sup-  workload  decision‐making stylenjt þ β12
port, developing a discharge plan, patient's mobility, and medication ad-  presence of head nurse  decision‐making stylenjt þ β13
ministration. These tasks were chosen based on results of a previous  presence of relatives  decision‐making stylenjt þ β14
qualitative study (Abdelhadi et al., 2020), as they represent both ill-  difficult patient  decision‐making stylenjt þ β15
structured (i.e., emotional support and developing discharge plan) and  clinical complexity  decision‐making stylenjt þ εnjt :
well-structured (i.e., patient's mobility and medication administration)
tasks. Nurses were randomly assigned to one of the four task types and where Unjt is the utility of nurses n derived from selecting incentive
completed the corresponding experiment. alternative j in choice task t, β1 to β15 are parameters to be estimated,
and εnjt is the error term that is assumed to be independently and
4.4.3. Decision-making style identically distributed following a Type 1 extreme value distribution.
Decision-making style was measured with the validated Unified Scale
to Assess Individual Differences in Intuition and Deliberation (USID). This 5. Results
two-scale questionnaire was originally developed to assess analytic and
intuitive personal decision-making styles (Pachur and Spaar, 2015). Six- 5.1. Sample characteristics
teen items assessed the analytic style (e.g., “Developing a clear plan is
very important to me”), and 16 items assessed the intuitive style The 387 participating nurses were mostly women (74%, n = 286),
(e.g., “When I make a decision, I trust my inner feeling and reactions”). holding a bachelor's degree (59%, n = 227), and working a 100% full-
Nurses were instructed to imagine a work situation that they had faced time equivalent (68%). Their ages ranged from 23 to 65 years (mean =
and to rate each item on a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 “I 35.97; standard deviation (SD) = 8.59), and their years of practice
disagree” to 5 “I 100% agree”) to indicate how they would have ranged from 1 to 45 (mean = 19.54; SD = 11.70). Furthermore,
decided in that situation. Cronbach's internal reliabilities were α = whereas 62% (n = 240) had earned a nursing advanced training certif-
0.94 and α = 0.87 for the analytic and intuitive subscales, respectively. icate, only 12% (n = 47) held a managerial role (Table 3).
For the current study, and in line with previous studies that found
that individuals are characterized by both decision-making styles 5.2. Internal consistency of the discrete choice questionnaire
(Shiloh et al., 2002; Jokić and Purić, 2019; Fletcher et al., 2012; Marks
et al., 2008; Witteman et al., 2009; Lu, 2015), the original questionnaire To test the internal consistency of the measure and the rationality of
scales were then categorized into four decision-making styles, based on participants, we added one scenario that, according to the study's hy-
the median score of the original scales: dominantly analytic (high ana- potheses, was superior to the alternative in every domain (Johnson
lytic/low intuitive), dominantly intuitive (low analytic/high intuitive), et al., 2019). Yet, since there is no ideal validity testing, we conducted
N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448 7

Table 3 the likelihood of missed nursing care: Workload × Task type (b =


Descriptive statistics for the study sample (N = 387). 0.252; p = 0.017), Patient clinical complexity ×Task type (b = 0.230;
Characteristic Mean (SD)/N (%) p = 0.013), and Difficult patient presence × Task type (b = − 0.219;
Gender (female) 286 (73.9%)
p = 0.044). To understand the nature of the interactions, post-hoc anal-
Age (in years) 35.97 (8.59) yses, using Fisher's LSD, were conducted.
Years of practice as a nurse (in years) 19.54 (11.70) First, for the interaction between workload and task type, post hoc
Education analyses demonstrated that the tendency to miss care was higher
Registered nurse 36 (9.3%)
under heavy than under regular workloads: the greatest disparity be-
BA 227 (58.7%)
MA 118 (30.5%) tween these was found for emotional support (b = −0.66; p-value =
PhD 6 (1.6%) 0.000), and the lowest for developing a discharge plan (b = −0.28; p-
Advanced nursing training certificate (yes) 240 (62%) value = 0.000). In addition, we observed no significant differences in
Full-time equivalent missed nursing care between the different task types under a regular
75% 81 (20.9%)
80–99% 43 (11.1%)
or a heavy workload. Yet the tendency to miss care under a heavy work-
100% 263 (68%) load was higher for patient mobility and emotional support than for dis-
Managerial role (yes) 47 (12.1%) charge planning (b = 0.182, p = 0.090 and b = − 0.200, p = 0.060,
respectively). Under a regular workload, the tendency to miss care
was higher for patient mobility than for emotional support (b =
an analysis to examine participants who failed to choose the inferior op- −0.27, p = 0.080).
tion in this scenario (Sargent, 2005; Holm et al., 2016). The analysis re- Second, for the interaction between patient complexity and task type,
vealed no significant differences between those who did and those who the probability of missed care in developing a discharge plan and medica-
did not choose the inferior option in this scenario. Thus, all participants tion administration was lower for patients characterized by high than by
were included in the final analysis. low clinical complexity (b = −0.21; p = 0.003; b = −0.22, p = 0.003,
respectively). Additionally, for high-clinical-complexity patients, the
5.3. Main effect of attributes on nurses' preference structures of missing care probability of missing a mobility task was higher than for missing a med-
ication administration task (b = 0.30; p = 0.042).
Table 4 presents the results of a conditional logit model evaluating Finally, for the interaction between patient difficulty and task type,
the likelihood of missed nursing care using the five attributes (model for emotional support (b = −0.308; p < 0.001), developing a discharge
1: main effects). Three of the five were significantly and negatively plan (b = −0.257; p < 0.001), and patient mobility (b = −0.228; p =
associated with missed nursing care: heavy workload (estimate = 0.002), nursing care had a lower probability of being missed for difficult
−0.46; p = 0.001), difficult patient (estimate = − 0.20; p = 0.001), than for non-difficult patients. However, no significant differences in
and patient clinical complexity (estimate = − 0.10; p = 0.006). medication administration were found between difficult and non-
Namely, the probability of missed care was lower for a heavy than for difficult patients.
a regular workload, when caring for patients having high comorbidity
compared with patients having low comorbidity, and when caring for 5.5. Interactions between preference structures (attributes) and decision-
patients labeled “difficult” (i.e., stimulating negative emotions) com- making style on nurses' missed nursing care
pared with non-difficult (i.e., stimulating positive emotions).
Only one significant two-way interaction, between preference struc-
5.4. Interactions between preference structures (attributes) and task type tures (attributes) and decision-making style on the likelihood of miss-
on nurses' missed nursing care ing care, was found (Table 4, model 2): Workload × Decision-making
style (b = − 0.48; p = 0.001). A post-hoc analysis (Fisher's LSD)
Results of the two-way interactions between task type and the five demonstrated that under a heavy workload, nurses characterized by
attributes, representing task structure, are presented in Table 4 (model dual preferences (b = −0.24; p = 0.029) or dominantly intuitive styles
2). Three of the five interactions were significantly associated with (b = −0.24; p = 0.021) were less likely to miss care than nurses char-
acterized by a disengaged style. In comparison, under a regular work-
Table 4 load, when compared with nurses characterized by a disengaged style,
Moderation conditional logistic models of the effects of attribute, task type, and decision- nurses characterized by a dominantly analytic style had the lowest
making style on probability of missed care. probability of missing care (b = −0.63, p = 0.001), followed by nurses
Attribute Model 1 Model 2
characterized by the dual style (b = − 0.39; p = 0.013) and nurses
Attributes only Attributes and characterized by the dominantly intuitive style (b = − 0.31; p =
interactions 0.033). Additionally, nurses characterized by a dominantly intuitive
Estimate p-Value Estimate p-Value style were more likely to miss care than nurses characterized by a dom-
inantly analytic style (b = 0.31; p = 0.038). No other significant inter-
Workload −0.461 <0.000 −0.356 <0.001
Complexity −0.101 0.006 −0.168 0.087 actions between workload and decision-making style were found.
Head nurse −0.043 0.241 −0.025 0.793
Relatives 0.018 0.959 0.015 0.872 6. Discussion
Difficult patient −0.209 <0.000 0.068 0.486
Workload × Task typea 0.252 0.017
By combining decision-making theory and discrete choice experi-
Complexity × Task typea 0.230 0.034
Head nurse × Task typea 0.074 0.481 ment methodology, we examined missed nursing care, drawing a com-
Relatives × Task typea 0.120 0.269 plex and nuanced picture of how, when, and why nurses miss care.
Difficult patient presence × Task typea −0.219 0.044 Apparently, nurses don't just react to external factors, such as overload
Workload × Decision-making styleb −0.485 >0.001
or a lack of resources, in their overt or covert decisions to miss care
Complexity × Decision-making styleb −0.147 0.154
Head nurse × Decision-making styleb −0.104 0.311 (Jones et al., 2020; Holm et al., 2016). Instead, the findings partially sup-
Relatives × Decision-making styleb −0.068 0.534 ported the hypothesized model (Fig. 1), demonstrating that nurses' de-
Difficult patient × Decision-making styleb −0.295 0.007 cisions are influenced by complex structural preferences that interact
a
Reference group = medication administration task. with task type and personal decision-making style. Importantly, three
b
Reference group = disengaged decision-making style. of the five attributes were predictors of lower likelihood to miss care:
8 N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448

extensive workload, difficult patients, and complex patients. These find- structure but features such as complexity, consequences, and monitoring
ings are not new, and they join robust findings from recent research re- level (Afsar and Umrani, 2020; Chen et al., 2022). Thus, because omitting
views (Jones et al., 2015; Mandal et al., 2020; Abdelhadi et al., 2020; or delaying a patient's mobility task can have long-run adverse conse-
Griffiths et al., 2018). The novelty of our findings lies in the interaction quences, this task is unlikely to activate the analytical decision-making
effects found relating to the moderating role of task type and personal process and may thus be overlooked (Jones et al., 2015; Ausserhofer
decision-making style in the relationship between the preference struc- et al., 2014) regardless of the patient's clinical complexity. In addition,
tures (i.e., attributes) and the likelihood of missing care. We were thus emotional support was not prioritized even for clinically complex pa-
able to contribute in a number of ways to the current literature. tients, since its ill-structured nature could not divert attention to analytic
First, contrary to Hypothesis 1 and previous research, perception of a decision-making processes, leading to missed care (Jones et al., 2015;
patient as difficult was linked to the likelihood of reduced missed nursing Cader et al., 2005; Hammond, 1996; Chegini et al., 2020).
care (Mamede et al., 2017; Schmidt et al., 2017; Smith and Zimny, 1988; Third, our findings indicated that work overload was linked to lower
Gerrard and Riddell, 1988). Medication administration was the only ex- likelihood of missed nursing care (Jones et al., 2015; Pavedahl et al.,
ception: no significant differences were found between difficult and 2022). However, our study adds to previous findings by revealing that
non-difficult patients. A methodological or theoretical explanation could nurses' likelihood to miss care in heavy- versus regular-overload circum-
clarify this discrepancy. In theory, understanding a difficult patient and stances varies depending on task type, with developing discharge plans
providing them full care may be the best way to handle the situation effi- missed the least, and providing emotional support missed the most
ciently and move on to the next patient (Gillette, 2000; Karahuseyınoglu under extreme workloads. As mentioned earlier, recent policy changes
and Oguzoncul, 2021). There is some evidence that nurses can de-escalate have resulted in discharge planning becoming more structured than ex-
difficult behaviors by offering instrumental or emotional support pected, and a fortiori than providing emotional support (Zhu et al.,
(Karahuseyınoglu and Oguzoncul, 2021). Furthermore, this result may 2015; Maramba et al., 2004). Thus, the combined effects of heavy work-
be due to our novel discrete choice experiment design, which allowed load and a well-structured task may signal nurses to stop, consider, and
us to examine the relationship between nurses' decision preferences make more analytical decisions in order to avoid missing nursing care.
and missed care, instead of the relationship between difficult patients By contrast, mobility was less prioritized under a heavy than under a reg-
and missed care alone. Namely, when considering different encounter fac- ular workload, even though it seemed to be a well-structured task. Task
tors together, treating difficult patients' needs immediately might be the type may include characters more than structures, such as complexity,
most efficient way to maximize the nurse's resources. consequences, and monitoring levels (Afsar and Umrani, 2020; Chen
Second, our findings indicated, in line with our Hypothesis 1 and et al., 2022). Putting off or delaying the mobility care of a patient is likely
previous findings, that a patient's clinical complexity results in more to have adverse long-term consequences, which is unlikely to activate an-
comprehensive care and lower likelihood of missed nursing care alytical thinking (Jones et al., 2015; Ausserhofer et al., 2014).
(Jones et al., 2015; Pavedahl et al., 2022). However, our study adds to Fourth, our findings provided important insights into nurses' per-
previous findings by revealing that nurses' decision-making about sonal decision-making styles as moderating the relationship between
whether to miss care of clinically complex patients varies depending decision-preference structures and missed nursing care. In partial sup-
on task type. Namely, in partial support of Hypothesis 2, when consider- port to Hypothesis 3, the findings indicated that under heavy compared
ing nurses' decision-preference matrix as a whole, rather than patient with regular workloads, nurses with a dominantly intuitive or dual-
complexity as a single factor, higher clinical complexity was associated preference style functioned better: missed care was less common than
with a lower likelihood of missed care for medication administration for nurses with disengaged decision-making styles (but not for nurses
and discharge-planning tasks, but not for emotional support or with analytic decision-making styles). Under heavier workloads, an
patient-mobility tasks. These findings provide partial support for the intuitive decision-making style may allow nurses to provide quick,
continuum cognitive theory (Cader et al., 2005; Hammond et al., efficient care, and to use their resources efficiently (Croskerry, 2013;
1987), arguing that nurses may give priority to well-structured tasks Hammond, 1988). In addition, nurses with a dual-preference decision-
when caring for patients characterized by high clinical complexity. making style are open-minded and switch between intuitive and
This theory suggests that the higher the task-structure level, the greater analytic styles when needed, allowing them to react flexibly. Therefore,
the chance that analytic decision-making processes will be facilitated, these decision-making styles seem better-suited to providing compre-
which will result in a more comprehensive patient-care plan and hensive care under heavier workloads (Nibbelink and Brewer, 2018;
fewer nursing-care gaps. That medication administration is a well- Jokić and Purić, 2019; Fletcher et al., 2012; Epstein et al., 1996). Under
structured task with detailed procedures that require nursing compli- heavy workloads, we also found that an analytic decision-making style
ance, especially for patients with high clinical complexity, may shift did not outperform a disengaged decision-making style in attenuating
the nurse's attention toward analytic decision-making and therefore re- the link between decision preference and missed care. It is likely that
duce missed care (Jones et al., 2015; Chaboyer et al., 2021; Mandal et al., such a decision-making style requires more time and cognitive re-
2020; Schubert et al., 2021). sources to develop tentative hypotheses and to weigh options before
At first glance, the fact that nurses prioritize developing discharge selecting the best one (Croskerry, 2013; Bjørk and Hamilton, 2011). In
plans for patients with higher clinical complexity may seem contrary addition, analyzing alternatives in a crowded environment can be
to the cognitive continuum theory (Hammond, 1988; Cader et al., extremely stressful for the nurse, preventing them from being able to
2005; Hammond et al., 1987), since discharge planning can seem to perform their duties efficiently (Oshodi et al., 2019).
be an ill-structured task. Yet, recently, health-system policymakers However, under a regular workload, a different picture emerged: as ex-
have realized that ill-structured discharge plans can lead to adverse pected, nurses having an analytical style were least likely to miss care,
events after discharge, hospital readmissions, and subsequent medical probably because they can use their resources optimally, calculate all the
costs (Kripalani et al., 2014; Zhu et al., 2015; Smith et al., 2018; alternatives, and make normative judgments (Shiloh et al., 2002). Nurses
Fredrickson and Burkett, 2019). It has therefore been necessary to struc- with a dominantly dual-preference style were the second best at reducing
ture discharge plans by monitoring the consequences of missed discharge the likelihood of missed care, followed by nurses with a dominantly intu-
plans and the discharge planning time (Zhu et al., 2015; Maramba et al., itive style. Disengaged nurses performed worst, regardless of their load
2004). Perhaps our finding that nurses prioritize developing discharge level, because they were less involved in their work (Shiloh et al., 2002;
plans for patients characterized by high clinical complexity reflects Jokić and Purić, 2019; Fletcher et al., 2012; Epstein et al., 1996).
these trends. Interestingly, when caring for patients with high clinical Finally, previous studies suggested that presence of the head nurse
complexity, patient mobility, a seemingly well-structured task, was not (Abdelhadi et al., 2020; Srulovici and Yanovich, 2022) and family mem-
prioritized by nurses. Perhaps task type encompasses not only task bers may have an important role in reducing missed nursing care
N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448 9

(Abdelhadi et al., 2020). However, we found no evidence for those rela- devastating effects. As a first point, we find that certain decision-
tionships in our study, through either main effects or interactions. The making styles (primarily intuitive and dual-preference) are better
study design may have led to these unexpected results given that we suited to decision-making and reduced missed care in heavy-
measured these attributes at only two levels—the presence or absence overload circumstances, whereas the primarily analytic decision-
of head nurses/relatives. Their presence may be perceived either as re- making style for regular overload circumstances. This may indicate
sources or as burdens, affecting the frequency of missed care (Hills that personal decision-making style could serve as a criterion for
et al., 2019; Coats et al., 2018; Miltner et al., 2015). For example, nurses selecting nurses and matching them with work environments that
may perceive relatives' presence as an unwanted interruption, increas- are conducive to their performance. It would also be helpful if nurses
ing their burden, resulting in missed care; or as a resource for decreasing were trained in and equipped with techniques for shifting between
workload, and thus decreasing missed care (Abdelhadi et al., 2020). intuitive decision-making and analytical decision-making under var-
Similarly, presence of the head nurse on the ward might be associated ious circumstances. Such training programs should encourage nurses
with better patient care when they are perceived as a clinical leader to identify those red alerts that require one to “stop and think” and to
(Page et al., 2021; Purdy et al., 2010) but with deteriorated patient deliberately enact an analytic decision-making style (Conroy, 2021).
care when they are perceived as punitive and as primarily monitoring Indeed, in a recent systematic review (Ludolph and Schulz, 2018) of
errors (Morsiani et al., 2017; Feather et al., 2015). Therefore, the insig- interventions to limit healthcare providers' intuitive decision-
nificant effects of head nurses'/relatives' presence might be interpreted making resulting in biases, around 70% of interventions were par-
with caution and should be investigated further. tially or completely effective. The authors concluded that combining
cognitive strategies primarily aimed at improving people's critical
6.1. Strengths, limitations, and suggestions for further research thinking skills with technological strategies that employ visual aids
to reduce errors, could reduce decision-making biases. Moreover,
This study used a novel discrete choice experiment to shed light on nurses characterized by a disengaged decision-making style should
bedside nurses' decision-making processes regarding missing care. be identified, and nurse managers should provide them internal
This design may have helped illuminate nurses' nuanced decision- and external motivational rewards for adopting critical thinking.
making processes about when is omitting or delaying care more likely, Second, despite findings suggesting that nurses struggling with diffi-
because (a) it simulates nurses' natural complex encounters, and (b) it cult patients cannot effectively process clinical judgments (Mamede
assesses nurses' decision preferences rather than singular encounter at- et al., 2017; Schmidt et al., 2017; Smith and Zimny, 1988), our findings
tributes, thereby overcoming the limitations of ranking singular- demonstrated that when nurses try to maximize their resources and
attribute data (Louviere and Woodworth, 1983). Despite their provide optimal care for all patients, they attend to difficult patients'
strengths, discrete choice experiments have several limitations. needs better. In this regard, nurses should be trained to offer solutions
First, using structured scenarios might limit the generalizability of to difficult patients so that they are less likely to miss nursing care
the findings to real-world situations. Discrete choice experiments may (Gillette, 2000; Karahuseyınoglu and Oguzoncul, 2021). Providing
not capture all the complexities of real-world choices (Liu et al., 2019; these patients empathy and trust is key to treating them (Gillette,
Berman et al., 2021). However, this limitation might be less serious in 2000; Karahuseyınoglu and Oguzoncul, 2021).
our study because nurses' decision-preference patterns were based on Third, the findings suggesting that structured tasks are less likely to
attributes and levels that emerged in a pilot qualitative study be missed call for better structuring of nursing tasks for patients with
(Abdelhadi et al., 2020) and matched those found in previous studies high clinical complexity, as missed care might have even more devastat-
(Jones et al., 2015) as recommended in the discrete choice experiment ing consequences for them (Recio-Saucedo et al., 2018). The nursing
protocol (Coats et al., 2018). Future studies should examine real- tasks of emotional support and patient mobility might be better struc-
world, bedside situations, to better understand nurse preferences and tured so that these vulnerable patients do not experience missed nurs-
to shed light on other factors involved in the likelihood to omit care. ing care. Consequently, nursing managers should provide nurses with
Second, data collection through social media networks may be vul- step-by-step instructions on how to break down tasks into sequential
nerable to self-selection bias: users of such platforms may be systemati- steps, as well as the actions that should be taken and their expected out-
cally different from those who do not. Additionally, social media comes. Examples of how the task can be completed are provided as a
networks do not necessarily contain the entire population of interest means of increasing nurses' understanding of the task and acting as a
(Morstatter and Liu, 2017; Mirabeau et al., 2013). Yet participants had model for how the task can be completed in general (Eiriksdottir and
to meet the inclusion criteria to join the study, which should partially Catrambone, 2011).
overcome this bias. In this vein, we acknowledge that our sample was Finally, the findings that likelihood to miss care are not made in iso-
younger than the nurses' population in Israel. However, as these repre- lation but instead reflect a complex decision preference call for inter-
sent the future generation of nurses, this limitation may not seriously vention programs that introduce nurses to complex situations, invite
bias our conclusions. Furthermore, we did not collect data on the them to reflect on their (sometimes conflicting) considerations, and dis-
nurse or about the ward in which the nurse worked (e.g., internal or sur- cuss ways to optimize their resources so that more complete care is pro-
gical ward). In first glance, one might expect differences in workloads vided to all patients.
across wards, which might bias our findings. However, because person-
nel in the wards is standardized according to the type of ward, and given 7. Conclusions
that our workload measurement was subjective (e.g., nurse perceptions
about whether the workload was below or above 100% occupancy), this The present study employed a novel discrete choice experiment
limitation may not significantly affect our results. design to highlight an understudied correlate of missed nursing care:
Finally, consistently presenting some attributes before others might the nurse's decision-making processes and styles. Apparently, choosing
bias the results. Yet it would be difficult for the individual if attributes' which care to omit or postpone is a complex decision, concomitantly
order was to change. However, future studies should consider random- influenced by many factors, including the decision-maker's personal
izing the attributes across individuals. style, the task type, and the nurse's decision-preference pattern,
encompassing ward and patient characteristics. These findings suggest
6.2. Practical implications that hospital managers might limit the frequency of missed care by bet-
ter structuring tasks. Alternatively, given appropriate training, nurses
The study's findings have important implications for hospital man- could learn how to better adjust their decision-making style to their
agers and policymakers seeking to reduce missed nursing care's work context.
10 N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448

Funding Conroy, M.E., 2021. A Qualitative Study on Entrustment Decision Making in the Intensive
Care Unit: About More Than the Learner. The Ohio State University.
Craiu, R.V., Duchesne, T., Fortin, D., Baillargeon, S., 2016. Two Step C Logit: Conditional Lo-
This study was supported by the Cheryl Spencer Institute of Nursing gistic Regression: A Two-step Estimation Method. R Package Version 1.2.5.
Research. Croskerry, P., 2013. From mindless to mindful practice—cognitive bias and clinical deci-
sion making. N. Engl. J. Med. 368 (26), 2445–2448.
Croskerry, P., 2014. ED cognition: any decision by anyone at any time. Can. J. Emerg. Med.
CRediT authorship contribution statement 16 (1), 13–19.
Dane, E., Pratt, M.G., 2007. Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making.
Nasra Abdelhadi: Methodology, Software, Data curation, Writing – Acad. Manag. Rev. 32 (1), 33–54.
De Bekker-Grob, E.W., Ryan, M., Gerard, K., 2012. Discrete choice experiments in health
original draft. Anat Drach-Zahavy: Supervision, Project administration. economics: a review of the literature. Health Econ. 21 (2), 145–172.
Einav Srulovici: Supervision, Project administration. Dolan, R.J., 2002. Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science 298 (5596), 1191–1194.
Drach-Zahavy, A., Dagan, E., 2002. From caring to managing and beyond: an examination
of the head nurse's role. J. Adv. Nurs. 38 (1), 19–28.
Data availability Driver, M.J., 1979. Individual decision-making and creativity. In: Kerr, S. (Ed.), Organiza-
tional Behavior. Grid, Columbus, OH.
The datasets collected and/or analyzed during the current study are Eiriksdottir, E., Catrambone, R., 2011. Procedural instructions, principles, and examples:
how to structure instructions for procedural tasks to enhance performance, learning,
available from the corresponding author on request. The corresponding
and transfer. Hum. Factors 53 (6), 749–770.
author had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., Heier, H., 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-
for integrity of the data and accuracy of the data analysis. experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71,
390–405. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390.
Feather, R.A., Ebright, P., Bakas, T., 2015. Nurse manager behaviors that RNs perceive to
Declaration of Competing Interest affect their job satisfaction. Nurs. Forum 50 (2), 125–136.
Ferris, G.R., Rowland, K.M., 1983. Social facilitation effects on behavioral and perceptual
No conflict of interest has been declared by the authors. task performance measures: implications for work behavior. Group Organ. Stud. 8
(4), 421–438.
Fletcher, J.M., Marks, A.D., Hine, D.W., 2012. Latent profile analysis of working memory ca-
Acknowledgments pacity and thinking styles in adults and adolescents. J. Res. Pers. 46 (1), 40–48.
Fredrickson, B.A., Burkett, E., 2019. Interventions to improve the continuity of medication
None. management upon discharge of patients from hospital to residential aged care facil-
ities. J. Pharm. Pract. Res. 49 (2), 162–170.
Gerrard, T.J., Riddell, J.D., 1988. Difficult patients: black holes and secrets. BMJ [Br. Med. J.]
References 297 (6647), 530.
Gillette, R.D., 2000. ‘Problem patients’: a fresh look at an old vexation. Fam. Pract. Manag.
Abdelhadi, N., Drach-Zahavy, A., Srulovici, E., 2020. (2020) The nurse’s experience of 7 (7), 57.
decision-making processes in missed nursing care: a qualitative study. J. Adv. Nurs. Grant, S.L., Langan-Fox, J., 2014. Stress and the unconscious in intuitive judgement. Hand-
76 (8), 2161–2170. book of Research Methods on Intuition. Edward Elgar Publishing, pp. 59–71.
Afsar, B., Umrani, W.A., 2020. Transformational leadership and innovative work behavior: Griffiths, P., Recio-Saucedo, A., Dall’Ora, C., Briggs, J., Maruotti, A., Meredith, P., Smith, G.B.,
the role of motivation to learn, task complexity and innovation climate. Eur. J. Innov. Ball, J., Missed Care Study Group, 2018. The association between nurse staffing and
Manag. 23 (3), 402–428. omissions in nursing care: a systematic review. J. Adv. Nurs. 74 (7), 1474–1487.
Aizaki, H., Nishimura, K., 2008. Design and analysis of choice experiments using R: a brief Hammond, R.M., 1988. Clinical intuition and clinical analysis: expertise and the cognitive
introduction. Agric. Inf. Res. 17 (2), 86–94. continuum. Professional Judgment: A Reader in Clinical Decision Making, pp. 78–105.
Alsubhi, H., Meskell, P., Shea, D.O., Doody, O., 2020. Missed nursing care and nurses’ inten- Hammond, K.R., 1996. Human Judgment and Social Policy: Irreducible Uncertainty, Inev-
tion to leave: an integrative review. J. Nurs. Manag. 28 (8), 1830–1840. itable Error, Unavoidable Injustice. Oxford University Press on Demand.
Ash, A.S., Zhao, Y., Ellis, R.P., Kramer, M.S., 2001. Finding future high-cost cases: comparing Hammond, K.R., Hamm, R.M., Grassia, J., Pearson, T., 1987. Direct comparison of the effi-
prior cost versus diagnosis-based methods. Health Serv. Res. 36 (6 Pt 2), 194–206. cacy of intuitive and analytical cognition in expert judgment. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man
Ausserhofer, D., Zander, B., Busse, R., Schubert, M., De Geest, S., Rafferty, A.M., Cybern. 17 (5), 753–770.
Schwendimann, R., 2014. Prevalence, patterns and predictors of nursing care left un- Harren, V.A., 1979. A model of career decision-making for college students. J. Vocat.
done in European hospitals: results from the multicountry cross-sectional RN4CAST Behav. 14, 119–133.
study. BMJ Qual. Saf. 23 (2), 126–135.
Hauber, A.B., González, J.M., Groothuis-Oudshoorn, C.G., Prior, T., Marshall, D.A.,
Bagnasco, A., Dasso, N., Rossi, S., Galanti, C., Varone, G., Catania, G., Sasso, L., 2020. Unmet
Cunningham, C., Ijzerman, M.J., Bridges, J.F., 2016. Statistical methods for the analysis
nursing care needs on medical and surgical wards: a scoping review of patients’ per-
of discrete choice experiments: a report of the ISPOR conjoint analysis good research
spectives. J. Clin. Nurs. 29 (3–4), 347–369.
practices task force. Value Health 19 (4), 300–315.
Berman, L., Nkhoma, L., Prust, M., McKay, C., Teshome, M., Banda, D., Kabambe, D., Gunda,
Hensher, D.A., Rose, J.M., Rose, J.M., Greene, W.H., 2005. Applied Choice Analysis: A
A., 2021. Analysis of policy interventions to attract and retain nurse midwives in rural
Primer. Cambridge University Press.
areas of Malawi: a discrete choice experiment. PloS one 16 (6), e0253518.
Higashi, T., Wenger, N.S., Adams, J.L., Fung, C., Roland, M., McGlynn, E.A., Shekelle, P.G.,
Bjørk, I.T., Hamilton, G., 2011. AClinical decision making of nurses working in hospital set-
2007. Relationship between number of medical conditions and quality of care. N.
tings. Nurs. Res. Pract. 2011.
Engl. J. Med. 356 (24), 2496–2504.
Burgess, L., Street, D.J., 2005. Optimal designs for choice experiments with asymmetric at-
Hills, P.J., Dickinson, D., Daniels, L.M., Boobyer, C.A., Burton, R., 2019. Being observed
tributes. J. Stat. Plan. Infer. 134 (1), 288–301.
caused physiological stress leading to poorer face recognition. Acta Psychol. 196,
Cader, R., Campbell, S., Watson, D., 2005. Cognitive continuum theory in nursing decision-
118–128.
making. J. Adv. Nurs. 49 (4), 397–405.
Holm, S., Lemm, R., Thees, O., Hilty, L.M., 2016. Enhancing agent-based models with dis-
Chaboyer, W., Harbeck, E., Lee, B.O., Grealish, L., 2021. Missed nursing care: an overview of
crete choice experiments. J. Artif. Soc. Soc. Simul. 19 (3) 3-3.
reviews. Kaohsiung J. Med. Sci. 37 (2), 82–91.
Charlson, M.E., Pompei, P., Ales, K.L., MacKenzie, C.R., 1987. A new method of classifying Johnson, F.R., Yang, J.C., Reed, S.D., 2019. The internal validity of discrete choice experi-
prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies: development and validation. ment data: a testing tool for quantitative assessments. Value Health 22 (2), 157–160.
J. Chronic Dis. 40 (5), 373–383. Jokić, B., Purić, D., 2019. Relating rational and experiential thinking styles with trait emo-
Chegini, Z., Jafari-Koshki, T., Kheiri, M., Behforoz, A., Aliyari, S., Mitra, U., Islam, S.M.S., tional intelligence in broader personality space. Eur. J. Psychol. 15 (1), 140.
2020. Missed nursing care and related factors in Iranian hospitals: a cross-sectional Jones, T.L., Hamilton, P., Murry, N., 2015. Unfinished nursing care, missed care, and implic-
survey. J. Nurs. Manag. 28 (8), 2205–2215. itly rationed care: state of the science review. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 52 (6), 1121–1137.
Chen, Y., Fang, W., Guo, B., Bao, H., 2022. The moderation effects of task attributes and Jones, T., Drach-Zahavy, A., Amorim-Lopes, M., Willis, E., 2020. Systems, economics, and
mental fatigue on post-interruption task performance in a concurrent multitasking neoliberal politics: theories to understand missed nursing care. Nurs. Health Sci. 22
environment. Appl. Ergon. 102, 103764. (3), 586–592.
ChoiceMetrics, 2014. NGENE user guide and reference manual. Australia. http://www. Kahneman, D., Klein, G., 2009. Conditions for intuitive expertise: a failure to disagree. Am.
choice-metrics.com/NgeneManual120.pdf. (Accessed 18 December 2020). Psychol. 64 (6), 515.
Chudner, I., Goldfracht, M., Goldblatt, H., Drach-Zahavy, A., Karkabi, K., 2019. Video or in- Kalánková, D., Žiaková, K., Kurucová, R., 2019. Approaches to understanding the phenom-
clinic consultation? Selection of attributes as preparation for a discrete choice exper- enon of missed/rationed/unfinished care-a literature review. Cent. Eur. J. Nurs. Mid-
iment among key stakeholders. Patient-Patient-Centered Outcomes Res. 12 (1), wifery 10 (1), 1005–1016.
69–82. Kalisch, B.J., Landstrom, G.L., Hinshaw, A.S., 2009. Missed nursing care: a concept analysis.
Clark, R.R., Lake, E., 2020. Burnout, job dissatisfaction and missed care among maternity J. Adv. Nurs. 65 (7), 1509–1517.
nurses. J. Nurs. Manag. 28 (8), 2001–2006. Kalisch, B.J., Doumit, M., Lee, K.H., El Zein, J., 2013. Missed nursing care, level of staffing,
Coats, H., Bourget, E., Starks, H., Lindhorst, T., Saiki-Craighill, S., Curtis, J.R., Doorenbos, A., and job satisfaction: Lebanon versus the United States. J. Nurs. Adm. 43 (5), 274–279.
2018. Nurses’ reflections on benefits and challenges of implementing family-centered Kang, D.S., Choy, S.P., 2015. The layers of relational communication in evaluating health
care in pediatric intensive care units. Am. J. Crit. Care 27 (1), 52–58. care services. Int. J. Pharm. Healthc. Mark. 9 (4), 349–368.
N. Abdelhadi, A. Drach-Zahavy and E. Srulovici / International Journal of Nursing Studies 139 (2023) 104448 11

Karahuseyınoglu, E.K., Oguzoncul, A.F., 2021. Perception of difficult patient and coping Pickering, C.E., Nurenberg, K., Schiamberg, L., 2017. Recognizing and responding to the
methods in primary healthcare institutions. Ann. Clin. Anal. Med. 12 (3), 281–285. “toxic” work environment: worker safety, patient safety, and abuse/neglect in nurs-
Kripalani, S., Theobald, C.N., Anctil, B., Vasilevskis, E.E., 2014. Reducing hospital readmis- ing homes. Qual. Health Res. 27 (12), 1870–1881.
sion: current strategies and future directions. Annu. Rev. Med. 65, 471. Purdy, N., Spence Laschinger, H.K., Finegan, J., Kerr, M., Olivera, F., 2010. Effects of work
Lancsar, E., Fiebig, D.G., Hole, A.R., 2017. Discrete choice experiments: a guide to model environments on nurse and patient outcomes. J. Nurs. Manag. 18 (8), 901–913.
specification, estimation, and software. PharmacoEconomics 35 (7), 697–716. Reber, A.S., Walkenfeld, F.F., Hernstadt, R., 1991. Implicit and explicit learning: individual
Lerner, J.S., 2001. Keltner D fear, anger, and risk. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81 (1), 146. differences and IQ. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 17 (5), 888.
Lerner, J.S., Gonzalez, R.M., Small, D.A., Fischhoff, B., 2003. Effects of fear and anger on per- Recio-Saucedo, A., Dall’Ora, C., Maruotti, A., Ball, J., Briggs, J., Meredith, P., Redfern, O.V.,
ceived risks of terrorism: a national field experiment. Psychol. Sci. 14 (2), 144–150. Kovacs, C., Prytherch, D., Smith, G.B., Griffiths, P., 2018. What impact does nursing
Liu, F.X., Witt, E.A., Ebbinghaus, S., Beyer, G.D., Basurto, E., Joseph, R.W., 2019. Patient and care left undone have on patient outcomes? Review of the literature. J. Clin. Nurs.
oncology nurse preferences for the treatment options in advanced melanoma: a dis- 27 (11-12), 2248–2259.
crete choice experiment. Cancer Nurs. 42 (1), E52. Ryan, M., 2004. Discrete choice experiments in health care. BMJ 328 (7436), 360–361.
Long, B.T., 2004. How have college decisions changed over time? An application of the Sargent, R.G., 2005. Verification and validation of simulation models. Proceedings of the
conditional logistic choice model. J. Econ. 121 (1–2), 271–296. 37th Winter Simulation Conference 130–14.
Louviere, J.J., Woodworth, G., 1983. Design and analysis of simulated consumer choice or Schmidt, H.G., Van Gog, T., Schuit, S.C., Van den Berge, K., Van Daele, P.L., Bueving, H.,
allocation experiments: an approach based on aggregate data. J. Mark. Res. 20 (4), Mamede, S., 2017. Do patients’ disruptive behaviors influence the accuracy of a doc-
350–367. tor’s diagnosis? A randomized experiment. BMJ Qual. Saf. 26 (1), 19–23.
Louviere, J.J., Flynn, T.N., Carson, R.T., 2010. Discrete choice experiments are not conjoint Schubert, M., Glass, T.R., Clarke, S.P., Schaffert-Witvliet, B., De Geest, S., 2007. Validation of
analysis. J. Choice Model. 3 (3), 57–72. the Basel extent of rationing of nursing care instrument. Nurs. Res. 56 (6), 416–424.
Lu, Y., 2015. Is experiential-intuitive cognitive style more inclined to err on conjunction Schubert, M., Ausserhofer, D., Desmedt, M., et al., 2013. Levels and correlates of implicit
fallacy than analytical-rational cognitive style? Front. Psychol. 6, 85. rationing of nursing care in swiss acute care hospitals—a cross sectional study. Int.
Ludolph, R., Schulz, P.J., 2018. Debiasing health-related judgments and decision making: a J. Nurs. Stud. 50, 230–239.
systematic review. Med. Decis. Mak. 38 (1), 3–13. Schubert, M., Ausserhofer, D., Bragadóttir, H., Rochefort, C.M., Bruyneel, L., Stemmer, R.,
Mamede, S., Van Gog, T., Schuit, S.C., Van den Berge, K., Van Daele, P.L., Bueving, H., Andreou, P., Leppée, M., Palese, A., RANCARE Consortium COST Action–CA 15208,
Schmidt, H.G., 2017. Why patients’ disruptive behaviors impair diagnostic reasoning: 2021. Interventions to prevent or reduce rationing or missed nursing care: a scoping
a randomized experiment. BMJ Qual. Saf. 26 (1), 13–18. review. J. Adv. Nurs. 77 (2), 550–564.
Mandal, L., Seethalakshmi, A., Rajendrababu, A., 2020. Rationing of nursing care, a devia- Schutijser, B.C.F.M., Jongerden, I.P., Klopotowska, J.E., Portegijs, S., de Bruijne, M.C.,
tion from holistic nursing: a systematic review. Nurs. Philos. 21 (1), e12257. Wagner, C., 2019. Double checking injectable medication administration: does the
Mangham, L.J., Hanson, K., McPake, B., 2009. How to do (or not to do). Designing a dis- protocol fit clinical practice? Saf. Sci. 118, 853–860.
crete choice experiment for application in a low-income country. Health Policy See, M.T.A., Chee, S., Rajaram, R., Kowitlawakul, Y., Liaw, S.Y., 2020. Missed nursing care in
Plan. 24 (2), 151–158. patient education: a qualitative study of different levels of nurses’ perspectives.
Maramba, P.J., Richards, S., Myers, A.L., Larrabee, J.H., 2004. Discharge planning process: J. Nurs. Manag. 28 (8), 1960–1967.
applying a model for evidence-based practice. J. Nurs. Care Qual. 19 (2), 123–129. Shields, C.G., Epstein, R.M., Fiscella, K., Franks, P., McCann, R., McCormick, K., Mallinger,
Marks, A.D., Hine, D.W., Blore, R.L., Phillips, W.J., 2008. Assessing individual differences in J.B., 2005. Influence of accompanied encounters on patient-centeredness with older
adolescents’ preference for rational and experiential cognition. Personal. Individ. Dif- patients. J. Am. Board Fam. Pract. 18 (5), 344–354.
fer. 44 (1), 42–52. Shiloh, S., Salton, E., Sharabi, D., 2002. Individual differences in rational and intuitive
Miltner, R.S., Jukkala, A., Dawson, M.A., Patrician, P.A., 2015. Professional development thinking styles as predictors of heuristic responses and framing effects. Personal.
needs of nurse managers. J. Contin. Educ. Nurs. 46 (6), 252–258. Individ. Differ. 32 (3), 415–429.
Mirabeau, L., Mignerat, M., Grangé, C., 2013. The Utility of Using Social Media Networks Shirey, M.R., Ebright, P.R., McDaniel, A.M., 2008. Sleepless in America: nurse managers
for Data Collection in Survey Research. cope with stress and complexity. J. Nurs. Adm. 38 (3), 125–131.
Morsiani, G., Bagnasco, A., Sasso, L., 2017. How staff nurses perceive the impact of nurse Smith, R.C., Zimny, G.H., 1988. Physicians emotional reactions to patients. Psychosomatics
managers’ leadership style in terms of job satisfaction: a mixed method study. 29, 392–397.
J. Nurs. Manag. 25 (2), 119–128. Smith, L.M., Keiser, M., Turkelson, C., Yorke, A.M., Sachs, B., Berg, K., 2018. Simulated inter-
Morstatter, F., Liu, H., 2017. Discovering, assessing, and mitigating data bias in social professional education discharge planning meeting to improve skills necessary for ef-
media. Online Soc. Netw. Media 1, 1–13. fective interprofessional practice. Prof. Case Manag. 23 (2), 75–83.
Mynaříková, E., Jarošová, D., Janíková, E., Plevová, I., Polanská, A., Zeleníková, R., 2020. Oc- Srulovici, E., Yanovich, O., 2022. Missed nursing care: testing the moderation-mediation
currence of hospital-acquired infections in relation to missed nursing care: a litera- energetic and motivational pathways via a nested design. J. Adv. Nurs. 78 (8),
ture review. Cent. Eur. J. Nurs. Midwifery 11 (1), 43–49. 2339–2348.
Nibbelink, C.W., Brewer, B.B., 2018. Decision-making in nursing practice: an integrative Starcke, K., Brand, M., 2012. Decision making under stress: a selective review. Neurosci.
literature review. J. Clin. Nurs. 27 (5–6), 917–928. Biobehav. Rev. 36 (4), 1228–1248.
Nishimura, H., Ok, E.A., 2018. Preference Structures. Mimeo, NYU. Thompson, C., Cullum, N., McCaughan, D., Sheldon, T., Raynor, P., 2004. Nurses, informa-
Nittas, V., Mütsch, M., Puhan, M.A., 2020. Preferences for sun protection with a self- tion use, and clinical decision making—the real world potential for evidence-based
monitoring App: protocol of a discrete choice experiment study. JMIR Res. Protocol. decisions in nursing. Evid. Based Nurs. 7 (3), 68–72.
9 (2), e16087. Tockhorn-Heidenreich, A., Ryan, M., Hernández, R., 2017. Discrete choice experiments.
Oshodi, T.O., Bruneau, B., Crockett, R., Kinchington, F., Nayar, S., West, E., 2019. The nurs- Patient Involvement in Health Technology Assessment. Adis, Singapore, pp. 121–133.
ing work environment and quality of care: content analysis of comments made by Uchmanowicz, I., Karniej, P., Lisiak, M., Chudiak, A., Lomper, K., Wiśnicka, A., Wleklik, M.,
registered nurses responding to the Essentials of Magnetism II scale. Nurs. Open 6 Rosińczuk, J., 2020. The relationship between burnout, job satisfaction and the ration-
(3), 878–888. ing of nursing care—a cross-sectional study. J. Nurs. Manag. 28 (8), 2185–2195.
Pachur, T., Spaar, M., 2015. Domain-specific preferences for intuition and deliberation in Vincelette, C., Thivierge-Southidara, M., Rochefort, C.M., 2019. Conceptual and methodo-
decision making. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 4 (3), 303–311. logical challenges of studies examining the determinants and outcomes of omitted
Pacini, R., Epstein, S., 1999. The relation of rational and experiential information process- nursing care: a narrative review of the literature. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 100, 103403.
ing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. J. Pers. Soc. Witteman, C., van den Bercken, J., Claes, L., Godoy, A., 2009. Assessing rational and intui-
Psychol. 76 (6), 972. tive thinking styles. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 25 (1), 39–47.
Page, A., Halcomb, E., Sim, J., 2021. The impact of nurse leadership education on clinical Zeleníková, R., Gurková, E., Friganovic, A., Uchmanowicz, I., Jarošová, D., Žiaková, K.,
practice: an integrative review. J. Nurs. Manag. 29 (6), 1385–1397. Papastavrou, E., 2020. Unfinished nursing care in four central European countries.
Papastavrou, E., Andreou, P., Efstathiou, G., 2014. Rationing of nursing care and nurse– J. Nurs. Manag. 28 (8), 1888–1900.
patient outcomes: a systematic review of quantitative studies. Int. J. Health Plann. Zhu, Q.M., Liu, J., Hu, H.Y., Wang, S., 2015. Effectiveness of nurse-led early discharge
Manag. 29 (1), 3–25. planning programmes for hospital in patients with chronic disease or rehabilitation
Pavedahl, V., Muntlin, Å., Summer Meranius, M., von Thiele Schwarz, U., Holmström, I.K., needs: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Clin. Nurs. 24 (19–20), 2993–3005.
2022. Prioritizing and meeting life-threateningly ill patients’ fundamental care needs
in the emergency room—an interview study with registered nurses. J. Adv. Nurs. 78
(7), 2165–2174.

You might also like