You are on page 1of 7

Psychotherapy © 2015 American Psychological Association

2015, Vol. 52, No. 1, 127–133 0033-3204/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038827

Countertransference in Successful and Unsuccessful Cases


of Psychotherapy

Jeffrey A. Hayes and Dana Lea B. Nelson James Fauth


The Pennsylvania State University Antioch University

Countertransference (CT) can provide psychotherapists with important information about relationship
dynamics with clients, the therapy process, and clinical decisions. CT also can lead therapists to view
clients and sessions inaccurately, feel unduly anxious, and behave in ways that primarily meet their own
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

needs at the expense of clients. In summarizing existing scholarship on CT, Fauth (2006) noted the need
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

for further research on therapists’ subjective experiences of CT to enhance current understanding of this
pantheoretical construct. To this end, we interviewed 18 therapists about their experiences of CT in a
recently terminated case; half of the therapists described CT in a case they judged to be successful, and
half described a case they thought was unsuccessful. Interview questions were designed to address the
5 components of CT proposed by Hayes (1995): origins, triggers, manifestations, effects, and manage-
ment. A grounded theory analysis was conducted and a model describing therapists’ experiences of CT
in successful and unsuccessful therapy was developed. Implications for practice, training, and research
are discussed.

Keywords: countertransference, therapist effects, qualitative research

Although the concept of countertransference (CT) originated in 2007). This definition does not delimit CT to the therapist’s
psychoanalytic theory, it has become a topic of interest to thera- unconscious, negative reactions to transference (Freud’s “classical
pists and researchers from a variety of theoretical orientations. definition”), but it does distinguish reactions that originate in the
Owing to the psychological scars inherent to the human condition, therapist’s unresolved issues (CT) from those that have other
all therapists experience CT, though its effects may be more causes, or any cause for that matter (the “totalistic definition”).
pronounced in theoretical approaches that are more relational than Given the restrictiveness of the classical definition and the over-
technical. Following decades of clinical and theoretical writings on inclusiveness of the totalistic one, most research on CT has utilized
CT, research conducted during the past 25 years has accumulated this moderate definition, as does the present study.
to the point where meta-analyses are now possible (Hayes, Gelso, Most early research on CT lacked a coherent theoretical framework
& Hummel, 2011). Research on CT is crucial because therapists’ to organize and integrate findings. Hayes (1995) generated such a
reactions to their clients, and the management of these reactions, framework, proposing that CT can best be conceptualized as consist-
can affect the process and outcome of psychotherapy, sometimes ing of five main components: origins, triggers, manifestations, effects,
radically so (Fauth, 2006; Gelso & Hayes, 2007; Hayes et al., and management. Origins are areas of unresolved conflict within the
2011). As a result, research that enhances current understanding of therapist. Triggers are therapy-related events that provoke the thera-
CT may be valuable to improving the quality of services that pist’s unresolved conflicts. Manifestations are the therapist’s cogni-
therapists provide. tive, affective, behavioral, and visceral reactions to these events.
A number of definitions of CT have abounded over the years, Effects are the subsequent results of these manifestations on therapy
but the one that has been used most consistently in the research process and outcome. Finally, management refers to the therapists’
literature construes CT as therapists’ reactions to clients that are ability to regulate their CT manifestations.
based on therapists’ unresolved conflicts (Gelso & Hayes, 1998, Researchers have assessed each of these five dimensions, and the
relationships among them, with mixed success. Quantitative measures
of CT origins have tended to assess fairly superficial constructs; in
contrast we believed that a qualitative approach in which the therapist
Jeffrey A. Hayes, Department of Educational Psychology, Counseling, had a trusting relationship with an interviewer might yield findings
and Special Education, The Pennsylvania State University; Dana Lea B. with greater depth. Previous research on CT triggers consisted largely
Nelson, Center for Counseling and Psychological Services, The Pennsyl- of analogue studies of client characteristics predicted to evoke CT
vania State University; James Fauth, Department of Clinical Psychology, (Gelso & Hayes, 2007). When considered alone, such triggers typi-
Antioch University.
cally failed to predict CT manifestations. Analogue studies, however,
We thank Helen Coble for her assistance with data collection. This
article is dedicated to Charlie Gelso, mentor, fishing partner, role model,
suffer from limited external validity; qualitative research conducted in
and friend. naturalistic settings represents one possible means of overcoming this
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jeffrey A. limitation. Research on the manifestations of CT suggests that anxi-
Hayes, 307 Cedar Building, Penn State University, University Park, PA ety, avoidance, and distorted perceptions are perhaps the most com-
16802. E-mail: jxh34@psu.edu mon CT reactions (Gelso & Hayes, 2007). They are by no means,

127
128 HAYES, NELSON, AND FAUTH

however, the only types of CT reactions, and we thought that to participants in advance and was asked during the interview (see
discovery-oriented research might reveal other common manifesta- the Appendix). Fifteen of the interviews were conducted by the
tions of CT. We also were particularly interested to know if an first and third authors; the three interviews on the West coast were
exploratory approach might suggest that CT reactions and their effects conducted by a research assistant.
vary as a function of the relative success of a case. The effects of CT
seem to be inversely, though modestly, related to therapy outcomes
(Hayes et al., 2011). Finally, studies of CT management have in- Data Analysis
volved assessing therapist characteristics, such as self-insight and
The analyses were performed by the three authors, two men and
empathic ability, which are postulated to help clinicians moderate CT
a woman, all of whom were European American. Both men had
reactions (Van Wagoner, Gelso, Hayes, & Diemer, 1991). The CT
doctoral degrees in counseling psychology, had worked in aca-
management factors that have been most commonly studied to date do
demia for 13 and 22 years, respectively, had conducted research on
not seem to help reduce CT reactions (r ⫽ ⫺.14) but they are strongly
CT, and worked part-time in private practices. The woman was an
associated with positive therapy outcomes (r ⫽ .56; Hayes et al.,
advanced doctoral student in clinical psychology who had com-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2011). We wondered if there might be other therapist characteristics


pleted her master’s degree. All three had experience conducting
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

or behaviors that aid in the management of CT and whether they


operate differently in cases of successful and unsuccessful therapy. qualitative research and were eclectic in their theoretical orienta-
We sought to explore these questions in the present study. tions. Each followed recommended procedures for conducting
The aim of the present study, then, was to further the exploration grounded theory to raise awareness of and manage their biases,
of therapists’ subjective experiences of CT, using Hayes’ (1995) such as documenting them in advance of data analyses and check-
structural model as a guide. We employed a grounded theory ing results against preexisting beliefs and expectations by record-
approach, which uses an inductive process to develop an empiri- ing their ideas during the data analytic process. These procedures
cally based theoretical model for a phenomenon, the details of were thought to be especially important given that the primary
which will be discussed further below. author developed the structural theory which guided the interview
questions (Hayes, 1995), he conducted some of the interviews, and
he participated in the data analysis process. The researchers needed
Method
to be particularly cautious, therefore, of a tautological process
wherein preconceptions about CT were simply reified by the
Participants research process. Multiple interviewers, bracketing of biases, and
Eighteen therapists in the United States were interviewed for the multiple data analysts all were employed to increase the likelihood
study. Therapists lived in a large city on the West coast (n ⫽ 3), that findings would yield modifications to Hayes’ (1995) theory
a college town in the Mid-Atlantic region (n ⫽ 6), and a large city and new discoveries about CT.
in New York (n ⫽ 9). Ten therapists were men and 8 were women. Interviews were transcribed and analyzed using the grounded
Their mean age was 52.0 years (range ⫽ 41– 63) and all but one theory method (Rennie, Phillips, & Quartaro, 1988). One re-
were European American; 1 therapist was African American. The searcher analyzed the successful cases, one analyzed the unsuc-
mean number of years of postdoctoral experience was 19.9 cessful cases, and the third provided feedback and integrated
(range ⫽ 6 –30). Therapists were asked to indicate on a 1-to-5 findings from all of the cases. After the interviews were tran-
scale the degree to which different theoretical orientations influ- scribed, transcripts were divided into “meaning units,” or segments
enced their work. The mean ratings were 3.65 for humanistic, 3.50 of text that contain a single idea or theme. Meaning units were then
for psychodynamic, and 3.00 for cognitive– behavioral. assigned labels that remained close to the language used by the
participants. Each meaning unit was compared to every other
Procedure meaning unit, developing categories that grouped units together
according to perceived similarities. The categories were then com-
Therapists in the two cities were recruited in person through pared with one another, again looking for similarities. Similar
researchers’ contacts with therapists. In the college town, all categories were combined, forming higher order “clusters” which
practicing psychologists listed in the telephone book were sent a
then were combined into “core categories.” This process of con-
letter describing the nature of the study and inviting their partici-
stant comparison continued until a hierarchical model was devel-
pation. All interviews were conducted in person, lasted approxi-
oped, one each for the successful and unsuccessful cases. These
mately an hour, and were audio-recorded. A semistructured format
two models were then compared by the third researcher, who
with an exploratory style of interaction was used, in which open-
denoted similarities and differences between them.
ended and nondirectional questions were asked to reduce the
likelihood of introducing researchers’ biases.
Prior to the interview, participating therapists were asked to Results and Discussion
choose a client with whom they had terminated in the last year and
whose case they would be willing to discuss. Therapists were Because many of the interview questions were informed by
randomly assigned to interview conditions in which they were Hayes’ (1995) structural model of CT, participants’ responses
asked to discuss cases where they believed that therapy had been naturally followed this framework. Thus, five core categories were
either successful (9 therapists) or unsuccessful (9 therapists). A set established relating to the origins, triggers, manifestations, effects,
of 17 questions, designed by the researchers to elicit information and management of CT. These core categories and their associated
about therapists’ experiences of CT with these clients, was mailed clusters and illustrative categories are presented in Table 1.
COUNTERTRANSFERENCE 129

Table 1
Core Categories, Clusters, and Categories of CT Components

Number of
Core Category Cluster Occurrences Illustrative Categories

Origins Unresolved personal issues 6 (S) Fear of others’ dependence


5 (U) Difficulty trusting others
Unresolved professional issues 6 (S) Difficulty confronting clients
4 (U) Doubts about competence
Triggers Client resemblance to T 6 (S) Cl and T both had distant fathers
1 (U) Cl and T both need to please others
Client resemblance to others 4 (S) Cl seen by T as similar to friend
1 (U) Cl seen by T as similar to parent
Differences between Cl and T 4 (S) T perceived Cl as more attractive
0 (U)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Client behaviors 6 (S) Cl discussed sexual escapades


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

6 (U) Cl disengaged from T


Client characteristics 1 (S) T perceived Cl as attractive
6 (U) T perceived Cl as dependent
Manifestations Pleasant feelings 5 (S) Warmth, caring, compassion
7 (U) Warmth, patience, hope
Unpleasant feelings 12 (S) Anger, worry, envy, guilt
7 (U) Anger, worry, self-doubt
Cognitions 7 (S) Confusion, forgetfulness
4 (U) Confusion, misconceptualization
Behaviors 7 (S) Colluding, working too hard
4 (U) Relaxing boundaries with C
Effects Facilitating 9 (S) T paid better attention to C
2 (U) Stronger working alliance
Hindering 2 (S) C was scared by T’s disclosure
2 (U) Weaker working alliance
Management Caution/Vigilance 7 (S) About not blaming C
0 (U)
In-session reminders 6 (S) To stay calm, to be present to C
0 (U)
Out-of-session behaviors 6 (S) Took better care of self, consulted
3 (U) Reflected on treatment, consulted
Self-insight 4 (S) Kept own values in perspective
1 (U) Did not recognize reactions as CT
Note. U indicates unsuccessful cases, S indicates successful cases.

Origins initially really challenged me. OK, let’s do more and more and
more and more to try to get this client better.”
The primary sources of CT for therapists in this study—personal
and professional—are consistent with those found in Hayes et al.
(1998). For instance, on a personal level, therapists in both studies Triggers
identified unresolved issues from one’s family of origin and the
therapist’s need to be liked as sources of CT reactions. For exam- Consistent with the findings of previous research (e.g., Cutler,
ple, one therapist described himself as “never a big part of the elite 1958; Hayes et al., 1998; Tishby & Vered, 2011), the content of
in-crowd . . . .the not-so-popular kid . . . with issues of being client material was a frequent trigger for many therapists (e.g., the
attractive, issues of popularity” whose insecurity was provoked client talked about sexually explicit behaviors and fantasies). Al-
working with a client he characterized as “the classic high school though many triggers could be considered objective events (e.g.,
football quarterback.” On a professional level, primary themes the client attempted suicide), others were highly influenced by
related to the role of therapist found in this study had to do with the therapists’ subjective perceptions, such as perceiving clients as
need to feel competent or the tendency to doubt one’s ability as a similar to other people in the therapist’s life (e.g., “my biggest
therapist, the tendency to feel overly responsible for clients’ prog- countertransference with her actually was that she reminded me of
ress or lack thereof—and therefore to feel inadequate (or, in the one of my closest friends and this was a little complicated”) or to
words of one therapist, like a “failure”) when treatment did not themselves. For instance, one therapist identified with her client’s
seem to be working. One therapist, speaking about his difficulty tendency to be overly accommodating:
trusting his reactions to clients, described having “unresolved I think I’m tuned in to issues of accommodation. That’s something
issues about my feelings of competence and confidence in myself I’ve had to struggle with . . . sometimes accommodating beyond my
and willingness to listen to what’s happening inside.” Another energy or beyond what I really should be offering . . . to somebody
therapist discussed her feeling overly responsible for a client by else who needs me . . . . How much do you give someone, and when
saying, “In terms of that rescuer/hero kind of feeling in me, she do you cut them off and stop giving? . . . I thought he was similar to
130 HAYES, NELSON, AND FAUTH

myself in that I go a very long way to try to keep people that I love It is interesting to note that therapists whose outcomes were
happy and promoted and I thought that he had a similar problem. successful articulated more unpleasant feelings and problematic
cognitive reactions than did therapists with unsuccessful outcomes.
Also consistent with previous research findings (Gelso & Hayes, This raises at least two possibilities: a) therapists whose outcomes
2007), therapists’ CT reactions were best understood in the context were successful were more aware of (and willing to discuss) their
of therapy-related events (i.e., triggers) that provoked their unre- CT reactions to clients, and b) therapists whose outcomes were
solved issues (i.e., origins). For example, one therapist who spoke successful managed their covert reactions so that they did not spill
about having unresolved issues with needing to be liked spoke out into CT behavior. In terms of the former, it is both plausible
extensively about how difficult it was for her to work with a client and consistent with prior research that awareness of CT reactions
who was quite passive and disengaged, because the client’s dis- would be associated with better psychotherapy outcomes (Gelso &
engagement made her feel as though the client did not like her, and Hayes, 2007). In terms of the latter, successful CT management
this made her feel restricted from “being [her]self.” Another ther- has been found to be strongly related to positive psychotherapy
apist who spoke about unresolved issues in her relationship with outcomes (Hayes et al., 2011).
her children and her concerns about how well she had done in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

promoting her children’s individuation reacted to a client who was


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

having difficulty individuating from her own mother. In this case, Effects
the therapist became overly invested in the client’s successfully Whereas the design of this study does not permit causal infer-
individuating and experienced feelings of wanting to be the “good ence regarding the effects of therapists’ CT, findings do provide
mother” (as compared to the client’s real mother, who was the important information about therapists’ perceptions of such ef-
“bad mother”). Gelso and Hayes (1998, 2007) recommend that fects. Perceived effects of CT on treatment process and outcomes
therapists take into account such interactions between their unre- included facilitating treatment by making better treatment choices,
solved issues and the events that trigger them to understand and supporting the client, strengthening the working alliance, and
manage their CT; examining origins or triggers in isolation is coming to understand the client more deeply. For example, one
probably of limited value. therapist whose codependency issues were similar to the client’s
remarked, “I think the facilitating part was really being able to
Manifestations empathize with the sense of ‘I want to be a good person’ and ‘I
want to be helpful to this person who I love but I feel a little
Therapists’ affective CT reactions included a wide variety of trapped by the work involved’ so I think I can empathize with
positive and negative feelings, both toward the client (e.g., anger, that.” CT reactions also interfered with treatment through hurting
envy) and about oneself (e.g., guilt, inadequacy). For instance, a the client’s feelings, frightening the client, and weakening the
therapist who was attracted to her client “found him a little bit working alliance. For example, one therapist expressed that her
intimidating . . . and I felt somewhat inadequate.” Another thera- discomfort with her client possibly not liking her caused her to feel
pist described feeling burdened by a client for whom the therapist restricted with the client and have more difficulty connecting with
had taken too much responsibility. Therapists’ cognitive reactions her. Another therapist indicated that her CT caused her to become
were uniformly undesirable, and they included forgetfulness, mis- overly invested in a particular outcome for the client, leading the
conceptualizing the client, making poor treatment decisions, and client to withhold important information for fear of disappointing
general confusion. Some therapists spoke about how their CT led the therapist. Among the most common negative effects, therapists
them to overlook important information that did not fit with their spoke about ways in which CT reactions caused them to misper-
initial impressions of their clients or to make assumptions that ceive their clients and thus inaccurately diagnose or conceptualize
were later disconfirmed (e.g., “I realized that I had missed a lot of their cases. Such misperceptions often led them to make treatment
his sadism and that what I saw as a victim, in fact, was a really choices that they later regretted. Several therapists also spoke
aggressive guy—and that I had been bamboozled on that”). An- about the negative impact of their CT reactions on the working
other therapist indicated “I really minimized his alcohol abuse . . alliance. Interestingly, both positive and negative feelings toward
. and I don’t normally deny or miss that stuff, but I think there was the client seemed to produce such effects. For instance, a therapist
this conjunction of what my family does, always deny and mini- who developed strong positive feelings toward her client and
mize a severe addiction when it is going on . . ..I think all of that became overly invested in the client’s individuation from her own
was so tinged with my own experiences with my brothers that it mother came to find out that her client had withheld information
was very hard for me to get a clear picture of him early on.” On a about a drug relapse for fear of disappointing the therapist.
behavioral level, the acting out of CT reactions took a variety of
forms, such as colluding (e.g., “Looking back at the CT stuff, I
Management
think that there were some ways I colluded with him early on
which made a very comfortable relationship”), arguing, showing Therapists discussed a number of different strategies for man-
off, working too hard, and not working hard enough. For instance, aging their CT reactions, seemingly with varying degrees of suc-
one therapist whose case was unsuccessful expressed she did not cess. Those therapists who indicated either that they were not
work as hard to keep her client in therapy as she might have aware of their CT enough at the time to adequately manage it or
normally. She said, “Maybe I prematurely suggested—when she that they simply tried to contain their CT tended to view their CT
made the motions about not coming every week—I [might] have as directly related to the negative outcomes of their cases. Thera-
said ‘ok’ . . . I don’t know . . . because it wasn’t very rewarding and pists who reflected— either by themselves or with their cli-
[it] was anxiety producing.” ents— on the relational dynamics of therapy or who sought con-
COUNTERTRANSFERENCE 131

sultation from colleagues tended to feel that they had more effects in successful cases. Successful and unsuccessful cases
successfully managed their CT and, when negative outcomes oc- differed markedly in the specificity, quality, and success of ther-
curred, tended to see poor outcomes as unrelated (or less related) apists’ efforts to manage their CT, and the downstream effects of
to their CT and more related to the limitations of their clients (e.g., CT on the case as a whole. Similar to the process of alliance
severity of pathology, lack of “readiness” for change, antisocial rupture and repair, in successful cases, therapists’ CT management
tendencies, etc.). Such findings are consistent with previous re- efforts helped them reframe, decenter, depersonalize, or otherwise
search, which suggests that therapists’ awareness of their feelings attain an enlarged perspective on the enactment taking place, and
toward clients is inversely related to demonstrations of CT behav- this new understanding allowed them to make adjustments and
ior (Hayes et al., 2011). work more productively with their clients (Safran, Muran, Sam-
Some therapists whose cases had unsuccessful outcomes de- stag, & Stevens, 2001).
scribed simply trying to “contain” their reactions, often without The results of this study not only provide additional detail to
articulating how, and sometimes were unaware of their CT reac- Hayes’ (1995) model of CT but they also supplement the work of
tions. One therapist said of her CT, “I don’t think I fully under- other CT scholars. The CT manifestations reported by our partic-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

stood it until she left therapy, so I don’t remember that I did ipants were consistent with common CT reactions—forgetting,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

anything . . . I don’t think I understood it to be a problem actually confusion, identifying with the client, and feelings of being help-
at the time . . . In hindsight I did.” less, overwhelmed, positive, overinvolved, withdrawn, disen-
Therapists who discussed cases with successful outcomes pro- gaged, and parental—that have been identified in previous studies
vided much more detailed strategies about managing their CT by other research teams (Betan, Heim, Zittel Conklin, & Westen,
reactions, such as by remaining vigilant in session about their own 2005; Hofsess & Tracey, 2010; McClure & Hodge, 1987; Tishby
internal experiences; reminding themselves in session to stay calm, & Vered, 2011). At the same time it should be noted that none of
objective, and attentive to clients; using their self-awareness in the participants in the current study identified bodily or somatic
session, and taking care of themselves outside of work. For exam- CT manifestations, as has been identified and described by a team
ple, one therapist said that she realized, “You can’t save everybody of Irish researchers (Booth, Trimble, & Egan, 2010).
and it helped me kind of face some things in myself that I can’t just
think I’m going to be able to save the world here . . . . I don’t know
if this sounds weird or not but I think that once I started really Limitations, Implications, and Recommendations for
taking better care of myself it helped, I think, free me up to say Future Research
I’ve taken this client as far as I can take it.” Another therapist
remarked: The inherent limitation of research based on interviews with
I can observe myself thinking about or framing . . . and I try therapists about their CT is that therapists may not be willing or
basically to be as objective as I can possibly be . . . I was able to able to share deeply personal, and perhaps unconscious, informa-
step back and say, ‘We’re overassuming here. We’ve got to stay tion about themselves. Furthermore, as Rennie et al. (1988)
where this person is and with the issues that he has that you are not pointed out, an additional limitation of grounded theory research
forced into here.’ That’s always running through my mind in a (and qualitative research more generally) is that it tends to involve
parallel fashion . . . [The reactions] were there and so was my a relatively small number of participants. What is gained in depth
awareness of their being there and how I was using or misusing is lost in breadth. Given the fact that this study explored the CT
them at a given time. reactions of only 18 therapists and each with only one case, these
The list of CT management strategies in Table 1 may be thought findings are of limited generalizability to other therapists or other
of as a helpful set of suggestions for therapists, trainees, and cases. Nonetheless, whereas these findings are no doubt incom-
supervisors to keep in mind when CT issues are affecting a case. plete, their consistency with and amplification of previous research
They supplement the five therapist characteristics that have been by a variety of scholars suggests that they have touched upon some
researched by Gelso and colleagues (Gelso & Hayes, 1998, 2007) of the major themes that tend to arise in therapists’ experiences
by providing possible behaviors in which therapists can engage to with their clients. Furthermore, the findings help to modify Hayes’
effectively manage CT. Of particular potential value may be in- (1995) skeletal model of CT, particularly in the area of CT man-
session reminders to oneself (e.g., to stay calm, objective, and agement where new, specific management strategies were identi-
present to both oneself and the client), exercising an internal fied. The more that studies of this kind are conducted (with
vigilance about one’s own reactions, and using one’s self- different types of therapists, clients, and treatment modalities), the
awareness (e.g., by distinguishing the client from other people in more comprehensive our knowledge of CT phenomena will be. It
the therapist’s life); it is important to note that these strategies were will be especially useful for other researchers to undertake such
used only in cases that had successful outcomes. studies since this study suffers from the possibility of reification.
That is, the primary author was involved with the planning, data
collection, and data analysis of a study designed to augment his
Integration of Successful and Unsuccessful Cases:
own structural theory of CT. Although we believe that the find-
A Grounded Theory
ings—particularly those pertaining to CT management—substan-
In successful and unsuccessful cases alike, CT was evoked when tially augment Hayes’ original theory, ultimately the reader must
therapists’ unresolved personal and professional issues were acti- judge how well findings met standard criteria for qualitative re-
vated by their perceptions of client characteristics and behaviors, search, including the extent to which results were coherent,
manifesting through a wide range of cognitive, affective, and grounded in observations versus preconceptions, and replicable
behavioral mechanisms that typically led to primarily helpful (Stiles, 1997).
132 HAYES, NELSON, AND FAUTH

Despite its limitations, the findings of this study have important Gelso, C. J., Latts, M. G., Gomez, M. J., & Fassinger, R. E. (2002).
implications, both for practice and for future research on CT. Countertransference management and therapy outcome: An initial eval-
Practicing therapists may use this framework and the observations uation. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, & Practice, 58, 861– 867.
of therapists who were part of this study to reflect on their own http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.2010
experiences of CT, possible pitfalls they may be able to avoid, as Hayes, J. A. (1995). Countertransference in group psychotherapy: Waking
well as the success of their current management strategies. When a sleeping dog. International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 45,
521–535.
therapists are aware of the various cognitions and emotions listed
Hayes, J. A., Gelso, C. G., & Hummel, A. M. (2011). Managing counter-
in Table 1, they would do well to trace their source to possible
transference. In J. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work
unresolved personal or professional issues that may require atten-
(2nd ed., pp. 239 –258). New York: Oxford University. http://dx.doi.org/
tion. Likewise, trainees and supervisors may benefit from using 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199737208.003.0012
this framework to facilitate discussions of CT in trainees’ work Hayes, J. A., McCracken, J. E., McClanahan, M. K., Hill, C. E., Harp, J. S.,
with clients and, more generally, in therapists own development. & Carozzoni, P. (1998). Therapist perspectives on countertransference:
Qualitative data in search of a theory. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

References ogy, 45, 468 – 482. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.45.4.468


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Hofsess, C. D., & Tracey, T. J. G. (2010). Countertransference as a


Baehr, A. (2004). Wounded healers and relational experts: A grounded theory prototype: The development of a measure. Journal of Counseling Psy-
of experienced therapists’ management and use of countertransference. chology, 57, 52– 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018111
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Educational Psychology,
McClure, B. A., & Hodge, R. H. (1987). Measuring countertransference
Counseling, and Special Education, Pennsylvania State University.
and attitude in therapeutic relationships. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Betan, E., Heim, A. K., Zittel Conklin, C., & Westen, D. (2005). Coun-
Research, & Practice, 24, 325–335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
tertransference phenomena and personality pathology in clinical prac-
h0085723
tice: An empirical investigation. The American Journal of Psychiatry,
Rennie, D. L., Phillips, J. R., & Quartaro, G. K. (1988). Grounded theory:
162, 890 – 898. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.890
A promising approach to conceptualization in psychology? Canadian
Booth, A., Trimble, T., & Egan, J. (2010). Body-centred countertransfer-
ence in a sample of Irish clinical psychologists. The Irish Psychologist, Psychology, 29, 139 –150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0079765
36, 284 –289. Safran, J. D., Muran, J. C., Samstag, L. W., & Stevens, C. (2001).
Cutler, R. L. (1958). Countertransference effects in psychotherapy. Journal of Repairing alliance ruptures. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, & Prac-
Consulting Psychology, 22, 349 –356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0044815 tice, 38, 406 – 412. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.38.4.406
Fauth, J. (2006). Toward more (and better) countertransference research. Stiles, W. B. (1997). Consensual qualitative research: Some cautions. The
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, & Practice, 43, 16 –31. http://dx.doi Counseling Psychologist, 25, 586 –598.
.org/10.1037/0033-3204.43.1.16 Tishby, O., & Vered, M. (2011). Countertransference in the treatment of
Gelso, C. J., & Hayes, J. A. (1998). The psychotherapy relationship: adolescents and its manifestation in the therapist-patient relationship.
Theory, research, and practice. New York: Wiley. Psychotherapy Research, 21, 621– 630.
Gelso, C. J., & Hayes, J. A. (2007). Countertransference and the thera- Van Wagoner, S. L., Gelso, C. J., Hayes, J. A., & Diemer, R. A. (1991).
pist’s inner experience: Perils and possibilities. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Countertransference and the reputedly excellent psychotherapist. Psy-
Erlbaum. chotherapy, 28, 411– 421.
COUNTERTRANSFERENCE 133

Appendix
Interview Questions

1. What was the client’s initial problem for which he or ity” of the therapy relationship and to what extent were
she sought therapy? they grounded in your own unresolved issues (i.e.,
countertransference based)?
2. What was your initial formulation of the client’s prob-
lem and how did that change over the course of therapy? 11. What happened in therapy to provoke or elicit your
countertransference reactions?
3. What was the outcome of the case? That is, how did the
client change, if at all? 12. How were those countertransference reactions related to
your own unresolved issues?
4. Please describe the working alliance at the beginning of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

therapy and discuss how it changed over time. 13. How did you countertransference reactions facilitate or
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

interfere with your understanding of the client? Did this


5. In what ways was the client similar to yourself? vary during the course of therapy?

6. In what ways did the client remind you of other people 14. How did you try to manage or therapeutically use your
in your life? countertransference reactions?

7. How genuinely did you and your client relate to each 15. How frequently did you self-disclose your countertrans-
other at the outset of therapy and how did this change ference reactions to your client and what influenced
over time? your decisions about doing so?

8. How accurately do you think you and your client per- 16. On the whole, then, in what ways did countertransfer-
ceived one another at the outset of therapy and how did ence affect your work with this client?
the accuracy of those perceptions change over time?
17. What else might be important for me to know in under-
9. What role did transference play in the client’s percep- standing the role of countertransference in this case?
tions of you and in the therapy?

10. Please describe the various reactions that you had to this Received January 6, 2014
client (e.g., thoughts, feelings, behaviors, images, etc.). Revision received February 20, 2014
To what extent were these reactions based in the “real- Accepted February 21, 2014 䡲

You might also like