You are on page 1of 17

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-022-05858-8

ORIGINAL PAPER

Differences in Praxis Errors in Autism Spectrum Disorder Compared


to Developmental Coordination Disorder
Gabriel Abrams1,2 · Aditya Jayashankar1,2 · Emily Kilroy1,2 · Christiana Butera1,2 · Laura Harrison1,2 · Priscilla Ring1,2 ·
Anusha Houssain1,2 · Alexis Nalbach1,2 · Sharon A. Cermak2 · Lisa Aziz‑Zadeh1,2 

Accepted: 30 November 2022


© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2022

Abstract
This study aimed to better understand how autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and developmental coordination disorder
(DCD) differ in types of praxis errors made on the Florida Apraxia Battery Modified (FAB-M) and the potential relation-
ships between praxis errors and social deficits in ASD. The ASD group made significantly more timing sequencing errors
in imitation of meaningful gestures, as well as more body-part-for-tool errors during gesture-to-command compared to the
other two groups. In the ASD group, increased temporal errors in meaningful imitation were significantly correlated with
poorer affect recognition and less repetitive behaviors. Thus, in ASD, aspects of imitation ability are related to socioemo-
tional skills and repetitive behaviors.

Keywords  ASD · DCD · Dyspraxia · Praxis · Imitation · Gesture

Introduction Mody et al., 2016; Williams et al., 2004). A better under-


standing of motor deficits that may be particular to ASD
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a pervasive develop- may improve early diagnosis, especially as motor deficits
mental disorder characterized by deficits in social commu- are much easier to test than social and emotional deficits
nication and emotional regulation, as well as restricted and (Dowell et al., 2009). Further, by understanding the rela-
repetitive behaviors (National Institute of Mental Health, tionship between social and motor deficits, we may better
2022). A growing body of research suggests a high prev- understand the underlying etiology of ASD. Indeed, in ASD
alence of motor deficits in individuals with ASD (Bhat, it has been noted that some praxis skills, such as imitation
2020; Chukoskie et  al., 2013; Edwards, 2014; Fournier ability, may be related to social impairments that are the
et al., 2010; Green et al., 2009; Hannant et al., 2018; Hil- core of the diagnosis, including joint attention (Bottema-
ton et al., 2012; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Ming et al., 2007; Beutel et al., 2019; Carpenter et al., 2002; Dadgar et al.,

* Lisa Aziz‑Zadeh Anusha Houssain


lazizzad@usc.edu ahossain@usc.edu
Gabriel Abrams Alexis Nalbach
gabrams@usc.edu analbach@usc.edu
Aditya Jayashankar Sharon A. Cermak
jayashan@usc.edu cermak@usc.edu
Emily Kilroy 1
Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern
ekilroy@usc.edu
California, 3620A McClintock Avenue, Los Angeles,
Christiana Butera CA 90089, USA
cbutera@usc.edu 2
Mrs. T. H. Chan Division of Occupational Science
Laura Harrison and Occupational Therapy, University of Southern
lauraloesch09@gmail.com California, 1540 Alcazar Street, Los Angeles, CA 90089,
USA
Priscilla Ring
pring@usc.edu

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

2017; Ingersoll & Schreibman, 2006; Rogers et al., 2003), on tasks that require verbal working memory (Alloway &
empathy skills (Adornetti et al., 2019; Dowell et al., 2009; Temple, 2007; Alloway et al., 2008).
McAuliffe et al., 2017; Mostofsky et al., 2006), social learn-
ing by imitation (Dziuk et al., 2007), functional and sym- Comparing Motor and Praxis Skills Between ASD
bolic play (Libby et al., 1997; Stone et al., 1997; Vivanti and DCD
et al., 2013), social reciprocity (McDuffie et al., 2007; Young
et al., 2011), cooperation (Colombi et al., 2009), and theory Performance on motor skills, such as balance, manual dex-
of mind (ToM; Perra et al., 2008). Further, imitation skills terity, and ball skills, are generally equally poor in ASD
have been correlated with autism severity (Gizzonio et al., and DCD groups compared to typically developing groups
2015; Ingersoll & Meyer, 2011; Pittet et al., 2022; Rogers (TD; Caçola et al., 2017; Kilroy et al., 2022). By contrast,
et al., 2003; Zachor et al., 2010). Here we aim to understand praxis performance encompassing skilled limb gestures
motor deficits in ASD, specifically by looking at errors made (e.g., gesture-to-command, tool use, imitation; Crucitti
during a measure of praxis skills compared to a group with et al., 2019), often differs between ASD and DCD groups,
developmental coordination disorder (DCD), as well as the with ASD performing worse (Dewey et al., 2007; Kilroy
relationship between praxis errors and social deficits in ASD et al., 2022). Dewey et al. (2007) found that only the ASD
and DCD. Here, we refer to motor performance skills as group (compared with DCD, DCD+ ADHD, and TD groups)
controlled and goal-directed actions measuring skills such as showed impairments in imitation of meaningful actions and
fine motor, ball, and balancing using quantitative metrics of gesture-to-command. Specifically, when looking at types
frequency, speed, etc. (Harris et al., 2015; Henderson et al., of errors made, during gesture-to-command, children with
2007). By contrast, we refer to praxis as representational or ASD made significantly more errors in delay, distortion, ori-
non-representational imitation and gesture production, with entation, body-part-as-object and incorrect actions. During
or without the use of tools (Ayres, 1989; Mostofsky et al., imitation, children with ASD made significantly more errors
2006) in extension, distortion, orientation, and body-part-as-object
(Dewey et al., 2007). These results are consistent with more
recent studies by our group and others showing that children
Developmental Coordination Disorder with ASD produce significantly more errors when imitating
gestures and performing gestures from verbal commands
Many of the motor deficits commonly seen in ASD also are than their TD counterparts (Gizzonio et al., 2015; Kilroy
seen in DCD, also known as dyspraxia (Gibbs et al., 2007; et al., 2022) and that accuracy in gesture-to-command and
Kilroy et al., 2021). A DCD diagnosis is based on impair- imitation of meaningful gestures is significantly poorer in
ments in dexterity, limb speed, and gross and fine motor ASD compared to DCD groups (Kilroy et al., 2022). The
skills that reduce one’s ability to engage in activities of latter results for praxis accuracy remain even after control-
daily living and achieve academically (American Psychiatric ling for social skills, theory-of-mind (ToM) skills, IQ, sex,
Association, 2013; Blank et al., 2019; Chang & Yu, 2016). and age (Kilroy et al., 2022). Further, while motor skills
While about 30% of the DCD population shows social dif- interact with praxis skills in the DCD group (Kilroy et al.,
ficulties on a social measure commonly used with ASD 2022), in the ASD group, deficits in praxis performance do
(Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS); Kilroy et al., 2021), not seem to be accounted for by deficits in motor coordina-
these deficits are not part of the diagnostic criteria and com- tion, perception, or basic motor control (Dewey et al., 2007;
monly are noted as secondary impairments stemming from Gizzonio et al., 2015; Kaur et al., 2018; Kilroy et al., 2022).
exclusion from social activities requiring motor coordina- Here, we aim to better understand praxis deficits in ASD
tion (i.e., sports teams, participation in musical ensembles, by directly investigating differences in error type in praxis
arts and crafts groups; Tal Saban & Kirby, 2019). However, skills between ASD, DCD, and TD groups. This builds
a moderate correlation between motor performance and upon our recent work that focused on accuracy comparisons
emotional cognition has been found in DCD (Vatandoust & between the three groups (Kilroy et al., 2022) and other prior
Hasanzadeh, 2018), and an increasing amount of data indi- work which investigated error types without also looking at
cate that social deficits may be more prominent in DCD than relations to social skills within groups (Dewey et al., 2007).
previously thought (Blank et al., 2019; Dewey et al., 2002; Here, we consider temporal, spatial, content, and body-part-
Karras et al., 2019; Kilroy et al., 2022; Sumner et al., 2016). for-tool errors using the Florida Apraxia Battery, modified
In addition, with regard to executive functions, children for children (FAB-M), which consists of subsections includ-
with DCD perform worse than children with ASD on tasks ing gesture-to-command, tool use, and imitation. We predict
involving visuospatial working memory (such as sillhouette that groups would significantly differ in the proportions of
detection, position discrimination, and cube analysis), while each error type each group would present, even after con-
children with ASD perform worse than children with DCD trolling for age and IQ. Based on our prior work (Kilroy

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

et al., 2022), we expected more errors in the ASD group Materials and Methods
compared to DCD and TD groups for gesture-to-command
and imitation of symbolic and pantomimed gestures. Specifi- Participants
cally, given data indicating significant visuospatial memory
deficits in DCD (Alloway et al., 2008), we hypothesized Ninety-five children (aged 8–17) participated in the current
that overall, the DCD group would present a significantly study. This sample is identical to that in Kilroy et al., 2022)
higher proportion of spatial errors across all praxis measures with the exception of 1 ASD participant removed for incom-
compared to the TD and ASD groups. Furthermore, due to plete data. Participants were recruited through social media,
significant difficulties in conceptual reasoning found in ASD and outreach to clinics, public schools, and private schools
children (Mostofsky et al., 2006), we hypothesized that ASD in the greater Los Angeles area. This study was part of a
participants would produce more conceptual motor errors larger study that included brain imaging components (Kilroy
(body-part-for-tool and content errors) compared to DCD et al., 2021). Inclusion criteria for all participants included:
and TD participants. (1) right-handed as measured by a modified Oldfield ques-
tionnaire (1971); (2) English speaking with at least 1 par-
ent proficient in English, (3) no history involving a loss of
Relationship Between Social and Motor Skills consciousness > 5 min; (4) born at or after 36 weeks of ges-
tation; and (5) an IQ score ≥ 80 on the Full Scale 4 [FSIV-
As described above, praxis skills have been posited to be IQ] or Verbal Comprehension Scale [VCI-IQ] assessed by
related to social skills in ASD. However, the relationship the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence 2nd Edition
between specific deviations in both praxis skills and social (WASI-2; Wechsler, 2011); (6) no other neurological or psy-
skills is less understood in ASD and DCD. Previous research chiatric diagnosis except for attention-deficit-hyperactivity-
has reported significant correlations between the error disorder (ADHD) and/or anxiety disorder in the ASD and
types in pantomimed gestures and the severity of social and DCD groups. Group characteristics can be seen in Table 1.
emotional ASD deficits also have been reported (Gizzonio
et al., 2015), though to our knowledge, similar comparisons
between error types and social skills in DCD have not been TD Group
made despite findings of elevated disturbances in social
skills (Kilroy et al., 2022; Sumner et al., 2016). Indeed, prior The TD group consisted of 35 participants (11 female and
research has shown that children with probable DCD per- 24 male). Additional inclusion criteria for the TD group
formed poorly on tasks involving recognition of emotional included: (1) a T-score < 65 on the Conners 3AI-Parent
facial expressions (Sumner et al., 2016). Moreover, children report (Conners 3); (2) at or above the 25th percentile on
with DCD showed differences compared to ASD and TD in the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC-2);
attending to socially relevant stimuli (Sumner et al., 2018). (3) a T-score > 60 on the Social Responsiveness Scale 2nd
Further, motor ability was a significant predictor of social Edition (SRS-2); (4) no DCD diagnosis or probable dysp-
behavior in DCD [using the social competence scale of the raxia based on the Developmental Coordination Disorder
Child-Behavior Checklist (CBCL)], even when control- Questionnaire (DCDQ), and no prior concerns of ASD.
ling for emotion recognition and visual-spatial organiza-
tion (Cummins et al., 2005). However, when considering
overall accuracy ratings of praxis skills as measured by the ASD Group
FAB-2, our previous work showed no relationships between
social and praxis skills in ASD or DCD groups (Kilroy et al., The ASD group consisted of 32 participants (7 female and
2022). Nevertheless, error type may be a more specific 25 male) Additional inclusion criteria included: (1) a pre-
marker, compared to accuracy, for exploring such relation- vious diagnosis of ASD and meeting criteria for an ASD
ships. Thus, to our knowledge, for the first time, here we diagnosis based on their scores on the ADOS-2 (adminis-
investigate relationships between social skills and error types tered by research certified assessor and reviewed by a cli-
in praxis skills in both ASD and DCD groups. To reduce the nician); (2) no other neurological or psychiatric disorders
number of comparisons, we focus on praxis skills known with the exceptions of ADHD and anxiety disorder. Based
to be most strongly affected in ASD compared to DCD, on parental reports 31 participants had clinical concerns
imitation of meaningful gestures, and gesture-to-command of ADHD and 6 for Anxiety disorder. Eleven ASD par-
(Kilroy et al., 2022). We predict correlations between these ticipants were prescribed and actively taking stimulants
praxis skills and social skills, ToM skills, affect recognition, medications for ADHD and/or anxiety and 1 individual
and autism severity (including measures of restricted and was on an antidepressant.
repetitive behaviors).

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 1  Demographic variables Characteristics of evaluated group


of the study sample
Group Age FSIQ ToM subscore ADOS-2
Social Restrictive and repeti- Compari-
affect tive behavior subscore son score
subscore

TD Mean 11.90 114.94 24.79


Std. deviation 2.22 12.64 1.99
N 35.00 35.00 35.00
ASD Mean 11.89 107.69 22.81 8.31 2.55 6.41
Std. deviation 2.28 19.87 3.08 3.61 1.39 1.98
N 32.00 32.00 27.00 32.00 31.00 32.00
DCD Mean 11.96 110.68 25.04
Std. deviation 2.35 17.04 2.49
N 28.00 28.00 28.00

No outliers (beyond 3 SD) were detected in our study sample for all variables

Probable DCD Group parent-report questionnaires (repetitive behavior question-


naires, social responsiveness scale, anxiety, AHDH measure).
The probable DCD group consisted of 28 participants (11
female and 17 male). Additional inclusion criteria included: Motor Measures
(1) scoring at the 16th percentile or lower on the MABC-2;
(2) no other neurological or psychiatric disorders with the Praxis
exceptions of ADHD and anxiety disorder; (3) no first-degree
relatives with an ASD diagnosis, and no concerns of ASD Praxis skills were measured by the Florida Apraxia Bat-
diagnosis. Additionally, if the participant’s SRS-2T-score was tery, modified for children (FAB-M; Mostofsky et al., 2006),
over 76 (“severe risk” for ASD) the participant was excluded which consists of Tool Use (TU), Gesture-to-Command
from the DCD category. Five DCD participants had SRS-2 (GTC), and Imitation (IMI). For each of the three sections,
scores between 65 and 75 (“moderate risk”); however, none the skilled gestures included Pantomime actions (actions that
met ADOS-2 criteria for an ASD diagnosis and were not involve the subject imagining the action and performing it,
excluded from the sample. Based on parental reports, there such as hammering nails into a wall in front of them). IMI
were 21 participants with concerns of ADHD and three par- and GTC further included Symbolic actions (actions that
ticipants with concerns of Anxiety. Four DCD participants represent more abstract concepts, such as “Show me how
were taking prescribed psychotropic medications for ADHD you tell someone to be quiet”). Pantomime and Symbolic
and/or anxiety (see Table 1). actions together are considered “Meaningful Gestures” and
have a communicative intent. The IMI section additionally
Procedures included a subsection of “Meaningless Gestures” (novel
movements with no explicit communicative meaning). In
After signing the consent and assent forms, all participants TU, the participants are provided with 17 different tools and
filled out demographic and handedness questionnaires. Par- asked to show how they would use each tool. In GTC, the
ticipants with ASD then completed the ADOS. All partici- participants were verbally instructed to perform 25 different
pants completed IQ measures, social affect and theory of mind gestures:17 pantomime questions and 8 symbolic items. In
measures, trait empathy and, and FAB-M next with a trained IMI, the participants are instructed to imitate 34 gestures
RA. They then performed the MABC-2 with a trained RA. shown to them by the administrator: 17 pantomime, 8 sym-
Simultaneously, in a separate room, parents completed all bolic, and 9 meaningless gestures. The pantomime actions

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 2  Sections and subsections of the FAB-M


Gesture-to-command (GTC) Imitation (IMI) Tool use (TU)

Pantomime (# of items = 17) Pantomime (# of items = 17) Pantomime (# of items = 17)


e.g., The administrator tells e.g., The administrator acts out the use of a hammer and e.g., The administrator gives the participant a toy
the participant to act out the nail and the participant must copy it hammer and asks him or her to show them how they
use of a hammer and nail would use the tool
Symbolic (# of items = 8) Symbolic (# of items = 8)
e.g., The administrator tells e.g., The administrator salutes and the participant must
the participant to act out as if copy it
they were saluting
Meaningless (# of items = 9)
e.g., The administrator makes meaningless arm circles,
and the participant must copy it

Table 3  Breakdown of all possible error types in the FAB-M

Error type Errors Description

Spatial Amplitude (A) Subject shows irregularity in the height and/or width of the motion (in the form of an
increase or decrease)
Internal configuration (IC) Subject does not position hand in the proper way to use the tool
External configuration (EC) Subject's tool movements do not align properly with the expected relationship between the
tool and object
Movement (M) Subject shows movement of the wrong joints when performing the task
Temporal Timing (T) Subject shows irregular timing in their action
Sequence (S) Subject does not follow the sequence of a gesture (can be the deletion or addition of parts of
a sequence)
Occurrence (O) Subject performs too little or too few cycles of the action
Content Concretization (C) Subject performs a pantomime action on an object unrelated to the task
Preservative (P) Subject performs a movement that uses parts or all the previous movement
Related (R) Subject performs a movement that is related to the movement solicited, but not the actual
movement
Non-related (N) Subject performs a gesture that was not solicited
Hand (H) Subject uses their hand in place of the imagined tool
Body part for tool Body Part for Tool (BPT) Subject uses a body part as a tool
Other No response (NR) Subject did not respond
Unrecognizable response (UR) Subject's response was unable to be recognized

in each category are identical actions amongst TU, GTC, 4 domains: spatial errors, temporal errors, content errors,
and IMI, (see Table 2). and other errors. We additionally included a separate fifth
Video recordings of each participant were collected at a domain, body-part-for-tool (BPT), following Gizzonio et al.
front-facing and over-the-shoulder angle in respect to the (2015), who removed these from the spatial error section. A
participant for the duration of the assessment. After the description of all possible errors is provided in Table 3. Sec-
assessment was completed, three occupational therapists tions and subsections of the FAB-M are broken down into 3
(OTs) reviewed and scored the assessment. Each item was sections, GTC, IMI, and TU. All three sections can further
either deemed correct or incorrect with a specified error be broken down into subsections. All three share the same 17
type as outlined by Gizzonio et al. (2015) (see Table 3). pantomime questions, GTC and IMI share 8 symbolic ques-
An agreement of 80% or higher was achieved between two tions, and IMI has an additional nine meaningless questions.
raters with the third rater acting as a tiebreaker. For items
where participants performed multiple error types, all types
of errors were recorded (this occurred in 12.22% of TU tri-
als, 1.39% of GTC, 15.19% of IMI).
Calculation of errors on the FAB-M followed Rothi
and Heilman (2014), who categorized each error into 1 of

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Other Behavioral, Socio‑emotional, and Cognitive Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder


Measures
Both the parent measure and the child self-report measure of
IQ the Conners 3rd edition ADHD Index (Conners, 2008) were
used as two separate scores.
The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI-II;
Wechsler, 2011) is a measure of intelligence normed for ages A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment
6–90 and contains four subtests including Block Design, (NEPSY‑II)
Vocabulary, Matrix Reasoning, and Similarities. Verbal
Comprehension Index (VCI) consists of Vocabulary and Each participant was assessed using the Social Perception
Similarities subtests and Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI) domain of the Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assess-
consists of Block Design and Matrix reasoning subtests. ment 2 (NEPSY-II; Korkman et al., 2007). The Affect Rec-
ognition (AR; 35 items) and Theory of Mind (ToM; 21
Measures of Autism Severity items) subtests within the social perception domain were
utilized. The ToM subtest contains a verbal and contextual
Participants with a previous diagnosis of ASD, or a sus- score. The sum of raw scores from each subtest are used to
pected diagnosis of ASD were given additional assessments inform a percentile rank, scaled scores, and a social percep-
to confirm the diagnosis and determine the severity of their tion total score. Higher scores indicate greater ability.
deficits. The ADOS-2 is a diagnostic exam that provides
standardized measures of a child’s social affect, ability to Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI)
communicate, and restricted and repetitive behaviors. Addi-
tionally, the diagnostic tool provides a comparison severity Each participant was administered the IRI to quantify empa-
score ranging from 1 to 10 (10 being the highest; Lord et al., thy (Davis, 1983). The assessment is broken down into four
2012). The ADI-R (Lord et al., 1994) is a clinical diagnostic traits that encompass empathy: Perspective Taking (PT; the
instrument for assessing autism. It is a structured interview ability to take on other perspectives), Fantasy (F; the extent
with the parent with open-ended questions and scored across to which one identifies with fictitious characters), Empathic
three domains: Language/Communication (LC), Reciprocal Concern (EC; the level of concern one has for others), and
Social Interactions (RSI), and Restricted, Repetitive, and Personal Distress (PD; negative feelings felt in response to
Stereotyped Behaviors (RRB). In this study, RSI was used as the negative circumstances of another person). The assess-
our index of autism social severity since we were primarily ment generates PT, F, EC, and PD subscores. Note, the F
interested in social severity. subscale has previously been reported to have reliability
concerns (Cox et al., 2012) and therefore results should be
Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors considered carefully.

The Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R) is a vali- Statistical Measures


dated 43-item questionnaire that measures restricted and
repetitive behaviors in children, adolescents, and adults Preliminary Analysis
with ASD, appropriate for ages 2–18 years (Hooker et al.,
2019; Schertz et al., 2016). Parents rate items on a 4-point Upon completion of data collection, the sample data was
Likert scale ranging from (0) “behavior does not occur” to assessed for potential outliers. From the original sample of
(3) “behavior occurs and is a real problem” within the past 96 participants, 1 ASD subject was removed because they
month (Lam & Aman, 2007). Bodfish et al. (1999) found the were missing their ADOS diagnostic scores. FSIQ, ToM, and
inter-rater reliability of the 6 subscales (Stereotyped Behav- ADOS scores were evaluated for outliers (3 standard devia-
ior, Self-injurious Behavior, Compulsive Behavior, Ritual- tions from their group average). No participants met these
istic Behavior, Sameness Behavior, and Restricted Behav- criteria. Additionally, a one-way ANOVA was conducted
ior) to be between 0.55 and 0.78, and test–retest reliability on both Age and FSIQ, with group as a factor, to determine
between 0.52 and 0.96. Raw scores are added to achieve the homogeneity of these variables within each group. Both
subscale scores, and the sum of subscores is used as the total FSIQ and Age (F(2,92) = 1.610, p > 0.2 and F(2,92) = 0.009,
score. Previous research recommends focusing on the sub- p > 0.9 respectively) showed no statistical significance, sug-
scores, as opposed to the total score, to capture the nuances gesting that both variables were homogenous within each
of such behaviors (Mirenda et al., 2010; Scahill et al., 2014). group. However, after running a preliminary ANCOVA
with Age, Sex, and FSIQ as covariates, both Age and FSIQ
(and not Sex) were found to be significantly associated with

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

performance on the praxis assessment (p < 0.005 for both). operationalized by scores on the ADOS-2 and ADI-R RSI.
For this reason, both variables were added to our model as We further correlated gesture errors with scores on the SRS-
covariates for the remainder of the analysis. 2, NEPSY-II subtests (ToM and Affect Recognition), IRI,
Performance on each praxis assessment was measured and RBS-R for both ASD and DCD participants. Of our 32
by the participant’s Error Performance (EP) in each section ASD participants, 5 were removed from the correlation with
and subsection. In some instances, not all participants were NEPSY-II due to missing scores, and 1 was removed from
administered all items (i.e., request to end session early or the correlation with the ADOS-2 due to a missing score.
missed item by administrator). EP was measured as a ratio of
incorrectly performed questions to the total number of ques-
tions administered for each section/subsection. For example, Results
a participant who performed two errors in GTC out of 17
administered Pantomime items would have a GTC Panto- Between Group Analysis of Errors on FAB‑M
mime EP score of 2/17 or 0.118, however, a participant who Components
performed 2 errors in the GTC out of 15 administered items
would have an EP score of 2/15 or 0.133. EP scores were Tool Use (TU) Analysis
calculated for each section/subsection combination.
Overall, the ASD and DCD groups performed a signifi-
cantly larger proportion of total TU errors compared to the
Individual Section Analysis
TD group (ps < 0.001; see full results in Table 4). Nota-
bly, the DCD group had significantly more temporal errors
A repeated measures ANCOVA (rmANCOVA) was con-
compared to the TD group (p = 0.009), and the ASD group
ducted with group (TD, DCD, & ASD) as the between-sub-
had significantly more content errors than the TD group
jects factor and the overall performance by subsection as the
(p = 0.002). However, there were no significant differences
within-subjects factor. This analysis was conducted for all
in errors between ASD and DCD groups. When we control
three sections of the praxis assessment individually. We fol-
for age, and total FSIQ there was no change in the signifi-
lowed up this analysis by examining the overall performance
cance of the results (all p < 0.05; see Table 5).
of each error type. Error type performance (SPAT, TEMP,
BPT, and CONTENT) was added as an additional within-
subjects variable. Analyses were conducted separately by
GTC Analysis
section.
The ASD group performed significantly more GTC errors
Pantomime Trial Analysis than the TD group (p < 0.001; See Fig. 1a and Table 4).
Specifically, the ASD group performed significantly more
Additional analysis was done on the EP of pantomime body-part-for-tool errors than either the TD or the DCD
actions across all 3 sections within each group (ASD, DCD, group (p < 0.001 and p = 0.003, respectively; See Fig. 2).
TD). This was completed to compare, within-group, per- When we control for age, and total FSIQ there was no
formance of the same pantomimed movements when given change in the significance of the results (all p < 0.05; see
different instructions (Tool Use, Gesture-to-Command, and Table 5).
Imitation). An rmANCOVA was conducted with section EP
as the within-subject factor (with 3 levels: GTC, IMI, TU) IMI Analysis
and group as a between-subjects factor. If the main-effects
were significant, post-hoc analyses with a Bonferroni cor- The IMI section was subdivided into pantomime, sym-
rection were performed in order to understand pairwise com- bolic, and meaningless gestures. Overall, as Fig. 1b shows,
parisons of EP between each group. the ASD group produced a significantly larger proportion
of imitation symbolic errors than both the DCD and TD
Correlation Analysis groups (p = 0.006 and p < 0.001 respectively; see Table 4),
while the DCD group performed significantly more errors
A Pearson correlation coefficient was calculated between than the TD group (p = 0.011). Notably, the ASD group per-
overall performance scores on the Imitation and Gesture- formed a significantly larger proportion of temporal errors
to-Command subsections of FAB-M, for error types that in both the pantomime and symbolic subsections compared
showed differences between ASD and DCD (determined to the TD and DCD groups (pantomime: p =  < 0.001 and
by the analyses above) and severity of behavioral defi- p = 0.012 respectively; symbolic: p = 0.006 and p = 0.013;
cits for ASD and DCD participants. ASD severity was See Fig. 3). In a post-hoc analysis, we found that temporal

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 4  Results of significant statistical comparisons (ANOVA) between groups without including age and IQ as covariates
Comparison ANOVA statistics Pairwise group comparisons
2
F-statistic (2, 92) p value η ASD-TD δ p value DCD-TD δ p value ASD-DCD δ p value

TU OP 27.842  < 0.001 0.377 0.258*  < 0.001 0.177*  < 0.001 0.08 0.104


TU Temp 4.808 0.01 0.095 0.022 0.18 0.036* 0.009 − 0.014 0.713
TU content 6.596 0.002 0.125 0.09* 0.002 0.063 0.059 0.027 0.945
GTC OP 10.135  < 0.001 0.181 0.157*  < 0.001 0.077 0.106 0.08 0.1
GTC pantomime BPT 8.886  < 0.001 0.162 0.071*  < 0.001 0.005 1 0.065* 0.003
IMI symbolic OP 20.831  < 0.001 0.312 0.26*  < 0.001 0.125* 0.011 0.135* 0.006
IMI pantomime Spat 19.14  < 0.001 0.294 0.249*  < 0.001 0.188*  < 0.001 0.061 0.523
IMI pantomime Temp 9.972  < 0.001 0.178 0.067*  < 0.001 0.019 0.706 0.048* 0.012
IMI symbolic Temp 6.338 0.003 0.121 0.052* 0.006 0.002 1 0.05* 0.013
Transitive trials OP 23.3  < 0.001 0.336 0.237*  < 0.001 0.155*  < 0.001 0.082 0.09
Transitive trials Spat 21.364  < 0.001 0.317 0.188*  < 0.001 0.128*  < 0.001 0.06 0.171
Transitive trials Temp 7.361 0.001 0.138 0.03*  < 0.001 0.021* 0.048 0.01 0.78

All transitive trials comparisons are repeated measure ANOVAs, while the rest are one-way independent sample ANOVAs
TU tool use, GTC​ gesture-to-command, IMI imitation, OP overall performance of errors, BPT body part as a tool errors, Spat spatial errors,
Temp temporal errors, δ group mean difference
*Significant pairwise comparison

error differences between the ASD group and TD/DCD in TU compared to both IMI and GTC trials (IMI-TU
groups were particularly driven by errors in sequencing δ = 0.287; GTC-TU δ = 0.184; all ps < 0.001). For ASD and
(p < 0.001), but not in irregular timing or performing too DCD groups, there were significantly fewer spatial errors
many or too few occurrences. Additionally, the ASD and for GTC compared to IMI pantomime trials (ASD: GTC-
DCD groups performed a significantly larger proportion of IMI δ = − 0.14; p < 0.001; DCD: GTC-IMI δ = − 0.13;
spatial errors for pantomime imitation compared to the TD p < 0.001).
group (ps < 0.001), but ASD and DCD groups did not sig-
nificantly differ in spatial errors. All results remained sig- Temporal Errors Across Pantomime Trials
nificant when we controlled for FSIQ (or VCI) and age (see
Table 5). The TD group performed significantly more temporal
errors in the GTC subsection compared to the TU subsec-
tion (GTC-TU δ = 0.023; p = 0.031). The ASD group per-
Pantomime Trials Across Sections formed significantly more temporal errors during imitation
pantomime trials than in GTC or TU (IMI-GTC δ = 0.047;
The TD group performed significantly better on pantomime p = 0.002 and IMI-TU δ = 0.05; p < 0.001, respectively). The
gestures across TU, GTC, and imitation trials than both ASD DCD group showed no significant differences in temporal
and DCD groups (ps < 0.001; see Table 4 for full results). errors between the three subsections.
There was no statistically significant difference in overall
performance between ASD and DCD participants. However, Correlations Between Praxis Measures and Other
pairwise comparisons revealed that TU had the least overall Behavioral Measures
errors compared to GTC and IMI trials for all groups. Below
we include analyses for spatial and temporal errors only, as In the ASD and DCD groups, correlations were used to eval-
the other error types (e.g., BPT, content) had floor effects uate the relationship between intellectual, social, emotional,
in some groups. and behavioral diagnostic scores and errors in each subsec-
tion of the praxis assessment that differed between ASD and
Spatial Errors Across Pantomime Trials DCD groups (Symbolic Imitation Overall error performance,
Symbolic Imitation Temporal errors, and Pantomime Imi-
ASD and DCD groups showed a significantly larger pro- tation Temporal errors, Gesture-to-Command BPT errors).
portion of spatial errors across pantomime trials compared There were no correlations between behavioral measures and
to the TD group but did not differ significantly from each praxis measures in the DCD group. The results for the ASD
other. All groups showed significantly fewer spatial errors group can be seen in Table 6 and in scatter plots depicted

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 5  Results of significant statistical comparisons (ANCOVA) between groups after including age and IQ as covariates
Comparison ANCOVA statistics Pairwise group comparisons
2
F-statistic (2, 90) p value η ASD-TD δ p value DCD-TD δ p value ASD-DCD δ p value

TU OP 29.875  < 0.001 0.399 0.249*  < 0.001 0.174*  < 0.001 0.075 0.097


TU Temp 5.745 0.004 0.113 0.023 0.12 0.037* 0.004 − 0.015 0.609
TU content 5.309 0.007 0.106 0.077* 0.007 0.056 0.086 0.021 1
GTC OP 10.325  < 0.001 0.187 0.141*  < 0.001 0.069 0.094 0.071 0.089
GTC pantomime BPT 8.01  < 0.001 0.151 0.059* 0.002 − 0.001 1 0.060* 0.003
IMI symbolic OP 20.928  < 0.001 0.317 0.249*  < 0.001 0.120* 0.009 0.129* 0.006
IMI pantomime Spat 22.382  < 0.001 0.332 0.24*  < 0.001 0.185*  < 0.001 0.055 0.502
IMI pantomime Temp 8.369  < 0.001 0.157 0.06*  < 0.001 0.015 1 0.045* 0.016
IMI symbolic Temp 5.73 0.005 0.113 0.049* 0.011  < 0.001 1 0.049* 0.016
Transitive trials OP 26.8  < 0.001 0.373 0.222*  < 0.001 0.148*  < 0.001 0.074 0.071
Transitive trials Spat 23.695  < 0.001 0.345 0.183*  < 0.001 0.126*  < 0.001 0.056 0.151
Transitive trials Temp 6.918 0.002 0.133 0.028* 0.001 0.02* 0.046 0.008 0.902

All transitive trials comparisons are repeated measure ANCOVAs, while the rest are one-way independent sample ANCOVAs
TU tool use, GTC​ gesture-to-command, IMI imitation, OP overall performance of errors, BPT body part as a tool errors, Spat spatial errors,
Temp temporal errors, δ group mean difference
*Significant pairwise comparison

***
*** *
*** **
***
* ***
*** *

Fig. 1  Error performance on subsections FAB-M for TD, ASD, and section. C depicts error performance in the Tool Use (TU) section.
DCD Groups. A depicts error performance in Geture-to Command *p < 0.05, **p < 0.005, ***p < 0.0005
(GTC) section. B depicts error performance in the imitation (IMI)

in Fig. 4. For the ASD group, we found that for imitation of


meaningful gestures (pantomime and symbolic), temporal
errors were significantly negatively correlated with affect **
GTC BPT Error Performance

**
recognition (pantomime: NEPSY-AR Scaled; R = − 0.383, . 12

p = 0.044; symbolic: NEPSY-AR Scaled; R = − 0.465,


.10
p = 0.013). However, on visual inspection of the association
of temporal errors in symbolic imitation and NEPSY-AR .08

Scaled, we found a strong floor effect, such that most ASD .06
participants did not perform temporal errors regardless of
their affect recognition score, and the correlation was instead .04

primarily driven by one prominent high error score. Hence, .02

we focus instead on the significant correlation for panto-


.00
mime temporal errors and affect recognition. Pantomime
Temporal errors during imitation of pantomime trials
were also significantly negatively correlated with restricted Fig. 2  Body-part-for-Tool (BPT) Errors during Gesture-to-Command
and repetitive behaviors (RBS-R Sameness Subscale; (GTC) comparing between groups. **p < 0.005

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

R = − 0.429, p = 0.020). For imitation of symbolic gestures, Tool Use (TU)
increased overall error performance was significantly cor-
related with less restricted and repetitive behaviors (RBS-R Both ASD and DCD groups performed significantly more
Restrictive Behaviors Subscale; R = − 0.537, p = 0.002). TU errors than the TD group, with predominantly more spa-
For Gesture-to-Command, body-part-for-tool errors were tial errors than the TD group (though spatial errors were the
significantly related such that higher scores on the RBS-R most common error for all groups). Compared to the TD
Sameness Subscale were related to lower body-part-for-tool group, the DCD group performed more temporal errors, and
errors (R = − 0.374, p = 0.045). the ASD group performed more content errors. The latter
is in line with difficulties in conceptual reasoning found in
ASD children (Mostofsky et al., 2006). However, there were
Discussion no significant differences between ASD and DCD groups,
indicating that TU errors are not capable of distinguishing
While motor impairments are the core deficit in DCD, between these two groups.
there is significant evidence that motor deficits are also
common to ASD (~ 80%; Bhat, 2020; Chukoskie et al., Gesture‑to‑Command (GTC)
2013; Edwards, 2014; Fournier et  al., 2010; Gizzonio
et al., 2015; Green et al., 2009; Hannant et al., 2018; Hil- Overall, for GTC, our sample performed significantly more
ton et al., 2012; Kilroy et al., 2019; Ming et al., 2007; errors on the pantomime than the symbolic items, consist-
Mody et al., 2016; Williams et al., 2004). However, how ent with previous studies (Gizzonio et al., 2015). This dif-
ASD and DCD groups differ in motor impairments has ference in performance is likely due to less motor practice
been less studied. Prior data indicate that one source of with pantomime gestures included in the assessment (e.g.,
motor differences between ASD and DCD groups may be hammering nails, painting a wall, or using a saw) as com-
in praxis skills, with ASD groups showing more praxis pared to symbolic gestures, which they may daily perform
impairments than DCD groups, especially in imitation (e.g., giving the “O-K” symbol, “shooing” someone away).
of meaningful gestures and gesture-to-command (Dewey Further, pantomime gestures may be more complicated to
et al., 2007; Green et al., 2002; Kilroy et al., 2022; Miller perform than symbolic gestures, consistent with adult litera-
et al., 2021; Paquet et al., 2019; Sumner et al., 2016; Wis- ture on apraxia, which reports greater errors in pantomime
dom et al., 2007). Here we focus on prominent results in vs. symbolic subsections (Power et al., 2010).
error type between our three groups while also controlling Further, we found that only the ASD group had signifi-
for age and IQ. Moreover, we discuss how different error cant GTC content errors compared to the TD group. How-
types are related to social functioning in ASD and DCD ever, a post-hoc analysis showed that these differences were
groups. A main finding is that two prominent ASD symp- no longer significant when controlling for verbal IQ. This
tomologies, socio-emotional processing and restricted and may reflect the strong communicative/language component
repetitive behaviors, correlate with pantomime imitation of GTC. Indeed, deficits in verbal working memory are com-
and gesture-to-command errors. These findings are dis- mon in youth with ASD children (Alloway et al., 2008), and
cussed further below. the large proportion of content errors in the GTC section

*
***
*

Fig. 3  (TEMP) errors during Imitation (IMI) of Pantomime and Symbolic gestures comparing between groups. Note, the symbolic and panto-
mime sections together constitute the imitation of meaningful gestures section of the FAB-M. *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.0005

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 6  Correlations of error performance within the ASD group with behavioral variables
NEPSY ToM—verbal ToM—con- ToM—total RBS ste- RBS self- RBS RBS ritualis- RBS sameness
AR—scaled textual reotyped injurious compul- tic Bx Bx
Bx Bx sive Bx

GTC panto- − 0.135 − 0.141 − 0.091 − 0.087 0.004 − 0.027 − 0.013 − 0.114 − 0.374*
mime BPT
IMI symbolic − 0.264 − 0.143 0.009 0.052 − 0.255 − 0.239 − 0.343 − 0.294 − 0.244
OP
IMI panto- − 0.383* − 0.164 − 0.06 − 0.136 − 0.089 − 0.059 − 0.003 − 0.066 − 0.429*
mime Temp
IMI symbolic − 0.465* − 0.178 0.306 0.145 0.009 0.13 0.198 0.059 0.106
Temp
RBS restricted Bx RBS total IRI PT IRI fantasy IRI EC IRI PD ADOS social affect ADI-R RSI

GTC pantomime BPT − 0.1 − 0.141 0.147 0.236 0.181 0.279 − 0.003 − 0.001
IMI symbolic OP − 0.537** − 0.29 0.048 0.121 0.152 − 0.035 − 0.168 − 0.166
IMI pantomime Temp − 0.114 − 0.311 − 0.12 − 0.141 − 0.17 0.027 0.078 − 0.217
IMI symbolic Temp − 0.174 0.129 − 0.201 0.05 0.266 0.026 0.024 0.079

GTC​gesture-to-command, IMI imitation, OP overall performance of errors, BPT body part as a tool errors, Temp temporal errors, AR affect rec-
ognition, ToM theory of mind, Bx behaviors, PT perspective taking, EC empathic concern, PD personal distress, RSI Reciprocal Social Interac-
tion
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01

may be due to difficulty following verbal task instructions Imitation (IMI)


for the ASD group.
Importantly, we found that the ASD group performed Overall, the ASD group performed significantly worse than
significantly more BPT errors during GTC than either the the TD group in all IMI subsections (symbolic, pantomime,
TD or DCD group, even after controlling for FSIQ (or VIQ) meaningless) and significantly worse than the DCD group
and age. These results corroborate prior findings indicat- in the symbolic subsection, consistent with prior studies
ing that frequent production of BPT errors may be a com- (Dewey et al., 2007; Kilroy et al., 2022). Moreover, the ASD
mon feature of ASD, much more so than in DCD (Dewey group performed a significantly larger proportion of tem-
et al., 2007; Fabbri-Destro et al., 2019; Gizzonio et al., 2015; poral errors during imitation of symbolic and pantomime
Mostofsky et al., 2006). Such BPT errors do not seem to be gestures than the DCD and TD groups. Thus, the ASD group
linked to VIQ, and instead may be associated with other showed a significant impairment compared to both TD and
ASD symptomatology. Indeed, there is evidence indicating DCD, suggesting a unique deficit in temporal frequency of
that BPT errors are not a result of production ease due to imitated meaningful gestures in children with ASD. These
less cognitive and motor demands than pantomimed actions, data are in line with previous studies that have shown that
but instead are related to increased brain activity than panto- temporal errors were among the most common error type
mimed gestures. In adults, BPT production, as compared to among children with ASD (Dewey et al., 2007; Gizzonio
pantomimed production is associated with increased bilat- et al., 2015; Mostofsky et al., 2006), and with our previous
eral parietal and IFG/premotor activity, specifically in the work showing deficits in meaningful imitation (including
right parietal supramarginal gyrus (Ohgami et al., 2004). both pantomime and symbolic sections) are particularly
It has been suggested that increased BPT errors following worse in ASD compared to DCD (Kilroy et al., 2021). Tem-
apraxia may follow from damage to the left hemisphere poral deficits may relate to problems following a particu-
(common in apraxia) which results in increased right hemi- lar sequence, irregular timing of imitation, not following
sphere processing, producing BPT errors (Ohgami et al., the sequencing of a gesture, or performing too many or too
2004). Whether such underlying mechanisms can also be few occurrences of the action. In a post-hoc analysis, we
found to explain increased BPT errors in ASD remain to be found that differences between the ASD group and TD/DCD
investigated. groups were particularly driven by errors in sequencing (but
not in irregular timing or performing too many or too few

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Fig. 4  Significant correlations between errors during gesture-to-com- with RBS Restricted behavior subscale, C, D Timing errors during
mand (GTC) or imitation (IMI) and behavioral measures in ASD. A imitation of pantomime gestures are related to NEPSY Affect Recog-
BPT body-part-for-tool errors during GTC pantomime trials are sig- nition and RBS Sameness subscale Note: Correlations for the DCD
nificantly correlated with Repetitive Behaviors (RBS) Sameness Sub- group were not statistically significant and for clarity, not depicted
scale, B Errors during symbolic imitation are significantly correlated here

occurrences). Thus, therapists working with individuals with affect recognition. Indeed, the inferior frontal gyrus oper-
ASD may want to particularly assess for sequence errors cularis (IFGop), a brain region that is involved in imitation
during screening assessments and focus on sequencing dur- (Heiser et al., 2003) and motor rhythmic and sequencing
ing imitation focused interventions. Such deficits in motor behavior (Sakreida et al., 2018), is also hypoactive in ASD
planning of imitation sequencing may also affect the utiliza- when observing (and imitating) facial expressions or hand
tion of these representations in understanding other people’s actions (Dapretto et al., 2006; Kilroy et al., 2021; Wang
actions, as prior studies have indicated (Fabbri-Destro et al., et al., 2004). This result also corroborates other studies that
2019). Whether this difficulty may be related to additional have found imitation ability correlates with aspects of social
social deficits in the ASD group is discussed further in the ability (ToM, joint attention, functional and symbolic play,
next section. social repricroticy, empathy; for a review, please see Vivanti
& Hamilton, 2014), as well as autism severity (Gizzonio
et al., 2015; Ingersoll & Meyer, 2011; Pittet et al., 2022;
Correlations of Praxis with Other Behavioral Rogers et al., 2003; Zachor et al., 2010), as previously dis-
Measures cussed. Further, a recent study found that praxis skills are
significantly correlated to the ability to understand errors
In the ASD group, increased timing errors in imitation of in other people’s actions (Fabbri-Destro et al., 2019). Here,
pantomimed gestures were significantly related to poorer we add to these prior data by showing that errors in timing

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

during imitation are particularly related to affect recognition. Limitations


Our data support the notion that intact motor representations
may be necessary for the ability to use one’s own motor We note that the FAB-M is not norm-referenced, with
system to understand and interpret other people’s actions, limited reporting of its psychometric properties, though
and poor imitative functioning may thus impact social pro- some of the concern was mitigated in the current study
cesses, though further work is needed (Cattaneo et al., 2007; by having at least two raters obtain scoring agreement.
Fabbri-Destro et al., 2019). We did not find any significant Further, unlike the ADOS and Affect recognition and
correlations in the DCD group, indicating that only in the ToM assessments, some of the other measures used here
ASD group are imitation difficulties linked to socio-emo- (IRI, ADHD, and RRB) were self-report or parent-report
tional skills. In the DCD group, and not in the ASD group, measures. Future studies using behavioral or observa-
we have previously shown that praxis skills are significantly tional measures of empathy, ADHD, and restricted and
related to motor ability rather than socio-emotional skills repetitive behaviors would increase the validity of the
(Kilroy et al., 2022). current results on those measures. Finally, we note that
Unexpectedly, in the ASD group, we found that our experimenters were not blind to the group of the par-
fewer errors (better performance) in pantomime imita- ticipants, which may bias results.
tion timing, overall symbolic imitation performance,
and gesture-to-command body-part-for-tool errors were
related to increased restricted and repetitive behaviors. Conclusions
These findings contradict findings that poorer motor
skills were related to increased restricted and repeti- Here we show that timing errors during imitation of panto-
tive behaviors and attention in ASD (Bhat et al., 2022; mimed actions and BPT errors during gesture-to-command
Ravizza et al., 2013), though to our knowledge this is are significantly more common in the ASD group compared
the first time a relationship with praxis skills has been to the DCD and TD group, and that these errors correlate
shown. Our findings may be due to a mediating factor with socio-emotional skills and restricted and repetitive
not analyzed here and needs further exploration in future behaviors. Indeed, it is noteworthy that unlike the DCD
studies. group, in ASD, praxis skills are not correlated with motor
Finally, we note that while some investigators have impairment but instead are correlated with core autism
found correlations between praxis skills and autism symptomatology. Thus, in ASD, deficits in praxis skill may
severity (Fabbri-Destro et  al., 2019; Gizzonio et  al., be related to underlying etiology. This data may support the
2015; Ingersoll & Meyer, 2011; Pittet et al., 2022; Rogers notion that having intact processing of imitative temporal
et al., 2003; Zachor et al., 2010), contrary to our predic- sequencing may be critical for socio-emotional understand-
tions, we did not find any relationship between temporal ing of others, though further work is needed. By contrast, in
errors during imitation, or BPT errors during GTC, with DCD, underlying motor and praxis impairments are corre-
autism severity nor with other behavioral measures we lated, but not related to social symptomology. Further stud-
examined (verbal IQ, theory-of-mind abilities, empathic ies are needed to investigate whether social disturbances in
processing, or ADHD symptomatology). We note that DCD are primary or secondary symptomology (Tal Saban
nearly all the prior studies (except for Gizzonio et al., & Kirby, 2019). Finally, these data suggest the need for
2015 and Fabbri-Destro et al., 2019) were conducted in individualized ASD interventions, focusing on timing dur-
toddlers and/or children younger than 5 years old, and ing pantomime gesture imitation, and BPT errors during
the study by Fabbri-Destro et al. (2019) focused on spa- gesture-to-command.
tial errors during GTC, which may explain discrepan-
cies with the current study. Nevertheless, the finding Acknowledgments  We thank all our participants and their families. We
also thank all lab members and research assistants for their contribu-
that both socio-emotional processing and restricted and tions to participant recruitment, data collection, and scoring, in particu-
repetitive behaviors—two of the diagnostic criteria of lar Riley McGuire and Trinh Nguyen. We also give special thanks to
ASD—correlate with imitation and/or GTC errors, sug- Stewart Mostofsky and his team for training in scoring the FAB-M and
gests that a subset of prominent autism-related behaviors Dr. Mary Margaret Windsor for her assistance and careful considera-
tion of the FAB-M scoring and discussion. Research reported in this
are associated with imitation and praxis difficulties, and publication was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National
may help better understand how motor and social deficits Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National
could stem from similar underlying biological processes Institutes of Health under Award Number R01HD079432. The content
in ASD. is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily rep-
resent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Author Contributions  LAZ and SAC performed the conceptualization.


LAZ, SAC, EK, CB, and LH performed the design. GA, AJ, EK, CB,

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

LH, PR, AH, and AN performed the data collection. GA and AJ per- Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Interrela-
formed the data analysis. GA, AJ, LAZ, and SAC performed the data tions among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism.
interpretation. GA, AJ, and LAZ prepared the manuscript initial draft. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 91–106.
GA, AJ, LAZ, SAC, EK, CB, LH, PR, AH, and AN performed the https://​doi.​org/​10.​1023/a:​10148​36521​114
manuscript editing. GA and AJ performed the figure creation. Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., Boria, S., Pieraccini, C., Monti, A.,
Cossu, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (2007). Impairment of actions chains
Funding  Funding was provided by Eunice Kennedy Shriver National in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Pro-
Institute of Child Health and Human Development (Grant no. ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(45), 17825–
R01HD079432). 17830. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1073/​pnas.​07062​73104
Chang, S. H., & Yu, N. Y. (2016). Comparison of motor praxis and per-
Declarations  formance in children with varying levels of developmental coordi-
nation disorder. Human Movement Science, 48, 7–14. https://​doi.​
Conflict of interest  The authors state no conflicts of interest. org/​10.​1016/j.​humov.​2016.​04.​001
Chukoskie, L., Townsend, J., & Westerfield, M. (2013). Motor skill
in autism spectrum disorders: A subcortical view. International
Review of Neurobiology, 113, 207–249. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​
B978-0-​12-​418700-​9.​00007-1
References Colombi, C., Liebal, K., Tomasello, M., Young, G., Warneken, F.,
& Rogers, S. J. (2009). Examining correlates of cooperation in
autism: Imitation, joint attention, and understanding intentions.
Adornetti, I., Ferretti, F., Chiera, A., Wacewicz, S., Żywiczyński, P.,
Autism, 13(2), 143–163. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​13623​61308​
Deriu, V., Marini, A., Magni, R., Casula, L., Vicari, S., & Valeri,
09851
G. (2019). Do children with autism spectrum disorders understand
Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners 3rd edition: Manual (Vol. 14). Multi-
pantomimic events? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1382–1382.
Health Systems.
https://​doi.​org/​10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2019.​01382
Cox, C. L., Uddin, L. Q., Di Martino, A., Castellanos, F. X., Milham,
Alloway, T. P., Rajendran, G., & Archibald, L. M. D. (2008). Work-
M. P., & Kelly, C. (2012). The balance between feeling and know-
ing memory in children with developmental disorders. Journal of
ing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain’s
Learning Disabilities, 42(4), 372–382. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​
intrinsic functional dynamics. Social Cognitive and Affective
00222​19409​335214
Neuroscience, 7(6), 727–737. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 093/s​ can/n​ sr051
Alloway, T. P., & Temple, K. J. (2007). A comparison of working
Crucitti, J., Hyde, C., & Stokes, M. A. (2019). Hammering that nail:
memory profiles and learning in children with Developmental
Varied praxis motor skills in younger autistic children. Journal of
Coordination Disorder and moderate learning difficulties. Applied
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(9), 3253–3262. https://​
Cognitive Psychology, 21, 473–487.
doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​019-​04136-4
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statisti-
Cummins, A., Piek, J. P., & Dyck, M. J. (2005). Motor coordination,
cal manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric
empathy, and social behaviour in school-aged children. Develop-
Association. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1176/​appi.​books.​97808​90425​596
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47(7), 437–442. https://​
Ayres, A. J. (1989). Sensory integration and praxis tests. Western Psy-
doi.​org/​10.​1017/​S0012​16220​50008​5X
chological Services.
Dadgar, H., Rad, J. A., Soleymani, Z., Khorammi, A., McCleery, J.,
Bhat, A. N. (2020). Is motor impairment in autism spectrum disorder
& Maroufizadeh, S. (2017). The relationship between motor,
distinct from developmental coordination disorder? A report from
imitation, and early social communication skills in children with
the SPARK Study. Physical Therapy, 100(4), 633–644. https://d​ oi.​
autism. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry, 12(4), 236–240.
org/​10.​1093/​ptj/​pzz190
Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M.,
Bhat, A. N., Boulton, A. J., & Tulsky, D. S. (2022). A further study
Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emo-
of relations between motor impairment and social communica-
tions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism
tion, cognitive, language, functional impairments, and repetitive
spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30. https://d​ oi.​
behavior severity in children with ASD using the SPARK study
org/​10.​1038/​nn1611
dataset. Autism Research.
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy:
Blank, R., Barnett, A. L., Cairney, J., Green, D., Kirby, A., Polatajko,
Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personal-
H., Rosenblum, S., Smits-Engelsman, B., Sugden, D., Wilson,
ity and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​
P., & Vinçon, S. (2019). International clinical practice recom-
0022-​3514.​44.1.​113
mendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention,
Dewey, D., Cantell, M., & Crawford, S. G. (2007). Motor and gestural
and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder.
performance in children with autism spectrum disorders, develop-
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 61(3), 242–285.
mental coordination disorder, and/or attention deficit hyperactivity
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​dmcn.​14132
disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,
Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F., & Lewis, M. (1999). The repetitive behavior
13(2), 246–256. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1017/​S1355​61770​70702​70
scale: Test manual. Western Carolina Center Research Reports.
Dewey, D., Kaplan, B. J., Crawford, S. G., & Wilson, B. N. (2002).
Bottema-Beutel, K., Kim, S. Y., & Crowley, S. (2019). A systematic
Developmental coordination disorder: Associated problems in
review and meta-regression analysis of social functioning corre-
attention, learning, and psychosocial adjustment. Human Move-
lates in autism and typical development. Autism Research, 12(2),
ment Science, 21(5–6), 905–918. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​s0167-​
152–175. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​aur.​2055
9457(02)​00163-x
Caçola, P., Miller, H. L., & Ossom Williamson, P. (2017). Behavio-
Dowell, L. R., Mahone, E. M., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). Associations
ral comparisons in autism spectrum disorder and developmental
of postural knowledge and basic motor skill with dyspraxia in
coordination disorder: A systematic literature review. Research in
autism: Implication for abnormalities in distributed connectivity
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 38, 6–18. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​
and motor learning. Neuropsychology, 23(5), 563–570. https://​
rasd.​2017.​03.​004
doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0015​640

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Dziuk, M. A., Larson, J. C., Apostu, A., Mahone, E. M., Denckla, M. disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1078–
B., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2007). Dyspraxia in autism: Association 1085. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​rasd.​2010.​12.​001
with motor, social, and communicative deficits. Developmental Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2006). Teaching reciprocal imitation
Medicine and Child Neurology, 49(10), 734–739. https://​doi.​org/​ skills to young children with autism using a naturalistic behavioral
10.​1111/j.​1469-​8749.​2007.​00734.x approach: Effects on language, pretend play, and joint attention.
Edwards, L. A. (2014). A meta-analysis of imitation abilities in indi- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(4), 487–505.
viduals with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 7(3), https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​006-​0089-y
363–380. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​aur.​1379 Jansiewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., Newschaffer, C. J., Denckla, M.
Fabbri-Destro, M., Gizzonio, V., Bazzini, M. C., Cevallos, C., Cheron, B., Landa, R., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2006). Motor signs distinguish
G., & Avanzini, P. (2019). The relationship between pantomime children with high functioning autism and Asperger’s syndrome
execution and recognition across typically developing and autis- from controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
tic children. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 61, 22–32. 36(5), 613–621. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​006-​0109-y
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​rasd.​2019.​01.​008 Karras, H., Morin, D., Gill, K., Izadi-Najafabadi, S., & Zwicker, J.
Fournier, K. A., Hass, C. J., Naik, S. K., Lodha, N., & Cauraugh, J. (2019). Health-related quality of life of children with developmen-
H. (2010). Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: A tal coordination disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
synthesis and meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Develop- 84, 85–95. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​ridd.​2018.​05.​012
mental Disorders, 40(10), 1227–1240. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​ Kaur, M., Srinivasan, S., & Bhat, A. N. (2018). Comparing motor
s10803-​010-​0981-3 performance, praxis, coordination, and interpersonal synchrony
Gibbs, J., Appleton, J., & Appleton, R. (2007). Dyspraxia or develop- between children with and without autism spectrum disorder
mental coordination disorder? Unravelling the enigma. Archives (ASD). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 72, 79–95.
of Disease in Childhood, 92(6), 534–539. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 136/​ https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​ridd.​2017.​10.​025
adc.​2005.​088054 Kilroy, E., Cermak, S. A., & Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2019). A review of func-
Gizzonio, V., Avanzini, P., Campi, C., Orivoli, S., Piccolo, B., Canta- tional and structural neurobiology of the action observation net-
lupo, G., Tassinari, C., Rizzolatti, G., & Fabbri-Destro, M. (2015). work in autism spectrum disorder and developmental coordination
Failure in pantomime action execution correlates with the sever- disorder. Brain Sciences, 9(4), 75. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​hbm.​
ity of social behavior deficits in children with autism: A praxis 25312
study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(10), Kilroy, E., Harrison, L., Butera, C., Jayashankar, A., Cermak, S.,
3085–3097. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​015-​2461-2 Kaplan, J., Williams, M., Haranin, E., Bookheimer, S., Dapretto,
Green, D., Baird, G., Barnett, A. L., Henderson, L., Huber, J., & Hen- M., & Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2021). Unique deficit in embodied simu-
derson, S. E. (2002). The severity and nature of motor impairment lation in autism: An fMRI study comparing autism and devel-
in Asperger’s syndrome: A comparison with specific developmen- opmental coordination disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 42(5),
tal disorder of motor function. Journal of Child Psychology and 1532–1546. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​hbm.​25312
Psychiatry, 43(5), 655–668. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​1469-​7610.​ Kilroy, E., Ring, P., Hossain, A., Nalbach, A., Butera, C., Harrison,
00054 L., Jayashankar, A., Vigen, C., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Cermak, S. A.
Green, D., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T. O. M., (2022). Motor performance, praxis, and social skills in autism
Simonoff, E., & Baird, G. (2009). Impairment in movement skills spectrum disorder and developmental coordination disorder.
of children with autistic spectrum disorders. Developmental Med- Autism Research, 15(9), 1649–1664. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​aur.​
icine and Child Neurology, 51(4), 311–316. https://​doi.​org/​10.​ 2774
1111/j.​1469-​8749.​2008.​03242.x Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY II: Clinical and
Hannant, P., Cassidy, S., Van de Weyer, R., & Mooncey, S. (2018). interpretive manual. Harcourt Assessment.
Sensory and motor differences in autism spectrum conditions Lam, K. S., & Aman, M. G. (2007). The repetitive behavior scale-
and developmental coordination disorder in children: A cross- revised: Independent validation in individuals with autism spec-
syndrome study. Human Movement Science, 58, 108–118. https://​ trum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​humov.​2018.​01.​010 37(5), 855–866. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​006-​0213-z
Harris, S. R., Mickelson, E. C. R., & Zwicker, J. G. (2015). Diagnosis Libby, S., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R. (1997). Imitation of
and management of developmental coordination disorder. Cana- pretend play acts by children with autism and down syndrome.
dian Medical Association Journal, 187(9), 659–665. https://​doi.​ Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(4), 365–383.
org/​10.​1503/​cmaj.​140994 https://​doi.​org/​10.​1023/a:​10258​01304​279
Heiser, M., Iacoboni, M., Maeda, F., Marcus, J., & Mazziotta, J. C. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism diagnostic obser-
(2003). The essential role of Broca’s area in imitation. European vation schedule–2nd edition (ADOS-2). Western Psychological
Journal of Neuroscience, 17(5), 1123–1128. https://​doi.​org/​10.​ Services.
1046/j.​1460-​9568.​2003.​02530.x Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic
Henderson, S. E., Sugden, D. A., & Barnett, A. L. (2007). Movement interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for
assessment battery for children (2nd ed.). The Psychological caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental
Corporation. disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24,
Hilton, C., Zhang, Y., Whilte, M., Klohr, C., & Constantino, J. (2012). 659–685. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​BF021​72145
Motor impairment in sibling pairs concordant and discordant for McAuliffe, D., Pillai, A. S., Tiedemann, A., Mostofsky, S. H., & Ewen,
autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 16(4), 430–441. https://​doi.​ J. B. (2017). Dyspraxia in ASD: Impaired coordination of move-
org/​10.​1177/​13623​61311​423018 ment elements. Autism Research, 10(4), 648–652. https://​doi.​org/​
Hooker, J. L., Dow, D., Morgan, L., Schatschneider, C., & Wetherby, 10.​1002/​aur.​1693
A. M. (2019). Psychometric analysis of the repetitive behavior McDuffie, A., Turner, L., Stone, W., Yoder, P., Wolery, M., & Ulman,
scale-revised using confirmatory factor analysis in children with T. (2007). Developmental correlates of different types of motor
autism. Autism Research, 12(9), 1399–1410. https://​doi.​org/​10.​ imitation in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Jour-
1002/​aur.​2159 nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(3), 401–412.
Ingersoll, B., & Meyer, K. (2011). Examination of correlates of differ- https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​006-​0175-1
ent imitative functions in young children with autism spectrum

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Miller, H. L., Sherrod, G. M., Mauk, J. E., Fears, N. E., Hynan, L. Rothi, L. J. G., & Heilman, K. M. (2014). Apraxia: The neuropsychol-
S., & Tamplain, P. M. (2021). Shared features or co-occurrence? ogy of action. Psychology Press.
Evaluating symptoms of developmental coordination disorder in Sakreida, K., Higuchi, S., Di Dio, C., Ziessler, M., Turgeon, M., Rob-
children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal erts, N., & Vogt, S. (2018). Cognitive control structures in the
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51(10), 3443–3455. imitation learning of spatial sequences and rhythms—An fMRI
Ming, X., Brimacombe, M., & Wagner, G. C. (2007). Prevalence of study. Cerebral Cortex, 28(3), 907–923. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1093/​
motor impairment in autism spectrum disorders. Brain and Devel- cercor/​bhw414
opment, 29(9), 565–570. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​brain​dev.​2007.​ Scahill, L., Dimitropoulos, A., McDougle, C. J., Aman, M. G., Feurer,
03.​002 I. D., McCracken, J. T., Tierney, E., Pu, J., White, S., Lecavalier,
Mirenda, P., Smith, I. M., Vaillancourt, T., Georgiades, S., Duku, E., L., Hallett, V., Bearss, K., King, B., Arnold, L. E., & Vitiello,
Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Fombonne, E., Roberts, W., Volden, J., B. (2014). Children’s Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale in
Waddel, C., & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2010). Validating the repetitive autism spectrum disorder: Component structure and correlates of
behavior scale-revised in young children with autism spectrum symptom checklist. Journal of the American Academy of Child
disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(12), and Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(1), 97–107. https://​doi.​org/​10.​
1521–1530. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​010-​1012-0 1016/j.​jaac.​2013.​09.​018
Mody, M., Shui, A. M., Nowinski, L. A., Golas, S. B., Ferrone, C., Schertz, H. H., Odom, S. L., Baggett, K. M., & Sideris, J. H. (2016).
O’Rourke, J. A., & McDougle, C. J. (2016). Communication defi- Parent-reported repetitive behavior in toddlers on the autism spec-
cits and the motor system: Exploring patterns of associations in trum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(10),
autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Journal of Autism and Devel- 3308–3316. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​016-​2870-x
opmental Disorders, 47(1), 155–162. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​ Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. D. (1997). Motor imita-
s10803-​016-​2934-y tion in young children with autism: What’s the object? Journal of
Mostofsky, S., Dubey, P., Jerath, V., Jansiewicz, E., Goldberg, M., & Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(6), 475–485. https://​doi.​org/​10.​
Denckla, M. (2006). Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to 1023/a:​10226​85731​726
imitation in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Sumner, E., Leonard, H. C., & Hill, E. L. (2016). Overlapping pheno-
the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(3), 314–326. types in autism spectrum disorder and developmental coordina-
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1017/​S1355​61770​60604​37 tion disorder: A cross-syndrome comparison of motor and social
National Institute of Mental Health » Autism Spectrum Disorder. skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(8),
(2022). Autism spectrum disorder. Retrieved March 27, 2021 2609–2620. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​016-​2794-5
from NIH.gov website: https://​www.​nimh.​nih.​gov/​health/​topics/​ Sumner, E., Leonard, H. C., & Hill, E. L. (2018). Comparing atten-
autism-​spect​r um-​disor​ders-​asd/​index.​shtml tion to socially-relevant stimuli in autism spectrum disorder
Ohgami, Y., Matsuo, K., Uchida, N., & Nakai, T. (2004). An fMRI and developmental coordination disorder. Journal of Abnormal
study of tool-use gestures: Body part as object and pantomime. Child Psychology, 46(8), 1717–1729. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​
NeuroReport, 15(12), 1903–1906. https://​d oi.​o rg/​1 0.​1 097/​ s10802-​017-​0393-3
00001​756-​20040​8260-​00014 Tal Saban, M., & Kirby, A. (2019). Empathy, social relationship and
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: co-occurrence in young adults with DCD. Human Movement Sci-
The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113. ence, 63, 62–72. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​humov.​2018.​11.​005
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​0028-​3932(71)​90067-4 Vatandoust, L., & Hasanzadeh, R. (2018). The study of the emotion
Paquet, A., Olliac, B., Golse, B., & Vaivre-Douret, L. (2019). Nature recognition and the cognitive failures of children with develop-
of motor impairments in autism spectrum disorder: A compari- mental coordination disorder. Iranian Rehabilitation Journal.
son with developmental coordination disorder. Journal of Clini- https://​doi.​org/​10.​32598/​irj.​16.2.​121
cal and Experimental Neuropsychology, 41(1), 1–14. https://​doi.​ Vivanti, G., & Hamilton, A. (2014). Imitation in autism spectrum dis-
org/​10.​1080/​13803​395.​2018.​14834​86 orders. In F. R. Volkmar, S. J. Rogers, R. Paul, & K. A. Pelphrey
Perra, O., Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Fraser, L., Benzie, H., & Per- (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disor-
rett, D. I. (2008). Imitation and ‘theory of mind’competencies ders, (4th ed., Vol. 1), Ser Diagnosis, development, and brain
in discrimination of autism from other neurodevelopmental dis- mechanisms (pp. 278–301). Wiley.
orders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(3), 456–468. Vivanti, G., Dissanayake, C., Zierhut, C., & Rogers, S. J. (2013). Brief
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​rasd.​2007.​09.​007 report: Predictors of outcomes in the early start Denver model
Pittet, I., Kojovic, N., Franchini, M., & Schaer, M. (2022). Trajec- delivered in a group setting. Journal of Autism and Develop-
tories of imitation skills in preschoolers with autism spectrum mental Disorders, 43(7), 1717–1724. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​
disorders. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 14(1), s10803-​012-​1705-7
1–13. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s11689-​021-​09412-y Wang, A. T., Dapretto, M., Hariri, A. R., Sigman, M., & Bookheimer,
Power, E., Code, C., Croot, K., Sheard, C., & Gonzalez Rothi, L. J. S. Y. (2004). Neural correlates of facial affect processing in chil-
(2010). Florida Apraxia Battery–extended and revised sydney dren and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
(FABERS): Design, description, and a healthy control sample. the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(4),
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(1), 481–490. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1097/​00004​583-​20040​4000-​00015
1–18. Wechsler, D. (2011). WASI-II: Wechsler Abbreviated Ccale of Intel-
Ravizza, S. M., Solomon, M., Ivry, R. B., & Carter, C. S. (2013). ligence. NCS Pearson.
Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: Williams, J., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of
The relationship of attention and motor deficits. Development and action imitation in Autistic Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism
Psychopathology, 25(3), 773–784. and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 285–299. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.​
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). 1023/B:​JADD.​00000​29551.​56735.​3a
Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with Wisdom, S. N., Dyck, M. J., Piek, J. P., Hay, D., & Hallmayer, J. (2007).
other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Can autism, language and coordination disorders be differentiated
Psychiatry, 44(5), 763–781. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​1469-​7610.​ based on ability profiles? European Child & Adolescent Psychia-
00162 try, 16(3), 178–186. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00787-​006-​0586-8

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Young, G. S., Rogers, S. J., Hutman, T., Rozga, A., Sigman, M., & Publisher's Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
Ozonoff, S. (2011). Imitation from 12 to 24 months in autism and jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
typical development: A longitudinal Rasch analysis. Developmen-
tal Psychology, 47(6), 1565. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0025​418 Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds
Zachor, D. A., Ilanit, T., & Itzchak, E. B. (2010). Autism severity and exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the
motor abilities correlates of imitation situations in children with author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted
autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disor- manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of
ders, 4(3), 438–443. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​rasd.​2009.​10.​016 such publishing agreement and applicable law.

13

You might also like