You are on page 1of 27

VOLUME 70, No.

2 THE QUARTERLY REVIEW OF BIOLOGY JUNE 1995

FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS

ALAIN BoissY
aux Milieux, INRA Centrede TheixClermont-Ferrand
Adaptationdes Herbivores
F-63122 Saint-Genes-Champanelle,
France
ABSTRACT
Persistence ofindividual differences inanimalbehavior in reactionstovarious environmental
challengescouldreflect basicdivergences intemperament, whichmight beusedtopredict details
ofadaptive response.Although studies havebeencarried outonfear andanxiety invarious species,
includinglaboratory, domestic andwildanimals, noconsistent definition
offearfulnessas a basic
traitoftemperament hasemerged.
After a classificationoftheevents thatmayproduce a stateoffear,thisarticle describes the
greatvariability in behavior and inphysiological patterns generallyassociated withemotional
Thedifficulties
reactivity. ofproposing fearfulness - thegeneral capacitytoreacttoa variety of
potentially
threatening situations - as a validbasicinternal variablearethen discussed.Although
therearemanystudiesshowing covariation amongthepsychobiological responsestodifferent
environmental challenges, other studies findno suchcorrelations and raisedoubtsaboutthe
interpretation
offearfulness as a basicpersonality trait.Aftera critical
assessment ofmethodologies
usedinfearandanxiety studies, it is suggested thatdiscrepancies among resultsaremainly due
tothemodulation ofemotional responses in animals,whichdepend on numerous genetic and
epigenetic
factors. It is difficult tocompare results obtained bydifferentmethods fromanimals
rearedunder various conditions andwithdifferent geneticorigins.
Theconcept offearfulness as aninner traitis best
supported bytwokinds ofinvestigations.First,
an experimental approach combining ethology andexperimental psychology produces undeniable
indicatorsofemotional reactivity. Second, genetic linesselectedforpsychobiologicaltraitsprove
usefulin establishing relationships between behavioral andneuroendocrine aspectsofemotional
It is suggested
reactivity. thatfearfulness couldbeconsidered a basicfeatureofthetemperament
ofeachindividual, onethat predisposes ittorespond similarlytoa varietyofpotentiallyalarming
challenges,butis nevertheless continually modulated during development bytheinteraction of
genetictraitsofreactivity withenvironmentalfactors, particularlyin thejuvenile period.Such
interactionmayexplainmuchoftheinterindividual observed
variability inadaptive responses.

The Quarterly
Reviewof Biology,June 1995, Vol. 70, No. 2
Copyright ? 1995 by The University of Chicago. All rights reserved.
0033-5770/95/7002-0002$1.00

165
166 THE QUARTERL Y REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

IT IS CLEAR thattherearebehavioraldif- subsequentlybeen challenged,or at least mod-


ferencesnot only among species but also erated, by an infusionof biological concepts
among individualsofthe same species(Plomin and rapid scientificdevelopment in the areas
et al., 1990). Several studies reveal that there of behavioral ecology and neuroendocrinol-
is considerable individual variabilityin many ogy. A reexamination of the validity of the
aspects of behavior, even when animals are conceptsoffearand fearfulnessis a goal ofthis
reared under the same conditions. Demon- article. In particular,a more global relevance
stratedconsistencyofinterindividual variability mightbe demonstratedon the basis of com-
over time and in response to environmental monalitiesin physiologicaland behavioral re-
changes could indicate the existence of basic sponses to dangers among and withinspecies,
dimensions ofpersonality,as has been argued forthere is evidence that genetics and devel-
already forhumans (Zuckerman, 1991). opmental experience subserve individual dif-
Among these psychologicalcharacteristics, ferencesin such responsivenessin predictable
an individual's emotional reactivity-the ca- ways.
pacityto perceive and react to potentiallyan- According to Gray (1987), the studyoffear
xiogenic situations-is oftenhypothesizedto involves two importantconcepts of psychol-
be an intervening variablemodulating a large ogy: motivation and personality.Fear and anxiety
range of fundamentalbehaviors arising from have often been considered motivators and
major life-challengingcircumstances. For ex- are defined here as emotional states that are
ample, studiesin laboratoryrodentsrelatedif- induced by the perception of any actual dan-
ferencesin reactionsto threateningeventsbe- ger (fear state) or potential danger (anxiety
tweenvirginand pregnantor lactatingfemales state) thatthreatensthe well-beingoftheindi-
to the capacity to demonstrate maternal be- vidual, and which are characterizedas a feel-
havior (Hard and Hansen, 1985; Ferreira et ing ofinsecurity.Fearfulness is consideredhere
al., 1989; Maestripieri and D'Amato, 1991). as a personalityor temperamenttraitdefining
Fleming and Luebke (1981) suggested that the general susceptibilityof an individual to
emotional reactivityprevents virgin females reactto a varietyofpotentiallythreateningsit-
frombehaving maternally. Moreover, inter- uations.
individualvariabilityin behaviormay be partly The Darwinian mainstream of behavioral
related to differencesin emotional reactions ecology, based on the concept ofevolutionary
among individuals. In mice, forexample, fe- adaptations, points out that fear has definite
males obtaining higher scores for maternal survivalvalue in wild animals. The lifeexpec-
defensein protectingoffspring against conspe- tancy of an animal is obviously increased if
cificswere less reactivein a previous stressful it can reactto avoid sources ofdanger. Defen-
testregardlessofreproductivestage (Maestri- sive reactionsincrease the chances of individ-
pieri and D'Amato, 1991). Such characteris- ual survival (by fightor flight,forinstance).
tic patternsof reactivitycould be used to pre- In nature, the most importantthreatsof in-
dict adaptive responses to a varietyof major jury that an individual encounters during its
life-challengingcircumstances. lifetimecome frompredators and competing
In the past, the concepts of fear and fear- or attackingconspecifics.There has been con-
fulnesswere a focusofintenseinterestin com- siderableinterestin recentdecades in theevolu-
parativepsychology.These concepts,however, tionofantipredatorand competitionstrategies.
were vague and lacked universally accepted These strategiesare regarded as the product
definition.With theriseofbehaviorism,these of strongselectionpressuresbecause inappro-
concepts, as well as those of emotions in ani- priate defensivebehaviors generallyproduce
mals in general, were criticized as being too rapid and disastrous consequences (Blanch-
anthropocentric(e.g., Skinner, 1953). Con- ard and Blanchard, 1990). Studies in natural
sequently, it became more common to speak environmentsprovide substantialinformation
descriptivelyof escape and avoidance behav- regarding the social functionof fear and de-
ior, withoutany assumptions about underly- fensive strategies.
ing unitary states or traits(e.g., Anisman et An importantpart of the research on fear
al., 1978). This reductionisticbehaviorismhas expressions as social signals is based on the
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 167

studyof alarm calls. Animals of various spe- tor behavior is directed against environmen-
cies vocalize whentheydetectpredators:mam- tal changes and against people. For instance,
mals likevervetmonkeys,Cercopithecus aethiops in large mammals, the detectionof a person
(Seyfarthet al., 1980) and California ground who remains at a considerable distance from
squirrels, Spermophilus beecheyl
(Leger et al., the animal may elicitno response. Closer ap-
1980), and birds like black-billed magpies, proach, however, which is oftennecessaryon
Pica pica hudsonia(Stone and Trost, 1991) and farms, often elicits a violent defensive reac-
domestic fowl,Gallusdomesticus (Gyger et al., tion (Seabrook, 1972). In poultry,tonic im-
1987). Alarm calls may be defined as signals mobility,characterizedby a reduced respon-
thatcommunicatepossibilitiesofdangerto con- sivenessinduced by physicalrestraint(as when
specifics.For example, in free-rangingvervet a human grabs a chicken), appears to be a
monkeys,flightresponses to recorded alarms defensivereactionagainst potentialpredators
are identical to those induced by the presence because it simulatesa predatoryepisode (Mc-
of predators (Seyfarthet al., 1980). Animals Farland, 1987). In spiteof theircaptivitythen,
are able to vary call structurein a graded fash- domesticanimals can express emotional reac-
ion to communicate informationabout spe- tivitysimilarto thatoffree-livingpopulations
cific predator context to other conspecifics, of wild animals. Fear and anxietyare gener-
as was shown in magpies (Stone and Trost, ally considered to be undesirable emotional
1991). Such predator-evokedcalls serveto rep- states that may reduce welfare, growthand
resent the type of predator encountered and reproductiveperformancein laboratory,zoo
help to coordinatean effective and appropriate and farm animals. In poultry,for instance,
response.By examiningthe reactionsto alarm reactions such as panic or violent escape are
calls,Seyfarthand his coworkers(1980) showed often inappropriate in an intensive farming
that distincttypes of alarm calls emitted by system,and the induction of a chronic state
vervet monkeys are associated with distinct ofanxietyor stressmay cause damage to indi-
sets of responses fromconspecifics,responses vidual organismsand adverselyaffectmilkor
whichappear to be adaptive strategiesforcop- egg production,growthrate, or disease resis-
ing withthehuntingbehavior ofthepredators tance (Craig and Adams, 1984; Jones, 1989;
involved. In response to the recorded alarm Mills and Faure, 1990). Thus, the develop-
calls of an individual exposed to a leopard, ment of studies aimed at reducing fear and
monkeysrun up into treeswhere theyappear defensivebehavior in animals reared in cap-
to be safestfromthe ambush styleof typical tivitycould be ofeconomic and ethical signifi-
attack of terrestrialpredators. The audition cance.
of calls emittedfollowingthe detection of an The purpose of this articleis firstto exam-
eagle causes them to look up and run into ine the statusof fear and anxietyin the study
cover, apparentlyto avoid an aerial predator's of animal life and then to develop the rele-
stoop. Some acts relatedto fearmay therefore vance ofthe conceptoffearfulnessin the anal-
be regardedas a means to communicatepossi- ysis of mechanisms underlyinginterindivid-
bilities of danger to conspecifics,and conse- ual variabilityin adaptive processes. Animal
quently to influencetheir behavior. fear can be assessed only if the fear-eliciting
Natural predatorsare largelyabsent in cap- stimuliand the magnitude ofthe correspond-
tivity,and it could be argued that reduced ing psychobiologicalresponsesare objectively
fearfulnessis a rnajorconcomitantofdomesti- taken into account. This article,therefore,re-
cation (Price, 1984). Populations of farmani- views the natureofthe frightening stimuliand
mals in range environments,however, may the correspondingbehavioral responses and
stillexperience severe predation by wild ani- neuroendocrine processes. Aftera brief de-
mals or dogs (Sheltonand Wade, 1979). More- scriptionofthevarious experimentalmethods
over, modern farming practice can impose and situations,theconsistencyamong indica-
severaldeviationsfromwhatmightbe regarded torsofemotional reactivityis discussed. Inter-
as the natural situation: A kind of artificial individual variabilityin reactionsto challenge
predation is practiced by humans, especially is exploredin lightof theconceptof fearfulness
in intensivehousing, where most antipreda- as a basic personalitytrait that is subject to
168 THE QUARTERLY REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

several geneticand epigeneticfactors.Finally, A stimulus can elicit fear by being associ-


I consider the possibilityof temperamentas- ated withotherthreateningevents as a result
sessment in predictingadaptive responses to of conditionedprocessesoflearning.
Learning plays
variousenvironmentalchallengesand vulnera- a role in emotional reactions by identifying
diseases and pathologies.
bilityto stress-induced new sources of danger. Boissy (1990) condi-
tioned heifersto be frightenedby a visual ob-
IDENTIFICATION OF FEAR ject, initially nonaversive, by associating it
NATURE OF FRIGHTENING STIMULI with an electrical shock.
Animals may be frightenedby a stimulus
Some years ago, Miller (1959) assertedthat because of thephysicalcharacteristics of its pre-
most stimulithat generatenegative emotions
sentation, such as its movement, intensity,
acquire thispower as a resultof classical con-
duration,suddenness,or proximity.According
ditioning. But there are substantial observa-
to Russel (1979), thesestimuliare not species-
tions on a large range of eventsthat are capa-
specificbut ratherare associated withthecon-
ble of causing fear that is not conditioned.
textof predation. For instance, a flyinghawk
Gray (1979) classifiesfear-producingstimuli
leads more easily to a flightresponse in black-
into fivesubdivisions. One consistsofdangers
billed magpies than the same predatorthatis
thatare part ofthe evolutionaryhistoryofthe
perched and not moving (Buitron, 1983). By
species, a second refersto novelty, a third
using video playback images to manipulate
resultsfromlearning, a fourthis the intensity
the salientfeaturesofthepredator,Evans and
of stimuli, and the fiftharises from interac-
Marler (1992) showed thatthe velocityof the
tions with conspecifics.
predatorand thedistancefromthepredatorto
Animalsexhibitnegativeemotionalresponses
the chickenare criticalwithregardto eliciting
towards specificstimulithat typicallyrelate to
alarm calls in the prey.
persistentdangers in the ecology of the spe-
Finally, and as already mentioned in the
cies. They may relateto physicaldangers such
as fear of heights,as is found in many mam- introductoryparagraphs, signalsarisingfrom
mals, fearofdarkness,as in vision-dependent interactions
withconspecifics can also induce re-
species like humans, or fear of dead bodies sponses related to negative emotions. These
thatmay transmitdisease or indicate thepres- triggeringsocial signals representparticular
ence of danger. Fear of potential predators cases of the previous types of fear-producing
has led to antipredatorstrategies,which are stimuli. They may be characterized by nov-
based mainly on associations between certain elty: Contact with a foreignindividual may
featuresin theanimal'senvironmentand adap- provokea flightresponsein heifers(Bouissou,
tivestrategiesforcoping. For example, as men- 1985). They may be innate, as in the case of
tioned above, vervetmonkeysgive alarm calls a few alarm signals: Exposure of chicks to
withdifferent acoustical featuresaccording to alarm calls recordedfromconspecificsexposed
the type of predator, eitherterrestrialpreda- to a predatormake them peep and run away,
torssuch as leopards or snakes, or aerial pred- whetheror not theyhave previouslyencoun-
ators such as eagles (Seyfarthet al., 1980). tered a predator(Duncan and Filshie, 1979).
Exposure of an animal to noveltyis one of These signals may be acquired, as seen typi-
themostpotentexperimentalconditionslead- callyin most antipredatorstrategiesand dom-
ing to a negative emotional response. The re- inance hierarchiesoccurringwithinthe social
sulting behavioral arousal is similar to that group. Learning from conspecifics, that is,
induced by a nociceptive stimulationsuch as social learning, to avoid futuredangerous en-
electricfootshock(Dantzerand Mormede,1983). counters with putative predators, has been
Novelty can be classified as a collative vari- reportedin birds (Curio, 1988) and primates
able because the recognitionof any stimulus (Mineka and Cook, 1988). In vervetmonkeys,
situation as being novel requires a compari- forinstance,while infantscan distinguishbe-
son with events that have been experienced tweenrelativelygeneralpredatorclasses, with
in thepast. Faure (1979) observed thatbehav- increased age theysharpentheassociation be-
ioral reactions decrease with repeated expo- tween the predator species and the type of
sure to a previously novel environment. alarm calls (Seyfarthet al., 1980). Responses
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 169

of low-rankingheifersto threatsfromdomi- activityin whichtheanimal is engaged may be


nant conspecificsalso reflectthis process, al- enhanced: The administrationofweak electric
thoughthe role of social learning in domestic shocks to a rat leads to an increase in food
animals is open to question (Bouissou, 1985). intake. But when fearis intense,behavior can
In highlygregariousspecies, such as domestic be disturbedor totallyinhibited:Regular nox-
fowl,theinterruption ious stimulican inhibitaggressiveinteractions
of social contactis stress-
fulsince the social motivationofthe individu- between rats (Archer, 1976c). Intermediate
als is a potentfactorin learning (Mills et al.,degrees of fear usually lead to a conflictbe-
1993). tween the expression of fear and the activity
An adaptive response to real or potential in which the individual is engaged (feeding,
danger comprisestwo complementaryfacets: sexual, aggressive, exploratory). For exam-
psychobehavioralchanges thatneutralize the ple, File (1980) showed that modulation of
effectsofthetriggeringstimulusand neuroen- anxietyby pharmacological manipulation in-
docrineadjustmentsneeded to maintaininter- duces changes in the amount of social interac-
nal homeostasis.In animals, emotionalstates, tion. Conflict between an intense emotional
such as fear or anxiety, can be indirectlyin- stateand a positivemotivationmay resultin a
ferredonly fromthese observable and mea- displacementactivity.Dantzer and Mormede
surable phenomena. (198 lb) observed thathungrypigs developed
a compulsive behavior (nibbling of a chain)
FEAR-RELATED BEHAVIORAL PARAMETERS when feedwas associated witha painfulstim-
Behavioralresponsesto aversiveeventsvary ulation (conflictsituation). Conflictbetween
greatlydepending on whetherthethreatstim- motivationsmay also provoke an intermedi-
ate activitycharacterized by the alternation
ulus is present (fear state) or only potential
of two typesof behaviors, as, forinstance, in
(anxiety state). The behavioral patternscan
some aggressive or sexual displays.
even be contradictory:Active defense(attack,
threat)or active avoidance (flight,hiding, es- FEAR-RELATED BIOLOGICAL MARKERS
cape) and immobilityor movementinhibition
can all be viewed as expressing a fear state. There are obviouslymany diversepotential
Two types of motor inhibitionmay be ob- biological correlates of emotional reactivity
served. "Freezing" is of shortduration and is and associated behavioral explanations. This
to
usually accompanied by signs of activationof article is purposely restricted neuroendo-
the vegetative system,such as an increase in crine correlates and does not deal with all of
the complex mechanisms ofthecentralnervous
pulse rate (Archer, 1976b). "Tonic immobil-
ity,"which is mainly observed in response to systemthat are involved in emotional states,
manual restraint(Gallup, 1977), is longer and such as neural circuits,neuromodulators,and
not accompanied by physiologicalevents that neurotransmitters, whichare reviewedby sev-
are controlledby the autonomic nervous sys- eral authors (e.g., LeDoux, 1990; Holstege,
is now extensive literatureon
tem. Tonic immobilityhas been successfully 1992). There
the neuroendocrine changes that accompany
used as an index of fear in chickens (Jones,
1984). exposure to environmental challenges. The
is
Other responses considered as fear indica- purpose here to attemptto show that vari-
tors include expressive movements(piloerec- ous physiologicalsystemscan respond specifi-
tion, facial expressions), vocalizations, and cally to psychologicalconditions,and thatthe
the productionof smells(pheromones). Some neuroendocrinestateaffectsthe behavioral re-
of these responses, such as the alarm calls, actions to danger.
are a means ofcommunicationin social envi-
Neuroendocrine SystemsInvolvedin
ronments.
EmotionalReactivity
Fear-producingstimulimay also affectother
patternsof behavior. Archer (1979) observed SympatheticAdrenal MedullarSystemand
that the effectsvary and depend on the emo- Hypothalamo-Pituitary-AdrenalAxis
tional intensityof the response to a threaten- Traditionally the sympatheticadrenal me-
ing event. When the level of fear is low, the dullar systemand thehypothalamo-pituitary-
170 THE QUARTERLY REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

adrenocortical (HPA) axis are viewed as the In spiteofmany differencesin theirspecific


major systemsthatare highlysensitiveto en- effects,glucocorticoidsact in combinationwith
vironmental challenges. Claude Bernard, in adrenal catecholamines to enable the organ-
thelast century,was the firstto recognize that ism to engage in rapid behavioral action by
the relativeconstancyofthe internalenviron- mobilizing available resources. They inhibit
ment is importantto the functionalintegrity glucose uptake and fattyacid storage or pro-
of the organism. Cannon (1935) noticed that tein synthesisat storage sites, and stimulate
emotional reactions, such as fear and rage, the release of energy substratessuch as glu-
pose a threatto the integrityof the body. In cose, amino acids, and freefattyacids (Mor-
theemergencyconcept,Cannon describedhow mede, 1991). It has been demonstratedthat
emotional reactions are characterizedin par- thereare numerous interactionsbetween the
ticular by the release of adrenal medullary neurochemicalcontrolsofthesetwomajor neu-
catecholamines. Besides the adrenal activa- roendocrine systems (Axelrod and Reisine,
tion, emotional disturbances also elicit a re- 1984; Plotskyet al., 1989). In addition, since
lease of catecholamines both of sympathetic the discoveryof serotoninin the centralner-
origin(Dodd and Role, 1991) and froma dense vous system,there have been extensive data
neuronal networkin subcortical areas in the supportingthenotionthatthereare close rela-
brain (Le Moal and Simon, 1991). These re- tionshipsbetweencentralserotonergicsystems
actions require immediate physiological ad- and the sympatheticnervous system or the
justmentsformaintaininghomeostasis,mostly pituitary-adrenalaxis (Chaouloff, 1993).
via energymetabolismand cardiovascularad-
aptations that allow a rapid redistributionof Other
Neuroendocrine
Systems
blood and energytowardmusclesand thebrain. Although this article focusesessentiallyon
The cardiovascularand metabolicadjustments the two major neuroendocrine systemslead-
have survival value in preparingthe body for ing to the ACTH-glucocorticoid and adrenal
an active response to acute threat, such as catecholamine secretions, there is now evi-
"fightor flight"(Kuchel, 1991). The measure- dence indicatingthat numerous otherneuro-
mentofcirculatingcatecholaminesis extremely endocrine systemsare altered under stressful
difficult due to theirlow concentrations,short conditions,involvinggonadal steroids,a large
duration of half-life,and high sensitivityto group of hypothalarno-pituitary neuropeptides
foreignevents. Thus, the activationofthe au- [prolactin,corticotropinreleasingfactor(CRF),
tonomicnervoussystemhas usuallybeen indi- oxytocin,vasopressin], and another group of
rectlyevaluated by theeffecton certainvegeta- peptides localized in brain, pituitaryand ad-
tivefunctions,suchas defecationfrequencyand renal medulla (13-endorphins,enkephalins).
changes in cutaneous resistance, or on some Many studies have shown that there are
cardiovascular measure, such as heart rate or differencesin fearand defensivereactionsbe-
blood pressure. tween the sexes. In response to challenges,
Since Selye (1936), numerous studies have female rodents generally exhibit a lower be-
demonstratedthata varietyofpsychophysical havioralreactivitythando males (Gray, 1987).
events cause release of adrenocorticotrophic The same pattern has been observed in do-
hormone (ACTH) fromthe pituitary,which mesticfowl(Jones, 1987b). Likewise, in dogs
in turnactivates the adrenal cortex to release and sheep, males are more reluctantthan fe-
glucocorticoid,which facilitatesenergyavail- males to approach and make physical contact
abilityover prolongedperiods. The conceptof with a human or a novel object (Lore and
stresswas proposed to characterizethe arousal Eisenberg, 1986; Vandenheede and Bouissou,
of the HPA axis and was firstdefined as "a 1993a, respectively). This interpretationof
non-specificresponse of the body to any de- the emotional levels of males and femaleshas
mand made upon it" (Selye, 1950). It took a been severelycriticizedbecause thereare many
long time before it was recognized that the physiologicaldifferences, such as body weight
profileof the activation of the HPA axis may or foodintake,thatcould accountforsex differ-
vary depending on the nature of the stressor encesin testsofanxiety(Archer,1975;Johnston
(Mason, 1971). and File, 1991). Studies in domesticungulates,
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 171

however, presentevidence forimplicationsof to pain. Central 1-endorphins, which share


gonadal steroidsin fear-relatedreactions. Fe- a precursorwithACTH (the pro-opiomelan-
males treatedwithtestosteronepropionateex- ocortin),are released simultaneouslywiththis
hibitedlowerfearreactionsthantheiruntreated neuropeptide in response to stressfulevents
counterpartsin cattle (Boissy and Bouissou, (Guillemin et al., 1977). There is substantial
1994) and sheep (Vandenheede and Bouis- evidence that exposure to aversive events can
sou, 1993b). produce a reduction in pain reactivitymedi-
It is well establishedthatprolactinsecretion ated by theopioid system.For example, when
by the anteriorpituitaryis also increased fol- returnedto an environmentpreviouslyasso-
lowing a variety of challenges. The plasma ciated withelectricshock, ratsexhibita loss of
levels of prolactin rose in rats subjected to the pain sensitivity;this autoanalgesic effect
electricshock(MormZede,Dantzer, Montpied, disappears afterthe injection of naloxone, an
Bluthe, Laplante, and Le Moal, 1984) and in antagonistof opioid receptors(Fanselow, 1984).
sheep testedduringa period of social isolation In the same way, rats exposed to a novel envi-
(Parrot and Thornton, 1989). In addition, ronmentalso develop analgesia (Netto et al.,
changes in the plasma levels of prolactin are 1987), which is suppressed by the injection
linked to adrenal responses to aggression. In of naltrexone, another antagonist of opioid
thelactatingfemalerat,whichhas highplasma receptors(Siegfried et al., 1987). Moreover,
levels of prolactin,the pituitary-adrenocortical the administrationof an anxiolyticbeforeex-
reactivityis lower in an open-fieldtest (Stem posure to threateningevents reduces not only
and Levine, 1974; Hard and Hansen, 1985). the length of freezingbut also the length of
The rise in prolactin levels is even positively analgesia (Fanselow and Helmstetter,1988).
correlatedwith the increase in corticosteroid Ecologically relevant stimuli associated with
concentrationin rats placed in an open-field social aggressionand predationhave also been
test (Seggie and Brown, 1975). The plasma shownto activateendogenous antinociceptive
level of prolactin, however, is not always af- opioid mechanisms (Kavaliers and Colwell,
fectedby threateningevents. In primates,the 1991). Endogenous opioids seem to reduce
behavior oflower-rankingindividuals, which the experienced intensityof noxious stimula-
is very differentfrom that of dominant ani- tion and therebyrender the animal less re-
mals, does not seem to be related to an in- sponsive to the challenge. Evidence that opi-
creased prolactin level (talapoin, Miopithecus ate peptides can enterthe brain (Kastin et al.,
talapoin:Eberhartet al., 1983; marmoset, Cal- 1991) confirmsthe influenceof the peripher-
lithrixjacchusjacchus: Abbott et al., 1981). ally released opiates on the activityofthe cen-
Some otherpeptides also seem to be closely tral nervous system.
relatedtotheneuroendocrineresponsesofemo- According to these data, the brain appears
tional reactivity.Corticotophin-releasingfac- to be the most importanttargetorgan forhor-
tor (CRF), released into the hypothalamic- mones released in challenging situations,
pituitaryportal vessels, is the main regulator although peripheral sites, such as the neuro-
of adrenocorticotropinsecretionby the pitu- muscular and cardiovascular systems,are im-
itarygland (Dunn and Berridge, 1990). CRF, portanttargetsof the adrenal corticosteroids
centralvasopressin, and oxytocinregulatethe and catecholamines,whichstimulatetheadap-
sympathetic nervous system and stimulate tiveresponsesthatincrease immediate energy
ACTH release eitherdirectlyor by modulat- availability forthe brain and muscles.
ing theeffectsofneuropeptides(Gibbs, 1986).
The influenceof these neuropeptides on the Modulationof the Neuroendocrine
peripheralendocrineresponsesto environmen- PatternofReactivity
tal challenges, however, is complex (Ivanyi The magnitude of neuroendocrine arousal
et al., 1991). depends not only on the physical properties
The endogenous ligands of opioid recep- ofthetriggerstimulilike electricshock or heat
tors,the 13-endorphins and enkephalins,have but also on psychologicalfactors.Psychologi-
aroused considerable interestin the study of cal discomfort,which may resultfromuncer-
emotions because of theirrole in the reaction tainty,conflict,frustration,or a high degree
172 THE QUARTERLY REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

ofnovelty,is responsibleforHPA axis activa- Rats thathave the opportunityto controldis-


tion(Hennessy and Levine, 1979). For exam- play a more moderateblood pressureresponse
ple, exposure to a novel environmentleads to challenge than rats withoutcontrol(Buch-
to increases in the plasma levels of corticoste- holz et al., 1981).
roids in the mouse (Hennessy and Levine, The predictabilityof an aggressive act may
1978) and the squirrelmonkey,Saimirisciureus also reduce its impact. When uncontrollable
(Coe et al., 1982). Catecholamines are re- electricshocksare combined witha briefstim-
leased in situationsthatrequire attentionand ulus followingshocktermination(i.e., a feed-
vigilance. For example, rats placed in a cage back stimulus),rats show no more fearin this
where theyhad been previously subjected to contextthanthoseexposedto controllableshock
electricshock exhibithigher plasma levels of (Mineka et al., 1984). Repeated exposure to
catecholamines than do naive rats (McCarty noveltyprovokes a lower activationof the pi-
and Kopin, 1978a). tuitary adrenal axis only if the exposure is
There is evidence indicatingthatthedegree regular, that is, according to a predictable
of control that an animal can exert over its pattern(expectancy), in both rats (Muir and
threateningenvironmentby suitable behav- Pfister,1987) and mice (Shanks et al., 1990).
may also determine Henry(1980) representsthedifferences
ior, that is, controllability, in neu-
the pattern of neuroendocrine arousal. This roendocrine patterns schematicallyaccording
was demonstratedby the fundamentalwork to the subject's possibility of controllingor
of Weiss (1972), who used two rats yoked to- predictingthe threateningevent. He suggests
getherby shockelectrodesattachedto thesame thatthemedullosympatheticsystemwould be
source. One rat could interruptthe electric predominant as long as the organism is chal-
shock (ability to control) and the other could lenged in its controlof the environment.By
not (no ability to control), but both subjects contrast,the loss of controlor the perception
received the same quantity of electric shock of failureto meet expectations would lead to
as a result of the yoking. The rat that con- a preferentialactivation of the HPA axis.
trolledthe stimulationexhibiteda lower pitu- There is ample evidence, therefore,to indi-
itary-adrenalarousal compared to yoked sub- cate that the biological response to a threatis
jects that had no such possibility of action. not stereotypedbut is influencedby psycholog-
Since then, some other studies have shown ical factorsand by behavioral strategiesthat
that the extentto which the animal perceives have a direct impact on the threat.
the danger as under its controlis of the great- Influenceofthe NeuroendocrineState
est importance in determiningthe intensity on EmotionalReactivity
of the emotion experienced. The increase in
While the neuroendocrine responseto threat-
plasma levels of corticosteroneobserved in
ening events is modulated by the possibility
rats (Barnett and Cowan, 1976) and in mice
ofbehavioral action, theneuroendocrinestate
(Misslin and Cigrang,1986) exposed to a novel
that results froman interactionbetween the
environmentceases once the animals have free
availability of hormones and the functional
access to theirfamiliarenvironment.The be-
propertiesof theirreceptors,both in the pe-
havioral action of the subject does not even
ripheryand the brain, may affectthe way in
need to be related to the aggression to be effi- whichindividuals react to aversive situations.
cient: Chewing a nonedible object reduces the Indeed,by bindingto receptorspresentthrough-
adrenal activation in mice exposed to a novel out the brain, glucocorticoidsexert negative
environment(M. B. Hennessyand Foy, 1987), effectson theirown secretionand also act on
The possibility of moving, however, corre- otherfear-activatedneural circuits(Bohus et
sponds generallyto an active behavioral strat- al., 1987). Such influencesoccur by at least
egy of coping, which leads to a decrease of two mechanisms: a basic hormonal effectand
the HPA axis arousal consecutiveto theexpo- a retroactivehormonaleffect,as demonstrated
sureto a threateningsituation(Dantzer, 1986). through methods of adrenalectomy and ad-
The sympatheticadrenal medullarysystemis ministrationof exogenous glucocorticoid to
also highlyinfluencedby the animal's ability intactanimals (Leshner, 1979). When an aver-
to control the occurrence of aversive events. sive event occurs, the basal hormonal levels
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 173

(basiceffect)
may affectthebehavioral response STUDY OF FEAR-RELATED RESPONSES: THE
by "preparing" the central nervous system. ASSESSMENT OF INTERINDIVIDUAL
For example, the duration of tonic immobil- DIFFERENCES
ity, considered as a specific fear-relatedre- VARIOUS METHODS FOR STUDYING FEAR
sponse in restrainedhens, is prolonged after
thebasal plasma level of corticosteroneis arti- Many kinds of laboratory situations have
been designed to studythe various behavioral
ficiallyincreased by a corticosteroneinfusion
and physiologicalprocessesofemotional reac-
at physiologicaldoses (Joneset al., 1988). And
tivityby attemptingto mimic conditions en-
adrenalectomy in rats decreases novelty-in-
counteredby animalsin naturalhabitats.These
duced locomotion(Veldhuis et al., 1982). The
experimentaltestsfacilitatecomparisonofpsy-
glucocorticoidresponse to a threateningsitu-
chobiological reactivityamong individuals in
ation in turnhas an effecton the centralner-
a standardized way that is difficultto attempt
vous system (retroactive effect)
and modulates
under natural conditions. It is not our intent
the development of the behavioral response.
to review all the experimental designs that
Mice exposed to trained attackers fightless
have been used in studies of fear defensive
and exhibitsubmissivebehavior more rapidly
reactions, but to focus on those most com-
when injected withACTH just beforethe en-
monlyused in the scientificliteratureon both
counter (Leshner and Politch, 1979). This is
laboratory and domestic animals.
due to release of corticosteroidsin response
Since the classic work of Hall (1934), the
to ACTH injection (excitabilityof the gluco-
"open-field"testhas been extensivelyused in
corticoidreceptors), since the behavior is not
rodents (Archer, 1973b). This experimental
observed in adrenalectomized animals whose
situation provides a number of threatening
plasma corticosteronelevels are kept constant
factorssuch as novelty,absence of shelterand
by corticosteroneinfusion.
landmarks, and bright lighting. Later, this
All of these observations suggest that the
testwas used fordomestic fowl(Faure et al.,
neuroendocrine state of the brain may be the
1983) because of its methodological facility
ultimate mechanism for the organization of
(easy use, rapidity,automatization,standardi-
emotional responses. Moreover, autonomic
zation, and repeatability). More recently,it
and neuroendocrine patternsgive additional
has been applied to domestic mammals and
informationabout the emotional state of the
in particularto cattle (Kilgour, 1975; Koval-
animal.
At this level of the analysis, attentionhas cikova and Kovalcik, 1982; Dantzer et al.,
to focus on the fact that the identificationof 1983; Boissy, 1990), pigs (Mormede, Dant-
fear-inducingstimulidepends on the type of zer, Bluthe, and Caritez, 1984; Taylor and
responsestheyproduce. By contrast,thesere- Friend, 1986), and sheep (Moberg et al., 1980;
sponses, labeled as fear-relatedacts, depend Lachaux et al., 1983).
on the type of stimuli inducing them. Fear- Some other tests also have been used to
producing stimuliand fear-relatedresponses assess neophobia. The introductionofa novel
are thusinextricably bound. Accordingto Hinde object into the familiarenvironmentand the
(1985), an emotional state cannot be consid- opportunityto leave thehome cage to explore
ered to be independent of the factorsthat in- unfamiliar surroundings,that is, the "emer-
duce itnor oftheresponsesexpressingit. These gence test,"have been used in fowl (Archer,
psychobiologicalresponsesare not stereotyped. 1976a; Murphy, 1977) and rodents (Barnett
A major determinantoftheemotionalresponse and Cowan, 1976). Reactions to novel objects
may be regarded as theresultofan interaction have also been studiedin heifers(Boissy, 1990),
between (1) the propertiesof the threatening goats (Lyons et al., 1988) and pigs (Monnede,
event, (2) the possibilitiesthat are offeredby Dantzer, Bluthe,and Caritez, 1984; Lawrence
the environmentto the individual to control et al., 1991).
the danger, thatis, cognitiveperception,and Hall (1941) described othermethodsforas-
(3) theneuroendocrinestateoftheindividual. sessing fear and anxietyin rodents: handling
The most efficientapproach to the identifica- by humans, exposureto visual or acousticfear-
tion of a fear or anxiety state in the animal producing stimuli,movementin a maze, and
is to consider these aspects interactively. administrationof inescapable electric shock.
174 THE QUARTERL Y REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

Fear has also been measured by the decrease challenges such as exposure to novelty(Kopin
in socialinteractionbetweenpairsofratsplaced et al., 1989). It also can be graded by modi-
in a novel enclosure(File, 1980). Most ofthese fyingthe alarm-causing characteristicsof the
tests were later used in fowl Uones, 1987a). same threateningevent.
Domestic ungulates have also been exposed Some resultssuggestthat the response can
to these threateningsituations. Confinement be reliably related to the degree of environ-
was testedin cattle(Stephensand Toner, 1975; mentalchallenge. In rodentssubjectedto elec-
Veissier et al., 1987), exposure to a stimulus tric shock, behavioral responses (Natelson et
associated withan unavoidable noxious event al., 1987), plasma catecholamine levels (Na-
was studied in pigs (Dantzer and Mormede, telson et al., 1981), and HPA axis reactions
1980), while exposure to humans was tested (Wiener and Levine, 1983) increase withthe
in goats(Lyons and Price, 1987), cattle(Boissy intensityof the stimulation.In a series of ex-
and Bouissou, 1988), and pigs (Lawrence et perimentsusing various species, it was dem-
al., 1991). Presentation ofa natural predator onstratedthatifthe gradientofnoveltyvaries
(dog) was used in sheep (Torres-Hernandez withchanges in thestimuluselements,thereis
and Hohenboken, 1979), and the response to a correlatedgraded psychobiologicalresponse
sound stimulationwas recordedin both sheep according to the degree of differencefromthe
(Ames and Arehart, 1972) and cattle (Boissy normal environmentof the organism. Faure
and Le Neindre, 1990). (1975, 1979) andJones (1977b) reportedthat
Another methodologywas also devised to the duration of tonic immobility(i.e., reac-
assess fearthatinvolvespassive avoidance con- tion induced by physical restraint)decreases
ditioning (avoidance of an unpleasant event in chickens once the animals become accus-
by the inhibitionofa previouslyrewarded be- tomed to the test. A similar result was ob-
havior) and active avoidance conditioning(es- tained in the same species when familiarob-
cape fromthe unpleasant event by displaying jectswereintroduced intothepen Uones, 1977a).
a particularbehavior,e. g., two-waytest)(Selig- The increase in the level ofnoveltyin the pen
man, 1975). In addition to experimentswith increases the defecationrate in rats (Ivinskis,
rodentsand fowl,theseconditioningtestshave 1970), the plasma level of corticosteroidsin
been used in pigs (Dantzer and Mormede, mice (M. B. Hennessy and Levine, 1978, M.
1981a). B. Hennessy and Foy, 1987) and rats (Arm-
Many other experimental situations have ario et al., 1986), the duration of freezing
been designed.As Archer(1973b) pointedout, in chicks Uones and Faure, 1982), and the
however, these testsdo not take into account frequencyofmovementsin calves(Dellmeier et
thebiological significanceofthebehavior pro- al., 1985). Likewise, exposure to increasingly
duced, and oftenhad littleor no relationto the unfamiliarsurroundingsincreases the ambu-
behavioral repertoireof the species studied. lation rate in sows (L. Taylor and Friend,
1986) and calves (Dellmeier et al., 1985), in
CHARACTERIZATION OF
which the level of plasma cortisol also rose
FEAR-RELATED RESPONSES
(Dantzer et al., 1983). However, correlations
RelationbetweenEnvironmental are not always found between the level of the
Challengeand EmotionalReactivity disturbanceand subsequentreactions.For ex-
In orderto assess the effectivenessof study- ample, the adrenal response of rats subjected
ing emotional reactivityin animals, attention to electricshock is not systematicallyrelated
has long focusedon correlations.One attempt to the intensityof the stimulus (Natelson et
to achieve an objectiveevaluationofemotional al., 1981, 1987).
reactivityassociated with fear is to measure
Differences
Assessmentof Interindividual
response afterexperimentallyvaryingthe de-
gree of threat. The degree of threat can be variationin thebehav-
A greatinterindividual
graded by changing the triggerevent from ioral and neuroendocrinereactivityto threat-
slight disturbances, such as gentle handling eningchallengeshas oftenbeen described.The
oftheanimal, to major physicaleventssuch as importance of this individual reactivityhas
restraintor electricshock, and psychological led to examination of whether:(1) the behav-
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 175

ioralpatternscould be linkedto particularneu- rate Uones et al. , 1981). For confinedrabbits,


roendocrine traits,(2) the scores obtained in tonic immobilityis longer when the plasma
differentenvironmental challenges correlate levels of corticosteroneare high (Carli et al.,
withinindividuals, and (3) thepsychobiologi- 1979).
cal responses to an alarming event are consis- Behavioral reactivityand endocrine char-
tent over time. acteristics are not always related, however.
The negative correlationbetween defecation
between
Relations BehavioralReactivity and ambulation in rodents subjected to an
andNeuroendocrine Traits open-fieldtestis sometimesnonsignificant (Har-
The significanceofinterrelationships is stud- rington, 1972; Ley, 1975; Misslin et al., 1976).
ied from behavioral and endocrine parame- And the magnitude of fear-relatedbehaviors
tersrecorded during the same environmental cannot be systematicallylinked to physiologi-
challenge. Abundant evidence shows thatbe- cal parameters. In ratstestedin an open field,
havioralcharacteristics are linkedto differential the duration of freezingis independentof the
physiologicaland neuroendocrineresponsesto increase in heart rate (Candland et al., 1967)
threateningsituations.Defecation rate in ro- and corticosteroidplasma levels (Stern et al.,
dents, the most frequentlyused index of fear 1973; Restrepo and Armario, 1987). In re-
in open-fieldtestssincetheworkofHall (1934), sponse to an acoustic startlestimulus,Svens-
is negativelycorrelatedwithambulation(Tachi- son and coworkers(1991) reporteda dissocia-
bana, 1982; Gamallo et al., 1986; Dishman tion between behavioral and cardiovascular
et al., 1988) and positivelycorrelatedwiththe reactivityin rats. In squirrelmonkeysexposed
increase in heart rate (Candland and Nagy, to novelty, there is interindividualvariation
1969; Blizard, 1971), plasma catecholamines in the magnitude of behavioral disturbance
(McCarty and Kopin, 1978a), and the initial but no differencein adrenocorticalresponses
plasma levels of corticosterone(Gamallo et (Coe et al., 1982). The relationsbetweenhor-
al., 1986). monal and behavioral responses accompany-
Such correlationsalso existin domesticani- ing fearare not more marked in domestic un-
mals. When chickensare subjectedto an open- gulates. Behavioral response to a threatening
field test, the duration of freezing varies in event is not related to heart rate in goats (Ly-
proportion to their adrenal response Uones ons and Price, 1987) nor to plasma levels of
and Merry, 1988). In sheep testedin a novel glucocorticoidsin cows (Lefcourtet al., 1986).
environment,Lankin (1976) found a positive Most testsusing novel environmentsthatare
correlationbetween the ambulation rate and performedon pigs (Dantzer and Mormede,
the subsequent increase in the plasma level 198lb), sheep (Moberg and Wood, 1982), and
ofcortisol.In Chinese pigs exposed to a novel calves (Arave et al., 1980; Dantzer et al., 1983)
environment,a high behavioral reactivityis have shown a dissociationbetweenlocomotor
negatively correlated with low plasma levels reactivityand activationof the pituitary-adre-
of ACTH and adrenaline (Mormede, Dan- nal axis. Furthermore,the differentphysio-
tzer, Bluthe, and Caritez, 1984). In the other logical indices seldom show high intercorrela-
experimentalchallenges, several correlations tions (Lacey, 1967).
between behavioral and physiological values
are also reported as significant.In rats ex- RelationsbetweenResponsesto DifferentChallenges
posed to electric shock, for example, the in- The interestin individual differenceshas
crease in behavioral reaction depends closely led researchersto ask whetherindividuals are
on the rise in the plasma levels of circulating consistentin theirresponses to differentchal-
catecholamines(McCarty and Kopin, 1978b). lenges, and many studies indicate that they
Exposure to an unexpected acoustic stimula- are. Brush and coworkers(1985) reportedthat
tionproduces an inverserelationshipbetween ratsexhibitinglow performanceduringactive
the amplitude of the behavioral reaction and avoidance conditioninghave a higherdefeca-
the increase of corticosteronelevels (Glowa tion rate in a novel environment.The defeca-
et al., 1992). Behavioral reactionsofhens ex- tion rates of rats in conditioned feartestsand
posed to humans are correlated with heart open-fieldtestsare positivelycorrelated(Ley,
176 THE QUARTERL Y RE VIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

1975). Correlationsare foundamongseveral pa- tionalityto describe the behavioral reactions


rametersmeasured in open-field,emergence, (mainlythereductionofactivity)and thephysi-
and tonic immobilitytestsand in response to ological changes (higherdefecationscores,for
a frighteningstimulus in chicks Jones and example) observedin a ratexposed to an open-
Mills, 1983), hens Uones, 1987c, 1988), and fieldtest.Later, thenumerouscross-testcorre-
Japanese quails, Coturnixcoturnixjaponica(Mills lationsobservedamong variousfear-related re-
and Faure, 1986). Good correlationsamong sponses suggested thatthese responses might
responses to a fewkinds offrighteningstimuli be mediatedby the same psychobiologicalpro-
have also been obtained in hens (Craig et al., cesses. Gray (1987) interpretsfearas a hypo-
1983), dogs (Goddard and Beilharz, 1984), and theticalstateofthe brain and neuroendocrine
cattle(Boissy, 1990). systemthat occurs under certain conditions
A limitednumber ofstudiesshow thatthere and resultsin certainformsofbehavior. Fear-
is no strikingcorrelation among the values fulnessmay thus be considered an inner trait
recorded in various experimental challenges. of the individual, an intermediarybetween
In rats (Overstreet et al., 1992) and in chick- perceived stimulations(or independent vari-
ens (Archer,1976a), behavioralreactionsmea- ables) and psychobiologicalresponses (or de-
sured in the open field are not significantly pendentvariables)(Ivinskis,1970; Gray, 1979).
correlatedwith any parameter recorded dur- Accordingly,fearfulnessmay be definedas a
ing the emergence test. basic constitutionaltrait that determinesthe
extentto which an individual becomes fright-
ofEmotional
Consistency ResponsesoverTime
ened in potentiallyalarming situations. We
Psychobiological is definedby those
reactivity can expect, therefore,that when an animal
individual characteristicsthatare consistently exhibitsa high degree of emotional reactivity
displayed, not only in differentsituationsbut in responseto one challenge, itwillmostlikely
also over time. Measurements of flightreac- display high reactivityin responseto another,
tions in response to threateninghumans have similar situation. With this notion of a single
illuminated the issue of fear consistencyover scale to representemotional reactivity,it be-
time because several studies in domestic ani- comes possible to classifysubjects by the im-
mals have looked at the same individuals over portanceof certaincharacteristicresponsesto-
long periods. For example, Lyons and cowork- ward a few challenges.
ers (1988) found that goats, when tested at Not all tests,however, support the notion
several ages, show high stabilityin theirreac- ofconsistencyin reactionsto diverse environ-
tivitytoward humans. In heifers,flightreac- mental challenges, and some authors, in par-
tion in response to a standardized approach ticularArcher(1973b) and Hinde (1985), have
of a human remained fairlyconstant up to questioned the unitaryconstructas a means
seven months of age (Kerr and Wood-Gush, of definingfearfulness.Archer (1973b) said
1987), and in poultry,individuals are consis- that Hall (1934) used emotionalityas "a con-
tent in their tonic immobilityresponse over venient concept for describing a complex of
time(Jones,1988). Websterand Hurnick(1990) factors"assumed to express sympatheticactiv-
found thatthe distressresponses measured in ity.Taking thevarietyoffear-related responses
chickens in an open-fieldtest at 17 weeks of as a basis, Archer (1979) proposed a more
age are related to their behavioral reactivity flexiblemodel in whichreactionsare governed
recorded at age 2 days. by multifactorialinfluencesratherthan by a
single, central state.
SEARCH FOR A UNIFYING
CONCEPT OF FEARFULNESS ORIGINS OF INTERLABORATORY DISCREPANCIES

IS FEARFULNESS A UNITARY TRAIT The conflictingfindingsreportedabove raise


OR A COMPLEX OF FACTORS? questions about the sources ofinterindividual
Attemptshave long been made to interpret differences in emotionalreactivity.Among the
the differencesin individual reactions to the possiblesourcesofvariationthatmay be identi-
same environmentalchallengein orderto pre- fied are variation in experimentaldesign and
dict psychobiologicalresponses to other chal- interactionbetween genetic and ontogenetic
lenges. Hall (1934) firstused the term emo- factors.
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 177

Diversityof EffectsOwing to rect response to a predator (avoidance and


ProceduralVariations attackreactions),as opposed to thoseoccurring
Thevariety ofprocedures. The varietyofmeth- in response to olfactorycues indicating the
ods used in studies of fear and anxiety may potential threatof a predator (approach pat-
partlyexplainthediscrepanciesin results(Walsh terns).
and Cummins, 1976). Maintenance condi- The social context.Because it can alter the
tions could be one source of difference.Har- individual's response to stressfulevents ("so-
rington and Blizard (1983) showed that the cial buffering"),the social context is also a
resultsofopen-fieldtestsare not directlycom- potentsource ofvariation among data. A vast
parable ifanimals are previouslymaintained amountofdata on antipredatorstrategiesshow
under radicallydifferent conditions.
light-cycle that alarm calls are sensitive to the caller's
Defecation scores are substantiallyhigherfor social context. For example, alarm calls in
rats maintained under a continuous 24-hour female vervet monkeys, Cercopithecus aethiops,
lightdiurnal cycle than forthose maintained are potentiated by the presence of offspring
in a light-darkdiurnalcycle.The physicalcon- (Cheney and Seyfarth,1985). The vocal be-
ditionsofthe experimentalsituationmay also havior of domestic chickensvaries according
account for differencesamong studies. For to thenatureofthesignaler'saudience: Chick-
example, in rodentsthat are mainly active at ens give more aerial alarm calls in response
night,increasinglightintensityduring expo- to overhead hawk models when they are in
sure to an open-fieldtest increases the level the presence of conspecificsthan when they
ofthreat.The changein thebrightnessgradient are alone (Evans and Marler, 1992). Several
is sufficientto cancel thesignificant
correlations laboratorystudieshave investigatedthe effect
betweenambulation and defecationscoresob- of an audience on otheremotional behaviors.
served when the same test is performedwith In rats (G. T. Taylor, 1981), fowl(Jones and
a mean lightintensity(Tachibana, 1982). Be- Merry, 1988), and monkeys(Coe et al., 1982),
cause physicalconditions,ofbothmaintenance exposure to noveltyproduces less behavioral
and experimentation,differfrom one study disturbance when animals are tested with a
to another, theirinfluenceis all the more im- social partner than when they are alone. In
portant. The area of the device used to test fact,the animal does not even need to be in-
cattle in a novel environmentmay vary be- volved in a social interactionwithitscompan-
tween 12m2 (Canali et al., 1986) and lOOm2 ions forthereactivityto be reduced. In a study
(Kovalcikova and Kovalcik, 1982), and itsform withheifers,themere presence ofconspecifics
can be square (Boissy and Bouissou, 1988), is sufficientto diminishthe threateningeffect
rectangular(Dellmeier et al., 1985), circular of an unexpected event (Boissy and Le Nein-
(Warnicket al., 1977), or even T-shaped (Veis- dre, 1990).
sier et al., 1987). Inappropriateexperimentalconditions
forthespe-
The psychophysical conditionspresentwhen ciesstudied.Testing animals in inappropriate
an alarming event occurs may also influence environments,which is likelyto resultin ab-
the emotional response. Chickens exposed to normal and maladaptive behavior, can also
a novel environmenthave differentresponses lead to an inaccurate estimationof emotional
to the same sound stimulus, depending on reactivityand explain the lack of correlations
whetherthe sound is emittedby an object that among studies. Conditioned avoidance tests,
is localizable in the immediate environment forinstance, have been extensivelyvalidated
(Archer, 1976b). While fear and anxiety are as a means of assessing fear responses in ro-
both induced by threat, they can be distin- dents (Gray, 1987). The same type of test,
guished based on how the threatis presented. however, does not always seem to be accurate
The frighteningevent may be actual (fear- in evaluating fearin fowl.While a conditional
state associated) or anticipated based on the stimulusdifferentiates hens according to their
presentationof cues (anxiety-stateassociated). strainwhen theymust respond activelyto the
Blanchardand Blanchard(1990) providedstrong signal to avoid the aversive event (i.e., active
supportforsuch a distinction,indicatingthat avoidance), the same stimulusdoes not differ-
verydifferent behavioral patternsoccur in di- entiatebetweentwo strainsifthe animals must
178 THE QUARTERL Y REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

refrainfrommoving to avoid the unpleasant reported even more often at the population
event (passive avoidance) (Rutter and Dun- level than at the individual level. Large differ-
can, 1988). This suggeststhatfowlare distin- ences in the behavioral and neuroendocrine
guished during the active avoidance test by reactionsto aversiveeventsare foundbetween
theirabilityto exhibitan active response and breeds or strainsof rodents(Olivero and Cas-
not, as was assumed, by the magnitude of an tellano, 1990). For instance, in mice exposed
absolute fear response. Moreover, it is not to uncontrollablefootshock,themagnitude of
well known whethertests measure the same the increase in corticosteroneconcentration
motivations in males and females, and the and in the time required for it to returnto
validation of a test for one sex cannot be as- basal values vary with strains(Shanks et al.,
sumed to necessarilyapply to the other(John- 1990). Similarly, exposure to an unexpected
ston and File, 1991). acoustic stimulationelicits larger behavioral
Smallsamplesizes.Small sample sizes, which responses in Lewis rats than in Fisher rats
are not suitable formost statisticalanalyses, (Glowa et al., 1992). Breed-dependentdiffer-
may also be responsible for a lack of signifi- ences exist also in domestic mammals. For
cant correlationsamong thedata (Gray, 1987). example, compared with European breeds,
Tachibana (1982) and thenJones(1987c) showed Chinese pigs displaya low behavioralreactivity
how difficultit is to obtain statisticallysignifi- in responseto a novelenvironment(Mornmede,
cant resultsforas complex a phenomenon as Dantzer, Bluthe, and Cartitez, 1984).
fear from a limited amount of data and too These findingssuggest that genetic vari-
small a sample. abilitymay be partlyresponsible forthe dis-
crepancies among laboratories in which sub-
Diversityof EffectsOwing to Preexisting jects ofdifferentgeneticoriginshave been used.
IndividualCharacteristics It is not surprisingto findthatvariation in the
Adaptive responses to environmentalchal- expressionofemotional reactivityis relatedto
lenges are affectedby preexistingcharacteris- genetic variability. On the other hand, lim-
ticsofreactivitythatare based on interactions ited genetic variability can affectthe likeli-
between an individual's geneticbackgroundand hood offindingdifferences in reactivityamong
past environmental on developmental
influences individualsofthe same population. Since lab-
and learning processes. oratoryand domesticanimalshave been highly
selected,theinterindividualvariabilitywithin
Genetic
Background each strainis considerablyreduced.From seven
As already mentioned,the sex ofan animal inbred strainsof rats tested in an open field,
is a major factorin interindividualvariability Van Der Staay and his coworkers (1990) showed
in defensiveresponses.Adaptive processesare thatthe variabilityamong individuals within
also, to a certainextent,under geneticcontrol each strainwas clearlysmallerthanthevariabil-
(Bouchard et al., 1990; Plomin, 1990). A ge- ity among individuals fromdifferentstrains.
netic component accounting for part of the By studying correlations over strain means
interindividualvariabilityin reactions to hu- (genetic correlations)where the strainsserve
mans has been foundin several domestic spe- the role ofindividuals, these authorsreported
cies. In a studycomparing dam-reared goats strongcorrelationsbetweendifferent measures,
withhuman-rearedgoats, Lyons and cowork- both withinthe open-fieldtestand across dif-
ers (1988) found that among pairs of twins, ferenttests(open-fieldand emergence tests).
a goat's rank within the dam-reared group Accordingly,itmay be harder to demonstrate
predictsitssibling'srank in thehuman-reared correlationsbetweenvalues measured in sub-
group. A consistentheritabilityof reactivity jects belonging to the same population than
also has been foundby studyingdefensivere- those recorded from individuals of different
actions to humans in dairy cattle (Dickson strains.
et al., 1970), pigs (Hemsworth et al., 1990),
poultry(Jones, 1986), and dogs (Goddard and PastEnvironmentalInfluences
Beilharz, 1984). The influenceof geneticfac- Previouslearning,characterizedby habitua-
tors on psychobiologicaltraitsof reactivityis tion and classical conditioning,interactswith
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 179

the genetic background of each individual to number ofhippocampal glucocorticoidrecep-


modulate emotional reactivity.In poultry,for tors,which are implicated in both behavioral
example, repeated exposure to human beings and endocrine regulations (Renner and Ro-
(i.e., habituation) or association of a food re- senzweig, 1987; Meaney et al., 1989, respec-
ward withhuman contact (i.e., classical con- tively).
ditioning)decreases subsequentbehavioral re- The influencesofprevious experiencesand
activity(Jones, 1986; Murphy and Duncan, early environmentare partly responsible for
1976, respectively).On the otherhand, prior divergentresultsbetweenstudieson emotional
negativeexperiencecan dramaticallyincrease reactivity.For example, the fact that Jones
emotionalreactivity. For example,chronicstress (1977b) but not Archer(1973a) notes thatthe
(generallyinescapable electricshockpretreat- repetitionofthe open-fieldtestincreasesloco-
ment) predisposes individuals to react more motor activityin chickensmay simplybe due
fearfully to subsequentalarmingevents,as was to differencesin social housing methods. Un-
shown in fowl (Jones et al., 1988) and rats like the animals testedby Archer, those used
(Tejedor del Real et al., 1991). byJoneshad been rearedin a group and there-
In addition, isolation or separation from forewere faced not only withnoveltybut also
conspecifics,as well as repeated social stress, with separation fromconspecificsduring the
such as chronicsocial defeator social instability test. Thus, their increased activitywith re-
and subordinationrelationships,determinein- peated testingmight reflectan active search
dividual neuroendocrine reactivity(Henry, forconspecificsstimulatedby progressivere-
1982). For instance,dailyrotationofratsamong duction ofemotionallyinduced locomotorin-
differentsocial groups, which causes social hibition.
instability,activates the sympatheticnervous Differencesin developmental maturityamong
system(Mormede et al., 1990) and may dif- animals may affectthe reliabilityof results.It
ferentiallyaffectindividual reactivityto aver- is indeed known thatage influencesthe acute
sive challenges.These social stress-related dif- response to alarming events, although these
ferencesare not found only between animals effectshave not received much attention.For
with distinct social status, that is, between example, adult deer flee danger while young
dominants or subordinates. For example, the fawns adopt the characteristicfreezing pos-
differencein the neuroendocrine profilesof ture(Espmark and Langvatn, 1979). Because
free-rangingolive baboons, Papio anubis, in thephenotypeand environment interactthrough-
response to a physical threatis not so much out life, one would not expect perfectconsis-
between dominants and subordinates as it is tency over long periods of time. Therefore
between certain dominant individuals when it is not surprising that correlations among
the hierarchy is unstable (Sapolsky, 1990): resultsrecorded at different ages seldom show
Certain dominants exhibit a release of corti- large coefficientsof variation. Goddard and
costeroid in response to physical threatsthat Beilharz (1986) foundin dogs thatthepredict-
is similarto the hormonal patternof the sub- ability of an individual's fearfulnessdimin-
ordinates. ishes withtime fromthe initialmeasurement,
Abundant evidence is available suggesting particularlywhen behavioral assessment be-
that increased environmentalmanipulations gins at a young age.
in early lifeleads to marked increases in later To summarize,discrepanciesin resultsfrom
reactivity.Early environmentalinfluencesact different observationsare partlyrelatedto nu-
especially during sensitive periods of brain merous variations in procedural conditions
development. For example, rats or fowlsthat that tend to affectthe degree of stressfulness
received substantial stimulation in infancy, ofthesituationand limitthepossibilityofdirect
such as environmentalenrichmentor regular comparisonsamongstudies.In addition,genetic
handling,were generallyless reactivein adult- predispositionsand developmentalexperiences
hood (Chevins, 1990; Jones and Waddington, shape individualtendenciesto be anxious, fear-
1992, respectively).Early environmentalen- ful,or secure, and thusdifferentiate individu-
richmentand handlingmay also influenceneu- als on the basis of their reactions to adverse
roanatomical development by increasing the environmentalchallenges. Because emotional
180 THE QUARTERL Y REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

dispositionhas so much effectupon responses wide varietyofenvironmentalchallenges. For


to environmentalchallenges, traditionalindi- instance, Roman rats geneticallyselected for
catorsoffearand anxietymay provide contra- a low degree of active avoidance behavior ex-
dictoryresultsifstudiesdo not properlytake it hibit higher levels of corticosteroneand pro-
into account. This concept of interindividual lactin followingexposure to an open-fieldtest
diversityin emotional reactivityis illustrated than do rats displayinghigh active avoidance
in programs of genetic selection. behavior (Walker et al., 1989; Castanon and
Mormede, 1994). Maudsley rats, selectedfor
GENETIC MODELS OF theirhigh reactivityin an open field,are also
PSYCHOBIOLOGICAL REACTIVITY more reactivein an escape-avoidance conflict
The existence of robust strain-dependent (Commissaris et al., 1986) and spend less time
differencesin adaptive responses to aversive in an open arm comparedto a closedone (Over-
stimulationhas suggestedthatthis character- streetet al., 1992).
isticcan be geneticallymanipulated. Bidirec- Similar resultshave been obtained in poul-
tional genetic selection for a large range of try. Hens selected over several generations
behavioral and neuroendocrinereactivityhas for low locomotor activityin an open field
been successfulin rodents(Brush, 1991). For exhibited lengthened tonic immobilityreac-
example, strains of mice have been selected tions, marked responsivenessto startlingvi-
on the basis of their behavioral reactivityin sual or acoustic stimuli,a longer waiting pe-
open-fieldtests (De Fries et al., 1978). Ge- riodbeforeventuringinto an unfamiliararea,
netic selection has been carried out in rats and decreasedacceptanceofnovel food(Faure,
according to theirbehavioral responses to en- 1981). InJapanese quails, chicksfroma strain
vironmental challenge. Roman rats, for in- bred selectivelyfortheirhigh tonic immobil-
stance, were originally selected for extreme ityin response to handling by humans exhib-
differencesin two-wayactive avoidance per- ited higherreactionsof fearin open-fieldand
formance(Bignami, 1965). Maudsley ratswere emergence tests Uones et al., 1991; Jones et
selectivelybred for differencesin open-field al., 1992). Such wide-rangingeffectssuggest
defecation rate (Blizard, 1971; Broadhurst, thatgeneticmanipulationmightexerta marked
1975). Wild Norwayratswereselectedthrough effect on fearand defensivepsychobiological re-
many generations, either for reduced or for sponses.
high aggressive reactivitywith respectto hu- The association between neuroendocrine
mans (Naumenko et al., 1989). Divergent status and behavioral responses to threaten-
strainsof poultryhave also been successfully ing stimuli may result from a genetic link.
developed. For instance, hens were selected Such coselection is demonstrated by recent
over several generationson the basis of high studies. For example, by making nonsegre-
or low activityin the open-field test (Faure gatingcrosses (parental lines and F1) and seg-
and Folmer, 1975), and Japanese quails were regating crosses (F2 and backcrosses) of the
selectivelybred forexaggeratedor reduced ad- Roman lines, Castanon and her coworkers
renocorticaland behavioralresponsesto immo- (1991) foundthatavoidance performanceand
bilizationstress(Satterleeand Johnson, 1988; prolactinreactivityare geneticallycorrelated.
Mills and Faure, 1991, respectively).A heri- Such results suggest that the behavioral and
tabilityindex of0.5 forthe reactionsofdefen- neuroendocrine responses to environmental
sive dogs has led to breeding of individuals challengesmay be intimatelyinterrelatedand
according to theirbehavioral reactivityin re- regulated by a common central mechanism
sponse to an environmentalchallenge (God- thatis geneticallymodulated. Rather thanbe-
dard and Beilharz, 1984, 1986). ing genetically related systematically,how-
In selectionprograms,even though strains ever, thebehavioral and endocrinecharacters
were selected according to one particularbe- ofemotionalreactivitymay insteadresultfrom
havioral or endocrinecharacteristicmeasured fortuitouscoselection.And selectionin defen-
in a particular test, there are many recent sive responses may partlyexplain divergence
studies reportingthat the animals also differ among the resultsreviewed above. As an ex-
in other psychobiologicaltraitsobserved in a ample, Hendley and her collaborators(1986)
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 181

initiated a recombinant inbreeding program The above analyses illustratethe complexity


to separatehypertensiveand hyperactivetraits of fear and anxietyconcepts, and emphasize
that are associated in the SHR strain, a line recognitionof the importance of interactions
derived fromtheWistar-Kyotorat. They pro- among various factorsin modulating individ-
duced a new strainof ratsthatwere behavior- ual responses to aversive environmentalchal-
allyhyperreactiveto environmentalchallenge lenges. Although a certain consistencydoes
like the original SHR strain,but withouthy- existin individualemotionaldispositions,fear-
pertension.Such psychobiogenetically selected relatedstrategiesdepend indeed on numerous
strains provide an extremelyuseful tool for interactionsbetween: (1) these individualten-
investigatingthe biological bases ofinterindi- dencies of reactivitywhich are shaped by ge-
vidual differences in emotionalreactivity.This netic background (species, strain and sex),
new approach oughtto furtheran understand- experiences (early experiences and learning
ing of how geneticfactorsinfluencebrain or- processes) and the stateoftheneuroendocrine
ganization subserving individual variability, systems,and age or maturation, and (2) the
particularlyindividual susceptibilityto being characteristicsof the experimental environ-
frightened. ment: social context, properties of stressors
(includingsize, lighting,color, substrate,nov-
FEARFULNESS AS A COMPLEX INTERMEDIATE
elty,and procedure), controllability,and pre-
VARIABLE IN BEHAVIOR MECHANISMS
dictability.Thus the variation among results
As suggestedthroughoutthisarticle,psycho- is mainly due to this continuous modulation
biologicalreactionsto emotionalchallengesare of psychobiological reactivity.If experimen-
only indicators of fear, and cannot be direct tal designs were carried out in a more stan-
measures ofthe emotional state. Fear mustbe dardized way and if care were taken when
evaluated by studyingnot onlytheintensityof extrapolatingfromone resultto another, di-
a unique response but also the strategyof the vergenceamong resultsshould be diminished.
response. The different mechanisms underly- Clearly thereare at least twolines ofexperi-
ing fear-relatedresponses probably depend mental data that emphasize the relevance of
on othermotivationalsystemsthatmay modu- the concept of fearfulnessto a better under-
late these responses. For instance, differences standing of the interindividualvariabilityin
in locomotor activityrecorded in a particular adaptive behavior. First, there are many re-
testbetween two strainsof rats are consistent sultsshowingcovariationbetween behavioral
with the high or normal activity for which
and neuroendocrine traitsof fear, both at in-
the animals are selected,whereas the opposite
dividual and at group levels, such as among
response is observed in another threatening
breeds or strains. Second, the existenceof ge-
situation (Rogers et al., 1988). Therefore, it
netic relationshipsbetween some behavioral
is difficultto attribute a given behavior to
and neuroendocrinetraitsof reactivityallows
any singleemotion or motivation.To say that
investigatationof the biological bases of fear-
behavior recorded in a given environmentis
fulness.
a fearindicatordoes not necessarilymean that
fear is the only cause of this behavior. And Consequently, most argumentsagainst the
variations in the patternof the fear response concept offearfulnessas a basic characteristic
do not necessarilyimplydifferencesin the de- may arise from the difficultyof comparing
gree of fear. Because of the complexityof the resultsobtainedwithdifferent approaches. The
phenomena that can affectemotional behav- wide variabilityof factorsand responses re-
ior, it is not possible to relyon resultsisolated lated to an emotional stateis not put forward
fromcontext. A measurement used as a fear here as a reason forabandoning the concept
indicator in one situation cannot be directly of fearfulness.All this variability,however,
extrapolatedto others,and it is impossible to makes itnecessaryto relativizethe interpreta-
assess the magnitude ofconcepts like fearand tion of this concept. Fearfulness can then be
anxiety on the basis of a single objective and definedas a basic characteristicofthe individ-
perfectmeasurement. It is likelythatthevari- ual that predisposes it to react in a similar
ety of fear-relatedresponses representdiffer- manner to a large range of potentiallychal-
ent coping strategies. lengingevents. This profileofgeneral reactiv-
182 THE QUARTERL Y REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

ityis obviouslyadaptable, since itresultsfrom to thereinforcing effects


ofthedrug(O'Brien et
the dynamicinteractionbetweengeneticback- al., 1986). Recent researchin rodentsshowed
ground and epigenetic factorssuch as early thatvulnerabilityto amphetamineself-admin-
influencesand previous experiences. istration,used as an experimental model of
drug addiction, may be predictedin individu-
FEARFULNESS AS AN INDEX OF VULNERABILITY als by assessing theiremotional reactivityto-
TO STRESS AND STRESS-RELATED DISEASES ward a novel environment(Piazza et al., 1991).
An imbalance between environmentalde- The higherthe locomotor and corticosterone
mand on the one hand, and psychobiological responses of rats to open-fieldexposure are,
reactionsand neuroendocrineemotionalstates the greater is their vulnerabilityto psycho-
on theother,may account forvarious psycho- stimulants.
somatic diseases. The role of neuroendocrine One problem with animal fear as a model
systemsas etiologic factorsin mental disor- forhuman emotionis thatfearin humans often
ders or pathological outcomes, such as hyper- concernspsychosocialthreatratherthan psy-
tension and immune suppression, has often chophysicalthreat.Indeed, as shownthrough-
been hypothesized (De Wied, 1979; Henry, out this article, the study of emotion in ani-
1988; Karasek and Theorell, 1990). Given mals is oftenbased on pain conditioning,conflict
this complex framework,questions arise not between punishment threat and hunger, or
only about the originsand correlatesof inter- the fear of extreme novelty or open spaces.
individual differencesin emotional reactions, The question is whetherthese animal models
but also about consequences ofthisvariability are the best strategies for investigatingthe
in long-termadaptive processes. Could indi- biological bases of the psychosocial reactivity
vidual differencesin the developmentofstress in humans. It may be suggested that animal
responses or stress-inducedphysiopathology models using psychosocialcues would be use-
be predicted by a particular set of emotional ful for investigatingthe human personality.
traits? The study of emotional reactivityis Indeed withinanimal colonies, emotional re-
likelyto bring considerable benefitsshould a action of individuals is often observed as a
set of fearfulnesstraitsbe identifiedthat pre- response to threatarising fromtheir conspe-
dict the likelihood of developing stress and cificgroup members, or at least is influenced
stress-relatedpathologies. A few studies sug- by the social context(Brain, 1990). Regard-
gest thatthe development of stresscan be in- ing their linkages with human anxiety sys-
fluencedby the susceptibilityto be more easily tems, such animal models mightbe useful in
frightened.For instance,it is observedin poul- providing homologies to a varietyof human
try and rats that the more intense the fear psychopathological conditionsarisingfromma-
reactionsare, thegreaterare thepsychobiologi- jor life-challengingcircumstances.
cal consequences fora chronicstressor(Jones, Investigation of emotional reactivitymay
1989; Tejedor del Real et al., 1991, respec- also be particularlyrelevantto assessment of
tively). A betterunderstanding of interindi- welfarein animal husbandry, since the well-
vidual variability in emotional reactivityis being of an individual depends on whetherit
thus essential to opening new avenues of re- can effectivelycope withenvironmentalchal-
search in the human clinical and applied ani- lenge (Broom, 1988). Artificialselection for
mal sciences. improved zootechnical performancehas been
Given their possible relevance for human accompanied by selectionforadaptabilityto in-
pathologies, animal models of psychobiologi- tensivehousing(Price, 1984). However, farm-
cal reactivitymay be useful for developing ing practice has changed too frequentlyand
noninvasive experimental tools for studying too rapidly for animal adaptive processes to
the physiopathologicalmechanisms underly- evolvesufficientlyas correlatedcharacters.More-
ing vulnerabilityto anxiety-relatedor stress- over, in intensivehusbandry,housing condi-
related disorders. For example, the clinical tionsprofoundlyrestrictthe degree of control
study of drug addiction has indicated that a the animals have. As we saw earlier, thisloss
major factorinfluencingpsychoactivedrug in- of control is often associated with increased
take in humans is an individual'svulnerability HPA axis activitythat may lead to various
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 183

psychosomatictroubles. A large range of ab- emotional behaviors (Blanchard et al., 1990).


normal and undesirable behaviors, such as Moreover, the vast amount of data on the
aggression,overeating,stereotypiesand with- contributionofsocialcues to antipredatorstrat-
drawal, occurs in modern farming(Dantzer egies, which are studied in free-rangingani-
and Mormede, 1983). A fewstudies associate mals, suggest a new alternativeforthe study
lower productivitywithan increased suscepti- of emotions in the laboratory: inducing fear
bilityto being frightened.For instance, goats or anxietythroughthe emotional reactionsof
thatare experimentallyratedas more reactive conspecifics.As compared withtraditionalin-
present a higher degree of milk ejection im- vestigations,whichstudyreactionsoftheindi-
pairment(Lyons, 1989). The findingsreported vidual directlyexposed to a challenge,models
throughoutthisarticlesuggestthatemotional based on thesocial communicationoffearmay
reactivityin domestic animals may be open avert subjective interpretationand anthropo-
to experimentalmanipulation geared toward morphism.
reducingtheincidenceofsome psychobiologi-
cal troubles. Because emotional reactivityis
CONCLUSION
to a certain degree under genetic control, a
selection program to reduce emotional reac- Emotional reactionshave an importantim-
tivitymay be undertaken in domestic mam- pact on the relationshipof an animal with its
mals, as ithas in rodentsand poultry.Because environment.The psychobiologicalresponses
experiencesduringparticularlysensitiveperi- to environmental challenges depend on the
ods are also important, changes in human- geneticbackground and earlyor previous ex-
animal relationshipsduringtheseperiodsmay periences of the individual. A complex inter-
be recommended(Boissy and Bouissou, 1988; action of these factorsis responsible for the
Boivin et al., 1992). The development of re- large range ofvariationin fear-relatedor anx-
search for assessing in domestic animals the iety-relatedresponses. Personality,tempera-
patternsof psychobiologicalreactivitymight ment or individual behavior exists in nonhu-
improveour understandingofthe factorsand man animals, and considerable progresshas
mechanismsgoverningthe elicitationof stress, to be made in the understandingof interindi-
and offernew strategiesforimprovingthewell- vidual differences.Fearfulnesshas to be con-
being of animals, and improvingproduction sidered as a componentof personalityand we
efficiencyas well. cannot dismiss the validity of fearfulnessas
The conceptofemotionalreactivityis there- an intermediatevariable that partlyexplains
fore importantand should be considered in the interindividualvariabilityobserved in an-
nonhuman animal studies. Moreover, animal imal behavior. Furtherstudies are needed to
models are needed to establish the biological better understand the modifiabilityof emo-
bases ofhuman personality,because theyoffer tional traitsand the relative contributionsof
an experimentalapproach thatis impossibleto heredity,early environmentalinfluences,and
employ in humans. Nevertheless, as shown learningin the genesis ofinterindividualvari-
throughoutthisarticle,anthropomorphism of- abilityin fear-relatedand anxiety-relatedre-
ten influencesthe definitionof emotion-induc- sponses. The developmentofnew approaches
ing events.The question is whethertraditional and technologiesis now essential forprovid-
animal models,essentiallybased on pain condi- ing appropriate strategiesfor the investiga-
tioningand conflict,are best forinvestigating tion of fear and adaptive processes. Social
emotions.It is suggestedthatsome approaches signals fromanimal communication that un-
realizedin semi-naturalistic
environmentsmay equivocally induce fear will allow us to study
be more useful, and that reactions in these psychobiologicalpatternsthatmightbe func-
settingsbettermimick responses to threatsin tionally interpretedas emotional reactions.
natural settings.Indeed, laboratory animals As Brain (1990) says, such developments"may
exposed to natural situationsassociated with enable us to make more sense of what is cur-
nonpainfulthreats(predators, partial preda- rentlya ratherconfused literature."In addi-
torystimuliand contextualstimuliassociated tion to the interestin predictingthe adaptive
with predators) display a broad spectrumof responses of individuals to various environ-
184 THE QUARTERL Y REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

mentalchallenges,the studyof fearfulness can ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

be used as a new approach to investigatethe I am gratefulto M. F. Bouissou,J. P. Signoret,


processesthatunderliethevariabilityin vulner- and P. Le Neindre fortheirhelpfulcomments on
abilityto stressand to stress-relateddiseases. the earlier version of the manuscript. I am greatly
Such approaches are sure to have consider- indebted to D. A. de Catanzaro forhaving offered
able relevance forhuman clinical and applied many useful suggestions, including correctingthe
animal sciences. English of the paper.

REFERENCES
Abbott, D. H., A. S. McNeilly, S. F. Lunn, M. J. Bignami, G. 1965. Selection for high rates and
Hulme, and F. J. Burden. 1981. Inhibition of low rates of avoidance conditioning in the rat.
ovarian functionin subordinate femalemarmo- Anim. Behav., 13:221-227.
set monkeys(Callithrixjacchusjacchus). J. Reprod. Blanchard, R. J., and D. C. Blanchard. 1990.
Fertil.,63:335-345. Anti-predatordefenseas models of animal fear
Ames, D. R., and L. A. Arehart. 1972. Physiologi- and anxiety.In P. F. Brain, S. Parmigiani,R. J.
cal response of lambs to auditory stimuli. J. Blanchard, and D. Mainardi (eds.), Fear and
Anim. Sci., 3:994-998. Defence,pp. 89-108. Harwood Academic Pub-
Anisman, H., D. de Catanzaro, and G. Reming- lishers, Chur.
ton. 1978. Escape performancefollowingexpo- Blanchard,R. J., D. C. Blanchard,J. Rodgers,
sure to inescapable shock: deficitsin motor re- and S. M. Weiss. 1990. The characterization
sponse maintenance.j Exp. Psychol. Anim.Behav. and modelling ofantipredatordefensivebehav-
Processes,4:197-218. ior. Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev., 14:464-472.
Arave, C. W., J. L. Walters, and A. J. Svejda. Blizard, D. A. 1971. Autonomic reactivityin the
1980. Responses to open field testing of Hol- rat: effectsofgeneticselectionforemotionality.
stein heifers reared under four differentsys- J. Comp. Physiol.Psychol.,76:282-289.
tems. J. Dairy Sci., 63:145. Bohus, B., R. F. Benus, D. S. Fokkema, J. M.
Archer,J. 1973a. The influenceoftestosteroneon Koolhaas, C. Nyakas, G. A. van Oortmerssen,
chick behavior in novel environments. Behav. A. J. A. Prins,A. J. H. de Ruiter,A. J. W.
Biol., 8:93-108. Scheurink, and A. B. Steffens.1987. Neuroen-
. 1973b. Tests for emotionalityin rat and docrine states and behavioral and physiological
mice: a review. Anim. Behav., 21:205-235. stressresponses. In E. R. de Kloet, V. M. Wie-
. 1975. Rodent sex differencesin emotional gant, and D. De Wied (eds.), Progressin Brain
and related behavior. Behav.Biol., 14:451-479. Research,pp. 57-70. Elsevier Science Publish-
. 1976a. Emergence tests in testosterone- ers, Amsterdam.
treated chicks. Physiol.& Behav., 16:513-514. Boissy, A. 1990. Les reactions emotives chez les
. 1976b. Testosterone and fearbehavior in Bovins domestiques femelles (Bos taurusL.):
male chicks. Physiol.& Behav., 17:561-564. quantification et variations sous l'influencede
facteursenvironnementauxet hormonaux. PhD
. 1976c. The organizationof aggressionand
thesis, University of Paris XIII, Paris.
fear in vertebrates. In P. P. G. Bateson and
Boissy, A., and M. F. Bouissou. 1988. Effectsof
P. Klopfer (eds.), Perspectives in Ethology,Vol.
early handling on heifers'subsequent reactivity
2, pp. 231-298. Plenum Press, New York.
to humans and to unfamiliar situations. Appl.
. 1979. Behavioural aspects of fear. In
Anim. Behav. Sci., 20:259-273.
W. Sluckin (ed.), Fear in Animalsand Man, pp.
, and . 1994. Effects of androgen
56-85. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York.
treatmenton behavioral and physiological re-
Armario, A.,J. L. Montero, andJ. Balasch. 1986. sponses of heifers to fear-elicitingsituations.
Sensitivity of corticosterone and some meta- Horm. Behav., 28:66-83.
bolic variables to graded levels of low intensity Boissy, A., and P. Le Neindre. 1990. Social influ-
stresses in adult male rats. Physiol. & Behav., ences on the reactivityof heifers: implications
37:559-561. for learning abilities in operant conditioning.
Axelrod, J., and T. D. Reisine. 1984. Stress hor- Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 25:149-165.
mones: theirinteractionand regulation. Science, Boivin, X., P. Le Neindre, J. M. Chupin, and
224:452-459. J. P. Garel. 1992. Influence of breed and early
Barnett, S. A., and P. E. Cowan. 1976. Activity, management on handling facilityand open-field
exploration, curiosityand fear: an ethological behaviour of heifers. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.,
study. Interdiscip.Sci. Rev., 1:43-62. 32:3 13-323.
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 185

Bouchard, T. J.,D. T. Lykken,M. McGue, N. L. Cannon, W. B. 1935. Stresses and strains of ho-
Segal, and A. Telleguen. 1990. Sources of hu- meostasis. Am. J. Med. Sci., 189:1-14.
man psychological differences:the Minnesota Carli, G., F. Farabollini, and C. Lupo Di Prisco.
study of twins reared apart. Science,250:223- 1979. Plasma corticosteroneand its relation to
228. susceptibilityto animal hypnosisin rabbits.Neu-
Bouissou, M. F. 1985. Contribution a l'etude des rosci.Lett., 11:271-274.
relations interindividuelleschez les Bovins do- Castanon, N., and P. Mormede. 1994. Psychobio-
mestiques femelles(Bos taurusL.). PhD thesis, genetics-adapted tools forthe study of the cou-
University of Paris VI, Paris. pling between behavioral and neuroendocrine
Brain, P. F. 1980. Adaptive aspects of hormonal traits of emotional reactivity. Psychoneuroendo-
correlates of attack and defence in laboratory crinology, 19:257-282.
mice: a studyin ethobiology. In P. S. McCon- Castanon, N., C. Sandi, J. Dulluc, M. Le Moal,
nell, G. J.Boer, H. J.Romijn, N. E. Van De and P. MormZede.1991. Genetic analysis ofthe
Poll, and M. A. Corner (eds.), Progress in Brain relationshipsbetween behavioral and neuroen-
Research,AdaptiveCapabilitiesof theNervousSys- docrine traits in Roman low and high avoid-
tem,pp. 391-414. Elsevier Science Publishers, ance lines of rats. Endocr.Soc., 73:498.
Amsterdam. Chaouloff, F. 1993. Physiopharmacological inter-
. 1990. A historical look at the concepts actions between stress hormones and central
of fear and defence and some conmments on serotonergicsystems.BrainRes. Rev., 18:1-32.
psychobiology. In P. F. Brain, S. Parmigiani, Cheney, D. L., and R. M. Seyfart. 1985. Vervet
R. J.Blanchard, and D. Mainardi (eds.), Fear monkey alarm calls: manipulation through
andDefence,pp. 1-21. Harwood Academic Pub- shared information?Behaviour,94:150-166.
lishers, Chur. Chevins, P. F. D. 1990. Early environmentalinflu-
Broadhurst, P. L. 1975. The Maudsley reactive ences on fear and defence in rodents. In P. F.
and non-reactive strains of rats: a survey. Be- Brain, S. Parmigiani, R. J. Blanchard, and
hav. Genet.,5:299-319. D. Mainardi (eds.), Fear and Defence,pp. 269-
Broom, D. M. 1988. The scientificassessment of 288. Harwood Academic Publishers, Chur.
animal welfare.Appl.Anim.Behav.Sci., 20:5-19. Coe, C. L., D. Franklin,E. R. Smith, and S. Le-
Brush, F. R. 1991. Genetic determinantsof indi- vine. 1982. Hormonal responses accompa-
vidual differencesin avoidance learning: be- nyingfearand agitationin the squirrelmonkey.
havioral and endocrine characteristics. Expe- Physiol.& Behav., 29:1051-1057.
rientia,47:1039-1050. Commissaris, R. L., G. M. Harrington, A. M.
Brush, F. R., S. Baron, J. C. Froehlich, J. R. Ortiz, and H. J. Altman. 1986. Maudsley reac-
Ison, L. J. Pellegrino, D. S. Phillips, P. C. tive and non-reactive rat strains: differential
Sakellaris, and V. N. Williams. 1985. Genetic performance in a conflicttask. Physiol. & Be-
differences in avoidance learningby Rattus nor- hav., 38:291-294.
vegicus: escape/avoidance responding, sensitiv- Craig,J. V., and A. W. Adams. 1984. Behaviour
ity to electric shock, discrimination learning and well-being of hens (Gallus domesticus)in al-
and open-fieldbehavior. J. Comp. Psychol.,99: ternativehousing environments. World'sPoult.
60-73. Sci.J., 40:221-240.
Buchholz, R. A., J. E. Lawler, and G. F. Barker. Craig, J. V., T. P. Craig, and A. D. Dayton.
1981. The effects ofavoidance and conflictsched- 1983. Fearful behavior by caged hens of two
ules on the blood pressure and heart rate of genetic stocks. Appl. Anim.Ethol., 10:263-273.
rats. Physiol.&Behav., 26:853-863. Curio, E. 1988. Cultural transmission of enemy
Buitron, D. 1983. Variability in the response of recognitionin birds. In T. R. Zentall and B. G.
black-billed magpies to natural predators. Be- Galef (eds.), SocialLearning.Lawrence Erlbaum
haviour,87:209-236. Associates, Hillsdale.
Canali, E., M. Verga, M. Montagna, and A. Baldi. Dantzer, R. 1986. Behavioral, physiological and
1986. Social interactions and induced behav- functional aspects of stereotyped behavior; a
ioural reactionsin milk-fedfemale calves. Appl. review and a reinterpretation. J. A nim.Sci., 62:
Anim. Behav. Sci., 16:207-215. 1776-1786.
Candland, D. K., and Z. M. Nagy. 1969. The Dantzer, R., and P. Mormede. 1980. Hormonal
open-field: some comparative data. Ann. N. Y. influences on conditioned fear in pigs. Appl.
Acad. Sci., 159:831-851. Anim. Ethol., 6:92-93.
Candland, D. K., K. D. Pack, and T. J. Mat- , and . 1981a. Influence du mode
thews. 1967. Heart rateand defecationfrequency d'elevage sur le comportementet l'activite hy-
as measures of rodent emotionality.J. Comp. pophyso-corticosurrenaliennedu porcelet. Re-
Physiol.Psychol.,64:146-150. prod. Nutr. Dev., 21:661-670.
186 THE QUARTERLY REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

, and . 1981b. Pituitary-adrenalcor- Fanselow, M. S. 1984. Shock-induced analgesia


relates of adjunctive activities in pigs. Horm. on the formalin test: effectsof shock severity,
Behav., 16:78-92. naloxone, hypophysectomy, and associativevari-
, and . 1983. Stressin farmanimals: ables. Behav. Neurosci.,98:79-95.
a need forreevaluation. J. Anim.Sci., 57:6-18. Fanselow, M. S., and F. J. Helmstetter. 1988.
Dantzer, R., P. Mormede, R. M. Bluthe, and Conditional analgesia, defensive freezing,and
J. Soissons. 1983. The effectof differenthous- benzodiazepines. Behav. Neurosci., 102:233-
ing conditions on behavioural and adrenocorti- 243.
cal reactions in veal calves. Reprod.Nutr.Dev., Faure, J. M. 1975. Etude des liaisons entre com-
23 :501-508. portement en open-field et emotivite chez le
De Fries,J. C., M. C. Gervais, and A. E. Thomas. jeune poussin. Ann. Genet.Sel. Anim., 7:197-
1978. Response to 30 generations of selection 204.
for open-field activityin laboratory mice. Be- 1979. Effetde l'habituation sur le compor-
hav. Genet.,8:3-14. tement en open-field du jeune poussin (Gallus
Dellmeier, G. R., T. H. Friend, and E. E. Gbur. domesticus). Biol. Behav., 4:241-248.
1985. Comparison of fourmethods of calf con- 1981. Bidirectionalselectionforopen-field
finement.II. Behavior. J. Anim. Sci., 60:1102- activityin young chicks. Behav. Genet.,11:135-
1109. 144.
De Wied, D. 1979. Schizophrenia as an inborn Faure,J. M., andJ. C. Folmer. 1975. Etude gene-
error in the degradation of f-endorphin: a hy- tique de l'activiteprecoce en open-fielddu jeune
pothesis. TrendsNeurosci.,2:79-82. poussin. Ann. Genet.Sel. Anim., 7:123-132.
Dickson, D. P., G. R. Barr, L. P. Johnson, and Faure, J. M., R. B. Jones, and W. Bessei. 1983.
D. A. Wieckert. 1970. Social dominance and Fear and social motivation as factorsin open-
temperament of Holstein cows. J. Dairy Sci., field behaviour of the domestic chick. A theo-
53:904-907. retical consideration. Biol. Behav., 8:103-116.
Dishman, R. K., R. B. Armstrong,M. D. Delp, Ferreira, A., S. Hansen, M. Nielsen, T. Archer,
R. E. Graham, and A. L. Dunn. 1988. Open- and B. G. Minor. 1989. Behavior ofmotherrats
fieldbehavior is not related to treadmillperfor- in conflicttests sensitiveto antianxietyagents.
mance in exercising rats. Physiol.& Behav., 43: Behav. Neurosci.,103:193-201.
541-546. File, S. E. 1980. The use of social interaction as
a methodfordetectinganxiolyticactivityofchlor-
Dodd, J., and L. W. Role. 1991. The autonomic
nervous system. In E. R. Kandel, J. H. diazepoxide-like drugs. J. Neurosci.Methods,2:
219-238.
Schwartz, and T. M. Jessell (eds.), Principles
Fleming A. S., and C. Luebke. 1981. Timidity
ofNeuralScience,3rd ed., pp. 761-775. Elsevier,
preventsthevirginfemaleratfrombeing a good
New York.
mother: emotionalitydifferencesbetween nul-
Duncan, I. J. H., and J. H. Filschie. 1979. The
liparous and parturientfemales. Physiol.& Be-
use of telemetrydevices to measure tempera-
hav., 27:863-868.
ture and heart rate in domestic fowl. In C. J.
Gallup, G. G., Jr. 1977. Tonic immobility: the
Amlaner and D. W. MacDonald (eds.), A Hand-
role of fear and predation. Psychol.Rec., 27:
bookonBiotelemetry and Radio Tracking,pp. 579- 41-61.
588. Pergamon Press, Oxford. Gamallo, A., A. Villanua, G. Trancho, and
Dunn, A. J., and C. W. Berridge. 1990. Physio- A. Fraile. 1986. Stress adaptation and adrenal
logical and behavioral responses to corticotro- activity in isolated and crowded rats. Physiol.
pin-releasing factor administration: Is CRF a & Behav., 36:217-221.
mediator of anxiety or stress responses? Brain Gibbs, D. M. 1986. Vasopressin and oxytocin:
Res. Rev., 15:71-100. hypothalamicmodulatorsof the stressresponse:
Eberhart,J. A., E. B. Keverne, and R. E. Meller. a review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 11: 131-140.
1983. Social influences on circulating levels of Glowa, J. R., M. A. Geyer, P. W. Gold, and
cortisol and prolactin in male talapoin mon- E. M. Sternberg. 1992. Differentialstartleam-
keys. Physiol.& Behav., 30:361-369. plitude and corticosterone response in rats.
Espmark, Y., and R. Langvatn. 1979. Cardiac Neuroendocrinology, 56:719-723.
responses in alarmed red deer calves. Behav. Goddard, M. E., and R. G. Beilharz. 1984. A
Processes.,4:179-186. factoranalysis of fearfulnessin potential guide
Evans, C. S., and P. Marler. 1992. Female ap- dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 12:253-265.
pearance as a factor in the responsiveness of , and . 1986. Early predictionofadult
male chickens during anti-predatorbehaviour behaviour in potential guide dogs. Appl. Anim.
and courtship. Anim. Behav., 43:137-145. Behav. Sci., 15:247-260.
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 187

Gray,J. A. 1979. Emotionalityin male and female . 1988. Emotions et hypertensionpsycho-


rodents. A reply to Archer. Br. J. Psychol.,70: sociale. Recl. Med. Vet., 164:751-766.
425-440. Hinde, R. A. 1985. Was "the expression of the
1987. The PsychologyofFear and Stress,2nd emotions" a misleading phrase? Anim. Behav.,
ed. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. 33:985-992.
Guillemin, R., T. Vargo, J. Rossier, S. Minick, Holstege, G. 1992. The emotional motor system.
N. Ling, C. Rivier, W. Vale, and F. Bloom. Eur. J. Morphol., 30:67-79.
1977. 13-endorphin and adrenocorticotropinare Ivanyi, T., V. M. Wiegant, and D. De Wied.
secretedconcomitantly.Science,197:1367-1368. 1991. Differentialeffectsof emotionaland physi-
Gyger, M., P. Marler, and R. Pickert. 1987. Se- cal stresson the centraland peripheralsecretion
mantics ofan avian alarm call system: the male of neurohypophysial hormones in male rats.
domestic fowl(Gallus domesticus). Behaviour,102:
Life Sci., 48:1309-1316.
15-40.
Ivinskis, A. 1970. A study of validityof open-field
Hall, C. S. 1934. Emotional behavior in the rat.
measures. Aust.J. Psychol.,22:175-183.
I. Defaecation and urination as measures of
Johnston, A. L., and S. E. File. 1991. Sex differ-
individual differencesin emotionality.J. Comp.
ences in animal tests of anxiety. Physiol.& Be-
Psychol.,18:385-403.
hav., 49:245-250.
. 1941. Temperament: a survey of animal
studies. Psychol.Bull., 38:909-943. Jones, R. B. 1977a. Open-field responses of do-
Hard, E., and S. Hansen. 1985. Reduced fear- mestic chicks in the presence or absence of fa-
fulness in the lactating rat. Physiol.& Behav., miliar cues. Behav. Processes,2:315-323.
35:641-643. . 1977b. Repeated exposure ofthedomestic
Harrington,G. M. 1972. Straindifferences in open- chick to a novel environment:effectson behav-
fieldbehavior ofthe rat. Psychon.Sci., 27:51-53. ioural responses. Behav. Processes,2:163-173.
Harrington,G. M., and D. A. Blizard. 1983. Open- . 1984. Experimental noveltyand tonic im-
field behavior in the Maudsley Reactive and mobility in chickens (Gallus domesticus).Behav.
Nonreactive strains: procedural variations. Be- Processes.,9:255-260.
hav. Genet.,13:91-94. . 1986. The tonic immobility reaction ol
Hemsworth, P. H., J. L. Barnett,D. Treacy, and the domestic fowl: a review. World'sPoult. Sci.
P. Madgwick. 1990. The heritabilityofthe trait J., 42:82-96.
fearofhumans and the association between this . 1987a. The assessment of fear in the do-
traitand subsequent reproductiveperformance mestic fowl. In R. Zayan and I. J. H. Duncan
of gilts. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 25:85-95. (eds.), Cognitive Aspectsof Social Behaviourin thz
Hendley, E. D., D. J. Wessel, andJ. Van Houten. Domestic Fowl, pp. 40-81. Elsevier, Amsterdam.
1986. Inbreeding of Wistar-Kyoto rat strain . 1987b. The social and environmental as-
with hyperactivitybut without hypertension. pects of fear in the domestic fowl. In R. Zayan
Behav. NeuralBiol., 45:1-16. and I. J. H. Duncan (eds.), Cognitive Aspectsoj
Hennessy, M. B., and T. Foy. 1987. Nonedible SocialBehaviourin theDomesticFowl, pp. 82-149.
material elicitschewing and reduces the plasma
Elsevier, Amsterdam.
corticosterone response during novelty expo-
. 1987c. The assessment of fear in adull
sure in mice. Behav. Neurosci.,101:237-245.
laying hens: correlational analysis of methods
Hennessy, M. B., and S. Levine. 1978. Sensitive
and measures. Br. Poult. Sci., 28:319-326.
pituitary-adrenalresponsiveness to varying in-
. 1988. Repeatability of fear ranks amonQ
tensities of psychological stimulation. Physiol.
adult laying hens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 19:
&Behav., 21:295-297.
Hennessy, J. W., and S. Levine. 1979. Stress, 297-304.
arousal, and thepituitary-adrenalsystem:a psy- . 1989. Chronic stressors,tonic immobility
choendocrine hypothesis.Prog.Psychobiol. Phys- and leucocytic responses in the domestic fowl.
iol. Psychol.,8:133-178. Physiol.& Behav., 46:439-442.
Henry, J.P. 1980. Present concept of stress the- Jones, R. B., G. Beuving, and H. J. Blokhuis.
ory. In E. Usdin, R. Kvetnansky, and I. J. 1988. Tonic immobilityand heterophil/lympho-
Kopin (eds.), Catecholamines andStress:RecentAd- cyteresponses ofthedomestic fowlto corticoste-
vances,pp. 557-571. Elsevier North Holland, rone infusion. Physiol.& Behav., 42:249-253.
New York. Jones, R. B., I. J. H. Duncan, and B. 0. Hughes.
. 1982. The relation of social to biological 1981. The assessment of fear in domestic hens
processes in disease. Social Sci. & Med., 16: exposed to a looming human stimulus. Behav.
369-380. Processes,6:121-133.
188 THE QUARTERL Y REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70
Jones, R. B., andJ. M. Faure. 1982. Open-field Lacey, J. I. 1967. Somatic responsepatterning
behaviour of male and female domestic chicks and stress:somerevisionsofactivationtheory.
as a functionof housing conditions, test situa- In M. H. Appleyand R. Trumball(eds.), Psy-
tions and novelty. Biol. Behav., 7:17-25. chologicalStress:
IssuesinResearch, pp. 14-44. Ap-
Jones, R. B., and B. J. Merry. 1988. Individual pleton-Century-Crofts, New York.
or paired exposure ofdomesticchicksto an open- Lachaux, M., M. F. Bouissou,J. C. Berges,and
field: some behavioural and adrenocorticalcon- P. Orgeur. 1983. Etude du comportement en
sequences. Behav. Processes,16:75-86. open-field de beliersIle-de-France soumisa dif-
Jones, R. B., and A. D. Mills. 1983. Estimation f6rentesconditionsd'elevage.Biol. Behav.,3:
of fearin two lines of the domestic chick: corre- 257-269.
lations between various methods. Behav. Pro- Lankin,V. S. 1976. Differences in the response
cesses,8:243-253. ofthepituitary-adrenal systemtopsychological
Jones, R. B., A. D. Mills, andJ. M. Faure. 1991. stressin sheepwithtwotypesofbehaviour.Isv.
Genetic and experiential manipulation of fear- Sib. Otd.Akad.Nauk.SSSR Ser.Biol.Nauk.,15:
related behavior in Japanese quail chicks (Co- 105-108.
turnixcoturnixjaponica). J Comp. Psychol.,105: Lawrence,A. B., E. M. C. Terlouw,and A. W.
15-24. Illius. 1991. Individualdifferences in behav-
Jones, R. B., D. G. Satterlee, and F. H. Ryder. iouralresponsesofpigs exposedto non-social
1992. Fear and distressinJapanese quail chicks and social challenges.Appl.Anim.Behav.Sci.,
of two lines geneticallyselected forlow or high 30:73-86.
adrenocorticalresponseto immobilizationstress. LeDoux, J. E. 1990. Fear pathwaysin thebrain:
Horm. Behav., 26:385-393. implications fora theoryoftheemotionalbrain.
Jones, R. B., and D. Waddington. 1992. Modifi- In P. F. Brain,S. Parmigiani,R. J. Blanchard,
cation of fear in domestic chicks, Gallus gallus and D. Mainardi (eds.), FearandDefence, pp.
domesticus, via regular handling and early envi- 163-178.HarwoodAcademicPublishers, Chur.
ronmentalenrichment.Anim. Behav., 43:1021- Lefcourt, A. M., S. Kahl, and R. M. Akers.1986.
1033.
Correlationof indicesof stresswithintensity
Karasek, R., and T. Theorell. 1990. HealthyWork:
of electricalshockforcows.J. DairySci., 69:
Stress,Productivity,
and theReconstructionof Work- 833-842.
ingLife. Basic Books, New York.
Leger,D. W., D. H. Owings,and D. L. Gelfand.
Kastin, A. J., M. A. Pearson, and W. A. Banks.
1980. Single-notevocalizationsof California
1991. EEG evidence that morphine and an en-
groundsquirrels:Gradedsignalsand situation-
kephalin analog cross the blood-brain barrier.
specificityofpredatorand sociallyevokedcalls.
Pharmacol.Biochem.Behav., 40:771-774.
Z. Tierpsychol.,52:227-246.
Kavaliers, M., and D. D. Colwell. 1991. Sex dif-
LeMoal, M., andH. Simon.1991.Mesocorticolim-
ferencesin opioid and non-opioidmediatedpred-
bic dopaminergic network: functionaland regu-
ator-induced analgesia in mice. BrainRes., 568:
173-177.
latoryroles.Physiol.Rev., 71:155-234.
Kerr, S. G. C., and D. G. M. Wood-Gush. 1987.
Leshner,A. I. 1979.Kinds ofhormonaleffects on
The developmentofbehaviourpatternsand tem- behavior:a newview. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.,
perament in dairy heifers. Behav. Processes,15: 3:69-74.
1-16. Leshner,A. I., andJ.A. Politch.1979.Hormonal
Kilgour, R. 1975. The open-fieldtestas an assess- controlofsubmissiveness in mice: irrelevance
ment of the temperamentof dairy cows. Anim. oftheandrogensand relevanceofthepituitary-
Behav., 23:615-624. adrenalhormones.Physiol.& Behav.,22:531-
Kopin, I. J., G. Eisenhofer, and D. Goldstein. 534.
1989. Adrenergic response following recogni- Ley, R. 1975. Open-fieldbehavior,emotionality
tion of stress. In S. Breznitz and 0. Zinder duringfearconditioning and fear-motivated in-
(eds.), MolecularBiologyofStress,pp. 123-132. strumental performance. Bull.Psychon. Soc.,6:
A. R. Liss, New York. 598-600.
Kovalcikova, M., and K. Kovalcik. 1982. Rela- Lore, R. K., and F. B. Eisenberg.1986. Avoid-
tionshipsbetween parameters of the open-field ance reactionsof domesticdogs to unfamiliar
test of cows and their milk production in loose male and femalehumansin a kennelsetting.
housing. Appl. Anim. Ethol., 9:121-129. Appl.Anim.Behav.Sci., 15:261-266.
Kuchel, 0. 1991. Stress and catecholamines. In Lyons,D. M. 1989.Individualdifferences intem-
G.Jasmin and M. Cantin (eds.), StressRevisited: peramentof dairygoatsand the inhibitionof
1 Neuroendocrinology of Stress,pp. 80-103. Kar- milkejection.Appl.Anim.Behav.Sci., 22:269-
ger, Basel. 282.
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 189

Lyons, D. M., and E. 0. Price. 1987. Relation- Mineka, S., M. Cook, and S. Miller. 1984. Fear
ships between heart rates and behavior of goats conditioned with escapable and inescapable
in encounters with people. Appl. Anim. Behav. shock: effectsof a feedback stimulus. J. Exp.
Sci., 18:363-369. Psychol.Anim. Behav. Processes,10:307-323.
Lyons, D. M., E. 0. Price, and G. P. Moberg. Misslin, R., R. Bouchon, and P. Ropartz. 1976.
1988. Individual differencesin temperamentof Signification de certains parametres compor-
domestic dairy goats: constancy and change. tementaux chez la souris placee dans un open-
Anim. Behav., 36:1323-1333. field. Physiol.& Behav., 17:767-770.
Maestripieri, D., and F. R. D'Amato. 1991. Anxi- Misslin, R., and M. Cigrang. 1986. Does neopho-
etyand maternal aggression in house mice (Mus bia necessarily imply fear or anxiety? Behav.
musculus):a look at interindividualvariability. Processes,12:45-50.
J. Comp. Psychol.,105:295-301. Moberg, G. P., C. 0. Anderson, and T. R. Un-
Mason,J. W. 1971. A re-evaluation ofthe concept derwood. 1980. Ontogeny of the adrenal and
of non-specificityin stress theory.J. Psychiatr. behavioralresponsesoflambs to emotionalstress.
Res., 8:323-333. J Anim. Sci., 51:138-142.
McCarty, R., and I. J. Kopin. 1978a. Changes Moberg, G. P., and V. A. Wood. 1982. Effect
in plasma catecholamines and behavior of rats of differentialrearing on the behavioral and
during the anticipation of footshock.Horm.Be- adrenocortical response oflambs to a novel en-
hav., 11:248-257. vironment. Appl. Anim. Ethol., 8:269-279.
, and . 1978b. Sympatho-adrenal Mormede, P. 1991. Du stressa la physiopathologie
medullary activityand behavior during expo- de l'adaptation. Rev. Med. Fonct.,23:273-303.
sure to footshockstress: a comparison of seven Mormede, P., R. Dantzer, R. M. Bluthe, and
rat strains. Physiol.& Behav., 21:567-572. J. C. Caritez. 1984. Differences in adaptive
McFarland, D. 1987. The OxfordCompaniontoAni- abilities ofthreebreeds ofChinese pigs. Behav-
malBehaviour.Oxford UniversityPress, Oxford. ioural and neuroendocrine studies. Genet.Sel.
Meaney, M. J., D. H. Aitken,S. Sharma, V. Viau, Evol., 16:85-102.
and A. Sarrieau. 1989. Postnatal handling in- Mormede, P., R. Dantzer, P. Montpied, R. M.
creases hippocampal type II glucocorticoid Bluthe, E. Laplante, and M. Le Moal. 1984.
receptorsand enhances adrenocorticalnegative- Influence of shock-induced fightingand social
feedback efficacyin the rat. Neuroendocrinology, factorson pituitary-adrenalactivity,prolactin
50:597-604. and catecholamine synthesizing enzymes in
Miller, N. E. 1959. Liberalization of basic S-R rats. Physiol. &Behav., 32:723-729.
concepts: extensionsto conflictbehavior, moti- Mormede, P., V. Lemaire, N. Castanon,J. Dul-
vation and social learning. In S. Koch (ed.), luc, M. Laval, and M. Le Moal. 1990. Multi-
Psychology:A Studyofa Science,pp. 196-292. Mc- ple neuroendocrine responses to chronic social
Graw-Hill, New York. stress: interaction between individual charac-
Mills, A. D., andJ. M. Faure. 1986. The estima- teristicsand situational factors. Physiol.& Be-
tion of fear in domestic quail: correlations be- hav., 47:1099-1105.
tween various methods and measures. Biol. Be- Muir, J. L., and H. P. Pfister. 1987. Time course
hav., 11:235-243. ofthe corticosteroneand prolactin response fol-
and . 1990. Panic and hysteria in lowing predictable and unpredictable novelty
domestic fowl: A review. In R. Zayan and stress in Rattus norvegicus. Physiol.& Behav.,
R. Dantzer (eds.), SocialStressinDomesticAnimals, 40: 103-107.
pp. 248-277. Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht. Murphy, L. B. 1977. Responses of domestic fowl
, and . 1991. Divergent selection for to novel food and objects. Appl. Anim. Ethol.,
duration of tonic immobility and social rein- 3:335-349.
statementbehavior in Japanese Quail (Coturnix Murphy, L. B., and I. J. H. Duncan. 1976. At-
coturnixjaponica) chicks.]. Comp. Psychol.,105: temptsto modifythe responses ofdomestic fowl
25-38. towards human beings. The effectof early ex-
Mills, A. D., R. B. Jones, J. M. Faure, and J. B. perience. Appl. Anim. Ethol., 4:5-12.
Williams. 1993. Responses to isolation injapa- Natelson, B. H., D. Creighton,R. McCarty, W. N.
nese quail geneticallyselected forhigh and low Tapp, D. Pitman, andJ. E. Ottenweller. 1987.
sociality. Physiol.& Behav., 53:183-189. Adrenal hormonal indices of stress in labora-
Mineka, S., and M. Cook. 1988. Social learning tory rats. Physiol.& Behav., 39:117-126.
and the acquisition of snake fear in monkeys. Natelson, B. H., W. N. Tapp,J. E. Adamus,J. C.
In T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef (eds.), Social Miller, and B. E. Levin. 1981. Humoral indi-
Learning.Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hills- ces of stress in rats. Physiol. & Behav., 26:
dale. 1049-1054.
190 THE QUARTERL Y REVIEW OF BIOLOGY VOLUME 70

Naumenko, E. V., N. K. Popova, E. M. Nikulina, Rogers, L. J., H. S. Sink, and J. W. Hambley.


N. N. Dygalo, G. T. Shishkina, P. M. Boro- 1988. Exploration, fear, and maze learning in
din, and A. L. Markel. 1989. Behavior, adre- spontaneously hypertensiveand normotensive
nocortical activity, and brain monoamines in rats. Behav. NeuralBiol., 49:222-233.
Norway rats selected for reduced aggressive- Russel, P. A. 1979. Fear-evoking stimuli. In
ness towards man. Pharmacol.Biochem.Behav., W. Sluckin (ed.), Fear in Animalsand Man, pp.
33:85-91. 86-124. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York.
Netto, C. A., B. Siegfried,and I. Izquierdo. 1987. Rutter, S. M., and I.J. H. Duncan. 1988. Mea-
Analgesia induced by exposure to a novel envi- suringfearin domesticfowlusing aversionlearn-
ronmentin rats: effectof concurrentand post- ing. In International Congresson AppliedEthology
trainingstressfulstimulation.Behav.NeuralBiol., inFarmAnimals,pp. 108-114. Skara (Sweden).
48:304-309. Sapolsky, R. M. 1990. Adrenocortical function,
O'Brien, C. P., R. N. Ehrman, andJ. N. Terns. social rank, and personality among wild ba-
1986. Classical conditioning in human opioid boons. Biol. Psychiatry, 28:862-878.
dependence. In S. R. Goldberg and I. P. Stolr- Satterlee, D. G., andW. A.Johnson. 1988. Selec-
man (eds.), BehavioralAnalysisof Drug Depen- tion ofJapanese quail forcontrastingblood cor-
dence,pp. 329-338. Academic Press, London. ticosterone response to immobilization. Poult.
Olivero, A., and C. Castellano. 1990. Genetic ap- Sci., 67:25-32.
proaches to fear and defence. In P. F. Brain, Seabrook, M. F. 1972. A study to determine the
S. Parmigiani, R. J. Blanchard, and D. Main- influence of the herdman's personalityon milk
ardi (eds.), Fear and Defence,pp. 144-162. Har- yield. J. Agric. Labour Sci., 1:1-45.
wood Academic Publishers, Chur. Seggie, J. A., and G. M. Brown. 1975. Stress
Overstreet, D. H., A. H. Rezvani, and D. S. Ja- response patternsof plasma corticosterone,pro-
nowsky. 1992. Maudsley reactiveand nonreac- lactin and growthhormone in the rat following
tive ratsdifferonlyin some tasksreflectingemo- handling or exposure to novel environment.
tionality. Physiol.& Behav., 52:149-152. Can. J. Physiol.Pharmacol.,53:629-637.
Parrot, R. F., and S. N. Thornton. 1989. Opioid Seligman, M. E. P. 1975. Helplessness:On Depres-
influenceson pituitaryfunctionin sheep under sion, Development, and Death. W. H. Freeman,
basal conditionsand duringpsychologicalstress. San Francisco.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14:451-459. Selye, H. 1936. A syndrome produced by diverse
Piazza, P. V.,J. M. Demini?ere,S. Maccari, M. Le nocuous agents. Nature,138:32-33.
. 1950. Stress:ThePhysiology and Pathology of
Moal, P. Mormede, and H. Simon. 1991. Indi-
vidual vulnerabilityto drug self-administration: Exposureto Stress.Acta Medica, Montreal.
Seyfarth,R., D. L. Cheney, and P. Marler. 1980.
action of corticosteroneon dopaminergic sys-
Monkey responsesto threedifferent alarm calls:
tems as a possible pathophysiological mecha-
evidence ofpredatorclassificationand semantic
nism. In P. WillnerandJ. Scheel-Kruger (eds.),
communication. Science,210:801-803.
TheMesolimbicDopamineSystem:FromMotivation
Shanks, N., J. Griffiths,S. Zalcman, R. M. Za-
to Action,pp. 473-495. Wiley & Sons, New
charko, and H. Anisman. 1990. Mouse strain
York.
differencesin plasma corticosteronefollowing
Plomin, R. 1990. The role ofinheritancein behav- uncontrollablefootshock.Pharmacol.Biochem.Be-
ior. Science,248:183-188.
hav., 36:515-519.
Plomin, R., J. C. De Fries, and G. E. McClearn. Shelton,M., and D. Wade. 1979. Predatorylosses:
1990. BehavioralGenetics:A Primer.W. H. Free- a serious livestockproblem. Anim. IndustryTo-
man, New York. day, 2:4-9.
Plotsky, P. M., E. T. Cunningham, and E. P. Siegfried,B., C. A. Netto, and I. Izquierdo. 1987.
Widmaier. 1989. Catecholaminergic modula- Exposure to noveltyinduces Naltrexone-rever-
tionofcorticotropin-releasing factorand adreno- sible analgesia in rats. Behav. Neurosci., 101:
corticotropinsecretion. Endocr. Rev., 10:437- 436-438.
458. Skinner, B. F. 1953. Scienceand Human Behavior.
Price, E. 0. 1984. Behavioral aspects of animal MacMillan, New York.
domestication. Q. Rev. Biol., 59:1-32 Stephens, D. B., and J. N. Toner. 1975. Hus-
Renner, M. J., and M. R. Rosenzweig. 1987. bandry influences on some physiological pa-
Enrichedand Impoverished Environments. on
Effects rametersofemotional responses in calves. Appl.
BrainandBehavior.Springer-Verlag,New York. Anim. Ethol., 1:233-243.
Restrepo, C., and A. Armario. 1987. Chronic stress Stern,J. M., M. S. Erskine, and S. Levine. 1973.
alters pituitaryadrenal functionin prepubertal Dissociation ofopen-fieldbehaviorand pituitary-
male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
12:393-398. adrenal function.Horm. Behav., 4:149-162.
JUNE 1995 FEAR AND FEARFULNESS IN ANIMALS 191

Stern, J. M., and S. Levine. 1974. Psychobio- makers. 1990. Genetic correlations in validat-
logical aspects of lactation in rats. Prog. Brain ing emotionality. Behav. Genet.,20:51-62.
Res., 41:433-444. Veissier, I., P. Le Neindre, and G. Trillat. 1987.
Stone, E., and C. H. Trost. 1991. Predators, risks The influenceof mother-youngrelationshipson
and contextformobbingand alarm calls in black- behavioural reactivityand learning in calves.
billed magpies. Anim. Behav., 41:633-638. Biol. Behav., 12:222-238.
Svensson, L., C. Harthon, and B. Linder. 1991. Veldhuis, H. D., E. R. De Kloet, I. Van Zosten,
Evidence fora dissociation between cardiovas- and B. Bohus. 1982. Adrenalectomy reduces
cular and behavioral reactivityin the spontane- exploratory activity in the rat: a specific role
ously hypertensive rat. Physiol. & Behav., 49: for corticosterone. Horm. Behav., 16:191-198.
661-665. Walker, C. D., R. W. Rivest, M.J. Meaney, and
Tachibana, T. 1982. Open-field test forrats: cor- M. L. Aubert. 1989. Differentialactivation of
relational analysis. Psychol.Rep., 50:899-910. the pituitary-adrenocorticalaxis afterstressin
Taylor, G. T. 1981. Fear and affiliationin domes- therat: use oftwo geneticallyselectedlines (RLA
ticated male rats.J. Comp. Physiol.Psychol.,95: and RHA) as a model. J Endocrinol.,123:
685-693. 477-485.
Taylor, L., and T. H. Friend. 1986. Open-field Walsh, R. N., and R. A. Cummins. 1976. The
testbehavior ofgrowingswine maintained on a open-fieldtest: a critical review. Psychol.Bull.,
concretefloorand a pasture. Appl. Anim.Behav. 83:482-504.
Sci., 16:143-148. Warnick, V. D., C. W. Arave, and C. Mickelsen.
Tejedor del Real, P., J. Gibert-Rahola, I. Leon- 1977. Effectsof group, individual and isolated
segui, andJ. A. Mico. 1991. Relationship be- rearing ofcalves on weightgain and behaviour.
tween emotivitylevel and susceptibilityto the J. Dairy Sci., 60:947-953.
learned helplessness model of depression in the Webster, A. B., andJ. F. Hurnik. 1990. Open-
rat. In AnimalModels in Psychopharmacology: Ad- fieldassessment ofbehavioral phenocypewithin
vancesin Pharmacological Sciences,pp. 217-224. genetic stocks of the White Leghorn chicken.
BirkhaiuserVerlag, Basel. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 27:115-126.
Torres-Hemandez, G., and W. Hohenboken. 1979. Weiss, J. M. 1972. Psychological factorsin stress
An attempt to assess traits of emotionality in and disease. Sci. Am., 226:104-113.
crossbred ewes. Appl. Anim. Ethol., 5:71-83. Wiener, S. G., and S. Levine. 1983. Influence of
Vandenheede, M., and M. F. Bouissou. 1993a. perinatal malnutrition and early handling on
Sex differencesin fearreactions in sheep. Appl. the pituitary-adrenalresponse to noxious stim-
Anim. Behav. Sci., 37:39-55. uli in adult rats. Physiol.& Behav., 31:285-291.
, and . 1993b. Effectof androgen Zuckerman, M. 1991. Basic dimensions ofperson-
treatmenton fear reactions in ewes. Horm. Be- ality. In M. Zuckerman (ed.), Psychobiology of
hav., 27: 435-448. Personality,pp. 1-43. Cambridge University
Van Der Staay, F. J., S. Kerbusch, and W. Raaij- Press, Cambridge.

You might also like