You are on page 1of 8

International Journal of Psychology

International Journal of Psychology, 2019


Vol. 54, No. 2, 197–204, DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12459

Moral foundations, worldviews, moral absolutism and


belief in conspiracy theories

Luigi Leone, Mauro Giacomantonio, and Marco Lauriola


Department of Social and Developmental Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, Rome,
Italy

I n the present research, we examined whether individual differences in basic moral concerns might be related to a greater
endorsement of conspiracy theories. Building on the notion that conspiracy theories often deal with super-individual
relevant events in which a group perspective is central, we proposed that individual differences in moral concerns
pertaining to group- and community-concerns (i.e., binding moral foundations) rather than to individual well-being (i.e.,
individualising moral foundations) would be positively associated with conspiracy beliefs. We further hypothesised that
such relations would be totally mediated by beliefs in a dangerous world and by embracing moral absolutism. We found
support for these predictions in two community samples (Ns: 319; 514). Theoretical implications were discussed.

Keywords: Moral foundations; Personality; Conspiracy theories; Worldviews; Moral absolutism.

Although conspiracy beliefs cover a wide range of issues frightful events as intentional plots originating from
and content they share one common theme: momentous malevolent agents (Douglas, Sutton, Callan, Dawtry, &
and negative social events are explained as intentional Harvey, 2016). Therefore, conspiracy theories apply to
plots enacted by powerful groups to pursue evil goals by the agent-harming-patient dyadic template triggering
unlawful means (e.g., Sutton & Douglas, 2014). Psycho- moral reasoning (Sinn & Hayes, 2016). Moral constructs
logical research has found several correlates of endorse- appear thus relevant in understanding conspiracy beliefs.
ment of conspiracy theories, such as psychopathological In this study, we have investigated whether prioritising
and maladaptive traits (e.g., Swami, Weis, Lay, Barron, different moral foundations (Graham et al., 2011) might
& Furnham, 2016); narcissism (e.g., Cichocka, March- facilitate or inhibit the endorsement of conspiracy the-
lewska, & de Zavala, 2016); and social attitudes such as ories. We anticipated that individual differences in the
anomie, distrust and political cynicism (e.g., Moulding binding foundations, those emphasising group cohe-
et al., 2016). Given that conspiracy theories are especially sion, and group-based traditional norms, would relate to
widespread about political issues, worldviews (Mould- conspiracy beliefs, and that such associations occur via
ing et al., 2016), and the socio-political constructs of threatening worldview beliefs (Moulding et al., 2016) and
right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orien- through the habitual use of an either/or disjunctive logic
tation have also been connected with conspiracy beliefs in making moral judgments—moral absolutism/splitting
(e.g., Wilson & Rose, 2014), with generally more reli- (MAS; Lauriola, Foschi, & Marchegiani, 2015).
able effects for authoritarianism than for social domi-
nance (e.g., Grzesiak-Feldman, 2015). Moral foundations and conspiracy beliefs
Such a rich body of research accounts for many
psychological routes favouring the adoption of con- Moral foundations theory (e.g., Graham et al., 2011)
spiracy theories. Notwithstanding this, we believe that distinguishes between two broad categories of moral
one important feature of conspiracy theories has been concern: (1) the binding foundations of authority/respect,
somewhat overlooked: conspiracy beliefs imply a moral in-group/loyalty, and purity/sanctity, which bind the
evaluation because they generally interpret negative and individual to the values and norms of the group in order

Correspondence should be addressed to Luigi Leone, Department of Social and Developmental Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, Via
dei Marsi 78, Rome 00145, Italy. (E-mail: luigi.leone@uniroma1.it.)
Luigi Leone wrote most of the paper and performed the analyses. Mauro Giacomantonio and Marco Lauriola managed data collection, prepared the
data and wrote some sections of the paper.

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


198 LEONE, GIACOMANTONIO, LAURIOLA

to foster the maintenance and flourishing of a group association between the individualising foundations and
or culture; and, (2) the individualising foundations of conspiracy beliefs, if any, should be negative.
harm/care and fairness/reciprocity, which emphasise
respect for individual rights in order to protect and foster
the well-being of the individual. Mediators
Here, we would propose that the moral foundations
should relate to conspiracy beliefs and, specifically, that Moral foundations and conspiracy beliefs differ in terms
individuals prioritising the binding foundations would of generality/specificity. Therefore, the associations
more strongly endorse conspiracy beliefs. The focus on described above are likely to rely on the intervention of
in-group cohesion and community of the binding foun- more proximal correlates of conspiracy beliefs. The bind-
dations (Graham et al., 2011) would match with the ing moral foundations reflect concern for the communal
us-versus-them mentality that features in most conspir- well-being. Consistently, the binding foundations should
acy beliefs (van Prooijen & van Lange, 2014). The typi- relate to dangerous world beliefs (DWB), a worldview
cal events triggering conspiracy-focused explanations are picturing the social world as threatening a cherished
of super-individual relevance because they are construed way-of-life (Federico, Weber, Ergun, & Hunt, 2013). In
as threats to nations (e.g., 9/11), faiths (e.g., jihadist) or turn, the high degree of uncertainty and anxiety implied
even larger groups (e.g., the people) in the case of eco- by DWB should make such a worldview a proximal
nomic recessions or environmental crisis. Because the correlate of conspiracy beliefs (Moulding et al., 2016).
victims in the conspiracy theories are frequently framed The individualising foundations should instead be nega-
as large groups as opposed to small cliques of powerful tively related with DWB because of the optimistic view of
individuals (van Prooijen & van Lange, 2014), this asym- human nature that goes hand-in-hand with prioritising the
metry easily activates an in-group/out-group perspective individualising foundations (Nilsson & Strupp-Levitsky,
that should resonate more with individuals emphasising 2016).
the moral foundations that bind individuals into groups. We also considered a different worldview based on
In addition, the events explained by conspiracy beliefs competitive jungle beliefs (CJB). These are correlated
invariably represent a threat to the foundations of soci- with DWB (e.g., Federico et al., 2013), and share some
ety, and such threats resonate strongly with the defensive conceptual features with the construct of Machiavel-
stance of a way of life embodied in the binding founda- lianism, which has been connected with conspiracy
tions. Thus, we anticipate a positive association between ideation (Douglas & Sutton, 2011). Such associations
individual differences in the binding foundations and con- could make CJB a suitable candidate to relate with
spiracy beliefs. conspiracy beliefs.1 However, CJB taps mainly into a
We maintain a more exploratory position in the role of dominance motive that should be kept as distinct from the
the individualising foundations. It could be argued that the threat-related motives underpinning DWB (e.g., Duckitt,
individualising moral foundations should be less in tune Wagner, du Plessis, & Birum, 2002). It could be argued
with conspiracy beliefs when compared with the binding that the threat-related motives reflected in DWB are key
foundations. Fairness/reciprocity and harm/care empha- in fostering conspiracy beliefs (Moulding et al., 2016),
sise an individualistic perspective and a more detached whilst dominance-related motives are relatively less
attitude towards communal cohesion. The threats focal to important (e.g., Grzesiak-Feldman, 2015). Hence, we did
conspiracy theorising are group-threats, and it has been not expect much meditational capability from CJB; this
argued that it is group deprivation—rather than individual anticipation is also consistent with previous research that
deprivation—that makes people more susceptible to con- found negligible associations of CJB with the binding
spiracy beliefs (Simmons & Parsons, 2005). Hence, we foundations (Federico et al., 2013).
would tentatively propose that the individualistic outlook We also propose that the binding moral foundations
of those prioritising the individualising foundation would should relate to a moral absolutist stance, which in turn
inhibit the activation of conspiracy theorising. Further- should be related to conspiracy beliefs (Oliver & Wood,
more, the importance assigned to individual rights and 2014). Binding moral foundations serve a communal and
protection from harm is coupled with a more optimistic even transcendent point of view. They unambiguously
view of human nature (Nilsson & Strupp-Levitsky, 2016), define the terms of the good life for the community.
which is at odds with the pessimism on human nature The absolute character of the binding moral foundations
that underpins the endorsement of conspiracy beliefs should be conducive to a splitting moral stance, where
(Moulding et al., 2016). Thus, we could expect that the there is an unambiguous splitting separating good and

1 DWB are tightly connected with right wing authoritarianism (e.g., Federico et al., 2013), which in turn has been connected with conspiracy ideation

(e.g., Wilson & Rose, 2014). We preferred to measure DWB instead than Authoritarianism because the DWB construct is at a broad level of generality
and does not measure directly, as Authoritarianism scales do, ideological orientations. Ideology could be connected with conspiracy ideation, but we
preferred to maintain a level of analysis focused on broad worldviews, instead than on ideological orientations.

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


MORAL FOUNDATIONS AND CONSPIRACY BELIEFS 199

evil, and no shades of grey may exist, especially when the B differed from Sample A not only because the former
communal life is threatened. Hence, the binding founda- was larger, but also because Sample B was arguably more
tions should relate positively with individual differences balanced and diverse. More specifically, Sample B was
in MAS (Lauriola et al., 2015). Those who are high on the more balanced as far as the gender distribution was con-
MAS tend to frame intergroup relations within a disjunc- cerned (i.e., no significant departure from an ideal 50/50
tive logic that create binary oppositions: good in-group, gender ratio), χ2 (1) = 1.31, p = .259). In addition, Sam-
evil out-group (Lauriola et al., 2015). Likewise, believ- ple B was less dependent on student participants, with
ers in conspiracy theories explain threatening events as a complementary significant increase in the proportion
originating from a thoroughly evil out-group agent (Sut- of non-student participants in Sample B (χ2 (1) = 12.18,
ton & Douglas, 2014). Recent research has shown that p < .001). Samples differed in their composition on other
a construct similar to the MAS, dubbed Manichaeism demographics. In Sample A, 6.6% of respondents did
(measured with a single item) was related to conspiracy not finish high school, 48% completed high school and
beliefs (Oliver & Wood, 2014). Therefore, we anticipated 45.5% had a college degree (in Sample B the figures
that MAS would be connected with conspiracy beliefs, for education were 6, 39.4 and 54.3%, respectively).
and mediate the association between the binding foun- In Sample A, a plurality of respondents were students
dations and conspiracy beliefs. In contrast, the individ- (30.4%), followed by employees (24.8%), and a 15%
ualising foundations assume a form of moral relativism of professionals (remaining respondents were dispersed
because the individualising stance emphasises the right of across various occupations). In Sample B, a plurality
the autonomous individual to his/her personal moral stan- of respondents were employees (33.1%), followed by
dards (Taylor, 2007). This is at odds with MAS, which professionals (19.3%), and then by students (16.7%,
should then relate negatively with individual differences the rest of respondents were dispersed across various
in the individualising foundations. occupations).2 Thus, we could confidently conclude that
Sample B was larger, less biased and relatively more rep-
resentative than Sample A, therefore providing favourable
METHOD
ground for replication. As a whole, successful replication
of results across the two samples could be considered as
Participants and procedure
encouraging evidence on the tenability of our hypotheses.
Given the novelty of our predictions concerning the role of
moral foundations, we aimed at investigating the expected Measures
associations in two independent samples, collected a few
months apart. A total of 362 individuals participated in The same scales were used in Samples A and B.
the first data collection (Sample A), and 539 individuals Responses for all variables were coded on 7-point scales.
(Sample B) participated in the second data collection. Moral foundations were measured with the moral
For each sample, a snowballing recruitment procedure foundations questionnaire (MFQ; Graham et al., 2011;
was used. Psychology students were offered course-credit Italian version: Bobbio, Nencini, & Sarrica, 2011).
for recruiting mostly non-student adult individuals; the The MFQ is composed by two distinct sections of
psychology students helping with the data collection were 15 items each. One section examines how much each
not allowed to fill out the questionnaire. They were also moral foundation is relevant for the respondents (e.g.,
instructed to avoid recruitment of family members or whether or not someone was harmed; whether or not
roommates, in order to reach a relatively diverse pool of someone violated standards of purity and decency).
respondents. As a result, non-student adult respondents The second section focuses on moral evaluations of
were a majority (69.4% in Sample A and 83.3% in Sample behavioural instances (e.g., If I saw a mother slap-
B). The questionnaires were administered on-line for both ping her child, I would be outraged; I would call some
samples. Because of incomplete responses and missing acts wrong on the grounds that they are unnatural
values, final Ns were 319 for Sample A (179 females, or disgusting). In our research, we only included the
M age = 35.42, SD = 13.68), and 514 for Sample B (270 moral relevance section to keep the questionnaire short
females, M age = 36.30, SD = 11). and suitable for online data-collection. Some previous
All participants in both samples were Italian native studies had similarly relied on the moral relevance
speakers. The decision to use two independent samples scores only (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009, Study 1).
was a focus of our research project from the outset, in In addition, we were mainly interested in the general,
order to increase confidence on the generalizability of worldview-like, level of analysis that is best provided by
findings. Samples A and B, although both non-random the moral relevance section. Also, the moral judgement
samples, differed considerably from each other. Sample section typically yields unsatisfactory reliabilities, whilst

2 These distributions differed significantly across samples: χ2 (2) = 6.25, p = .044, for education; χ2 (7) = 25.2, p = .001, for occupation.

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


200 LEONE, GIACOMANTONIO, LAURIOLA

the relevance scales attain in general satisfactory relia- hypothesis of invariance of the variance/covariance matri-
bilities (e.g., Graham et al., 2009; Sinn & Hayes, 2016.) ces using the appropriate procedures within the LISREL
We averaged the scores for the binding foundations software (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996). The hypothe-
(authority/respect, in-group/loyalty and purity/sanctity) sis of equality could not be rejected (χ2 (21) = 30.27,
and for the individualising foundations (harm/care and p = .087; Root Mean Square Error of Approximation
fairness/reciprocity). Reliabilities were satisfactory for (RMSEA) = .033; Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = .99).
both the binding (αs .85 and .79 in Samples A and B, The associations (Table 2) could, therefore, be deemed
respectively), and the individualising foundations (αs .85 invariant across samples. It should be noted that the
and .86 in Samples A and B, respectively). associations of the binding and individualising founda-
Worldviews were measured using the 10-item belief tions with conspiracy beliefs were small in magnitude.
in a dangerous world scale, and the 20-item belief in a Nonetheless, the binding moral foundations were related
competitive jungle world scale (Duckitt et al., 2002). In significantly to conspiracy beliefs. The binding founda-
Sample B, a shorter (10-item) version of the latter was tions correlated, as expected, with DWB and MAS, but
administered. DWB scores showed satisfactory reliability not with CJB. Instead, the individualising foundations
(αs = .80 in both samples), as it was the case for CJB correlated negatively with CJB. DWB and MAS corre-
scores (αs .88 and .79 in Samples A and B, respectively). lated with conspiracy beliefs, as expected, whilst CJB
MAS, the rigid and stereotyped view that reflects appeared unrelated at the bivariate level with conspiracy
black-and-white thinking related to the ambiguous beliefs.
behaviours in terms of moral opposites, was measured To more closely test for our anticipated pattern of
with a 10-item scale comprising the seven items of the associations, we tested in both samples a path analy-
original Lauriola et al.’s (2015) measure of the construct, sis with multiple mediators with the LISREL software
and three additional items which broadened the scope of (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996). The correlation matrices
the measure and strengthened the reliability of the scale: in Table 2 were used as input. The model specified
“That who lies once, would lie forever”; “When honesty the moral foundations as the exogenous variables and
and loyalty are concerned, there are no shades of gray, beliefs in dangerous world, competitive jungle, and
just black or white”; “It’s not true that one can find some MAS as mediators; conspiracy beliefs were modelled
good even in an evil person: if one is evil, he/she is evil.” as the final criterion (Figure 1). This was a relatively
Reliabilities were satisfactory in both samples (αs .88 parsimonious total mediation model where no direct
and .86 in Samples A and B, respectively).3 effects of the moral foundations were estimated (James,
A total of 13 conspiracy beliefs were measured Mulaik, & Brett, 2006). Because the model fitted the data
to include a multiplicity of events and concerns such nicely in both samples (Sample A: χ2 (2) = .03, p = .98;
as 9/11, diseases, economic crisis. Some items were RMSEA = .00; CFI = 1.00; Sample B: χ2 (2) = .10,
adapted from beliefs investigated in other measures p = .94; RMSEA = .00; CFI = 1.00) a total mediation
(i.e., Swami et al., 2016), others were generated focus- hypothesis could be retained.
ing on beliefs with wide currency in Italy (vaccines, Figure 1 summarises the standardised parameter esti-
chemtrails—Mancosu, Vassallo, & Vezzoni, 2017). A mates. The binding foundations related positively with
fictitious conspiracy belief (item 10) was also included DWB (.34 in both Samples A and B) and MAS (.50
to tap into the “monological” processes that use one in Sample A; .42 in Sample B), but non-significantly
conspiratorial ideation as evidence for other – even with CJB (again, in both Samples A and B). Instead,
fictitious – conspiracy beliefs (Swami et al., 2011). Reli- the individualising moral foundations related negatively
ability was satisfactory in both samples (αs .88 and .89 in with CJB (−.26 and −.10 in Samples A and B, respec-
Samples A and B, respectively). Table 1 reports the list tively) and DWB (Sample A: −.13; Sample B: −.09),
of beliefs, as well as the component loadings obtained although more weakly so in Sample B. The individu-
in the two samples for a one-dimensional solution. To alising moral foundations were also negatively related
gauge the measure’s structural stability across samples, with MAS in both samples, with very similar coefficients
we computed a measure of factor congruence (Tucker’s (−.19 and −.17 in Samples A and B, respectively). The
phi, which reflects the correlation of the factor patterns effects of the supposed mediators on conspiracy beliefs
across samples) and obtained a coefficient of .99. were positive for DWB (.17 and .33 in Samples A and B,
respectively) and MAS (Sample A: .18; Sample B: .11)
RESULTS but non-significant for CJB, a pattern that replicated con-
sistently across samples. The proportion of variance in
As a preliminary step, we ascertained the similarity of the conspiracy beliefs accounted for by the effects included
zero-order associations across samples and tested the null in the model was .09 for Sample A, and .15 for Sample B.

3 The pattern of results was unaffected whether the original 7-item measure described by Lauriola et al. (2015) or the 10-item scale described herein

was used.

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


MORAL FOUNDATIONS AND CONSPIRACY BELIEFS 201

TABLE 1
Principal component analysis of conspiracy beliefs in Samples A and B

Component loadings
Item Sample A Sample B

1. The U.S. government did not prevent the 9/11 attacks in order to justify the Afghanistan .59 .71
and Iraq campaigns
2. A cure for cancer already exists, but the pharmaceutical industrial complex keeps it secret .70 .75
in order to maximise its profits
3. The 2008 financial crisis and the ensuing recession represent the intended result of .63 .64
deliberate actions by banks, financial speculators and multinational corporations
4. The financial crises of the last decade have been deliberately caused by political and .67 .67
financial authorities
5. The U.S. government knew in advance about the 9/11 attacks, and deliberately decided not .67 .76
to stop them
6. Soft-drinks corporations add addictive substances in their products .63 .52
7. Infective diseases, such as Ebola, are deliberately spread by governments and .74 .79
pharmaceutical multinationals
8. The so-called Islamic State does not really exist; it is a smoke-screen concocted by Western .61 .62
Governmental Agencies
9. LGBT groups have a plan to turn people into homosexuals by means of bogus “gender .44 .47
theories”
10. The assassination of judge Giacomini was committed by mafia killers with the political .67 .57
complicity of the Italian Government
11. Vaccines are useless and dangerous, they are only instrumental to the financial interests of .62 .59
pharmaceutical companies
12. Groups of powerful people use chemtrails to poison the air, or to manipulate the .64 .70
environment
13. Few groups of powerful individuals (Bilderberg Group, Trilateral Commission, the .68 .72
Rotschild family, the Free Masonry, etc.) secretly rule the world

LGBT = Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual and Transgender.

TABLE 2
Descriptive statistics

1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Binding — .50** .29** −.07 .50** .13*


2. Individualising .40** — .08 −.11** .04 .02
3. Dangerous world .29** .01 — −.02 .39** .38**
4. Competitive jungle −.08 −.24** .18* — .09* .04
5. Moral absolutism/splitting .41** .00 .42** .13* — .24**
6. Conspiracy beliefs .12* .01 .24** .01 .26** —
Means (SD) Sample A 3.84 (.82) 4.73 (.67) 4.04 (.86) 2.62 (.70) 3.62 (1.31) 2.79 (.77)
Means (SD) Sample B 3.87 (.87) 4.78 (.72) 4.18 (.92) 2.29 (.72) 3.62 (1.24) 2.75 (.71)

Note: Intercorrelations: Sample A (N = 319) below the diagonal and Sample B (N = 514) above the diagonal.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

This stable pattern of associations yielded indirect −.012) and DWB (Sample A: −.022; Sample B: −.029),
effects for the moral foundations, with opposite signs. and more weakly by CJB (Sample A: −.013; Sample B:
As expected, a positive indirect effect of the binding −.003). The indirect effect of the binding foundations
foundations was detected in both Sample A (.15, SE = .03, was channelled mainly through DWB (Sample A: .058;
p < .0001), and Sample B (.16, SE = .03, p < .0001). A Sample B: .112) and MAS (Sample A: .09; Sample B:
weaker—albeit significant—and negative indirect effect .047). The portion of the effect of the binding founda-
of the individualising foundations on conspiracy beliefs tion channelled by CJB was negligible in both samples
was similarly found in both samples: −.07, SE = .02, (Sample A: .002; Sample B: .001).
p < .001, and −.05, SE = .02, p < .01, for Samples A and
B, respectively. The indirect effects, although reliable,
were nonetheless small. DISCUSSION
Such indirect effects could be decomposed further.
The indirect effect for the individualising foundations As anticipated, the moral foundations related positively
was channelled by MAS (Sample A: −.034; Sample B: to conspiracy beliefs were those of the binding kind. The

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


202 LEONE, GIACOMANTONIO, LAURIOLA

Figure 1. Path analysis model: Standardised parameters. Sample B in parentheses. Note: † < .10; *p < .05. **p < .01.

positive association was channelled through perceptions subtle connections between moral perceptions of self and
of insecurity and an either/or disjunctive moral logic. other and conspiracy ideation.
An indirect association of the individualising foundations Individuals prioritising the individualising foundations
also materialised, but with a negative sign. The indirect appeared unresponsive to the us-versus-them mindset that
association materialised because the individualising foun- is inherent in conspiracy thinking. The finding that pri-
dations related negatively with both moral absolutism oritising the individualising foundation, which comprises
and with the pessimistic evaluation of human-to-human harm/care and fairness/reciprocity concerns, was nega-
interactions that transpires from DWB. In turn, moral tively related with conspiracy beliefs might be considered
absolutism and DWB related as expected with con- somewhat puzzling. Conspiracy theories generally deal
spiracy beliefs (Moulding et al., 2016; Oliver & Wood, with adverse events whereby individuals are unfairly
2014). treated, harmed, even deprived of their lives. Harm/care
The results were consistent with the line of research and fairness/reciprocity concerns should be activated by
that conceptualises endorsement of conspiracy theories such events, but apparently not in the group-focused way
as an inter-group phenomenon that is activated to face that fosters conspiracy-based explanations (van Prooijen
threats traced back to groups outside the in-group’s bor- & van Lange, 2014). It may be speculated that the individ-
ders (e.g., van Prooijen & van Lange, 2014). The bind- ualising foundations do indeed represent an outcome of
ing moral foundations are inherently social because they the “great moral narrowing” of the Enlightenment (Haidt
emphasise the group over the individual and they are & Kesebir, 2010, p. 798): the Enlightenment successfully
designed to foster the flourishing of the community and narrowed the moral domain dislodging an older moral
its internal cohesion around binding practices. For those order built around collective identities and moral tradi-
individuals whose moral stances are rooted in binding, tions with a modern moral understanding focused on the
group-centred concerns, events perceived as threatening individual (Taylor, 2007). Current differences in moral
can readily be framed within an us-versus-them moral foundations across liberals and conservatives reflect such
mould that fits with conspiracy ideation. To the best “narrowing,” as liberals assign moral relevance mainly to
of our knowledge, the present research was the first to the individualising foundations, whilst conservatives are
emphasise that moral foundations would be important in sensitive to the whole spectrum of the moral foundations
understanding conspiracy ideation, inasmuch conspiracy assessed by the MFQ (Graham et al., 2009). The tradi-
beliefs unambiguously identify victims and culprits and tional “broader” moral order interpreted social events
discriminate right from wrong, good from evil. Future as of relevance for the whole community; consequently,
research could investigate how other morally relevant any social threat represented a threat to the group. A
constructs, as moral identity and personality traits linked conspiracy mindset fits with such a moral order, and its
with morality, relate with conspiracy ideation and conspir- embodiment in moral foundations of the binding kind.
acy mentality. Implicit measures of honesty and morality Instead, the moral foundations of the individualising
(Perugini & Leone, 2009) might be used to gauge more kind do not frame momentous social events as threats for

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


MORAL FOUNDATIONS AND CONSPIRACY BELIEFS 203

the individual’s identity. The liberal post-Enlightenment of conspiracy beliefs had its limitations that are nonethe-
morality gravitating around individualising moral con- less shared by other validated scales focusing on specific
cerns also implied a more benign perception of the beliefs (i.e., some degree of cultural specificity, and sensi-
world as more sympathetic to human flourishing and tivity to the vagaries of the news cycle) as has been noted
populated by well-meaning individuals (Taylor, 2007). elsewhere (e.g., Brotherton, French, & Pickering, 2013).
As a consequence, conspiracy narratives do not fit with
the optimistic individualising moral sensibility as they do
with the pessimistic binding moral sensibility. CONCLUSION
The diverging pessimistic/optimistic views of human-
ity and moral stances relying on the absolutism/relativism Results emphasised the importance of different moral
dimension might be interpreted as the crossroads where foundations as the moral background that could favour or
the associations of the moral concerns with conspiracy inhibit a conspiracy-laden interpretation to negative social
beliefs bifurcated into opposite paths. Interestingly, the events. The indirect associations of individual differences
pathways leading to conspiracy-based accounts of trou- in moral foundations with conspiracy beliefs open a novel
bling events might be perceived as consistent with a com- interpretation of conspiracy theories as morally infused
plex and dynamic interpretation of conspiracy thinking perceptions of social events. A programme aiming at
(Moulding et al., 2016) where some elements (threaten- reducing the recognised negative impact of conspiracy
ing perceptions, dangerous worldview) may work to raise mindsets at the social level (e.g., van der Linden, 2015)
uncertainty, and others (Manichaeism, Moral absolutism) could take into account that conspiracy beliefs are not
to resolve it by embracing a simplified account of reality, simply sheer consequences of cognitive failures or mal-
as that provided by conspiracy beliefs. Such complex pat- adaptive personality traits, but that they are also based on
tern is reminiscent of other dynamic processes, as those moral stances on what is right or wrong, and who is to
peculiar to the anxious and avoidant attachment styles blame. Perceiving moral injustices is key to social ame-
(e.g., Dykas & Cassidy, 2011), where a similar unstable liorative action, and moral sources are important to spur
sequence of perceptions of insecurity and rigid responses social engagement and civic behaviours (Taylor, 2007).
can be observed. Such dynamics appear to be an interest- However, morally rigid (Manichean) perceptions of the
ing area for future research on the psychological function world and conspiracy beliefs breed suspicion and social
of conspiracy theories and beliefs. impotence (e.g., Douglas, Sutton, Jolley, & Wood, 2015).
Aiming at weakening the link between individual differ-
ences in binding moral foundations and a rigid either/or
Limitations disjunctive logic in making social judgments (MAS) may
rouse the attention to social problems, and avoid yielding
The main limitation of the present study resides in the cor-
to cynicism and social impotence.
relational and cross-sectional nature of the data, which
prevented us from making causal inferences. However,
Manuscript received March 2017
the main goal of the present work was to establish a pat- Revised manuscript accepted August 2017
tern of associations, with conspiracy beliefs differently First published online September 2017
associated with the binding compared with the individu-
alising foundations, and with such associations operating
through indirect pathways. REFERENCES
Generalizability of results can be regarded as another Bobbio, A., Nencini, A., & Sarrica, M. (2011). Il Moral Foun-
limitation of the present research. We tried to deal with dation Questionnaire: Analisi della struttura fattoriale della
this issue by analysing two independent samples. The versione italiana. Giornale di Psicologia, 5, 7–18.
two samples differed in some demographic characteris- Brotherton, R., French, C. C., & Pickering, A. D. (2013). Mea-
tics, and therefore the stability of results observed across suring belief in conspiracy theories: The generic conspiracist
samples might be interpreted as an encouraging finding, beliefs scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 279.
as far as replicability was concerned. Of course, stud- Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., & de Zavala, A. G. (2016).
ies relying on more representative samples could provide Does self-love or self-hate predict conspiracy beliefs? Nar-
more solid evidence on the generalizability of our find- cissism, self-esteem, and the endorsement of conspiracy the-
ories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(2),
ings. It is also apparent that the associations hypothe-
157–166.
sised explained only a relatively small amount of varia- Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Does it take one to
tion in conspiracy ideation. Yet, we would emphasise that know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influenced
because of the novelty of the proposed association, even by personal willingness to conspire. British Journal of Social
small associations could provide valuable insights for Psychology, 50, 544–552.
a multifaceted conceptualization of conspiracy ideation. Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J.,
Finally, it should be also acknowledged that our measure & Harvey, A. J. (2016). Someone is pulling the strings:

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


204 LEONE, GIACOMANTONIO, LAURIOLA

Hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy Moulding, R., Nix-Carnell, S., Schnabel, A., Nedeljkovic, M.,
theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 22(1), 57–77. Burnside, E. E., Lentini, A. F., & Mehzabin, N. (2016). Bet-
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Jolley, D., & Wood, M. J. (2015). ter the devil you know than a world you don’t? Intolerance
The social, political, environmental, and health-related con- of uncertainty and worldview explanations for belief in con-
sequences of conspiracy theories. In M. Bilewicz, A. spiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 98,
Cichocka, & W. Soral (Eds.), The psychology of conspiracy. 345–354.
(). Oxford, U.K.: Taylor and Francis. Nilsson, A., & Strupp-Levitsky, M. (2016). Humanistic and nor-
Duckitt, J., Wagner, C., du Plessis, I., & Birum, I. (2002). mativistic metaphysics, epistemology, and conative orienta-
The psychological bases of ideology and prejudice: Testing tion: Two fundamental systems of meaning. Personality and
a dual process model. Journal of Personality and Social Individual Differences, 100, 85–94.
Psychology, 83, 75–93. Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and
Dykas, M. J., & Cassidy, J. (2011). Attachment and the process- the paranoid style (s) of mass opinion. American Journal of
ing of social information across the life span: Theory and Political Science, 58(4), 952–966.
evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 19–46. Perugini, M., & Leone, L. (2009). Implicit self-concept and
Federico, C. M., Weber, C. R., Ergun, D., & Hunt, C. (2013). moral action. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(5),
Mapping the connections between politics and morality: 747–754.
The multiple sociopolitical orientations involved in moral van Prooijen, J. W., & van Lange, P. A. (2014). The social
intuition. Political Psychology, 34(4), 589–610. dimension of belief in conspiracy beliefs. In J. W. van
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and con- Prooijen & P. A. M. van Lange (Eds.), Power, politics, and
servatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal paranoia: Why people are suspicious of their leaders. (pp.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1029–1046. 237–253). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Simmons, W. P., & Parsons, S. (2005). Beliefs in conspiracy
Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of theories among African Americans: A comparison of elites
Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 366–385. and masses. Social Science Quarterly, 86(3), 582–598.
Grzesiak-Feldman, M. (2015). Are the high authoritarians more Sinn, J. S., & Hayes, M. W. (2016). Replacing the moral
prone to adopt conspiracy theories? The role of right-wing foundations: An evolutionary-coalitional theory of
authoritarianism in conspiratorial thinking. In M. Bilewicz, liberal-conservative differences. Political Psychology.
A. Cichocka, & W. Soral (Eds.), The psychology of conspir- https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12361
acy. (pp. 99–117). New York, NY: Routledge. Sutton, R. M., & Douglas, K. M. (2014). Examining the mono-
Haidt, J., & Kesebir, S. (2010). Morality. In S. Fiske, D. T. logical nature of conspiracy theories. In J-W. van Prooijen &
Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychol- P. A. M. van Lange (Eds.), Power, politics, and paranoia:Why
ogy. (pp. 797–832). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons. people are suspicious of their leaders. (pp. 254–272). Cam-
James, L. R., Mulaik, S. A., & Brett, J. M. (2006). A tale of two bridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
methods. Organizational Research Methods, 9, 233–244. Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A.,
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2009). Machiavellianism. In Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation
M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief
differences in social behavior. (pp. 93–108). New York, NY: system and associations between individual psychological
Guilford Press. differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories.
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8: User’s refer- British Journal of Psychology, 102, 443–463.
ence guide. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International. Swami, V., Weis, L., Lay, A., Barron, D., & Furnham, A. (2016).
Lauriola, M., Foschi, R., & Marchegiani, L. (2015). Integrating Associations between belief in conspiracy theories and the
values and cognitive style in a model of right-wing radical- maladaptive personality traits of the personality inventory for
ism. Personality and Individual Differences, 75, 147–153. DSM-5. Psychiatry Research, 236, 86–90.
van der Linden, S. (2015). The conspiracy-effect: Exposure Taylor, C. (2007). A secular age. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases University Press.
pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personality and Wilson, M. S., & Rose, C. (2014). The role of paranoia in a
Individual Differences, 87, 171–173. dual-process motivational model of conspiracy belief. In J.
Mancosu, M., Vassallo, S., & Vezzoni, C. (2017). Preva- W. van Prooijen & P. A. M. van Lange (Eds.), Power, poli-
lence and determinants of conspiracy theory believes tics, and paranoia: Why people are suspicious of their lead-
in Italy: an exploratory analysis. Retrieved from ers. (pp. 273–291). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University
www.carloalberto.org/research/working-papers Press.

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science

You might also like