You are on page 1of 5

15206777, 2013, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/nau.22272 by INASP/HINARI - ALGERIA, Wiley Online Library on [08/12/2022].

See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Neurourology and Urodynamics 32:43–47 (2013)

The Relationship Between Urinary Bladder Control


and Gait in Women
Joanne Booth,1* Lorna Paul,2 Danny Rafferty,1 and Carolyn MacInnes3
1
School of Health and Life Sciences, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK
2
Nursing & Healthcare, School of Medicine, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
3
NHS Greater Glasgow & Clyde, Glasgow, UK

Aims: Urinary incontinence and OAB are associated with increased falls risk in older people suggesting a potential
relationship between bladder functioning and control of gait. To begin to understand the possible interaction between
gait and bladder control this exploratory study aimed to examine the effects of controlling the bladder on gait param-
eters in healthy adult women. Methods: Thirty-six continent women (mean age 50.8  15.8 years), participated in
this observational cohort study. Subjects walked three times along an electronic walkway under three different bladder
conditions; first desire to void (FDV), strong desire to void (SDV), and post void (PV). Spatial and temporal parameters
of gait and continence status were recorded for each condition. Results: A significant reduction in gait velocity
(P < 0.025) was found at the SDV compared with the PV condition. Stride length decreased significantly (P < 0.001) at
the SDV compared with the FDV and PV conditions. No significant differences were found between FDV and PV
conditions. In addition, the variability of gait increased significantly with respect to cadence (P < 0.05) and stride times
(P < 0.05) at the SDV compared to the PV condition. This was not observed between the FDV and the PV conditions,
nor the FDV and the SDV. Conclusion: In healthy continent women, speed and rhythmicity of gait are different
when a strong desire to void is experienced. This suggests an interaction may exist between urinary bladder control
and control of gait. Further investigation is necessary to understand this relationship and begin to explain the
increased risk of falls associated with urinary bladder functioning. Neurourol. Urodynam. 32:43–47, 2013.
ß 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Key words: attention; falls; gait; gait changes; urinary bladder control; urinary incontinence; variability

BACKGROUND There is a known increased risk of falls among community


dwelling older people with all types of urinary inconti-
In the healthy person, urinary continence involves success-
nence9,10 those rising at night to use the toilet11 and those
ful storage of urine, urethral sphincter regulation, and bladder
with urinary incontinence associated with an urgent desire to
emptying. Unconscious sub-cortical and reflex control is
void (urgency urinary incontinence).12 This suggests a poten-
continuously modulated by higher cortical centers, allowing
tial association between continence control, postural stability,
voluntary control of bladder functioning.1 The processes of
and gait. Our group were interested in investigating this
bladder filling and voiding are separate activities and the
relationship and sought to determine whether maintaining
intensity of the sensation of bladder filling escalates as
urinary continence, with a strong desire to void affects gait.
the bladder distends,2 increasing awareness and thus the
As this is a novel concept, the aim of this exploratory study
perceived need to gain relief through voiding.3–5 A significant
was to examine the influence of controlling the bladder on
contribution to continence is made by the biomechanical
gait parameters (measurable factors that define gait and
contribution of the pelvic floor muscles which actively
determine its qualities) of healthy adult women. The study
support the pelvic organs including the bladder, rectum,
hypothesized that, in healthy, continent women, spatial and
uterus, and vagina (in women). The anterior and cephalad
temporal gait parameters assessed with a strong desire to
contraction of these muscles helps to close and maintain
closure of the pelvic openings. The motor cortex is crucial to
the voluntary control of these pelvic floor muscles and their
activities.6 Maintaining control of the bladder and voiding Mickey Karram led the peer-review process as the Associate Editor responsible for
behavior involves complex cognitive and sensory processes, the paper.
not yet fully understood, involving various components and Conflict of interest: none.
Authors’ contributions: JB conceived, designed and coordinated the study, con-
levels of the nervous system. tributed to data collection, analysis and interpretation, prepared the manuscript;
Previous beliefs that walking is solely an automatic activity LP conceived and designed the study, contributed to data collection, analysis, in-
controlled by central pattern generators within the spinal terpretation and manuscript preparation; DR collected data, performed the statis-
cord have been modified and more recent work has confirmed tical analysis, interpreted findings and helped prepare the manuscript; CM
collected data and contributed to interpretation and manuscript preparation. All
that maintaining safe gait involves both cognitive and authors read and approved the final manuscript.
sensori-motor processes.7,8 Therefore like bladder control, *Correspondence to: Joanne Booth, School of Health and Life Sciences, Glasgow
control of gait is a complex neurological process involving Caledonian University, Cowcaddens Road, Glasgow G4 0BA, UK.
multiple levels and components of the nervous system and E-mail: jo.booth@gcu.ac.uk
Received 7 November 2011; Accepted 23 April 2012
evidence from clinical practice suggests there may be a Published online 12 June 2012 in Wiley Online Library
relationship between the two although to date this has not (wileyonlinelibrary.com).
been investigated. DOI 10.1002/nau.22272

ß 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


15206777, 2013, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/nau.22272 by INASP/HINARI - ALGERIA, Wiley Online Library on [08/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
44 Booth et al.
void would be different to the same gait parameters assessed activities such as individual and group conversations were ap-
with no desire to void. plied, to ensure that the point of SDV was reached. As some
subjects were concerned at the outset that they might experi-
ence an episode of urinary incontinence, absorbent pads were
METHODS
provided and used if required. The walkway was positioned
along the direct route to the bathroom. When they had emp-
Subjects
tied their bladder (PV) subjects were asked to walk along the
This cross sectional study recruited a convenience sample of walkway again three times to complete the study. Each sub-
healthy women volunteers. To be included in the study, the ject was asked if they had experienced any urinary leakage
women had to be adults aged 18 and above with no medical during the testing procedure.
conditions which would affect their gait, including lower limb
dysfunction or neurological impairment. They were also re-
Statistical Analysis
quired to be continent (defined as no urinary leakage reported
in the previous 3 months), or no or minimal known urinary For each gait parameter, at each of the three time points,
storage symptoms (frequency, urgency, nocturia) or voiding averages and standard deviations of the three ‘‘walks’’ were
symptoms (slow stream, intermittency, straining) or post- calculated. The temporal and spatial parameters and variation
micturition symptoms (feeling of incomplete emptying) as de- in these parameters as calculated by coefficient of variation
termined by a score of 0–7 (indicating mild symptoms) when (CoV; standard deviation/average) were analyzed. The tempo-
screened using the American Urological Association ral and spatial parameters of gait were found to be normally
International Prostate Symptom Score (IPSS).13 The study was distributed and thus a two way ANOVA (subject/condition)
approved by the University Ethics Committee. All subjects followed by Tukey post hoc analysis with Bonferroni correc-
gave written, informed consent. tion to adjust for multiple comparisons was employed to iden-
tify where the differences lay. All coefficient of variations
were not normally distributed thus these were analyzed using
Measures
Signed Wilcoxon tests. Significance was taken as P < 0.05.
The temporal and spatial parameters of gait were assessed
using the GAITRite walkway. The GAITRite Gold (CIR systems,
RESULTS
Inc., USA) has been shown to be a valid and reliable device.14
The GAITRite system was used to measure step (the heel con- Initially 39 women were recruited to the study; three wom-
tact of the current footfall to the heel contact location of the en were subsequently excluded as their IPSS scores were
next footfall on the opposite foot) and stride length (the dis- above 7. Thirty-six women were thus included (mean age
tance between the heel contact of two consecutive footfalls of 50.8 þ 15.8 years; range 25–85 years). The median IPSS score
the same foot) and time; swing time (the period of time when was 2. All women reported complete continence throughout
the foot is not in contact with the ground), duration of single testing and there were no indications of urinary leakage by
support (the period of time when only one foot is in contact any subject at any of the three assessment points, including
with the ground) and double support (the period of time SDV.
when both feet are in contact with the ground); velocity (the Table I presents the results of the spatial and temporal
total distance walked divided by the time taken to walk it) parameters of gait, the velocity and the cadence. There were
and cadence (the number of steps the person would take in no statistically significant differences between FDV and PV for
1 min). Taped marks were made on the floor 1.5 m beyond any of the variables however at SDV (i.e., with full bladder)
each end of the walkway to allow an additional length for gait velocity was found to be significantly reduced compared
acceleration and deceleration (thus each ‘‘walk’’ was over to PV. Stride length for both sides at SDV was significantly
6.5 m/21 feet). Each subject walked at their self-selected walk- reduced in comparison to both PV and FDV conditions. The
ing pace, three times for each of the three conditions. The temporal parameter of stride time showed no change attribut-
three bladder conditions were: first desire to void (FDV) de- able to bladder condition (Table I). Similarly, no change in the
fined as ‘‘the feeling that would lead you to pass urine at the temporal parameters of step time, single and double support
next convenient moment but voiding can be delayed if neces- time, swing time, and cadence were observed between condi-
sary;’’15 strong desire to void (SDV) defined as ‘‘the volume at tions (data not shown).
which you feel you can no longer delay micturition and have In terms of the variability of each of the gait parameters, as
a strong and persistent desire to void’’15 and post void (PV) determined by the CoV, no statistically significant changes
defined as ‘‘the immediate period after passing urine and sub- were observed for step length, single support, double support
jectively emptying the bladder.’’ All three bladder conditions and swing times (data not shown). Similarly, stride lengths
are subjective sensations for which there are no reliable meas- showed no change in variability across the three testing con-
ures as yet.16 ditions (Table II). However, cadence and left/right stride times
showed significantly increased variability at SDV in compari-
son to PV.
Procedures
Testing took place on one occasion. On attending the labora-
DISCUSSION
tory, the study was fully explained to the subjects and each
was provided with, and encouraged to drink, non-alcoholic This study sought to establish whether maintaining urinary
fluid of their choice (water, juice, tea, coffee). Gait assessment continence with a strong desire to void affects gait in healthy
was undertaken when the subject indicated their FDV, follow- adult women by comparing the effects of three physiological
ing which they were asked to postpone voiding, controlling bladder conditions (PV, FDV, and SDV) on parameters of gait.
their bladder and maintaining continence as long as possible The findings that gait velocity and stride length were signifi-
to the point of SDV. At the point of SDV subjects completed cantly reduced between the SDV and the PV, but not the PV
further gait assessment. Distraction techniques including and FDV, suggests that the experience of a strong desire to

Neurourology and Urodynamics DOI 10.1002/nau


15206777, 2013, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/nau.22272 by INASP/HINARI - ALGERIA, Wiley Online Library on [08/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Urinary Bladder Control and Gait 45
TABLE I. Velocity, Cadence, Spatial, and Temporal Gait Parameters Under the Three Tested Bladder Conditions

Value average (SD) Tukey post hoc comparison P-values

Variable PV FDV SDV ANOVA P-value SDV vs. PV SDV vs. FDV FDV vs. PV

Velocity (ms1) 1.37 (0.18) 1.39 (0.19) 1.32 (0.19) 0.006a 0.008a 0.030 0.857
Cadence (steps per minute) 118 (7) 119 (8) 119 (9) 0.658 — — —
Spatial data
Stride length (L) (m) 1.40 (0.14) 1.40 (0.15) 1.32 (0.14) <0.001a <0.001a <0.001a 0.89
Stride length(R) (m) 1.39 (0.14) 1.40 (0.15) 1.33 (0.14) <0.001a <0.001a <0.001a 0.98
Temporal data
Stride time (L) (s) 1.02 (0.06) 1.01 (0.07) 1.02 (0.08) 0.909 — — —
Stride time (R) (s) 1.02 (0.06) 1.01 (0.07) 1.01 (0.08) 0.823 — — —

PV, post void; FDV, first desire to void; SDV, strong desire to void.
a
Significant result (P < 0.025; n ¼ 36).

void led to compensatory adjustments in these elements of while reaching a suitable and safe place to void, then control-
gait. Intuitively it might have been anticipated that gait veloc- ling a full bladder in this situation can be seen as an attention
ity and stride length would have increased at SDV, especially demanding task. As continence was maintained by all partic-
as the walkway was positioned so that participants gait was ipants in this study, there was no deterioration in the bladder
assessed as part of their route to the bathroom, however this control task, therefore, we hypothesize that the observed
was not found to be the case. Our study also showed increased changes in gait represented a prioritization of bladder control
variability in some temporal gait parameters (cadence and over gait control as a consequence of a divided attention situ-
stride times) at SDV. It is possible that the strong desire to ation. A study designed to determine the effects of urge uri-
void potentially interferes with the cortical control of gait nary incontinence (UUI) on performance of voluntary
resulting in a concomitant deterioration in rhythmic gait behavioral tasks that depend on cognitive processes in the
quality. As this was a cross sectional study it was not possible prefrontal cortex17 found that women with UUI had signifi-
to determine causality of the relationship between controlling cantly poorer working memory than continent women. This
a full bladder and the gait changes we observed. There are, suggested that UUI in this instance was attentionally
however, a number of potential explanations for these demanding resulting in an impaired ability to effectively
results. manage the two tasks. Divided attention is known to impact
It is possible that this experiment represents a dual task or on temporal and spatial parameters of gait18 particularly in
divided attention situation. Divided attention theory postu- older people where an increased risk of falls is the result.7,19
lates that attentional capacity, defined as a person’s informa- In studies investigating divided attention the primary task is
tion processing capacity, is a finite resource and each activity most commonly postural control or walking and a variety of
undertaken will require a proportion of the available resource. secondary tasks have been investigated including: cognitive
In a dual task paradigm, if the two tasks combined require tasks such as verbal memory tasks,20 arithmetic tasks,21
more information processing capacity than is available then motor tasks such as coin transference22 or carrying trays with
the performance of one or both tasks will be affected through full cups of water23 and visual–spatial tasks such as balancing
mechanisms of prioritization of one task over the other, or a in a moving room.24 No study to date has focused on
deterioration in the performance of both. If the sensation of a controlling the urinary bladder as a secondary task, although
strong desire to void leads to the requirement for concentra- this is a fundamental and regularly performed human
tion and higher executive functioning to maintain continence activity. However, a recent study25 found a significant

TABLE II. Results of CoV for Velocity, Cadence, Spatial, and Temporal Gait Parameters Under the Three Tested Bladder Conditions

Value median (IQR) Signed Wilcoxon P-values

Variable PV FDV SDV SDV vs. PV SDV vs. FDV FDV vs. PV

Velocity 2.70 (1.83–4.71) 3.65 (1.82–5.92) 3.49 (1.94–6.58) 0.056 0.76 0.26
Cadence 1.59 (1.29–2.33) 1.75 (1.10–2.88) 2.28 (1.29–2.96) 0.014a 0.48 0.55
Spatial data
Stride length (L) 2.13 (1.45–2.71) 2.35 (0.87–3.46) 2.51 (1.28–4.40) 0.32 0.53 0.89
Stride length (R) 2.20 (1.45–2.71) 2.35 (0.87–3.46) 2.51 (1.28–4.40) 0.17 0.63 0.93
Temporal data
Stride time (L) 1.67 (0.89–2.59) 1.92 (1.12–3.43) 2.10 (1.39–3.39) 0.009a 0.19 0.08a
Stride time (R) 1.76 (0.94–2.45) 1.72 (1.25–3.10) 2.13 (1.40–2.94) 0.032a 0.95 0.37

PV, post void; FDV, first desire to void; SDV, strong desire to void.
a
Significant result (P < 0.05; n ¼ 36).

Neurourology and Urodynamics DOI 10.1002/nau


15206777, 2013, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/nau.22272 by INASP/HINARI - ALGERIA, Wiley Online Library on [08/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
46 Booth et al.
correlation between the severity of mixed urinary inconti- walking speed and quality and maintenance of urinary conti-
nence and deterioration in functional performance, which nence when the bladder is perceived to be full and a strong
was more pronounced in older women. If actively controlling desire to void is experienced. It is important to develop this
the bladder while walking to a bathroom does represent a work further to investigate the effects of a strong desire to
dual task situation, our data suggest that maintaining urinary void on gait in older women with overactive bladder and
continence takes precedence over achieving a rhythmic and urinary incontinence. It is also important to conduct studies
controlled gait. to establish whether a strong desire to void is attentionally
An alternative potential explanation for our observed demanding or not, to test the hypothesis that increased risk of
results is that they may represent an altered biomechanical falls in older women is a consequence of divided attention
situation brought about by the effects of muscle group activa- inducing deterioration in gait quality. Further studies will also
tion on stability. Bladder filling is known to be associated be designed to investigate the role of the CoM and co-
with increased trunk and abdominal muscle activity.26 contraction of the pelvi-perineal muscles in explaining the
Increased trunk muscle activity has been shown to negatively gait changes observed.
affect balance in adult women because the contraction and
resultant stiffness of the muscle groups reduces the capacity
for postural correction when perturbations occur.26 Vertical REFERENCES
and lateral excursions of the body’s center of mass (CoM) 1. Holstege G. The emotional motor system and micturition control. Neurourol
reduce as gait velocity decreases and gait velocity is known to Urodyn 2009;29:42–8.
affect the kinematic (joint angles), kinetic (movement forces), 2. Dompeyre P, Fritel X, Bader G, et al. Bladder sensitivity testing using a visual
analogue scale: Comparative cystometric study on women. Neurourol
as well as the temporal and spatial parameters of gait. The Urodyn 2007;26:350–5.
kinematic and kinetic aspects of gait are not within the scope 3. Wyndaele J, De Wachter S. Cystometrical sensory data from a normal popu-
of this study, however, our data demonstrate a reduction in lation. Comparison of two groups of healthy young volunteers examined
gait velocity at SDV mediated (reduced) not by taking fewer with 5 years interval. Eur Urol 2002;42:34–8.
4. Athwal BS, Berkley K, Hussain I, et al. Brain responses to changes in bladder
steps, borne out by the lack of change in cadence, but by volume and urge to void in healthy men. Brain 2001;124:369–77.
taking shorter steps. One possible explanation is that our 5. De Wachter S, Heeringa R, van Koeveringe G, et al. On the nature of bladder
observed reduction in gait velocity and stride lengths at SDV sensation: The concept of sensory modulation. Neurourol Urodyn 2010;30:
is a strategy adopted to control CoM excursions in order to 1220–6.
6. Madersbacher H. Neurourology and pelvic floor dysfunction. Minerva Gine-
maintain balance. Thus, a second hypothesis is that the col 2004;56:303–9.
observed changes in gait represented an adaptive response to 7. Hollman J, Kovash F, Kubik J, et al. Age-related differences in spatiotemporal
maintain stability. Given that our results show a similar markers of gait stability during dual task walking. Gait Posture 2007;
reduction in gait speed and stride times to those reported by 26:113–9.
8. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and
Orendurff et al.27 the excursions and velocity of the CoM at gait: A review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 2002;16:
SDV is worthy of further exploration, as a possible explana- 1–14.
tion for the gait changes observed. 9. Foley A, Loharuka S, Barrett JA, et al. Association between the Geriatric
An increase in abdominal muscle activity is also associated Giants of urinary incontinence and falls in older people using data from the
Leicestershire MRC Incontinence Study. Age Ageing 2012;41:35–40.
with bladder filling.25 This increases the pressure on the 10. Tromp A, Pluijm S, Smit J, et al. Fall-risk screening test: A prospective study
bladder and thus the demand on the pelvic floor musculature on predictors for falls in community-dwelling elderly. J Clin Epidemiol
to avoid urinary leakage by stabilizing the bladder neck 2001;54:837–44.
and increasing intra-urethral pressure to maintain closure.26 11. Stewart R, Moore M, May F, et al. Nocturia: A risk factor for falls in the elder-
ly. J Am Geriatr Soc 1992;40:1217–20.
Thus, a further hypothesis is that observed gait changes oc- 12. Chiarelli P, Mackenzie L, Osmotherly P. Urinary incontinence is associated
curred secondary to rises in intra-abdominal pressure and were with an increase in falls: A systematic review. Aust J Physiother 2008;55:
brought about by co-contraction of the pelvi-perineal muscula- 89–95.
ture (bracing) to cope with the pressure on the pelvic floor. 13. Barry M, Fowler F, O’Leary M, et al. The American Urological Association
symptom index for benign prostatic hyperplasia. The Measurement
A number of limitations have been identified in this study. Committee of the American Urological Association. J Urol 1992;148:1549–
Determining the strength of the desire to void is a very subjec- 57.
tive measure and open to error. Whilst every effort was made 14. Webster K, Wittwer J, Feller J. Validity of the GAITRite walkway system for
to distract participants to wait until their bladder was the measurement of averaged and individual step parameters of gait. Gait
Posture 2005;22:317–21.
perceived to be as full as possible before undertaking the SDV 15. Abrams P, Cardozo L, Fall M, et al. The standardisation of terminology of
test it is likely that there was variation in the perceived lower urinary tract function: Report from the standardisation subcommit-
strength of the urge to void. In addition, it was not possible to tee on the International Continence Society. Neurourol Urodyn 2002;21:
objectively measure the individual’s level of continence as 167–78.
16. Morris C. Differences in the function of the prefrontal cortex between
participants were asked to self report any leakage of urine women with urge urinary incontinence and continence cohorts Neurourol-
during the walking task Embarrassment may have influenced ogy and Urodynamics 2007, Proceedings of ICS Conference Rotterdam. New
the accuracy of reported continence. A further consideration is investigator award for podium presentation.
the age range of the sample, which may be thought to poorly 17. Chapple C, Artibani W, Cardozo L, et al. The role of urinary urgency and its
measurement in the overactive bladder symptoms syndrome: Current con-
represent the older population in whom gait and bladder cepts and future prospects. BJU Int 2005;95:335–40.
dysfunction are most prevalent. However, the study was 18. Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Lecordroch Y, et al. Walking speed-related changes
exploratory and designed to establish the possible association in stride time variability: Effects of decreased speed. J Neuroeng Rehabil
between gait control and bladder control with a view to then 2009;6:32.
19. Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Allali G, et al. Recurrent falls and dual task-
investigating clinical populations if and when the association related decrease in walking speed: Is there a relationship? JAGS 2008;56:
was established. 1265–9.
20. Huxold O, Li S, Schmiedek F, et al. Dual-tasking postural control: Aging and
the effects of cognitive demand in conjunction with focus of attention.
CONCLUSION Brain Res Bull 2006;69:294–305.
21. Springer S, Giladi N, Peretz C, et al. Dual-tasking effects on gait variability:
This study demonstrates an association between gait and The role of aging, falls and executive function. Movement Disord 2006;21:
bladder condition, suggesting there is a relationship between 950–7.

Neurourology and Urodynamics DOI 10.1002/nau


15206777, 2013, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/nau.22272 by INASP/HINARI - ALGERIA, Wiley Online Library on [08/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Urinary Bladder Control and Gait 47
22. O’Shea S, Morris M, Iansek R. Dual task interference during gait in people 25. Dumoulian C, Lussier M, Bherer L, et al. Impaired executive function is asso-
with Parkinsons disease: Effects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. ciated with mixed urinary incontinence in older, community-dwelling
Phys Ther 2002;82:888–97. women. Neurourol Urodyn 2010;29:30.
23. McGilchrist C, Paul L, Ellis B, et al. The effect of a cognitive or motor task on 26. Smith M, Coppieters M, Hodges P. Postural response of the pelvic floor and
the gait pattern of those with diabetes, with and without neuropathy. abdominal muscles in women with and without incontinence. Neurourol
Diabetic Med 2009;26:234–9. Urodyn 2007;26:377–85.
24. Weerdesteyn V, Schillings A, van Galen G, et al. Distraction affects the 27. Orendurff M, Segal A, Klute G, et al. The effect of walking speed on center of
performance of obstacle avoidance during walking. J Motor Behav 2003;35: mass displacement. J Rehabil Res Dev 2004;6A:829–34.
53–63.

Neurourology and Urodynamics DOI 10.1002/nau

You might also like