You are on page 1of 13

933878

research-article20202020
SGOXXX10.1177/2158244020933878SAGE OpenFathi et al.

Original Research

SAGE Open

The Effect of Listening Strategy


April-June 2020: 1­–13
© The Author(s) 2020
DOI: 10.1177/2158244020933878
https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244020933878

Instruction on Second Language journals.sagepub.com/home/sgo

Listening Anxiety and Self-Efficacy


of Iranian EFL Learners

Jalil Fathi1 , Ali Derakhshan2  , and Saeede Torabi3

Abstract
The aim of the present study was to evaluate the effectiveness of listening strategy instruction on second language (L2)
listening comprehension ability, listening anxiety, and listening self-efficacy of Iranian English as a foreign language (EFL)
learners. To this end, a sample of 52 English major learners of two intact classes from a university in Iran was employed as
the participants of the study. The intact groups were randomly assigned to an experimental group and a control group. The
experimental group (N = 27) received the listening strategy instruction based on the framework proposed by Yeldham and
Gruba, whereas the participants in the control group (N = 25) were instructed traditionally without receiving any strategy
instruction. To collect the required data, the listening section of the International English Language Testing System (IELTS),
Foreign Language Listening Anxiety Scale (FLLAS), and Second Language Listening Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (SLLSQ) were
administered to assess the listening comprehension, listening anxiety, and listening self-efficacy of the learners before and
after the intervention. The findings of the study indicated that listening strategy instruction significantly improved learners’
listening comprehension ability and reduced learners’ L2 listening anxiety. However, it was revealed that listening strategy
intervention failed to significantly improve L2 listening self-efficacy of the learners. In light of the gained results, the implications
of this study are discussed with respect to L2 teachers, learners, and curriculum developers.

Keywords
EFL, listening anxiety, listening self-efficacy, listening strategies, strategy instruction

Introduction (Goh, 2017; Graham, 2017; Ngo, 2019; Vandergrift &


Tafaghodtari, 2010; Yeldham, 2016). Listening strategies are
Second or foreign language (L2) listening is a crucial skill subsumed under language learning strategies that are viewed
that provides the L2 learners with the ability to process L2 as procedures, techniques, or deliberate activities carried out
input and to have interaction with speakers of other lan- by learners so as to enhance learning, processing, and remem-
guages in their real-life everyday communications (Xu & bering of both linguistic and schematic knowledge (Chamot,
Huang, 2018). L2 listening is conceptualized as a perceptual 2005). L2 learning strategies have been conceptualized as one
process requiring learners to employ auditory phonetics of the most influential individual difference factors in L2
to organize, detect, and overcome lexical segmentation learning (Lu & Liu, 2011). A significant research base on lan-
in­adequacies (Field, 2003). Moreover, it is a complex and guage learning strategies asserts that teaching language learn-
demanding process that involves invoking both linguistic ing strategies assists language learners in fostering the
knowledge and world knowledge to comprehend the aural effective use of strategies (Chamot, 2005), and that effective
texts (Vandergrift & Baker, 2015). In comparison with other strategy use and L2 achievements are positively correlated
L2 skills such as reading and writing, listening has not been
adequately researched (Goh, 2017). 1
University of Kurdistan, Sanandaj, Iran
The current view toward L2 listening is the fact that it 2
Golestan University, Gorgan, Iran
should not be considered as a skill that is developed naturally 3
Islamic Azad University, Science and Research Branch, Tehran, Iran
on its own, but it is a skill that requires explicit instruction to
Corresponding Author:
be developed (Goh, 2010; Ngo, 2019). As a result of this con- Jalil Fathi, Assistant Professor in TEFL, University of Kurdistan, Pasdaran
ceptualization of the skill, numerous researchers have investi- Street, Sanandaj, Kurdistan 66177-15177, Iran.
gated the effectiveness of strategic instruction for L2 listening Email: jfathi13@yahoo.com

Creative Commons CC BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License
(https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of
the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages
(https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
2 SAGE Open

(Oxford et al., 2004). Listening strategy instruction is regarded acknowledging the fact that listening cannot be naturally
as a means of fostering the learners’ competence in selection developed through just exposure to language input. Given
and coordination of appropriate strategies for their listening the fact that the research evidence on strategy instruction has
objectives and needs so as to be able to comprehend more yielded inconclusive results, and with respect to the dearth of
effectively (Cohen, 2011b). research on listening strategy instruction (Siegel, 2015), the
As research into the use of L2 strategies accumulated, present research can hone our understanding of the effective-
numerous researchers attended to the employment of strate- ness of strategy instruction on the listening comprehension
gies in particular L2 tasks and skills (Cohen, 2011a; Oxford, ability, listening anxiety, and self-efficacy.
1990). Skill-specific strategies are effective in helping As one variable under the investigation of this study, lis-
learners to make up for L2 inadequacies in doing particular tening self-efficacy is mainly based on the assumptions of
L2-related skills or tasks (Nakatani, 2010). In line with this self-efficacy in mainstream education (Graham, 2011).
heightened interest in L2 strategies, listening strategies Vandergrift (2003) persuasively mentioned that empirical
were also received with some research attention by evidence has substantiated that the effective use of metacog-
researchers (Vandergrift, 2003, 2004, 2007). As successful nitive listening strategies plays a pivotal role in successful
comprehension is a function of the balanced coordination listening comprehension, assists learners to boost their self-
and synchrony of top-down and bottom-up strategies regulation and autonomy in listening (Vandergrift, 2002),
(Siegel, 2015), one way to help the learners to be able to and has an important relation with listening self-efficacy
effectively synchronize these processes is strategy use. (Vandergrift, 2005). Considered mainly as the belief in one’s
Furthermore, listening strategies are a set of direct and own capability and being assigned a sense of agency to exert
deliberate techniques employed to improve listening and to control over the surrounding situations and activities
remedy experienced or predicted comprehension inadequa- (Bandura, 1997), self-efficacy, rooted in sociocognitive the-
cies or breakdowns (Field, 2003). Because of the warranted ory, is one of the most important affective variables influ-
merits associated with successful use of listening strategies, encing the individuals’ selections of actions and devotion of
listening strategy instruction programs received research efforts in doing tasks. As far as L2 research is concerned, a
attention by numerous researchers (e.g., Graham, 2017; burgeoning body of empirical evidence has revealed that
Graham & Macaro, 2008; Vandergrift & Baker, 2015; self-efficacy beliefs affect language achievement, are cor-
Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari, 2010). Strategy instruction related with a set of significant learning attributes, and can
programs have been set to provide the listeners with a col- be pedagogically influenced (e.g., Graham & Macaro, 2008;
lection of workable strategies to help them successfully Mills et al., 2007). From Bandura’s (1986) perspective,
carry out listening tasks in real-life contexts (Mendelsohn, sources of self-efficacy consist of mastery experience,
1994). The strategic instructions can be designed to enhance vicarious experience, persuasions, and psychological states.
top-down processes of learners to guess meaning and to As the other variable under the examination of this study,
make more accurate predictions about the aural text L2 listening anxiety is viewed as a category of foreign lan-
(Vandergrift, 2007). These instructional programs should guage anxiety (FLA). Considered as the anxiety felt when a
also pay due attention to bottom-up processes that can context or task needs the use of L2 by learners who are not
enhance meaning-centered comprehension and sentence- very competent language users (MacIntyre & Gardner,
level linguistic processing necessary for successful com- 1994), FLA is claimed to be existing in all dimensions of L2
prehension (Tsui & Fullilove, 1998). such as the four skills (Elkhafaifi, 2005; Horwitz et al., 1986;
With regard to strategy instruction approaches or models, Vogely, 1999). Listening anxiety pervasively exists in doing
the recent intervention studies have used either more explicit L2 listening tasks mainly because of the variables such as
instructional approaches (e.g., Graham & Macaro, 2008) or unintelligibility, perceived difficulty, unfamiliarity of tasks,
more implicit models mainly designed for instruction of and fear of failure in comprehension (Elkhafaifi, 2005). A
metacognitive strategies (e.g., Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari, bulk of L2 listening research states that L2 listening anxiety
2010). Oxford (2011) conceptualizes explicit strategy is separate from global FLA and has detrimental effects on
instruction as a “completely informed strategy training” L2 listening performance (Elkhafaifi, 2005; Vogely, 1999).
(p. 181), in which strategies are named, demonstrated, taught, In spite of the fact that FLA has received much research
and practiced. Learners are made cognizant of metacognitive attention by L2 researchers (Dewaele & MacIntyre, 2014;
knowledge and strategies’ contingent benefits in academic Horwitz et al., 1986; MacIntyre & Gardner, 1994), skill-­
and real-world listening. These strategies can tell us what the specific L2 anxiety (i.e., listening anxiety) has remained
strategy is and what it does and, thus, culminating in the more of a less researched construct (Elkhafaifi, 2005). By
maintenance and transfer of strategies to other contexts and the same token, although numerous studies have indicated
tasks (Carrier, 2003). However, strategy use is integrated in that FLA is closely related with the use of L2 strategies
doing various language tasks in more implicit models (Lu & Liu, 2011; W. Zhang & Liu, 2013), the associations
(Oxford, 1990). The underlying tenet governing either of between L2 skill-specific anxiety and strategy use have not
these models is the necessity of strategy instruction and been widely researched (Liu, 2016).
Fathi et al. 3

The Present Study these theoretical and empirical backdrops and to bridge the
gaps pointed out above, the present study was set to investi-
In spite of the existence of a significant bulk of literature on gate the effects of listening strategy instruction on listening
L2 listening comprehension, research into affective variables ability, listening anxiety, and listening self-efficacy of Iranian
and individual differences in L2 listening is essentially scarce EFL learners.
compared with other skills (Andringa et al., 2012). Because
listening seems to be a formidable and complex skill for
many L2 learners (Graham, 2011), the investigation of the Research Questions
effect of instructional interventions, especially strategy Research Question 1: Does listening strategy instruction
instruction on L2 listening affective factors (i.e., anxiety and significantly enhance Iranian EFL learners’ L2 listening
self-efficacy), is much needed. Because of the complex comprehension ability?
nature of listening process, successful listening is highly reli- Research Question 2: Does listening strategy instruction
ant on an array of individual and affective characteristics of significantly reduce Iranian EFL learners’ L2 listening
L2 listeners (Bang & Hiver, 2016). It is argued that in addi- anxiety?
tion to cognitive dimensions of linguistic knowledge as well Research Question 3: Does listening strategy instruction
as strategy use in L2 listening, affective variables have a sig- significantly improve Iranian EFL learners’ L2 listening
nificant share of variance in positively affecting L2 listening self-efficacy?
success (Vandergrift & Goh, 2012). Affective variables,
which can positively cooperate with other cognitive and lin-
guistics aspects, play a very influential role in enhancing L2 Review of the Related Literature
listening ability (Dewaele & MacIntyre, 2014). Overall, L2 As discussed above, although listening strategy instruction in
listening as well as its psychological factors has remained an L2 and EFL contexts has received some attention by the
underresearched area with relatively few empirical studies. researchers (e.g., Goh, 2017; Graham, 2017; Ngo, 2019;
Therefore, replication studies are called for to shed more Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari, 2010; Yeldham, 2016), an
light on cognitive and affective variables in L2 listening empirical investigation of the effects of strategy instruction
(Vandergrift & Cross, 2017). on affective variables and individual variables related to L2
Furthermore, it is worth noting that the use and effective- listening seems to be absent on L2 listening research agenda.
ness of language leaning strategies are context dependent In this line of enquiry, some studies are found in the litera-
(Griffiths, 2013; Ngo, 2019). From this perspective, strategy ture. For example, Goh and Taib (2006) investigated the
use and strategy instruction may yield different results in the effect of metacognitive listening intervention on a sample of
particular socioeducational context of Iran. Concerning L2 listeners. The participants of this small-scale research
English language instruction in Iran, it is worth noting that included a number of primary school pupils participated in
English language is a compulsory subject in school curricu- the study intervention. The intervention included eight par-
lums from Grade 7 to Grade 12 in public schools. However, ticular lessons encompassing listening exercises, personal
the curriculum for public schools is mainly focused on gram- thinking on their conducted listening activities, and teacher-
mar instruction than on communication skills and it fails to led talks encouraging learners’ evaluation and application of
develop students’ communicative competence effectively their personal and group metacognitive knowledge and strat-
(Dahmardeh, 2009). Therefore, Iranian English as a foreign egy use. Findings revealed that the participants gained a
language (EFL) learners prefer to attend private language more in-depth knowledge of the nature and the requirements
centers so that they can learn spoken language and oral skills of listening. It was also found that learners became more
more effectively (Haghighi & Norton, 2016). Nevertheless, confident in carrying out listening tasks and acquired the
Iranian learners are very unlikely to be exposed to spoken effective strategies to cope with difficult listening tasks.
English outside the class, making it more difficult for them to Similarly, Cross (2009) did a quasi-experimental research to
improve their speaking and listening. In addition, other fac- find out the effectiveness of listening strategy instruction on
tors such as educational facilities, large class sizes, students L2 listening comprehension. The Japanese advanced-level
with heterogeneous language backgrounds, inadequate learners in the experimental group received a 12-hr listening
materials, and inappropriate evaluation procedures have hin- strategy instruction, encompassing the presentation, practice,
dered the effective development of communicative skills. and review of listening strategies, whereas the control group
Moreover, listening instruction in Iran is based on the prod- was taught traditionally with no explicit strategy instruction.
uct approach in which no real instruction may occur, and stu- Results revealed that the experimental group outperformed
dents are just required to listen and answer the questions. In the control group significantly.
the Iranian EFL classrooms, strategy instruction appears to In another study, Graham and Macaro (2008) explored
be absent in L2 listening instruction, which is more con- the impact of teaching strategies on the listening achieve-
cerned with doing listening activities and tasks than with ment and self-efficacy perceptions. The participants of the
enhancing and teaching listening. Therefore, in the light of study were a sample of lower intermediate students of
4 SAGE Open

French in the United Kingdom. The results of the study selected and divided into experimental (n = 30) and control
revealed that listening strategy instruction was effective (n = 25) groups. The students of the experimental group
both in enhancing listening performance and fostering the were exposed to strategy instruction according to the model
self-efficacy of the participants. Moreover, the interplay delineated by Vandergrift and Tafaghodtari (2010) for a
between listening strategy use and listening individual vari- period of eight sessions, whereas the control group was
ables such as anxiety and self-efficacy has been the focus of taught traditionally. To collect the data for the objective of
some correlational studies. For instance, Wong (2005) this research, listening section of Test of English as a Foreign
examined the relationship between language learning strate- Language (TOEFL), the Metacognitive Awareness Listening
gies and language self-efficacy among a number of preser- Questionnaire (MALQ), and Foreign Language Listening
vice teachers. A sample of 74 graduate L2 preservice Anxiety Scale (FLLAS) were also administered to the par-
teachers in Malaysia took part in the present study. The ticipants. The findings of the study indicated that the meta-
results obtained from Pearson correlation coefficients indi- cognitive strategy instruction significantly improved
cated that a significant positive relationship existed between learners’ listening proficiency and metacognitive awareness.
language learning strategies and language self-efficacy. The Furthermore, it was found that strategic instruction contrib-
results of qualitative data from interviews confirmed the uted to reducing listening anxiety of the participants.
findings of the correlational analyses, suggesting that higher By the same token, Rahimirad and Zare-ee (2015)
self-efficacy was correlated with more frequent employ- explored the impact of metacognitive strategy instruction on
ment of more number of language learning. listening self-efficacy among EFL learners. Forty homoge-
In a recent study, Xu and Huang (2018) also examined the nized female students of English literature served as the par-
mediating influence of listening metacognitive awareness ticipants of the study and were randomly assigned to the
between listening anxiety and listening performance, also experimental (n = 20) and the control (n = 20) groups. The
between test anxiety and listening performance. The partici- experimental group received 8 hr of metacognitive strategy
pants of the study were 402 Chinese EFL learners who instruction during eight sessions according to the model
answered the questionnaires. Data analysis from structural introduced by Vandergrift (2003), whereas the control group
equation modeling revealed that metacognitive knowledge was taught traditionally without any explicit strategy instruc-
of listening mediates not only the correlation between listen- tion. The results of this research showed that teaching listen-
ing anxiety and listening performance but also the correla- ing metacognitive strategies significantly improved listening
tion between test-related anxiety and listening performance. self-efficacy of Iranian EFL learners.
Likewise, in a recent study in which the role of context in In a similar line of inquiry, Mohamadpour et al. (2019)
strategy instruction was emphasized, Ngo (2019) investi- recently investigated the effectiveness of metacognitive strat-
gated the effects of a listening strategy instruction program egy instruction in reducing listening anxiety of Iranian EFL
on a sample of 27 Vietnamese EFL learners. The listening learners. Sixty-three intermediate EFL learners who were ran-
strategy instruction program designed based on the needs domly selected and randomly assigned to two experimental
included 22 sessions for a period of 11 weeks. A series of and two control groups served as the participants of the study.
focus group discussions were carried out to explore the learn- As for the strategy instruction framework, two models of
ers’ perceptions of changes in using listening strategies Integrated Experiential Learning Task (IELT; Goh, 2010) and
because of their experience in the strategy instruction pro- Metacognitive Pedagogical Sequence (MPS; Vandergrift,
gram. The findings obtained from the focus group interviews 2004) were employed. All the students in the four classes
indicated that the participants reported to be able to use lis- were instructed by the same teacher, and the same listening
tening strategies more effectively. These changes and effects comprehension materials were used for all the groups. One of
of strategy instruction were explained in the light of cultural the experimental classes was taught listening strategies based
context of English learning and teaching in Vietnam. Overall, on IELT and the other experimental group was taught accord-
the findings of this study underscores a more learner-cen- ing to MPS model, whereas both control groups were taught
tered atmosphere in which the learners are scaffolded and based on the traditional product-oriented listening instruction.
mediated by their instructor, peers, and materials to enhance To collect the data, FLLAS and the listening component of
their use of listening strategies. Preliminary English Test (PET) were given to all groups
before and after the instruction. The findings of the study
revealed that both IELT and MPS were useful in reducing lis-
Empirical Studies in the Iranian Context
tening anxiety. However, it was found that the MPS was more
Carrying out a recent study in the Iranian context, Movahed effective in lowering listening anxiety of the learners.
(2014) examined the effects of metacognitive strategy In another study, Golzadeh and Moiinvaziri (2017)
instruction on the listening achievement, metacognitive abil- explored the interplay between listening anxiety and meta-
ity, and listening anxiety of EFL learners. A number of 55 cognitive strategy awareness among 105 Iranian EFL learn-
Iranian EFL learners studying English translation were the ers. The participants were of upper intermediate and
participants of the study. The participants were randomly advanced proficiency levels. To gather the required data for
Fathi et al. 5

Table 1.  Distribution of the Participants Across Each Group. Table 2.  Results of the OPT for Each Group.

Groups Number Male Female Groups M (SD) T Significance


Experimental group 27 9 18 Experimental 42.66 (11.35) −0.58 .56
Control group 25 10 15 Control 44.50 (11.14)  
Total number 52 19 33
Note. OPT = Oxford Placement Test.

this correlational study, MALQ and FLLAS were adminis- elementary students (A2). Students whose scores are between
tered. The results of data analysis revealed that a negative 30 and 39 are in the lower intermediate group (B1). Those
correlation existed between the learners’ listening anxiety students with scores of 40 to 47 are labeled as upper interme-
and their metacognitive strategy use. diate (B2) and the students with scores 48 to 54 and 54 to 60
are categorized as advanced (C1) and very sophisticated (C2)
Method levels, respectively. The internal consistency of OPT as esti-
mated by Cronbach’s alpha was reported to be .86 in this
This study is a quantitative research in nature. Because it was study. OPT is a time-saving and reliable English language
not possible to select and divide the participants randomly in proficiency test, which was developed by Cambridge Eng-
this research context (Islamic Azad University), quasi-exper- lish for Speakers of Other Languages (ESOL) and Oxford
imental design was used. This design is usually employed in University Press and was validated in 20 countries by more
case it is not logistically possible to carry out a randomized, than 6,000 students.
controlled trial study (Ary et al., 2019). In this design, par- To compare the OPT mean scores of the two groups, an
ticipants are not randomly assigned to groups. Two intact independent-samples t test was carried out. The results
classes served as the participants of this quasi-experimental presented in Table 2 demonstrated that there was no statis-
research design. tically significant difference in between the mean scores of
the experimental group (M = 42.66, SD = 11.35) and the
Participants control group (M = 44.50, SD = 11.14), t(50) = −0.58,
p > .05, revealing that both groups were homogeneous in
To achieve the aims of this research, a sample of 52 English terms of their general English ability before initiating the
major learners from two intact classes at one of the branches intervention.
of Islamic Azad University in Iran were recruited as the par-
ticipants of this research (see Table 1). They were both male International English Language Testing System (IELTS) listening
(N = 19) and female (N = 33) learners whose age ranged section.  To measure the listening comprehension ability of
from 19 to 24 years. The two intact groups were randomly the participants prior to the intervention and after the inter-
assigned to an experimental group (N = 27) and a control vention, the IELTS listening practice tests (Scovell et al.,
group (N = 25). To make sure about the homogeneity of the 2004) were administered. There are four parts in the IELTS
participants with regard to their English proficiency, Oxford listening module. The first two parts are concerned with top-
Placement Test (OPT; Allan, 2004) was given to the students ics of general interest. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability
of both groups. The scores of students on OPT revealed that coefficient indices for the pre- and posttests were .79 and
the participants can be categorized as upper intermediate in .82, respectively.
general English proficiency. All the participants had at least
a 5-year experience of learning English in language learning FLLAS.  To measure listening anxiety of the participants
institutes. Both groups were instructed by the same instruc- before and after the intervention, FLLAS that was devel-
tor, and an identical textbook and the same learning materials oped by Kim (2000) was administered to the participants.
were used for both classes. The strategy instruction interven- FLLAS is a self-report scale in the form of a 5-point
tion lasted for about 16 weeks. Likert-type questionnaire designed mainly according to
Horwitz et al.’s (1986) scale, which was developed and
validated to measure general FLA. FLLAS includes 33
Instruments
items measuring three dimensions of L2 listening anxiety:
English proficiency test.  OPT (Allan, 2004) was used to inves- tension and worry (10 items), lack of confidence (seven
tigate the homogeneity of the students of both groups in items), and problems encountered (16 items). Kim (2000)
terms of their general English proficiency. OPT can be given reported the internal consistency of the scale to be .93. The
to various learners of different abilities to identify their lev- reliability coefficient of the scale, as measured by Cron-
els. OPT consists of a six-rating scale; students whose scores bach’s alpha formula, was .81 in the present study. With
lie between 0 and 17 are categorized as basic (A1), and stu- respect to its validity, it has been administered in the Ira-
dents whose scores fall between 18 and 29 are considered as nian context by other researchers.
6 SAGE Open

Second Language Listening Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (SLLSQ).  Table 3.  Description of Listening Strategy–Based Instruction.
To assess L2 listening self-efficacy of the participants, SLLSQ
Phases Description of activities
that was developed and designed by Kassem (2015) was
employed in the present study. This scale includes 40 items in Preparation - Teacher made students think about the
the form of a 5-point Likert-type scale measuring five under- strategies they had learned before (or in the
lying subscales: progress (one’s perception of present perfor- previous session)
mance in comparison with past performance), observational Presentation - By thinking aloud, teacher modeled how to
employ a particular strategy with examples
comparison (one’s perception of his or her performance in
- Teacher provided a definition of the listening
comparison with that of peers), physiological states (the strategy and clarified when and how to
learner’s feelings while doing listening tasks), strategic employ the strategy
awareness (learner’s ability in dealing with listening task and - Students were asked to discuss the
overcoming difficulties), and challenge (the learner’s willing- presented strategy with peers
ness to do such a task). The internal consistency of the ques- Practice - Students were required to practice using the
tionnaire, as estimated by Cronbach’s alpha formula, was strategies they had learned in doing listening
reported to be .79 in this study. tasks
- Students had peer discussions on which
strategies to be selected and how to use
Procedure them
- Teacher provided the students with
The present research was a part of a bigger project wherein necessary support if they had difficulty doing
the effects of a listening strategy instruction program on listening tasks or selecting appropriate
several L2 listening cognitive and affective variables were strategies
investigated. Nevertheless, this article reports the details of a Evaluation - Students had reflection on their own
section of the procedure in which the effect of strategy strategy use and their comprehension of
instruction on listening anxiety and self-efficacy was under listening texts
- Students provided comments on strategy
investigation.
use and listening comprehension of their
One session prior to initiating the intervention of the peers
study, the English proficiency test (OPT) was given to the Expansion - Students were given further listening tasks
learners of both groups. The purpose was to make sure that as homework
the intact groups are not of different level of language profi- - Students were encouraged to use all their
ciency. Besides, the listening section of the IELTS was learned strategies on their own and develop
administered to measure the listening comprehension ability their individual combination of listening
of the students before (i.e., as pretest) and after (i.e., as post- strategies while doing listening assignments
outside the class
test) the strategy instruction. Then, the two scales of listening
anxiety and listening self-efficacy were administered to the
students as the pretests of the study.
During the first week of the intervention for the experi- consists of three categories of top-down, bottom-up, and
mental group, the teacher discussed the significance of teach- metacognitive set of strategies. Concerning the top-down
ing listening strategies very briefly and provided the learners strategies, the learners of the experimental group were taught
with a general description of the instructional program for on how to make predictions about aural content and lexical
the whole strategy instruction period. He also briefly items, infer and deduce the meaning of unknown vocabular-
explained the strategy instruction framework utilized in the ies in texts, guess the new aspects of text, make use of con-
program. Yeldham and Gruba’s (2014) model was adopted as textual clues to understand the aural text, and utilize textual
the framework for strategy instruction in the current research. markers to predict complicated content and the succeeding
Yeldham and Gruba believed that such a mixture could be content. As for the bottom-up strategies, the learners were
more productive and could solve the likely shortcomings instructed on how to make use of cues and jot down key-
in either approaches. They maintained that a totally embed- words during listening activities to improve their own com-
ded approach can be tedious for learners (Vandergrift & prehension, use discourse and text markers to enhance their
Tafaghodtari, 2010), whereas very explicit procedures might understanding, and recognize stressed content words in spo-
be too prescriptive too be interesting to learners. ken language. Regarding the metacognitive strategies, the
The strategy teaching framework employed for the pres- students were taught on how to grasp an overview of various
ent study was cognitive academic language learning approach listening strategies, concentrate on metacognitive orchestra-
(CALLA; Chamot & O’malley, 1994) developed by Chamot tion of different strategies, and monitor more successfully
et al. (1999), constituting five key phases of preparation, pre- and productively, for example, how to monitor text for mal-
sentation, practice, evaluation, and expansion (see Table 3). formed utterances and to monitor utterances including con-
Moreover, the strategy instruction model used for this study fusing schema (Yeldham & Gruba, 2014). Following what
Fathi et al. 7

Vandergrift et al. (2006) identify, metacognitive listening Table 4. Paired-Samples t Test for Listening Scores in Each
strategies are categorized into five types of strategies, encom- Group.
passing problem solving, planning and evaluation, mental Pretest Posttest
translation, person knowledge, and directed attention.
Vandergrift et al. mention that problem solving manifests a Groups M SD M SD t Significance
group of strategies that listeners need to draw on to predict Experimental 17.77 5.70 22.93 6.42 −5.84 .00
what they do not infer in the process of listening and to moni- Control 18.43 5.14 20.23 6.47 −2.36 .02
tor these inferences. Planning and evaluation strategies per-
tain to those strategies that listeners utilize to prepare
themselves for listening tasks and to evaluate the results of Finally, at the end of the completion of the strategy
their listening efforts. They further argue that mental transla- instruction intervention, the learners of both groups were
tions are those strategies that listeners need to avoid if they invited to answer FLLAS and L2 Listening Self-Efficacy
want to become tactful listeners (Vandergrift, 2003). Person Questionnaire as the posttests of the study. It is noteworthy
knowledge strategies encompass listeners’ perceptions and that during both pretest and posttest, the teacher was present
attitudes with respect to the difficulty of the listening task to clarify any sort of misunderstanding while completing the
and their self-efficacy about second language (L2) listening. questionnaires.
Directed attention entails strategies that listeners embark on
to concentrate and stay on listening task. Vandergrift et al.
postulate that the listeners’ awareness of these five types of Data Analysis
strategies that can help them regulate the process of L2 lis-
tening comprehension. To answer the research questions, three one-way between-
During the sessions of strategy instruction intervention, groups analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) were performed.
listening strategic instruction was incorporated into the regu- According to Pallant (2013), ANCOVA can be used when
lar instruction. This was carried out using the instructional there is a pretest/posttest design (e.g., comparing the effects
cycle proposed by Vandergrift (2007) to assist learners in of two different interventions, taking before and after mea-
coordinating their used strategies. In these sessions, the stu- sures for each group). The scores on the pretest are consid-
dents were also taught on how to make predictions about the ered as a covariate to “control” for preexisting differences
content of the aural texts after having listened to its initial between the groups. Moreover, to demonstrate the within-
utterances or to identify its topic. In addition, upon being group change from pretest to posttest for each variable,
exposed to the text for the first time, the learners checked paired-samples t tests were also carried out.
their predictions as well as their guesses and compared their
understanding of the text with that of their peers. When they
Results
listened for the second time, they reexamined their own
understanding, and talked about their comprehension and Research Question 1: Does Listening Strategy
interpretations of the aural text with their classmates. While Instruction Significantly Enhance Iranian EFL
listening for the third time, the learners reflected on their
Learners’ L2 Listening Comprehension Ability?
comprehension and made planning for similar subsequent
listening activities. In the meantime, the learners in the con- Because participants’ listening comprehension ability plays
trol group were taught traditionally without receiving any an indispensable role before and after the intervention as one
strategy instruction. By traditional, we mean that they did not of the variables of the study, paired-samples t tests were car-
work on different types of three categories of top-down, ried out to compare the listening scores in the pretest and
bottom-up, and metacognitive set of strategies. Moreover, posttest. The results demonstrated that there was a significant
the teacher did not have any discussion on strategy use, nei- increase of the mean scores for both experimental and control
ther did he engage the participants in any systematic attempt groups. Table 4 reveals that the listening mean scores of the
to reflect on their employed approach to listening. More par- experimental group significantly improved (t = −5.84, p <
ticularly, the traditional listening instruction was based on .05); moreover, the performance of the control group statisti-
the listen–answer–check (Vandergrift, 2004) approach, in cally improved (t = −2.36, p < .05). The results indicated
which the participants were required to listen to numerous that the mean score of the experimental group was 17.77 on
aural texts and then answer the following listening compre- the pretest and it increased to 22.93 on the posttest, which was
hension questions. The aural texts were either dialogues or statistically significant. Likewise, the listening pretest mean
monologues with various task demands including multiple- score for the control group rose from 18.43 to 20.23 on the
choice items, fill-in-the-blank questions, and open-ended posttest, which led to the significant difference.
items (i.e., responding to Wh-questions). The control group In addition, a one-way between-groups ANCOVA was
received the same listening activities and tasks from the run to find out the effects of the two types of listening instruc-
identical course book. tions utilized in the control group and the experimental
8 SAGE Open

Table 5.  ANCOVA Results for Listening Comprehension Scores.

Source Type III sum of squares df M square F Significance Partial η2


Corrected model 293.766 2 146.883 9.115 .000 .271
Intercept 216.441 1 216.441 13.432 .001 .215
Pretest listening 198.991 1 198.991 12.349 .001 .201
Group 122.007 1 122.007 7.571 .008 .134
Error 789.595 49 16.114  
Total 25,423.168 52  
Corrected total 1,083.361 51  

Note. ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.

Table 6. Paired-Samples t Test for L2 Listening Anxiety Scores. instruction (i.e., strategy-based instruction or traditional),
and the dependent variable was the scores of the participants
Pretest Posttest
on the posttest of listening anxiety. The pretest scores of
Groups M SD M SD t Significance listening anxiety were considered as the covariate in the
ANCOVA. Preliminary analyses were conducted to make
Experimental 44.37 10.56 37.00 10.31 37.03 .00
sure that the assumptions of normality, linearity, homogene-
Control 45.62 11.79 43.08 11.43 9.17 .00
ity of variances, homogeneity of regression slopes, and the
reliable measurement of the covariate were not violated. The
results of the ANCOVA (see Table 7) show that a statistically
group. The strategy instruction was the independent variable
significant difference existed between the two groups in the
and the dependent variable was the scores on the listening
mean scores on the posttest of L2 listening anxiety, F(1, 49)
test; students’ scores on the pretest of the listening test served
= 225.10, p = .000, partial η2 = .82. These results reveal
as the covariate in this analysis. The researchers were ascer-
that teaching L2 listening strategies contributed to reducing
tained that there was no violation of normality, linearity,
listening anxiety of the experimental group more than the
homogeneity of variances, homogeneity of regression slopes,
traditional L2 listening instruction.
and reliable measurement of the covariate. As Table 5 illus-
trates, there was a significant difference between the two
groups on posttest scores of listening comprehension ability, Research Question 3: Does Listening Strategy
F(1, 49) = 7.57, p = .008, partial η2 = .13. Instruction Significantly Improve Iranian EFL
Learners’ L2 Listening Self-Efficacy?
Research Question 2: Does Listening Strategy The third research question aimed to examine the effect of
Instruction Significantly Reduce Iranian EFL listening strategy instruction on EFL learners’ L2 listening
self-efficacy. To answer this question, a paired-samples t test
Learners’ L2 Listening Anxiety?
was conducted to investigate the mean scores of L2 listening
To investigate the effect of listening strategy instruction on self-efficacy for both the experimental and control groups
listening anxiety of the participants, a paired-samples t test from the pretest to posttest. In fact, the purpose of this analy-
was carried out to compare the mean scores of both groups sis was to investigate the effects of the strategy-based instruc-
from the pretest to posttest. As presented in Table 6, the tion and traditional listening instruction on the listening
results show that there was a statistically significant change self-efficacy of the participants. As Table 8 indicates, there
from the pretest to posttest of L2 listening anxiety for both was a statistically significant increase in the mean scores of
the experimental group, t(26) = 37.03, p < .00, and the con- L2 listening self-efficacy from the pretest to posttest for both
trol group, t(24) = 9.17, p < .00. As seen in Table 6, the lis- the experimental group, t(26) = −2.25, p < .05, and the con-
tening anxiety mean score for the experimental group trol group, t(24) = −2.39, p < .05. As presented in Table 6,
decreased from 44.37 on the pretest to 37.00 on the posttest. the mean score of L2 listening self-efficacy for the experi-
Similarly, the listening anxiety mean score of the control mental group increased from 46.77 on the pretest to 50.74 on
group decreased from 45.62 on the pretest to 43.08 on the the posttest. Likewise, the listening self-efficacy mean scores
posttest. These results indicated that listening anxiety of both of the control group was raised from 44.04 on the pretest to
groups was significantly reduced. 48.56 on the posttest.
In addition, a one-way between-groups ANCOVA was Furthermore, a one-way between-groups ANCOVA was
carried out to compare the effects of the two types of L2 lis- also run to compare the effectiveness of the two types of L2
tening instructions employed in the present study on the L2 listening instruction. The type of intervention (i.e., strat-
listening anxiety. The independent variable was the type of egy-based instruction or traditional) was the independent
Fathi et al. 9

Table 7.  ANCOVA Results for L2 Listening Anxiety Scores.

Source Type III sum of squares df M square F Significance Partial η2


Corrected model 6,316.842 2 3,158.421 2,315.054 .000 .990
Intercept 34.839 1 34.839 25.536 .000 .343
Pretest anxiety 5,836.989 1 5,836.989 4,278.387 .000 .989
Group 307.110 1 307.110 225.105 .000 .821
Error 66.851 49 1.364  
Total 89,264.000 52  
Corrected total 6,383.692 51  

Note. ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.

Table 8. Paired-Samples t Test for L2 Listening Self-Efficacy. proved to be effective in developing learners’ comprehension
ability. Because this model is a combination of the explicit
Pretest Posttest
and embedded approaches, it yields the most successful
Groups M SD M SD t Significance results, which have been substantiated by other studies
(Oxford, 2011; O’Malley & Chamot, 1990). Unlike most
Experimental 46.77 8.08 50.74 8.23 −2.25 .03
previous studies that focused on few participants and have
Control 44.04 6.14 48.56 8.02 −2.39 .02
focused on the explicit teaching of strategies (Yeldham &
Gruba’s, 2014), the present study was conducted with a big-
ger sample of Iranian EFL students of upper intermediate
variable, and the learners’ scores on the posttest of L2 lis- proficiency level and employed a quasi-experimental design.
tening self-efficacy served as the dependent variable. Also, Furthermore, the previously conducted bodies of research
the learners’ scores on the pretest of L2 listening self-­ focused on the metacognitive strategies and top-down pro-
efficacy were considered as the covariate in the ANCOVA. cessing (e.g., Goh, 1998; Vandergrift, 1997), but this study
Preliminary analyses revealed that there was no violation of employed bottom-up skills and strategies to teach listening
normality, linearity, homogeneity of variances, homogene- as well. This finding is consistent with that of Yeldham and
ity of regression slopes, and reliable measurement of the Gruba, concluding that drawing on just bottom-up strategy
covariate. The results of the ANCOVA (see Table 9) indi- instruction, without the integration of top-down skills and
cated that there was no statistically significant difference strategies, is less likely to boost the learners’ listening com-
between the two groups in the mean scores on the posttest prehension. This study embarked upon presenting and mod-
of L2 listening self-efficacy; F(1, 49) = 0.358, p = .552, eling strategies, and students were given enough practice,
partial η2 = .00. These results show that listening strategy which ultimately resulted in the better listening performance
instruction failed to improve Iranian EFL learners’ L2 lis- of the participants.
tening self-efficacy. The improved listening comprehension of the partici-
pants in the experimental group may be justified in light of
sociocultural theory (Vygotsky, 1978), in a sense that, listen-
Discussion and Conclusion
ing strategy use was mediated by the scaffolding of the
This research sought to examine the effects of teaching lis- teacher as a more capable person in the learning process of
tening strategies on the listening comprehension, listening the students (Gardner, 2010). Explaining and modeling the
anxiety, and listening self-efficacy of Iranian EFL learners. strategy use, the teacher helped the EFL learners to use lis-
To accomplish the objectives of this study, Yeldham and tening strategies more effectively. As such, the learners
Gruba’s (2014) model of strategy instruction was incorpo- might have been empowered to fill the gap between the
rated into a 16-week treatment period. Results of the first strategies they knew and their actual competence for using
research question indicated that the experimental group sig- them, an effective situation that helped the learner to
nificantly outperformed the control group on the listening improve their listening comprehension.
performance test, corroborating that the listening strategy The results of the second research question demonstrated
instruction may be effective in boosting participants’ listen- that listening strategy instruction significantly reduced
ing comprehension. The findings of this study resonate with Iranian EFL learners’ listening anxiety. This finding concurs
those of previous studies (Graham & Macaro, 2008; with a significant body of L2 listening research in emphasiz-
Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari, 2010; Yeldham & Gruba, 2014) ing the role of strategy instruction in decreasing listening
and are not in alignment with those of Rees-Miller’s (1993) anxiety (Goh & Taib, 2006; Mohamadpour et al., 2019;
and Rossiter’s (2003). Furthermore, the findings of this study Movahed, 2014; Vandergrift, 2007). Such a finding also veri-
are suggestive of the fact that Yeldham and Gruba’s model fies the findings of correlational studies (e.g., Golzadeh &
10 SAGE Open

Table 9.  ANCOVA Results for L2 Listening Self-Efficacy.

Source Type III sum of squares df M square F Significance Partial η2


Corrected model 309.128 2 154.564 2.475 .095 .092
Intercept 1,568.588 1 1,568.588 25.118 .000 .339
Pretest self-efficacy 247.397 1 247.397 3.962 .052 .075
Group 22.373 1 22.373 0.358 .552 .007
Error 3,059.948 49 62.448  
Total 131,774.000 52  
Corrected total 3,369.077 51  

Note. ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.

Moiinvaziri, 2017; Xu & Huang, 2018), which indicated that listening anxiety. The effect of strategy use in reducing L2
a negative correlation exists between strategy use and listen- anxiety has been acknowledged in L2 literature (Lu & Liu,
ing anxiety. This finding is attributed to some reasons. First 2011). The findings of the present study are also partially
and foremost, one key cause of listening anxiety in L2 is consistent with those of Sioson (2011), who found that the
exposure to new vocabularies (Vogely, 1998; X. Zhang, use of metacognitive language strategies could reduce L2
2013). As the strategy instruction program helped the listeners anxiety by enabling the learners to become more competent
to make informed guesses about the meaning of unfamiliar in setting goals, planning, and self-monitoring their learning
words and have less difficulty processing new vocabularies, process. This also verifies the claim made by Vandergrift
the participants of the experimental group felt less listening (2002), who stated that metacognitive instruction or raising
anxiety after receiving strategy instruction. Guessing the learners’ metacognitive awareness can equip the L2 learners
meanings from the text that has been frequently employed by with listening competencies to be able to successfully trans-
the communicative L2 teaching approaches is reported to be fer what they have learned in listening tasks to authentic tar-
conducive in reducing listening anxiety (X. Zhang, 2013). In get language situations outside the class.
addition, strategy instruction might have increased the par- More particularly, the findings of the present study con-
ticipants’ risk-taking level, which encouraged the learners firmed the effectiveness of the adopted strategy instruction
not to be resistant in guessing incorrectly or making mistakes framework (Yeldham & Gruba, 2014) in lowering listening
in L2 classrooms (Yan & Horwitz, 2008). This increased risk anxiety. One likely reason might be the fact that this frame-
taking of the participants could have reduced listening anxi- work was a composite of the three sets of top-down, bottom-
ety of the learners. up, and metacognitive strategies. It is argued that overreliance
Moreover, as the listening strategy instruction involved on limited bottom-up strategies makes the learners become
further interaction among students and the teacher through too preoccupied with word-level processing and consume
teacher’s regular feedback, the participants perceived less much attentional processing at the expense of ignoring higher
distance between themselves and the teacher. Therefore, a order semantic processing of the text (Field, 2003). However,
more comfortable and nonthreatening learning environment Yeldham and Gruba’s (2014) model helped EFL learners to
was created for the learners who enjoyed doing listening use their top-down strategies and schematic knowledge to
tasks and might have reduced their listening anxiety. This process the aural texts more effectively. In the meantime,
stress-free learning environment is likely to have changed metacognitive strategies of the model encouraged the learn-
the perceptions of participants on listening and to have cre- ers to plan, monitor, and orchestrate their employed strate-
ated positive attitudes among them. It is also likely that par- gies more effectively. Therefore, the perceived success in
ticipants of the experimental group, perceiving the less comprehending texts, accompanied by less cognitive load
distance with the teacher, might have verbalized their feel- due to receiving strategic instruction, is likely to have
ings and shared their listening difficulties with the teacher reduced the anxiety of the participants. This is consistent
who could have provided them with solutions or workable with the findings of I. Chen and Chang (2009) who found
strategies, which contributed to reducing their listening that further cognitive load and less working memory pro-
anxiety. cessing could increase L2 listening anxiety.
Furthermore, it can be argued that strategy instruction Moreover, concerning the third research question, it was
might have encouraged the participants to manage and moni- revealed that teaching L2 listening strategies failed to
tor their learning, take responsibility of their learning, and improve listening self-efficacy of the students more than the
gain more self-confidence in doing learning tasks, thereby traditional listening instruction. This finding is at variance
reducing their anxiety. Appropriate strategy use may assign with numerous previous studies (e.g., Graham & Macaro,
the learners the competence and confidence to take control of 2008; Rahimirad & Zare-ee, 2015; Wong, 2005) revealing
their own learning process and feel more autonomy (Siegel, that strategic instruction can affect self-efficacy of the par-
2015), all of which might have contributed to reducing ticipants. The listening self-efficacy of the two groups in the
Fathi et al. 11

present study increased in the posttest, and there was no sig- the effectiveness of strategic instruction in culminating in the
nificant difference between the two groups. This finding most favorable outcomes in both cognitive and affective
might be accounted for by the fact that both groups were variables related to the particular L2-related tasks or skills
engaged in experiencing listening activities by which they (Oxford, 1990). Furthermore, L2 teacher education programs
have gained a sense of mastery that contributed to increasing should equip the practitioners with necessary theoretical and
listening self-efficacy of both groups. Although the control practical competencies to be able to effectively implement
group did not get exposed to any strategy instruction, they strategic instructions in their classrooms. This issue becomes
had adequate exposure to various listening tasks and activi- more crucial as far as listening instruction is concerned
ties during their instruction. Successful experience of the because it is argued that teachers play an influential role in
control group students in doing listening activities has served teaching listening effectively (Graham & Santos, 2015).
as a key source of listening self-efficacy for the experimental
and control groups. According to Bandura’s (1997) notion of Limitations and Directions for Further Research
self-efficacy grounded in sociocognitive theory, mastery
experience or successful doing of something acts as the most Given the fact that L2 listening has been more underre-
significant source of self-efficacy beliefs. Moreover, the two searched than other skills (Vandergrift & Goh, 2012) as well
groups were taught by the same teacher who tried to encour- as given the invisible and intricate nature of listening process
age the students of both groups by providing them with rela- (Vandergrift & Cross, 2017), doing further empirical research
tively identical verbal persuasion as the other source of on L2 listening and its related psychological factors (i.e.,
self-efficacy. Another plausible reason for the insignificant anxiety, self-efficacy, and motivation) across different con-
difference between the experimental and control groups’ texts using various interventions seems to be very warranted.
level of listening self-efficacy might be due to the relatively In addition, because strategy use is a covert process, some
short period of the instruction. However, the treatment con- researchers have recommended using qualitative research
ducted by Graham and Macaro (2008), which confirmed the procedures to gain a more comprehensive and in-depth
effectiveness of strategy instruction in enhancing listening insight into learners’ use of listening strategies (Y. Chen,
self-efficacy, lasted for a period of 6 months. 2005; Ngo, 2019). More specifically, the use of quantitative
Moreover, it is argued that students’ verbalization of self-report scales for measuring affective variables may not
listening strategies after being modeled by a teacher could provide a deep insight into the effects of strategy instruction
improve listeners’ self-efficacy because verbalization on such affective variables as anxiety and self-efficacy. Also,
encouraged learners to pay more attention to the taught strat- participants’ general sense of efficacy and academic self-
egies and foster their encoding (Graham, 2011; Schunk & efficacy might have affected the results of this study with
Rice, 1984). The participants of the experimental group did regard to the effectiveness of strategy instruction on listening
not practice to repeat the used strategies verbally. In fact, the self-efficacy. As a result, future researchers may investigate
teacher modeled the use of listening strategies and explained the moderating effects of general and academic self-efficacy
them out loud, but the participants did not repeat them ver- on L2 listening self-efficacy.
bally prior to implementing them to listening activities. This Future researchers are recommended to replicate similar
may be considered as another possible justification for the studies using qualitative or mixed-methods research designs.
fact that listening strategy instruction did not significantly The use of stimulated recalls and think-aloud protocols can
enhance L2 listening self-efficacy of the learners. help future researchers to cast a better picture of the effect of
As far as the Iranian EFL context is concerned, L2 practi- listening strategy instruction programs on listening affective
tioners seem to be neither competent nor willing to teach lis- variables such as anxiety and self-efficacy. Also, future
tening strategies in their own classrooms (Eslami & Ranjbary, researchers may employ delayed posttests to investigate
2003). Nor do EFL curriculum developers and syllabus whether the impacts of listening strategy instruction could be
designers pay adequate attention to teaching listening strate- maintained over time. Finally, future researchers can exam-
gies explicitly. Most of the listening class hours are devoted ine the effect of other models of listening strategy instruction
to completing tasks and doing activities by the students. As a on listening ability, anxiety, and self-efficacy of bigger sam-
result, focusing on just the end points and products in the ples of learners at different levels of language proficiency
form of carried-out listening exercises, activities, and tasks is using different listening tasks with various genres.
likely to increase listening anxiety of the EFL learners.
Therefore, integration of listening strategic instruction into Declaration of Conflicting Interests
regular classrooms might be a promising venue for EFL The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
teachers not only to improve the listening proficiency of their to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
students but also to reduce their anxiety as well as enhance
their motivation and self-efficacy in doing listening tasks. Funding
By the same token, EFL teachers should inculcate in their The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
pupils a positive attitude toward using strategies to increase ship, and/or publication of this article.
12 SAGE Open

ORCID iDs Field, J. (2003). Promoting perception: Lexical segmentation in L2


listening. ELT Journal, 57(4), 325–334.
Jalil Fathi   https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1146-1024
Gardner, R. C. (2010). Motivation and second language acquisi-
Ali Derakhshan   https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6639-9339 tion: The socio-educational model. Peter Lang.
Goh, C. C. M. (1998). How ESL learners with different listening
References abilities use comprehension strategies and tactics. Language
Allan, D. (2004). Oxford placement test. Oxford University Press. Teaching Research, 2, 124–147.
Andringa, S., Olsthoorn, N., van Beuningen, C., Schoonen, R., Goh, C. C. M. (2010). Listening as process: Learning activities
& Hulstijn, J. (2012). Determinants of success in native and for self-appraisal and self-regulation. In N. Harwood (Ed.),
non-native listening comprehension: An individual differences English language teaching materials: Theory and practice
approach. Language Learning, 62(1), 49–78. (pp. 179–206). Cambridge University Press.
Ary, D., Jacobs, L. C., Sorensen, C., & Walker, D. (2019). Goh, C. C. M. (2017). Cognition, metacognition, and L2 listening.
Introduction to research in education (10th ed.). Cengage. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook of research in second language
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A teaching and learning (pp. 214–228). Routledge.
social cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall. Goh, C. C. M., & Taib, Y. (2006). Metacognitive instruction in
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control (pp. 3– listening for young learners. ELT Journal, 60(3), 222–232.
604). W.H. Freeman. Golzadeh, N., & Moiinvaziri, M. (2017). Metacognitive strategy
Bang, S., & Hiver, P. (2016). Investigating the structural rela- awareness and listening anxiety: The role of gender and profi-
tionships of cognitive and affective domains for L2 listen- ciency level among Iranian EFL learners. International Journal
ing. Asian-pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language of Foreign Language Teaching and Research, 5(19), 91–109.
Education, 1(1), Article 7. Graham, S. (2011). Self-efficacy and academic listening. Journal of
Carrier, K. A. (2003). Improving high school English language English for Academic Purposes, 10(2), 113–117.
learners’ second language listening through strategy instruc- Graham, S. (2017). Research into practice: Listening strategies in
tion. Bilingual Research Journal, 27(3), 383–408. an instructed classroom setting. Language Teaching, 50(1),
Chamot, A. U. (2005). Language learning strategy instruction: Current 107–119.
issues and research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, Graham, S., & Macaro, E. (2008). Strategy instruction in listening
112–130. for lower-intermediate learners of French. Language Learning,
Chamot, A. U., Barnhardt, S., El-Dinary, P. D., & Robbins, J. 58(4), 747–783.
(1999). The learning strategies handbook. Pearson Education. Graham, S., & Santos, D. (2015). Strategies for second language
Chamot, A. U., & O’malley, J. M. (1994). The CALLA handbook: listening: Current scenarios and improved pedagogy. Springer.
Implementing the cognitive academic language learning Griffiths, C. (2013). The strategy factor in successful language
approach. Addison-Wesley. learning. Multilingual Matters.
Chen, I., & Chang, C. C. (2009). Cognitive load theory: An empiri- Haghighi, F. M., & Norton, B. (2016). The role of English language
cal study of anxiety and task performance in language learning. institutes in Iran. TESOL Quarterly, 51(2), 428–438.
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. B., & Cope, J. (1986). Foreign language
7(2), 729–746. classroom anxiety. The Modern Language Journal, 70(1),
Chen, Y. (2005). A qualitative study on the effect of language learn- 125–132.
ing strategy training [Paper presentation]. The Conference Kassem, H. M. (2015). The relationship between listening strategies
on English Teaching and Learning, National Taiwan Normal used by Egyptian EFL college sophomores and their listening
University, Taipei City. comprehension and self-efficacy. English Language Teaching,
Cohen, A. D. (2011a). Second language learner strategies. In E. 8(2), 153–169.
Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook of research in second language teach- Kim, J. H. (2000). Foreign language listening anxiety: A study of
ing and learning (Vol. II, pp. 681–698). Routledge. Korean students learning English (Unpublished doctoral dis-
Cohen, A. D. (2011b). Strategies in learning and using a second sertation). The University of Texas at Austin.
language (2nd ed.). Longman. Liu, M. (2016). Interrelations between foreign language listening
Cross, J. (2009). Effects of listening strategy instruction on news anxiety and strategy use and their predicting effects on test per-
videotext comprehension. Language Teaching Research, 13, formance of high-and low-proficient Chinese university EFL
151–176. learners. The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher, 25(4), 647–655.
Dahmardeh, M. (2009). English language teaching in Iran and Lu, Z., & Liu, M. (2011). Foreign language anxiety and strategy
communicative language teaching (Unpublished doctoral dis- use: A study with Chinese undergraduate EFL learners. Journal
sertation). University of Warwick, Coventry. of Language Teaching and Research, 2(6), 1298–1305.
Dewaele, J. M., & MacIntyre, P. D. (2014). The two faces of Janus? MacIntyre, P. D., & Gardner, R. C. (1994). The effects of induced
Anxiety and enjoyment in the foreign language classroom. Studies anxiety on three stages of cognitive processing in computerized
in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 4(2), 237–274. vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
Elkhafaifi, H. (2005). Listening comprehension and anxiety in the 16(1), 1–17.
Arabic language classroom. Modern Language Journal, 89, Mendelsohn, D. (1994). Learning to listen: A strategy-based
206–222. approach for the second-language learner. Dominie Press.
Eslami, -R. Z., & Ranjbary, R. (2003). Metacognitive strategy Mills, N., Pajares, F., & Herron, C. (2007). Self-efficacy of college
training for vocabulary learning. http://www-writing.berkeley. intermediate French students: Relation to achievement and
edu/TESLEJ/ej26/a5.html motivation. Language Learning, 57(3), 417–442.
Fathi et al. 13

Mohamadpour, P., Talebinejad, R., & Tabatabaei, O. (2019). Vandergrift, L. (2005). Relationships among motivation orienta-
Impact of metacognitive strategy instruction on Iranian EFL tions, metacognitive awareness and proficiency in L2 listening.
learners’ listening anxiety. International Journal of Foreign Applied Linguistics, 26(1), 70–89.
Language Teaching and Research, 7(26), 57–68. Vandergrift, L. (2007). Recent developments in second and for-
Movahed, R. (2014). The effect of metacognitive strategy instruc- eign language listening comprehension research. Language
tion on listening performance, metacognitive awareness and lis- Teaching, 40(3), 191–210.
tening anxiety of beginner Iranian EFL students. International Vandergrift, L., & Baker, S. (2015). Learner variables in second
Journal of English Linguistics, 4(2), 88–99. language listening comprehension: An exploratory path analy-
Nakatani, Y. (2010). Identifying strategies that facilitate EFL sis. Language Learning, 65(2), 390–416.
learners’ oral communication: A classroom study using Vandergrift, L., & Cross, J. (2017). Replication research in L2 lis-
multiple data collection procedures. The Modern Language tening comprehension: A conceptual replication of Graham &
Journal, 94(1), 116–136. Macaro (2008) and an approximate replication of Vandergrift
Ngo, N. (2019). Understanding the impact of listening strategy & Tafaghodtari (2010) and Brett (1997). Language Teaching,
instruction on listening strategy use from a socio-cultural per- 50(1), 1–10.
spective. System, 81(1), 63–77. Vandergrift, L., & Goh, C. (2012). Teaching and learning sec-
O’Malley, J. M., & Chamot, A. U. (1990). Learning strategies in ond language listening: Metacognition in action. Taylor &
second language acquisition. Cambridge University Press. Francis.
Oxford, R. L. (1990). Language learning strategies: What every Vandergrift, L., Goh, C., Mareschal, C., & Tafaghodtari, M.
teacher should know. Heinle & Heinle. (2006). The metacognitive awareness listening questionnaire:
Oxford, R. L. (2011). Teaching and researching language learning Development and validation. Language Learning, 56(3), 431–
strategies. Longman. 462.
Oxford, R. L., Cho, Y., Leung, S., & Kim, H. (2004). Effect of the Vandergrift, L., & Tafaghodtari, M. H. (2010). Teaching L2 learn-
presence and difficulty of task on strategy use: An exploratory ers how to listen does make a difference: An empirical study.
study. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Language Learning, 60(2), 470–497.
Teaching, 42(1), 1–48. Vogely, A. J. (1998). Listening comprehension anxiety: Students’
Pallant, J. (2013). SPSS survival manual. McGraw-Hill. reported sources and solutions. Foreign Language Annals,
Rahimirad, M., & Zare-ee, A. (2015). Metacognitive strategy 31(1), 67–80.
instruction as a means to improve listening self-efficacy among Vogely, A. (1999). Addressing listening comprehension anxiety.
Iranian undergraduate learners of English. International In D. J. Young (Ed.), Affect in foreign language and sec-
Journal of Instruction, 8(1), 117–132. ond language learning: A practical guide to creating a low-
Rees-Miller, J. (1993). A critical appraisal of learner training: anxiety classroom atmosphere (pp. 106–123). McGraw-Hill
Theoretical bases and teaching applications. TESOL Quarterly, College.
27(4), 679–689. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning and devel-
Rossiter, M. J. (2003). The effects of affective strategy instruction opment. Readings on the Development of Children, 23(3),
in the ESL classroom. The Electronic Journal for English as 34–41.
a Second Language, 7(2). http://www.tesl-ej.org/wordpress/ Wong, M. S. L. (2005). Language learning strategies and language
issues/volume7/ej26/ej26a2/ self-efficacy: Investigating the relationship in Malaysia. RELC
Schunk, D. H., & Rice, J. M. (1984). Strategy self-verbalization Journal, 36(3), 245–269.
during remedial listening comprehension instruction. The Xu, J., & Huang, Y. T. (2018). The mediating effect of listen-
Journal of Experimental Education, 53, 49–54. ing metacognitive awareness between listening test anxiety
Scovell, D., Pastellas, V., & Knobel, M. (2004). 404 essential tests and listening test performance. The Asia-pacific Education
for IELTS: Academic module. Adams & Austen Press. Researcher, 27(4), 313–324.
Siegel, J. (2015). Exploring listening strategy instruction through Yan, J. X., & Horwitz, E. K. (2008). Learners’ perceptions of
action research. Palgrave Macmillan. how anxiety interacts with personal and instructional fac-
Sioson, I. C. (2011). Language learning strategies, beliefs, and anxiety tors to influence their achievement in English: A qualitative
in academic speaking task. Philippine ESL Journal, 7(1), 3–27. analysis of EFL learners in China. Language Learning, 58(1),
Tsui, A. B., & Fullilove, J. (1998). Bottom-up or top-down pro- 151–183.
cessing as a discriminator of L2 listening performance. Applied Yeldham, M. (2016). Second language listening instruction:
Linguistics, 19(4), 432–451. Comparing a strategies-based approach with an interactive,
Vandergrift, L. (1997). The comprehension strategies of second lan- strategies/bottom-up skills approach. TESOL Quarterly, 50(2),
guage (French) listeners: A descriptive study. Foreign Language 394–420.
Annals, 30, 387–409. Yeldham, M., & Gruba, P. (2014). Toward an instructional approach
Vandergrift, L. (2002). It was nice to see that our predictions were to developing interactive second language listening. Language
right: Developing metacognition in L2 listening comprehen- Teaching Research, 18(1), 33–53.
sion. Canadian Modern Language Review, 58(4), 555–575. Zhang, W., & Liu, M. (2013). Evaluating the impact of oral test
Vandergrift, L. (2003). Orchestrating strategy use: Toward a model anxiety and speaking strategy use on oral English performance.
of the skilled second language listener. Language Learning, The Journal of Asia TEFL, 10(2), 115–148.
53(3), 463–496. Zhang, X. (2013). Foreign language listening anxiety and listen-
Vandergrift, L. (2004). Listening to learn or learning to listen? ing performance: Conceptualizations and causal relationships.
Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 24, 3–25. System, 41(1), 164–177.

You might also like