You are on page 1of 26

Journal of Cognitive Psychology

ISSN: 2044-5911 (Print) 2044-592X (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pecp21

A meta-analysis of executive functioning in


dyslexia with consideration of the impact of
comorbid ADHD

Aoife Lonergan, Caoilainn Doyle, Clare Cassidy, Shane MacSweeney Mahon,


Richard A.P. Roche, Lorraine Boran & Jessica Bramham

To cite this article: Aoife Lonergan, Caoilainn Doyle, Clare Cassidy, Shane MacSweeney Mahon,
Richard A.P. Roche, Lorraine Boran & Jessica Bramham (2019) A meta-analysis of executive
functioning in dyslexia with consideration of the impact of comorbid ADHD, Journal of Cognitive
Psychology, 31:7, 725-749, DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2019.1669609

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2019.1669609

Published online: 03 Oct 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pecp21
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY
2019, VOL. 31, NO. 7, 725–749
https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2019.1669609

REVIEW

A meta-analysis of executive functioning in dyslexia with consideration of


the impact of comorbid ADHD
Aoife Lonergana, Caoilainn Doyleb, Clare Cassidyc, Shane MacSweeney Mahona, Richard A.P. Roched,
Lorraine Boranb and Jessica Bramhama
a
Department of Psychology, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland; bSchool of Nursing and Human Sciences, Dublin City
University, Dublin, Ireland; cDepartment of Psychology, National University of Ireland, Galway, Ireland; dDepartment of Psychology,
Maynooth University, Kildare, Ireland

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


This meta-analysis examined inhibition, switching attention and auditory working Received 28 October 2018
memory in children with dyslexia. As a secondary outcome the impact of comorbid Accepted 7 September 2019
ADHD on executive functions in dyslexia was examined. Twenty-six controlled studies
KEYWORDS
examining executive functions in children with dyslexia alone and/or comorbid Executive function; dyslexia;
dyslexia/ADHD were reviewed. Outcomes were reaction times, errors and accuracy on ADHD; comorbid
measures of inhibition, switching attention and auditory working memory. Children
with dyslexia demonstrated difficulty with inhibition, switching attention, and auditory
working memory, with a medium to large effect relative to controls. Children with
comorbid dyslexia/ADHD exhibited relatively the same degree of inhibition, switching
attention and auditory working memory impairment compared to children with
dyslexia alone. Findings support the presence of executive function deficits in children
with dyslexia. Executive functioning may be a shared deficit, underpinning variants of
neurodevelopmental disorders. Future studies may benefit from examining executive
functions in children with dyslexia while controlling for attention difficulties.

Introduction
and clinical assessment (American Psychiatric
Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental disorder charac- Association, 2013).
terised by slow and inaccurate word recognition While theoretical reports of dyslexia highlight the
(Peterson & Pennington, 2012). It is the most primary deficit to be in the phonological system
common learning disability, accounting for approxi- (Hulme & Snowling, 2009; Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowl-
mately 80% of all learning difficulties, and affecting ing, & Scanlon, 2004), deficits in the executive
between 8% and 12% of the population (Eden & system have also been reported (Pickering & Gather-
Vaidya, 2008). Under the DSM 5, dyslexia falls cole, 2004; Swanson, 2006). It has been suggested
under the neurodevelopmental disorder category that these problems are over and above deficits in
of specific learning disorder, with impairment in the phonological system (Swanson, 2006). Studies
reading (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). examining difficulties with reading comprehension
For a diagnosis of specific learning disorder, with suggest working memory skills (Hulme & Snowling,
impairment in reading, hereafter referred to as dys- 2009) and the ability to update and inhibit distrac-
lexia multiple criteria must be met. Difficulties must tors are impaired (Palladino, Cornoldi, De Beni, &
be present for 6 months, despite the provision of Pazzaglia, 2001). Difficulties in the ability to update
interventions that target those difficulties (American working memory and inhibit distractors has been
Psychiatric Association, 2013). The affected reading implicated in research on reading difficulties (Palla-
skill must be below that expected for the individuals dino et al., 2001). These higher order cognitive pro-
chronological age and cause significant interference cesses fall under the umbrella of executive function’s
with academic/occupational functioning, as (EF) and may have implications for responsiveness
confirmed by standardised achievement measures to reading intervention (Sesma, Mahone, Levine,

CONTACT Jessica Bramham jessica.bramham@ucd.ie


© 2019 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
726 A. LONERGAN ET AL.

Eason, & Cutting, 2009; Swanson, 2006). EFs are pro- Lange, 2005). Booth, Boyle, and Kelly (2010) con-
cesses that control and regulate thought and action ducted a meta-analysis of 48 studies comparing
(Friedman, Miyake, Corley, Young, DeFries, & Hewitt, the performance of children with reading disability
2006) and engagement in goal directed behaviour with their typically developing peers on tasks of
(Lyon & Krasnegor, 1996). A growing body of evi- EF. An overall effect size of 0.57 was obtained, indi-
dence suggests that executive functioning is cating that children with reading disability had
related to reasoning ability (Engle, Tuholski, Laugh- impairments on tasks of EF (Booth et al., 2010).
lin, & Conway, 1999; Gathercole & Pickering, 2000; However, moderator analysis found that the task
Kyllonen & Christal, 1990) and is involved in aca- modality and IQ achievement discrepancy
demic abilities such as reading (Daneman & definitions of reading disability influenced the mag-
Merikle, 1996; De Jong, 1998; Swanson & Sachse- nitude of effect (Booth et al., 2010). Booth et al.
Lee, 2001). However, currently the exact EF profile (2010) found that the EF impairment of children
of dyslexia is unclear. with reading disability is not uniform across all
Epidemiological and aetiological studies of dys- tasks, indicating that some areas of EF may be
lexia have consistently found that Attention Deficit more impaired than others. In attempting to
Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is the most explain the heterogeneity of dyslexia, Pennington
common co-occurring disorder (Carroll, Maughan, (2006) proposed a multiple deficit model that
Goodman, & Meltzer, 2005; Maughan & Carroll, regards dyslexia as the result of multiple risk and
2006). It is estimated that 15–40% of children with protective factors, both environmental and genetic.
dyslexia are also diagnosed with ADHD (Semrud-Cli- According to this framework, each symptom’s
keman et al., 1992; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000). dimension can be predicted by several underlying
When left untreated, dyslexia and ADHD pose sig- cognitive factors (Peterson et al., 2017). Comorbidity
nificant barriers for school success and later adap- among symptom dimensions or disorders arises
tation in a variety of settings (Cornwall & Bawden, because some of the underlying cognitive skills are
1992; Mannuzza, Klein, Bessler, Malloy, & Hynes, shared by disorders (Peterson et al., 2017). Until
1997). These problems are greatly compounded in further research allows the exact nature of the EF
individuals with co-morbidity. A significant volume impairment to be isolated, it is not possible to say
of research indicates that the presence of executive whether the impairment is specific to areas such as
dysfunction is well established in ADHD (Barkley, inhibition and working memory, or involves more
1997; Eden & Vaidya, 2008; Nigg, 2001, 2006; Pen- areas, but just perhaps to differing degrees (Booth
nington, 2002; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Willcutt, et al., 2010).
Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005). However, While EF deficits have been found in children
far less is known about the impact of comorbid with ADHD (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Sergeant,
ADHD on EF in dyslexia. EF as a possible area of Geurts, Huijbregts, Scheres, & Oosterlaan, 2003), it
shared or additive difficulty in both dyslexia and is unclear, whether the comorbidity of ADHD with
ADHD has received increasing attention in recent dyslexia has an effect on EFs, as EF deficits may
research. The gaps and current limitations in the lit- also occur in children with dyslexia only (Donfran-
erature regarding the EF profile of children with dys- cesco, Mugnaini, & Dell’Uomo, 2005). The cognitive
lexia makes it an important area for further subtype hypothesis suggests that children with
investigation to inform EF theories of dyslexia. both ADHD and dyslexia have a different or more
severe form of dyslexia or ADHD than children
with either disorder alone (Rucklidge & Tannock,
Executive functioning in dyslexia and ADHD
2002). Willcutt et al. (2001) found evidence for this
Factor analytic studies have validated at least three hypothesis, reporting a double dissociation
EFs; inhibition, set shifting and updating working between ADHD and reading disability, as children
memory as independent but correlated EFs with ADHD had difficulties in EF (e.g. inhibition
(Miyake et al., 2000; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996). and set shifting), while children with dyslexia exhib-
While some studies have found children with dys- ited phonological and working memory deficits.
lexia to be impaired on measures of EF (Beneventi, Children with comorbid ADHD/reading disability
Tønnessen, Ersland, & Hugdahl, 2010; Brosnan were more impaired on inhibition and phonological
et al., 2002), others have reported little or no EF measures than ADHD, reading disability or control
impairment (Bental & Tirosh, 2007; Reiter, Tucha, & groups (Willcutt et al., 2001). This suggest that
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 727

children in the comorbid group had a more severe 2012; Lijffijt, Kenemans, Verbaten, & van Engeland,
subtype of ADHD and reading disability than the 2005; Martinussen, Hayden, Hogg-Johnson, &
children with reading disability alone (De Jong, Oos- Tannock, 2005; Willcutt et al., 2005). However, exam-
terlaan, & Sergeant, 2006; Willcutt et al., 2001). This ination of possible shared EF deficits in ADHD and
may have implications for EF as a target of assess- dyslexia has been rare and has produced conflicting
ment and intervention for underlying deficits findings. In exploring the overlap between reading
across both disorders. disability and ADHD, Shanahan, Pennington, and
Yerys (2006) showed that processing speed was
likely a shared cognitive deficit that could account
Inhibition
for the comorbidity between these disorders.
For conscious, reflective problem solving to occur, Extending this work, McGrath et al. (2011) tested a
inhibition of automatic responses is required to multiple cognitive deficit model of reading disability
allow strategic processing in favour of a long-term and ADHD. They found that processing speed was
goal (Altemeier, Abbott, & Berninger, 2008). Based the only cognitive predictor shared by both
on their findings, Miyake et al. (2000) and other symptom dimensions. These results suggest that
researchers, argue that inhibition might be the key comorbidity between dyslexia and ADHD may be
factor, as all EFs involve inhibitory processes primarily attributable to shared genetic influences
(Barkley, 1997; van der Sluis, de Jong, & van der that lead to slow processing speed. A possible
Leij, 2004). For example, updating working reason for the inconsistencies between studies of
memory requires the discarding and suppression EF in dyslexia may be differences in measurement
of irrelevant incoming information (van der Sluis tools used (Wang, Tasi, & Yang, 2012). As most
et al., 2004). Similarly, shifting attention requires studies in dyslexia have used only a single task to
the suppression of an irrelevant mental set in assess EFs of interest, results are nearly always a
favour of a new one (van der Sluis et al., 2004). mixture of non-EF, common EF (inhibition) and
This indicates that while EFs are all distinct con- specific EF components, making interpretation of
structs, they are not entirely unrelated (Booth results difficult (Snyder, Miyake, & Hankin, 2015).
et al., 2010). Inhibition is thought to affect memory
at both the encoding and retrieval stages (De Ribau-
Switching attention
pierre, 2001). Efficient retrieval of the phonological
codes for letters and letter units, for example, may Set shifting, or switching attention, is the ability to
be affected by the ability to suppress irrelevant flexibly shift attention as task demands change
codes and quickly search and retrieve the relevant (Altemeier et al., 2008). Cognitive control, such as
response (Altemeier et al., 2008). In the decoding the ability to react in a flexible way to information
process inhibition and set shifting occur moment in our environment, has been explored with a
to moment in linking letters and sounds (Altemeier variety of experimental paradigms. Using a task
et al., 2008). Inhibitory processes mediate selective switching paradigm, Poljac et al. (2010) found
attention and the inability to inhibit distractors that children with dyslexia demonstrated a signifi-
may underlie the distractibility frequently reported cantly larger switch cost, suggesting difficulties
by individuals with dyslexia (West, 1997). Earlier specifically related to switching between different
studies demonstrated that children with reading dis- tasks (Poljac et al., 2010). However, Stoet and
orders have deficits in inhibitory processing (Närhi & Snyder (2007) found no evidence for specific
Ahonen, 1995; Nigg, 1999; Purvis & Tannock, 2000; impairments in task switching in undergraduate
van der Schoot, Licht, Horsley, & Sergeant, 2000; Will- students with dyslexia. A possible explanation
cutt, Pennington, Olson, Chhabildas, & Hulslander, for the different finding could be a difference
2005). However, several studies have produced between the ages of participants and the stimuli
inconsistent results (Altemeier et al., 2008; used in these studies. While Poljac et al. (2010)
Landerl, Moll, Fussenegger, & Willburger, 2009; van used only incongruent stimuli, half of the stimuli
der Sluis et al., 2004). used by Stoet and Snyder (2007) were congruent.
Meta-analyses indicate that deficits in working Congruency effects have been found to modulate
memory (weighted effect sizes 0.47–0.85) and inhi- the size of the switch cost so that incongruent situ-
bition (weighted effect sizes 0.58–0.61) are strongly ations produce a relatively larger switch cost
associated with ADHD (Kasper, Alderson, & Hudec, (Meiran, 2000).
728 A. LONERGAN ET AL.

Attention shifting and interference control show et al., 2002). This suggests sensitivity to distraction
less consistent differences between ADHD and may be related to the diminished working memory
control children across studies (Willcutt et al., capacity in dyslexia. A smaller working memory
2003). Effect sizes of EF measures are small (d = .4 capacity will reach capacity earlier and place
− .6) in light of the wide overlap in the group distri- demands on inhibitory resources earlier (Brosnan
butions (Eden & Vaidya, 2008). Furthermore, almost et al., 2002; Spironelli, Penolazzi, & Angrilli, 2008).
all other developmental disorders also exhibit The process of learning to read is highly inte-
some level of problems with EF, suggesting that grated with working memory (Gathercole &
executive dysfunction is a pervasive, shared Alloway, 2008). The multicomponent subsystem
outcome of multiple pathological developmental model of working memory initially presented by
processes (Eden & Vaidya, 2008). In examining set Baddeley and Hitch (1974) is well supported by
shifting some studies have identified no deficit in research (Alloway, Gathercole, Willis, & Adams,
children with reading disability alone (van der Sluis 2004). According to the revised model, working
et al., 2004; Willcutt et al., 2013). However, van der memory consists of four integral components.
Sluis et al. (2004) found that children with both arith- There are two slave storage systems; the phonologi-
metic disability and reading disability were impaired cal loop which temporarily stores phonological
on both set shifting and inhibition tasks. It was information, and the visuospatial sketchpad which
shown that reading plus arithmetic disabled chil- temporarily maintains visual or spatial information
dren experienced the additive combination of pro- (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974). The episodic buffer, inte-
blems that characterise children with a single grates and stores information from the phonological
learning deficit (van der Sluis et al., 2004). Further loop and visuospatial sketchpad with long term
research is needed to identify if this is also the memory (Baddeley, 2000). These three storage
case with comorbid dyslexia and ADHD. systems work under the influence of the central
executive, which is responsible for retrieving and
manipulating information, and directing resources
Working memory
to the storage components (Baddeley & Hitch,
Working memory is the cognitive system that allows 1974). These components function together to
the storage and manipulation of a limited amount of create working memory, allowing for the completion
information over a short period. Its functioning is of many complex cognitive tasks such as reading
essential for a wide range of complex cognitive (Dawes, Leitão, Claessen, & Nayton, 2015).
tasks, such as reading, general reasoning, and Working memory has been found to be less
problem solving (Shah & Miyake, 1996). In reading, effective in children with dyslexia than in typically
working memory is involved in both single word reading children (Barnea, Lamm, Epstein, & Pratt,
decoding (Christopher et al., 2012; Swanson, Howard, 1994; Breznitz, 1997; Chiappe, Siegel, & Hasher, 2000;
& Saez, 2006; Swanson, Zheng, & Jerman, 2009) and Poblano, Valadéz-Tepec, de Lourdes Arias, & García-
comprehension of longer texts (Christopher et al., Pedroza, 2000; Vargo, Grosser, & Spafford, 1995; Will-
2012; Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Sesma et al., 2009; cutt et al., 2001). In a meta-analysis of 28 studies exam-
Swanson et al., 2006). Individual differences in ining working memory in children with reading
working memory are significantly impacted by indi- disabilities, Jerman and Lee (2005) found a large
vidual differences in ability to control attention mean effect size 0.89 (SE = 0.8) indicating that children
(Arrington, Kulesz, Francis, Fletcher, & Barnes, with reading disability were impaired on tasks asses-
2014). These attentional processes are crucial for sing working memory. Furthermore, Martinussen
cognitive processes supporting language and and Tannock (2006) found impaired performance
reading comprehension (Gathercole & Baddeley, across both verbal and visuospatial working memory
2014). Increasing demands on working memory domains for children with reading disabilities.
resources results in increased difficulty in controlling However, some studies have found that the perform-
inhibition (Pennington, Bennetto, McAleer, & ance of children with reading disability is comparable
Roberts, 1996). Working memory is perhaps one of to that of typically developing controls (e.g. McGee,
the most widely studied EFs in dyslexia. A Brodeur, Symons, Andrade, & Fahie, 2004).
common paradigm for assessing working memory In examining working memory, Rucklidge and
capacity is the digit span task. In dyslexia, digit Tannock (2002) found evidence in support of the
spans decrease with verbal distractors (Brosnan cognitive subtype hypothesis. A double dissociation
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 729

was reported in that naming and working memory 1998), the identification of areas of difficulty
deficits were characteristic of reading disability, specific to dyslexia is complex. Children with dys-
and inhibition deficits were characteristic of ADHD. lexia exhibit impairments in many of the same
In contrast, the comorbid ADHD and reading disabil- domains as children with ADHD, including proces-
ity group exhibited both deficits in inhibition and sing speed (De Jong et al., 2009; Ghelani, Sidhu,
working memory. The comorbid group exhibited Jain, & Tannock, 2004; Rucklidge & Tannock, 2002;
more severe naming problems than the two single Shanahan et al., 2006; Smith, Taylor, Rogers,
disorder groups (Willcutt et al., 2001). This suggests Newman, & Rubia, 2002; Tannock, Martinussen, &
that the comorbid group had a more severe automa- Frijters, 2000; Willcutt, Pennington, et al., 2005);
tisation problem than the children with ADHD or attention, auditory working memory (De Jong
reading disability only. et al., 2009; Rucklidge & Tannock, 2002; Tridas,
More recently, Peterson et al. (2017) tested a mul- 2007; Willcutt et al., 2003), response inhibition (De
tiple cognitive predictor model of word reading, Jong et al., 2009; Purvis & Tannock, 2000; Willcutt
math ability and attention in twins, aged 8–16 with et al., 2003) and visuospatial working memory (De
a history of attention, reading or math difficulties. Jong et al., 2009; Martinussen & Tannock, 2006;
Each symptom was predicted by multiple cognitive Purvis & Tannock, 2000).
dimensions, some of which were shared across symp- Numerous theories for the co-occurrence of dys-
toms, e.g. processing speed predicted reading, math lexia and ADHD have been suggested. Models that
and attention; verbal comprehension predicted propose a single cognitive dysfunction that is
reading and math (Peterson et al., 2017). There was unique to each disorder cannot easily account for
no evidence that EF skills accounted for covariation the pervasive comorbidity between different dis-
among these skill domains. Instead, specific EFs orders (Willcutt et al., 2010). Studies of dimensional
differentially related to certain outcomes, for measures of reading and ADHD symptoms report sig-
example inhibition to attention. Peterson et al nificant correlations that are low to moderate in mag-
(2017) interpreted their findings within the context nitude (r = .2 − .5) (Bauermeister et al., 2005; Nigg,
of a multiple deficit framework for neurodevelop- Hinshaw, Carte, & Treuting, 1998; Willcutt et al.,
mental disorders. Peterson et al.’s (2017) study is 2001; Willcutt, Doyle, et al., 2005). Findings such as
limited by a number of factors. Firstly, as participants this, have precipitated a reconceptualization of theor-
were not formally diagnosed with dyslexia or ADHD it etical models of dyslexia and ADHD (Willcutt et al.,
may be that they present with a less severe symptom 2010). The multiple deficit model describes complex
profile than those meeting these criteria. Secondly, disorders such as dyslexia and ADHD as hetero-
the age range of participants is quite broad. This is geneous conditions that arise from the additive and
important due to the potential impact of brain devel- interactive effects of multiple genetic and non-
opment during the age of 8–16 years on EF. genetic risk factors that lead to weaknesses in multiple
Many studies examining dyslexia and comorbid cognitive domains (Pennington, 2006; Sonuga-Barke,
dyslexia/ADHD mix the domains of working Sergeant, Nigg, & Willcutt, 2008; Willcutt, Sonuga-
memory they are assessing which may contribute Barke, Nigg, & Sergeant, 2008). The high comorbidity
to mixed results. If working memory is divided into rates across these disorders suggest a similar cogni-
verbal or auditory and visuospatial components, tive profile involving executive dysfunction.
auditory working memory deficits are more often Systematic studies of EFs in dyslexia, ADHD and
associated with dyslexia than with ADHD (Martinus- co-morbid dyslexia/ADHD groups indicate that,
sen & Tannock, 2006; Rucklidge & Tannock, 2002; while each disorder exhibits their own characteristic
Swanson, Mink, & Bocian, 1999; Willcutt et al., deficits (e.g. poor response inhibition in ADHD, pho-
2001), whereas visuospatial working memory nological coding deficits in dyslexia, and both in the
deficits are more pronounced in ADHD than dyslexia co-morbid groups), all groups also exhibit abnormal-
(Marzocchi et al., 2008). ities with processing speed (Rucklidge & Tannock,
2002) and auditory working memory (Willcutt
et al., 2001). Furthermore, Willcutt et al. (2001) exam-
Understanding the co-morbidity of dyslexia
ined the performance of twin children with reading
and ADHD
disability, ADHD or reading disability and ADHD.
As dyslexia is known to co-occur with many other Findings indicated that the reading disability and
developmental disorders, such as ADHD (Tannock, comorbid ADHD group was most impaired on
730 A. LONERGAN ET AL.

virtually all measures, including inhibition, set shift- working memory deficits in children with dyslexia
ing and auditory working memory (AVM) (Willcutt and comorbid dyslexia and ADHD. The primary
et al., 2001). It is difficult to conclude from the outcome measure is response time as well as error
current research whether EF problems in comorbid and accuracy rates in children with dyslexia. The sec-
groups are (a) due to the presence of ADHD, (b) a ondary outcome measures are these variables in
combination of both deficits found in dyslexia and children with co-morbid Dyslexia/ADHD. These
ADHD, or (c) a more severe profile in the co- outcome measures have been chosen due to their
morbid group than either disorder alone. Clarity consistent use in controlled studies of dyslexia and
regarding these issues has theoretical and practical ADHD. This meta-analysis will add to the literature
implications for understanding the role of EF by minimising the impact of the task impurity
across these highly comorbid conditions and may problem by using consistent outcome variables
inform possible EF intervention across disorders. and comparing different EFs.
Dyslexia is known to consist of difficulties with the
phonological components of language (Hulme &
Rationale and novelty of current study
Snowling, 2009; Vellutino et al., 2004). In light of this,
Many studies have argued against the existence of traditional interventions for dyslexia have focused on
EF deficits in particular populations based on null training phonological skills. However, increasing evi-
results that may have arisen from power limitations dence demonstrates that impairments associated
due to poor task sensitivity and reliability or small with dyslexia extend beyond reading and phonologi-
sample size, rather than reflecting a lack of impair- cal skills alone (Swanson, 2006). Growing research
ment (Snyder et al., 2015). Currently, there is substan- points to the wide reaching detrimental impact of dys-
tial discrepancy in the diagnostic terminology used lexia on the academic functioning, attention, socio-
in research examining impairments in reading, emotional well-being and adjustment of affected chil-
including dyslexia, reading disability/difficulty and dren. These difficulties have the potential for a lifetime
learning difficulty (Vellutino et al., 2004). To over- negative impact on children’s mental health and occu-
come these limitations, the proposed meta-analysis pational and social functioning as they move into
will primarily aim to review controlled studies exam- adulthood. These difficulties have their counterparts
ining inhibition, set shifting and auditory working in the cognitive domain and cumulative research
memory deficits in children with dyslexia. This aim points to the role of executive functions (EFs). This
will help inform EF theory as it relates to the research is being conducted in order to make a
symptom profile of children with dyslexia. This has novel contribution to EF theory by exploring the role
implications for informing possible interventions tar- of EFs in difficulties associated with dyslexia and the
geting EF in children with dyslexia. Currently, no potential benefits of targeting EF for intervention.
known meta-analysis has systematically reviewed Research exploring these skills has neglected examin-
these three EFs in children with dyslexia. ation of the possible impact of highly comorbid
It is well known that all these EFs are problematic difficulties, such as ADHD on the symptoms profile
in ADHD through previous meta-analyses (e.g. of dyslexia. While ADHD is well known to be associated
Kasper et al., 2012; Lijffijt, Kenemans, Verbaten, & with EF deficits, it is unclear if EF difficulties in children
van Engeland, 2005; Martinussen et al., 2005; Will- with the comorbid conditions purely reflect the
cutt, Doyle, et al., 2005). However, considerably less influence of ADHD or are also present in children
is known about the impact of comorbid ADHD on with dyslexia. Increasing evidence indicates that chil-
EFs in children with dyslexia. The secondary aim of dren who face the additive problems of both disorders
this review will be to compare the EF profile of chil- are at a greater risk for academic failure, psychosocial
dren with dyslexia alone, and comorbid Dyslexia/ consequences and poor long-term outcomes that
ADHD in order to gain an understanding of possible persist into adulthood. This meta-analysis aims to
shared and additive EF deficits across these groups. address this gap, by also examining the impact of
This may contribute to EF theory as it relates to ADHD symptoms on EF in children with dyslexia.
deficits in reading alone and comorbid ADHD. This
can then inform EF intervention across disorders.
Method
This meta-analysis aims to draw conclusions from
controlled studies, in examining the presence of The primary aims of this meta-analytic review were
inhibition, switching attention and auditory to identify and describe published frameworks of
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 731

the role of inhibition, auditory working memory and conference abstracts, research reports, book chap-
switching attention in dyslexia and to analyse ters, unpublished data, dissertations and policy
whether these EFs are deficits in children with dys- documents). The reference lists from included
lexia as compared to typically developing controls studies were screened to ensure data saturation.
(TDCs). A secondary aim was to understand EF Grey literature was examined through advanced
deficits in children with dyslexia alone and co- search options in Google Scholar. A search of the
morbid dyslexia/ADHD as compared to TDCs. major databases, PsycINFO, MEDLINE and CINAHL,
These aims were chosen to help develop EF theory was conducted using combinations of search
as it relates to dyslexia alone as well as co-morbid terms relating to each EF of interest, i.e. (inhibit*
with ADHD which may inform possible EF interven- OR “interference control” OR “response suppress*”
tions across disorders. OR “executive function*” OR “executive dysfunc-
tion*” OR “dysexecutive syndrome” OR “executive
control”); (“switch* attention*” OR “attention*
Study design
switch*” OR “mental flexib*” OR “cognitive flexib*”
A literature search was conducted with the aim of OR “switch cost” OR “switch dysfunc*” OR “cognitive
identifying controlled trials on EF in children with control*” OR “switch deficit*” OR “response switch*”
dyslexia and co-morbid Dyslexia/ADHD. The EFs OR “attention* shift*” OR “shift* attention*” OR “set
switching attention, inhibition and auditory shift*” OR “task switch*” OR “switch* task”);
working memory were the EFs of interest. The (“working memory” OR “updating memory” OR
meta-analytic review was designed according to “updating working memory” OR “memory updat-
the methodology checklist and quality guidelines ing”) were combined with terms for dyslexia and
proposed by PRISMA (2009). As the focus of the Dyslexia/ADHD, i.e (Dyslexi* OR “Reading Disab*”
review will be to explore the presence of EF OR “Reading difficult*” OR “Learning disab*
deficits, the systematic review method will be reader*” OR “Reading disorder*” OR “Specific learn-
based on quantitative analysis. ing disab*” OR “Specific learning difficult*” OR
“Learning difficult*”). and (ADHD OR “Attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder*” OR ADD OR “Atten-
Search methods
tion deficit disorder” OR “Attention-deficit/hyperac-
Previous meta-analytic and systematic reviews of tivity disorder*” OR AD/HD OR inattention) and
dyslexia and comorbid dyslexia/ADHD were con- terms for children or adolescents (i.e. “child* and
sulted to develop and implement search strategies adol*” OR child* OR adol* OR “young people” OR
to identify evidence relevant to the review objec- teen* OR juvenile OR “school age”) and finally with
tives. The search strategy was designed to be as terms for controlled studies (i.e. control*).
extensive as possible to identify all possible eligible As both dyslexia alone and co-morbid dyslexia/
studies, which were refined according to the ADHD were of interest, a separate search was con-
inclusion and exclusion criteria detailed below. ducted using the search terms for both. The last
ADHD only was not included as significant research search was conducted on the 14 August 2016. In
has already identified EF as a deficit in this disorder. total 295 articles were identified. After an initial
screening of titles, non-relevant articles, e.g. dupli-
cates, review articles, single case studies and uncon-
Search terms
trolled articles (k = 160) were removed. Initial
Research where EFs were assessed, such as profiling screening of titles was conducted by two indepen-
or intervention studies, were included if control con- dent researchers. Studies in which no diagnosis of
ditions involving a TDC group were present to allow dyslexia was present (e.g. reading disability/
for the presence of EF deficits to be isolated. Only difficulty), were excluded to control for the high
studies published in English from 2000 to 2016 in degree of variability in how reading disorders were
peer-reviewed journals or relevant grey literature assessed and defined across studies. Studies were
were included. Grey literature can be defined as selected for review if they had a group design,
materials produced on levels of governmental, aca- included a homogenous group of cases, were con-
demic, business and industry in print and electronic trolled studies and utilised reliable baseline
formats, but which is not controlled by commercial measures of target EFs. A second independent
publishers (Hopewell, Clarke, & Mallett, 2005) (e.g. researcher extracted data relating to
732 A. LONERGAN ET AL.

methodological features present in a randomly the EFs of interest collected by researchers. All
selected sub-sample of 23% of studies (k = 6). studies included participants diagnosed with dys-
Inter-rater agreement was 86% on methodological lexia. The statistical significance of differences
features present. Where inconsistencies were between groups was reported in all studies. Data
present this was discussed and agreement reached on concurrent treatment at the time of assessment
on appropriate coding. were given in five studies; all of these related to
Applying the above search criteria, 133 articles the use of medication. No studies detailed the pres-
were screened at abstract level and 124 studies ence of any current dyslexia interventions that may
were assessed at full text level. A second indepen- impact EF. Twenty-two studies provided information
dent researcher screened all titles to ensure consist- relating to the assessment of IQ or general cognitive
ency of selection for full review. Inter-rater abilities in both clinical and control groups. This con-
agreement was 91% on articles selected for full trolled for the potential impact of differences in
review. A third independent researcher screened general cognitive functioning outside of EFs on
articles where there was disagreement and indi- symptomology. The studies reviewed here are
cated if these should be screened at full article sufficiently well designed to allow relatively reliable
level. In total 26 studies were selected for inclusion conclusions to be drawn about the types of EFs they
in this meta-analysis. The literature search flow is dis- evaluated.
played in Figure 1.
Substantive findings
Overview of studies
Tables 3 and 4 contain summaries of the results of all
The characteristics of the 26 studies selected for 26 studies. Studies were grouped into three cat-
review are given in Table 1. All studies were pub- egories based on type of EF: (i) inhibition (ii) switch-
lished between 2000 and 2016. Studies were cate- ing attention, and (iii) auditory working memory. The
gorised into three groups based on type of EF in primary outcome was performance of the dyslexia
order to control for the possible differential profile group on measures of reaction time, error rates
of EFs; (i) inhibition (k = 12) (ii) switching attention and accuracy. The performance of children with
(k = 8); and (iii) auditory working memory (k = 14). co-morbid dyslexia/ADHD on these same measures
The limited number of controlled studies examining was a secondary outcome of interest. All studies uti-
EFs in children and adolescents diagnosed with dys- lised a researcher-administered standardised assess-
lexia and/or co-morbid dyslexia/ADHD necessitated ment of the EFs of interest. Cohen’s d standardised
the use of a relatively small number of studies. The mean difference was calculated using DeFife’s
combined number of children and adolescents (2009) effect size calculator for all variables of inter-
across studies is N = 1,611; in the dyslexia (N = 729), est. The effect sizes calculated where weighted
co-morbid dyslexia/ADHD (N = 82) and control based on sample size. Following Cohen’s (1992)
groups (N = 800). Participants’ ages ranged from 5 interpretation guidelines, effect sizes of 0.2 were
to 18 years, with the majority being male. Partici- considered small, 0.5 medium, and 0.8 large. Given
pants included in selected studies were referred many participants required medication as part of
through multiple sources including schools, mental their standard medical care for ADHD, use of medi-
health and medical professionals. All studies cation was not exclusionary as long as this was con-
included a measure of the EFs inhibition, switching trolled for via the use of a comparison group. As
attention or auditory working memory. many studies took part in different countries, type
of language orthography was not used as an exclu-
sionary criterion. It is important to note that studies
Methodological features
from different countries/orthographies mean that
Methodological features of all studies included in dyslexia is measured/differentially captured to
this review are summarised in Table 2. All studies studies with English as the primary language. As per-
included a measure of EF, a control group and diag- formance on reading tasks was not assessed, the
nostically-homogeneous groups. Six studies detailed impact of orthography on outcomes is likely
the presence of co-morbid difficulties, while the limited. Due to the high variability in the operationa-
remaining studies highlighted co-morbid difficulties lisation of reading difficulties, absence of a diagnosis
as exclusion criteria. All studies included measures of of dyslexia was exclusionary. However, differences in
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 733

Records identified through database Additional records identified through


searching (k = 295) other sources (k = 9)

Records after duplicates removed


(k =133)

Records screened (k = 133)

Full-text articles excluded,


with reasons (k = 98)

No control group,
participants not children or
Full-text articles assessed for eligibility adolescents, no diagnosis of
(k = 124) dyslexia, review paper, case
study, single group design or
process articles, presence of
co-morbid difficulties beyond
ADHD, inconsistent
measures, did not assess EFs
of interest, Pre 2000, required
Studies included in meta-analysis (k = 26) data for meta-analytic
calculations not provided,
non-English language.

Meta-analyses:

1. Inhibition: dyslexia (k =12), comorbid


dyslexia/ADHD (k = 1)
2. Switching attention: dyslexia (k = 8);
comorbid dyslexia/ADHD (k = 2)
3. AWM: dyslexia (k = 14); comorbid
dyslexia/ADHD (k = 3)

Figure 1. Literature search and study categorisation flow.

how this is diagnosed across countries in which in either the inattention or hyperactivity/impulsivity
English is not the primary language is an important domain, as rated by either their parent or teacher. Of
limitation to note as it may impact severity of poss- the 42 children meeting these criteria, 15 already
ible EF deficits in children diagnosed with dyslexia had a diagnosis of ADHD. Eight studies classified
across countries. the presence of a co-morbid psychiatric disorder as
exclusionary criteria. Control groups consisted of
children and adolescents with no known difficulties
Inhibition and average intellectual ability.
Design
Participants across all 12 inhibition studies were
Measures
demographically similar and assigned to a control
or dyslexia group based on diagnosis. Inclusion cri- Reaction time was measured in ten studies. Six of
teria were children aged under 18 years, meeting these assessed reaction time during the interference
diagnostic criteria for dyslexia and/or dyslexia and trial of the Stroop task, while three studies measured
comorbid ADHD. One study classified a group as reaction time using the SSRT of the stop signal task.
dyslexia/co-morbid ADHD symptoms. Children The Stop Signal Reaction Time (SSRT) is calculated by
were classified as having significant ADHD symp- subtracting each child’s mean stop signal delay from
toms in this study if they had six or more symptoms their mean reaction time, providing an index of the
734
Table 1. Characteristics of studies included in meta-analysis.
Study Study Mean age & Co-morbid
No. type Authors Year Country N per gp range Gender diagnosis Measure EF measured Referral Study setting

A. LONERGAN ET AL.
1 CS Bexkens, van den 2015 Netherlands D = 28 D = 10.11 D = m 60% None present SSRT Inhibition School teachers, parents, –
Wildenberg, and Tijms C = 31 C = 11.2 D = f 40% psychological &
9–12y C = m 53% educational services
C = f 47%
2 CS Gooch, Snowling, and 2011 United D = 17 10.23y m 61% 20% D/ADHD/AS SSRT Listening recall Inhibition, CAMHS School –
Hulme Kingdom D/ADHD = 25 5.58y – 14.75y f 39% AWM
C = 42
3 CS Moores and Andrade 2000 United D=9 15y – None present Go Gap Stop task RT, Inhibition – University
Kingdom C=7 14–16y Errors
4 CS Schmid, Labuhn, and 2011 Germany D = 20 D = 9.7 m 58% None present SSRT Inhibition Psychiatrists Schools University
Hasselhorn C = 16 C = 9.3 f 42%
5 CS Helland and Asbjørnsen 2000 Norway D = 25 D = 13.07y D = m 76%, f 24% 53% pre-school SLI SI RT & Accuracy Inhibition, Schools Schools OP
C = 20 C = 12.11y C = m 80%, f 20% (excluded from WCST Perseverative SA Clinicians clinic
analysis) errors
6 CS Faccioli, Peru, Rubini, 2008 Italy D=7 8–10y m 57%, f 43% None reported Stroop, Colour word Inhibition – –
and Tassinari C=7 RT
7 CS Protopapas, Archonti, 2007 Greece D = 16 D = 10 – 13y D = m 75%, f 25% None present SI RT & Errors Inhibition Psychiatric Hospital Psychiatric
and Skaloumbakas C = 72 C = 11y – 13y C = m 49%, f 51% School Hospital
School
8 CS Reiter et al. 2005 Germany D = 42 D = 10.8y m 62%, f 38% None present Stroop; Interference Inhibition, Psychologist –
C = 42 C = 10.6y RT & errors SA, AWM
MCST; Accuracy, PE,
BDS
9 CS Kapoula et al. 2010 France D = 10 D = 15.1 – None present SI errors Inhibition School –
C = 14 C = 14.3
12–15y
10 CS de Lima, Azoni, and 2013 Brazil D = 20 9y D = m 55%,, f 45% None present Stroop; Colour Word Inhibition, OP clinic University
Ciasca C = 20 7–11y C = m 50%, f 50% RT, Errors SA School
TMT B RT, Errors
11 CS Brosnan et al. 2002 United D = 30 D = 14y – None reported GEFT Accuracy Inhibition School –
Kingdom C = 30 C = 13.8y
13–14y
12 CS Tiffin-Richards et al. 2008 Germany D = 20 10–14y m 75%, f 25% ADHD 34% WCST adaptation SA, AWM OP clinic OP clinic
D/ADHD = 20 11.8y Perseverative error, School
C = 19 BDS
13 CS Moura, Simões, and 2015 Portugal D = 50 8–12y D = m 74%, None present TMT B RT SA Medical, Psychological, –
Pereira C = 50 9.8y f 26% educational professions
C = m 64%, f 36% and parents
14 CS Cruz-Rodrigues, Barbos, 2014 Brazil D = 39 8–12y m 64%, f 36% None present WCST Accuracy, BDS SA, AWM Medical, Psychological, OP clinic &
Toledo-Piza, Miranda, C = 34 10.66y educational professions Schools
and Buen & parents
15 CS Palmer 2000 United D = 16 14y m 69%, f 31% None present WCST Perseverative SA Schools –
Kingdom C = 16 12 −18y errors
16 CS Poljac et al. 2010 Netherlands D = 25 12–18y m 80%, f 20% ADHD 4% Matching task RT, SA Schools
C = 27 errors
17 CS Lewandowska et al. 2013 Poland D = 57 9–13y m 67%, f 33% None present BDS AWM Psychological, University
C = 40 11y counselling centre Schools
18 CS Maehler and Schuchardt 2016 Germany D = 31 7–10y m 61%, f 39% D/ADHD 37% BDS AWM Counselling centre –
D/ADHD = 37 9y
C = 31
19 CS Maehler and Schuchardt 2011 Germany D = 20 7–10y m 53%, f 47% None present BDS AWM – –
C = 25 8y
20 CS Schuchardt, Maehler, 2008 Germany D = 30 7–10y D = m 60%, f 40% None present BDS AWM Counselling centre Schools
and Hasselhorn C = 30 9y Schools
21 CS Göthe, Esser, Gendt, and 2012 Germany D = 21 7–8y – None present BDS AWM Research study School, home,
Kliegl C = 20 7.8y laboratory
22 CS Layes, Lalonde, Mecheri, 2015 Algeria D = 36 10.7y m 71%, f 29% None present Word span AWM __ __
and Rebaï C = 20
23 CS Chung, Ho, Chan, Tsang, 2013 China D = 26 12–15y m 56%, f 44% None present Listening recall AWM Schools –
and Lee C = 26 13.8y
24 CS Helland and Asbjørnsen 2004 Norway D = 12 10–16y m 66%, f 34% Preschool language BDS AWM Psychologist OP clinic
C = 20 12.4y impairment 6%
25 CS Chik et al. 2012 China D = 101 9.7y – None present Listening recall AWM Schools –
C = 101
26 CS Jeffries and Everatt 2004 United D = 21 10.9y m 88%, f 13% None present Stroop interference Inhibition, Schools –

JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY


Kingdom C = 40 RT, BDS AWM
Note: CS = A controlled study. D = Dyslexia, C = Control, D/ADHD = comorbid dyslexia & ADHD, AWM = Auditory Working Memory, SA = Switching attention, m = male, f = female, RT = reaction time, SSRT = Stop signal
response task reaction time, SI = Stroop Interference, GEFT = group embedded figures test, TMT = Trail making Test, BDS = Backward Digit Span, WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Task, CAMHS = Child and Adolescent
Mental Health Service.

735
736
A. LONERGAN ET AL.
Table 2. Methodological features of executive function studies.
Study Number
Feature S1 S2 S3 S4 S5 S6 S7 S8 S9 S10 S11 S12 S13 S14 S15 S16 S17 S18 S19 S20 S21 S22 S23 S24 S25 S26
Control group 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Diagnostic homogeneity 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Comparable for co-morbidity 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
Demographic similarity 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Baseline assessment 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Diagnosis present 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Parent’s ratings 0 1 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0
Child’s symptom assessed 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Teacher ratings 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0
Researcher ratings 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Clinician ratings 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0
Differences in general cognitive abilities/IQ assessed 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 1 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0
Statistical significance of difficulties assessed 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
Data on concurrent treatment given 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
Total 9 13 9 10 11 10 10 10 10 11 9 13 11 11 10 10 11 13 9 9 9 9 9 12 9 8
Note: S = study. 1 = design feature was present. 0 = design feature was absent.
Table 3. Summary of results of effects of executive function tasks on reaction time, error rate and accuracy.
Study Number and condition
S1 S2 S3 S4 S5 S6 S7 S8 S9 S10 S11 S12 S13 S14 S15 S16 S17 S18 S19 S20 S21 S22 S23 S24 S25 S26
D D D/ADHD D D D D D D D D D D D/ADHD D D D D D D D/ADHD D D D D D D D D
V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V V
Variable C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C
Reaction time
Inhibition 0.3 0.2 0.5 0.1 0.4 1.2 1.7 1.2 0.5 – 0.6 – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – 0.0
Set shifting – – – – – – – – – – 0.6 – – – 0.8 – – 0.7 – – – – – – – – – – –
Working memory – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –
Error rate
Inhibition – – – 0.2 – – – 0.5 0.3 0.9 0.7 – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –
Set shifting – – – – – 0.3 – – 0.3 – 0.9 – 0.5 0.8 – – 3.3 .6 – – – – – – – – – – –
Working Memory – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –
Accuracy
Inhibition – – – – – 1.0 – – – – – 1.2 – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – – –

JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY


Set shifting – – – – – – – – 0.4 – – – – – – 3.8 – – – – – – – – – – – – –
Working Memory – 0.2 0.3 – – – – – 0.5 – – – 1.1 1.5 – 4.5 – – 0.4 0.6 0.4 0.9 0.7 0.2 1.7 1.3 1.3 0.7 1.5
Note: D = Dyslexia group, C = Control group, D/ADHD = comorbid dyslexia and ADHD group.

737
738
Table 4. Summary of main findings of 26 evaluation studies of executive function.
Study Study Group
No. type Authors Year N per group EF differences Key Findings

A. LONERGAN ET AL.
1 CS Bexkens et al. 2015 1. D = 282. C= 31 Inhibition 1=2 . Children with dyslexia differed from controls on naming speed, literacy skill, phonological processing
and processing speed, but not on inhibition as measured by SSRT.

2 CS Gooch et al. 2011 1. D = 17 a. Inhibition a. 1=2=3 . There were non-significant trends for children with dyslexia to demonstrate a longer SSRT than
2. D/ADHD = 25 b. AWM b. 1=2=3 controls, reflecting greater difficulty with interference control. The comorbid D/ADHD group
3. C = 42 demonstrated a greater mean trend of difficulty in this area than both controls & dyslexia alone. D/
ADHD children demonstrated a mean trend of weaker AWM than controls & dyslexia alone.

3 CS Moores and 2000 1. D = 9 Inhibition 1=2 . Children with dyslexia demonstrated no significant difference in errors or RT during the GGS test of
Andrade 2. C = 7 inhibition compared to controls. Response inhibition may be an important component of the standard
SART, in which children with dyslexia were significantly weaker than controls.

4 CS Schmid et al. 2011 1. D = 20 Inhibition 1=2 . Children with dyslexia demonstrated a mean trend of longer SSRT than controls, suggesting greater
2. C = 16 difficulty inhibiting interference. The authors interpret findings as suggesting activation of
phonological strategies during performance of the stop signal task in controls, but not in dyslexia.

5 CS Helland and 2000 1. D = 25 a. Inhibition a. 1 > 2 . Children with dyslexia demonstrated a significantly longer RT & errors when demands where placed
Asbjørnsen 2. C = 20 b. SA b. 1=2 on their inhibition skills.
. Children with dyslexia showed a non-significant greater number of mean perseverative errors than
controls indicating a difficulty in SA. This difficulty only reached significance when comparing groups
based on overall errors.

6 CS Faccioli et al. 2008 1. D = 7 Inhibition 1>2 . Children with dyslexia responded significantly slower to incongruent stimuli during a Stroop task than
2. C = 7 controls. Greater demands placed on inhibition skills may result in a significant delay in response for
children with dyslexia.

7 CS Protopapas et al. 2007 1. D = 16 Inhibition 1>2 . Children with dyslexia had significantly greater deficits inhibiting non target stimuli in incongruent
2. C = 72 conditions as compared to congruent conditions. This was evident in significantly longer RT during
interference trials. However when examining number of errors on the colour word trial in isolation, no
significant differences were identified

8 CS Reiter et al. 2005 1. D = 42 a. Inhibition a. 1 > 2 . Children with dyslexia required significantly longer to inhibit during the Stroop & demonstrated a non-
2. C = 42 b. SA b. 1 = 2 significant mean trend of greater errors during interference trials. In SA, children with dyslexia
c. AWM c. 1 > 2 demonstrated a greater number of perseverative errors during the MCST which did not reach
statistical significance.

9 CS Kapoula et al. 2010 1.D = 10 Inhibition 1>2 . Teenagers with dyslexia made significantly more errors & required significantly more time than
2. C = 14 controls to inhibit interference. However insufficient data was available to calculate ES for RT.

10 CS de Lima et al. 2013 1. D = 20 a. Inhibition a. 1 > 2 . Children with dyslexia made significantly more errors during the interference trial of the Stroop than
2. C = 20 b. SA b. 1 > 2 controls. Children with dyslexia also demonstrated a mean trend of longer RT inhibiting distracting
stimuli. Findings suggest children with dyslexia have deficits in inhibitory control.
. Children with dyslexia demonstrated significantly greater difficulty than controls when SA, evident in a
significantly greater number of errors & longer RTs.
11 CS Brosnan et al. 2002 1. D = 30 Inhibition 1>2 . Children with dyslexia had significantly greater difficulty than controls accurately inhibiting the
2. C = 30 processing of distracting visual context to identify target parts of the stimuli.
12 CS Tiffin-Richards 2008 1. D = 20 a. SA a. 1 = 2 = 3 . In SA, children with dyslexia and D/ADHD did not demonstrate a significant deficit in comparison to
et al. 2. D/ADHD =20 b. AWM b. 2>1>3 controls as measured by perseverative errors.
3. C = 19 . Both dyslexia & D/ADHD groups demonstrated a significant deficit in AWM which may reflect a
cognitive overlap partly explaining the high rate of co-occurring dyslexia & ADHD. Mean trends
indicated children with co-morbid D/ADHD had greater AWM impairment than dyslexia alone.
13 CS Moura et al. 2015 1. D = 50 SA 1>2 . Children with dyslexia were found to take significantly longer than controls to SA during the TMT B
2. C = 50 task. They took significantly longer to complete TMT B in comparison to the sequencing TMT A,
thereby isolating a specific deficit in SA.
14 CS Cruz-Rodrigues 2014 1. D = 39 a. SA a. 1 > 2 . Children with dyslexia demonstrated significant deficits in AWM and SA as compared to controls.
et al. 2. C = 34 b. AWM b. 1>2 Deficits were identified in backward DS tasks & the WCST.
15 CS Palmer 2000 1. D = 16 SA 1>2 . Children with dyslexia demonstrated a significant deficit SA compared to controls, evidenced by a
2. C = 16 greater number of perseverative errors than controls.
16 CS Poljac et al. 2010 1. D = 25 SA 1>2 . Children with dyslexia demonstrated a significant switch-specific delay relative to controls. They took
2. C = 27 longer to switch task & made significantly more errors than controls. Authors interpret their findings as
reflecting switch specific deficits in dyslexia additional to deficits in general information processing
reported in the literature.
17 CS Lewandowska 2013 1. D = 57 AWM 1=2 . No significant difference between groups AWM measured by the backward DS was identified.
et al. 2. C = 40
18 CS Maehler and 2016 1. D = 31 AWM 1 = 2=3 . Findings identified no significant difference between groups AWM measured by backward DS.
Schuchardt 2. D/ADHD =37
3. C = 31
19 CS Maehler and 2011 1. D = 20 AWM 1>2 . Children with dyslexia performed significantly worse than controls on tasks of AWM. This pattern was
Schuchardt 2. C = 25 repeated on multiple AWM tasks not detailed in this review e.g. word span backwards.
20 CS Schuchardt et al. 2008 1. D = 30 AWM 1>2 . Children with dyslexia were found to have deficits in AWM evidenced by significantly weaker
2. C = 30 backwards DS compared to controls.
21 CS Göthe et al. 2012 1. D = 21 AWM 1=2 . Children with dyslexia exhibited a non-significant, mean lower backward DS than controls. Findings
2. C = 20 may reflect children with dyslexia performing equally to controls in AWM.
22 CS Layes et al. 2004 1. D = 36 AWM 1>2 . Children with dyslexia were found to perform significantly weaker than controls on word span task
2. C = 20 assessing AWM. Children with dyslexia accurately recalled significantly less words than controls.
Regression analysis showed that AWM was significantly associated with literacy.
23 CS Chung et al. 2013 1. D = 26 AWM 1>2 . Children with dyslexia exhibited significant deficits in AWM, recalling significantly less target words
2. C = 26 than controls. Approximately 50% of the adolescent readers with dyslexia had a deficit in AWM

JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY


associated with difficulties in processing, storage & retrieval of information & performing concurrent
cognitive activities.
24 CS Helland et al. 2004 1. D = 12 AWM 1>2 . A subgroup of children with dyslexia, without language comprehension impairments performed
2. C = 20 significantly worse than controls on a backward DS task assessing AWM. When not allowed to use
strategies e.g. finger counting, children with dyslexia accurately recalled significantly less digits than
controls.
25 CS Chik et al. 2012 1. D = 101 AWM 1>2 . Children with dyslexia demonstrated significant deficits in AWM compared to controls in a sentence
2. C = 101 recall task.
26 CS Jeffries and Everatt 2004 1. D = 21 a. AWM a. 1 > 2 . Children with dyslexia demonstrated a significant AWM deficit compared to controls in a backward DS
2. C = 40 b. Inhibition b.1 = 2 task.
. Children with dyslexia had a comparable RT to controls during the interference trial of the Stroop,
indicating no inhibition deficit impacting RT.
. Children with dyslexia demonstrated significant deficits in AWM compared to controls, on a backward
DS task.

Note: CS = Controlled Study, D = Dyslexia group, C = Control group, D/ADHD = comorbid dyslexia and ADHD group, TDC = typically developing controls, SA = Switching Attention, AWM = Auditory Working Memory, DS =
digit span, RT = reaction time, WCST = Wisconsin card sorting task, SSRT = Stop signal response time.

739
740 A. LONERGAN ET AL.

duration of the inhibitory process, independent of stimuli, with a medium effect, in comparison to con-
mean reaction time (Gooch et al., 2011). A long trols. None of the studies identified included infor-
SSRT is indicative of poorer inhibition. One study mation relating to inhibition error rates for children
assessed reaction time during the Go Gap Stop with co-morbid Dyslexia/ADHD.
Test (GGST) of response inhibition. All these tests Two studies examined inhibition accuracy. Effect
assessed children’s speed of inhibition in compari- sizes for accuracy were 1.0 and 1.2. The mean
son to typically-developing controls. Error rates effect size was 1.1 (large). Both of these studies
were collected in five studies. Four of these assessed found that controls were significantly more accurate
error rates during the interference trial of the Stroop than children with dyslexia when inhibiting irrele-
task while one study assessed error rate during the vant stimuli. No studies identified included inhi-
GGST. Only two studies examined participant’s accu- bition accuracy information for children with co-
racy during measures of inhibition. One of these uti- morbid dyslexia/ADHD.
lised the group embedded figure test (GEFT) while
another examined accuracy during the interference
trial of the Stroop colour word test. Switching attention
Design
Results Participants across all eight switching attention
Children with dyslexia were compared to controls studies were demographically similar and
based on reaction time across ten inhibition assigned to a control or dyslexia group based on
studies. Effect sizes for reaction time during inhi- diagnosis. Inclusion criteria were children aged
bition tasks ranged from 0 to 1.7. The mean effect under 18 years meeting diagnostic criteria for dys-
size was 0.62 (medium). For five studies, results indi- lexia and/or comorbid dyslexia/ADHD. Four
cated that children with dyslexia had a significantly studies classified the presence of a co-morbid psy-
longer reaction time than controls on tasks of inhi- chiatric disorder as an exclusionary criterion. One
bition. However, in five studies differences in reac- study identified a single participant in the dyslexia
tion time between dyslexia and controls were non- group as possessing a diagnosis of ADHD; no
significant. In three of these, mean trends indicated other co-morbid difficulties were present. One
that children with dyslexia responded slower than study contained comorbid ADHD as an inclusion
controls. criterion, while the presence of medication or
For the co-morbid dyslexia/ADHD group reaction any other comorbidity was exclusionary. Control
time data were available for one study comparing groups consisted of children and adolescents
this group and controls. The effect size was 0.5 with no known difficulties and average intellectual
(medium). These findings indicated that children ability.
with comorbid dyslexia/ADHD demonstrated a
mean trend of longer reaction times, with a
Measures
medium effect size in comparison to controls. This
impairment was found to be somewhat worse Reaction time was measured in three studies. Two of
than that of dyslexia alone in Gooch et al.’s (2011) these utilised the Trail Making Task, Part B, while one
study. used a matching task. Error rate data was collected
Dyslexia error rate was compared to controls in six studies. Four of these assessed perseverative
across five studies when inhibiting non target errors when required to switch task in the Wisconsin
stimuli. Effect sizes for error rates ranged from 0.2 Card Sorting Test (WCST) or an adaptation of a card
to 0.9. The mean effect size was 0.5 (medium). For sorting task. Two studies assessed overall errors
two studies, results indicated children with dyslexia during a switching attention task, as measured by
made significantly more errors than controls on the TMT B and the matching task. Two studies exam-
tasks requiring inhibition. However, in three ined accuracy while switching attention. One of
studies differences in error rates between dyslexia these examined the number of categories in which
and controls demonstrated a non-significant mean participants successfully completed switching atten-
trend for higher errors in dyslexia. These findings tion tasks in the WCST while another utilised the
indicate that children with dyslexia have an Modified Card Sorting Test (MCST) to assess switch-
impaired ability to accurately inhibit irrelevant ing accuracy.
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 741

Results group based on diagnosis. Inclusion criteria were


children aged under 18 years meeting diagnostic cri-
The reaction times of children with dyslexia were
teria for dyslexia and/or co-morbid Dyslexia/ADHD.
compared to controls across three studies. Effect
Three studies classified a group as having dyslexia
sizes for reaction times during switching attention
and co-morbid ADHD symptoms. Eight studies
tasks ranged from 0.6 to 0.8. The mean effect size
classified the presence of a co-morbid psychiatric
was 0.7 (medium effect). For two studies, results indi-
disorder as an exclusionary criterion. Control
cated children with dyslexia had a significantly
groups consisted of children and adolescents with
longer reaction time than controls when required
no known difficulties and average intellectual
to switch attention. For the co-morbid dyslexia/
ability. All tasks assessing auditory working
ADHD group reaction time data were not available.
memory did so in relation to participant accuracy.
These findings suggest that children with dyslexia
Consequently no reaction time or error rate data
have impaired reaction times, with a medium
are available for this variable.
effect, when required to switch attention in compari-
son to controls.
The outcome measure, error rate, was collected in Measures
six studies examining switching attention. Effect
The primary outcome, accuracy, was measured in
sizes for switching attention errors ranged from 0.3
14 studies. Ten of these assessed auditory
to 3.3. The mean effect size was 0.9 (large). For two
working memory accuracy using the backward
studies, results indicated that children with dyslexia
digit span task. Three studies utilised a listening
made significantly more switching attention errors
recall task in which participants were required to
than controls. For the comorbid dyslexia/ADHD
hold increasing verbal information in memory
group error rate data were available for one study.
while answering a comprehension question
The effect size for this study was 0.8 (large). Mean
related to the sentence, followed by recall of the
trends indicated that children with co-morbid dys-
last word in each sentence. One study measured
lexia/ADHD exhibited greater switching impairment
auditory working memory accuracy based on
than children with dyslexia alone and controls.
ability to recall a list of animals presented verbally,
However, this difference was not significant.
in a reorganised order.
Accuracy data were collected in two studies
assessing switching attention in children with dys-
lexia. The effect size for these studies varied con- Results
siderably from 0.4 to 3.8, the mean effect size was
Auditory working memory accuracy for children
2.1. For one study, results indicated that children
with dyslexia was compared to controls in 14
with dyslexia demonstrated a mean trend of accu-
studies. Effect sizes for accuracy during auditory
rately completing fewer items during a switching
working memory tasks ranged from 0.2 to 4.5. The
attention task; this effect was on the borderline of
mean effect size was 1.1 (large). Ten studies demon-
significance at .05 (Reiter et al., 2005). In contrast,
strated that children with dyslexia were significantly
Cruz-Rodrigues et al. (2014) found that children
less accurate in tasks assessing auditory working
with dyslexia were significantly less accurate than
memory than controls.
controls when switching attention. It is noteworthy
For the co-morbid dyslexia/ADHD group accuracy
that Cruz-Rodrigues et al. (2014) utilised ambiguous
data were available for three studies comparing
cards in the WCST while Reiter et al. (2005) did not in
these groups with controls. The effect sizes ranged
the MCST. No accuracy or reaction time data were
from 0.3 to 1.5. The mean effect size was 0.7
available in relation to comorbid dyslexia/ADHD
(medium). For one study the co-morbid group was
when switching attention.
significantly less accurate than both controls and
children with dyslexia alone. One study identified a
Auditory working memory non-significant trend towards reduced accuracy on
measures of auditory working memory than both
Design
controls and children with dyslexia alone. One
Participants across all 14 auditory working memory study identified no significant difference between
studies were demographically similar and assigned both controls and children with dyslexia alone and
to a control, dyslexia or co-morbid dyslexia/ADHD comorbid dyslexia/ADHD.
742 A. LONERGAN ET AL.

These findings indicate that children with dys- impairment was found to be somewhat worse
lexia have impaired accuracy on auditory working than that of dyslexia alone, but not to a significant
memory tasks, with a large effect size, in comparison degree (Gooch et al., 2011). The relatively similar
to controls. In contrast, a three-way comparison of effect sizes for the dyslexia alone and co-morbid
children with dyslexia only, co-morbid dyslexia/ Dyslexia/ADHD studies reviewed contradicts
ADHD and controls produced more varied results. studies that found comorbid Dyslexia/ADHD rep-
The comorbid group was found to demonstrate resented a more severe subtype of ADHD and
somewhat impaired auditory working memory accu- reading disability than the children with reading dis-
racy, with a medium effect, in comparison to con- ability alone (De Jong et al., 2006; Willcutt et al.,
trols. Interpretation of results is limited by the 2001). However, the comorbid Dyslexia/ADHD reac-
small number of studies examining a comorbid dys- tion time data in the current meta-analysis are based
lexia/ADHD. on one study (Gooch et al., 2011), limiting interpret-
ation of results. Findings may support a multiple
deficit model in which shared EF deficits may
Discussion
underlie both disorders.
This meta-analysis aimed to draw conclusions from
26 controlled studies, in examining the presence of
Switching attention
inhibition, switching attention and auditory
working memory deficits in children and adoles- Children with dyslexia demonstrated greater impair-
cents with dyslexia. As a secondary outcome the ment than controls in switching attention reaction
impact of comorbid ADHD on EF in dyslexia was time (d = 0.7) with a medium effect. They also
also examined. demonstrated a greater magnitude of errors (d =
0.9) and lower accuracy rates (d = 2.1) than controls,
with a large effect size. The majority of studies ident-
Inhibition
ified deficits ranging from mean trends to statisti-
In terms of inhibition, children with dyslexia demon- cally significant difficulties. This indicates that
strated a greater impairment than TDCs, with a children with dyslexia experienced greater
medium effect, based on a longer reaction time (d difficulty in switching attention than controls. The
= 0.62) and a higher number of errors (d = 0.5). Fur- particularly large effect size for switching attention
thermore, children with dyslexia were less accurate supports the findings of Poljac et al. (2010) in
when required to inhibit (d = 1.1) non-target infor- which children with dyslexia were slower and less
mation than controls with a large effect size. accurate on a switching attention task than control
Approximately half of the studies reported a signifi- children. This difficulty with general task execution
cant difference between children with dyslexia and could be due to difficulties in visual processing,
controls on this domain, while the majority of including possible difficulties in inhibition of the sur-
remaining studies reported mean trends indicating rounding context (Brosnan et al., 2002), or due to
children with dyslexia experienced greater deficits in working memory already reported in the
difficulty inhibiting non-target stimuli. This is literature on dyslexia.
largely consistent with literature reporting inhibitory Only one study was available which examined
processing deficits in children with a reading disabil- switching attention errors in children with comorbid
ity (Närhi & Ahonen, 1995; Nigg, 1999; Purvis & Dyslexia/ADHD. Children with Dyslexia/ADHD
Tannock, 2000; van der Schoot et al., 2000; Willcutt demonstrated a greater number of errors when
et al., 2005). However, as with previous research required to switch attention (d = 0.8) than controls
(Altemeier et al., 2008; Landerl et al., 2009; van der with a large effect size (Tiffin-Richards, Hasselhorn,
Sluis et al., 2004), results are inconsistent, with Woerner, Rothenberger, & Banaschewski, 2008).
several studies identifying non-significant deficits Mean trends indicated that this difficulty was
or no difficulties with inhibition at all. worse than that of dyslexia alone, but not to a signifi-
These results are similar to the comorbid Dys- cant degree (Tiffin-Richards et al., 2008). This sup-
lexia/ADHD group who also demonstrated a mean ports the possible presence of a shared deficit in
trend of greater impairment in speed of response switching attention in both dyslexia alone and co-
during inhibition tasks (d = 0.5) than controls, with morbid Dyslexia/ADHD (Shanahan et al., 2006; Will-
a medium effect (Gooch et al., 2011). This cutt et al., 2003; Willcutt, Pennington, et al., 2005).
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 743

Auditory working memory Conclusions


Children with dyslexia were found to be less accu- Overall, children with dyslexia were found to have
rate when required to recall verbally presented difficulty with inhibition, switching attention, and
information (d = 1.1) than controls, with a large auditory working memory (with a medium to
effect. The large majority of studies included ident- large effect) relative to TDCs. Across all EFs, children
ified these deficits as significantly worse than for with dyslexia were found to have greater difficulty
controls. This indicates that children with dyslexia relative to typically-developing children when per-
exhibit deficits in auditory working memory as com- formance was assessed based on accuracy (large
pared to controls. This is consistent with previous effect) and error rates (medium-large effect),
literature identifying working memory deficits in rather than reaction times (medium effect). This
children with reading disability (Barnea et al., indicates that the chosen metric for interpreting
1994; Breznitz, 1997; Chiappe et al., 2000; Poblano performance may be a factor leading to mixed
et al., 2000; Vargo et al., 1995; Willcutt et al., results relating to EFs in dyslexia. This supports
2001). Additionally, it supports previous meta-ana- Wang et al.’s (2012) argument that a possible
lyses identifying a large effect of working memory reason for the inconsistencies between studies of
deficits in children with reading disabilities as com- EF in dyslexia may be differences in measurement
pared to controls (Jerman & Lee, 2005). These tools used. Children with comorbid Dyslexia/
findings contradict studies which found the per- ADHD exhibited relatively the same degree of
formance of children with reading disability to be impairment in terms of inhibition, switching atten-
comparable to that of typically-developing controls tion and auditory working memory as children
(e.g. McGee et al., 2004). This may be due to the with dyslexia alone. Findings have theoretical and
consistent use of auditory working memory clinical implications for the need to assess for and
measures in this meta-analysis, while numerous intervene when EF deficits are identified in children
previous studies have mixed the use of auditory with dyslexia. Furthermore, findings have prelimi-
and visual working memory tasks. It is possible nary theoretical implications for supporting EF as
results would be different if all measures used a possible a shared deficit underpinning variants
assessed working memory in the visual domain, or of neurodevelopmental disorders. This may make
were cross-modal. EF an important area for intervention where comor-
Children with comorbid Dyslexia/ADHD were bidities of neurodevelopmental disorders such as
found to be less accurate in tasks of auditory dyslexia and ADHD are present. However, these
working memory (d = 0.7) than controls, with a findings are tentative due to the limitations dis-
medium effect. For one study the comorbid group cussed below.
was significantly less accurate than both controls
and children with dyslexia alone (Tiffin-Richards
et al., 2008). However, the other two studies Limitations and further research
included did not identify a significant difference
This systematic review is limited by the relatively
between both controls and children with dyslexia
low numbers of studies, particularly in relation to
alone and comorbid dyslexia/ADHD (Gooch et al.,
co-morbid Dyslexia/ADHD. Studies from varied
2011; Maehler & Schuchardt, 2016). This suggests
languages of mixed orthographic depth were
that comorbid ADHD does not result in more
included. This may have implications for the assess-
severe auditory working memory deficits than in
ment and diagnosis of dyslexia as well as symptom
dyslexia alone. However, it does indicate that both
severity across countries included. However, as per-
clinical groups present with auditory working
formance on reading tasks was not assessed, the
memory deficits in comparison to controls. This
impact of this is likely minimal. In order to draw
suggests auditory working memory may be a
on studies examining diagnostically-homogeneous
shared deficit across neurodevelopmental disorders.
groups this review only included studies in which
Broadly, these results support previous research
a diagnosis of dyslexia was made. There is consider-
identifying working memory deficits in both ADHD
able debate over the validity of distinguishing
and dyslexia (Barkley, 1997; Castellanos & Tannock,
between children who have a large discrepancy
2002; De Jong, 1998; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996;
between reading ability and IQ (dyslexia) and
Willcutt, Pennington et al., 2001).
those who do not have this discrepancy (Fletcher,
744 A. LONERGAN ET AL.

Coulter, Reschly, & Vaughn, 2004). Lack of consen- the cognitive sub-type hypothesis and multiple
sus surrounding the choice of diagnostic measure deficit theory could not be directly tested.
of reading disability is an important limitation to However, as the presence of EF deficits in children
existing research (Sexton, Gelhorn, Bell, & Classi, with ADHD alone is well established in current
2012). This review is also limited by the task impur- research, inferences can be made as to whether
ity problem whereby most studies on EF have used these theories fit with the broad results of this
only a single task to assess the EF processes of study. Studies directly testing these theories with
interest, often resulting in a mixture of non-EF, the addition of an ADHD only group would
common EF (inhibition), and specific EF component provide more conclusive evidence in this regard.
effects, making interpretation of the results difficult Finally, the large majority of included studies did
(Snyder et al., 2015). This limitation was managed, not indicate that the presence of ADHD symptoms
where possible, by using the same measures were screened for at baseline assessment. This
across studies to address the specific EFs of inter- means that the possibility of an undiagnosed
est. As there have been few controlled studies ADHD profile in children with dyslexia alone
examining EF in children with dyslexia and comor- cannot be ruled out.
bid dyslexia/ADHD a review of the literature has, at This meta-analysis highlights a number of impli-
times necessitated the inclusion of research which cations for future research. Firstly, future studies
is relatively old. This highlights the novelty of this may benefit from examining the presence of EFs in
meta-analysis and reflects the current gap in the lit- children with dyslexia where attention has been
erature for research examining this topic. The limit- specifically assessed and controlled for. This may
ation was managed by the inclusion of controlled shed light on the potential impact of an ADHD
studies published between 2000 and 2016 in profile on dyslexia symptomology. Secondly, future
order to reduce the likelihood of excluding relevant research may benefit from utilising multiple
studies while also including as recent studies as measures of each EF to limit the task impurity
possible. problem. Further well-controlled studies are
A limitation of this, and any systematic review is needed to examine the specific profiles of dyslexia
possible publication bias. Publication bias occurs alone or combined with ADHD in comparison to
when the publication of studies depends on the controls. This could potentially help explain
nature and the direction of the results, so that pub- whether the deficits experienced by children with
lished studies may be systematically different from dyslexia or co-morbid Dyslexia/ADHD represent sep-
those of unpublished studies (Dickersin, 1990; arate profiles, shared deficits, or an additive effect of
Song, Hooper, & Loke, 2013). In general studies EF difficulties. This could add to our theoretical
with statistically significant or positive results are understanding of EF in the development of these
more likely to be published than those with non- highly comorbid difficulties. Overall these findings
significant or negative results (Song et al., 2010) provide preliminary support for the presence of EF
leading to systematic reviews potentially drawing deficits in children with dyslexia. This indicates this
on biased information. In order to help manage is an important area for further research to inform
this, relevant grey literature was included in the sys- EF theory in dyslexia and possible intervention.
tematic search in order to screen unpublished rel- Better understanding of EF deficits in dyslexia may
evant studies for inclusion as well as published have implications for evidence-based assessment
studies. As this review was examining baseline and intervention advancement, including enhan-
measures of EF, rather than the impact of an inter- cing screening, prevention and treatment (Snyder
vention publication bias is also minimised. The et al., 2015).
small number of studies for each EF meant statistical
methods such as a funnel plot would not be appro-
priate in this study. However future studies may Disclosure statement
benefit from the statistical examination of possible No potential conflict of interest was reported by the
publication bias if possible. authors.
As the purpose of this meta-analysis was not to
test theories of comorbidity, it was beyond the
scope to include a group consisting of children Funding
with ADHD only. This meant that theories such as The author’s have no funding to report.
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 745

References and children with developmental dyslexia.


Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 2144–2155.
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. Carroll, J. M., Maughan, B., Goodman, R., & Meltzer, H.
(2004). A structural analysis of working memory and (2005). Literacy difficulties and psychiatric disorders:
related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Evidence for comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology
Experimental Child Psychology, 87(2), 85–106. and Psychiatry, 46(5), 524–532.
Altemeier, L. E., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2008). Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of
Executive functions for reading and writing in typical lit- attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The search for
eracy development and dyslexia. Journal of Clinical and endophenotypes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(8),
Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(5), 588–606. 617–628.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Chiappe, P., Siegel, L. S., & Hasher, L. (2000). Working
statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®) (5th). memory, inhibitory control, and reading disability.
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Pub. Memory & Cognition, 28(1), 8–17.
Arrington, C. N., Kulesz, P. A., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., & Chik, P. P., Ho, C. S., Yeung, P., Wong, Y., Chan, D. W., Chung,
Barnes, M. A. (2014). The contribution of attentional K. K., & Lo, L. (2012). Contribution of discourse and mor-
control and working memory to reading comprehen- phosyntax skills to reading comprehension in Chinese
sion and decoding. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(5), dyslexic and typically developing children. Annals of
325–346. Dyslexia, 62(1), 1–18.
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new com- Christopher, M. E., Miyake, A., Keenan, J. M., Pennington, B.,
ponent of working memory? DeFries, J. C., Wadsworth, S. J., … Olson, R. K. (2012).
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. Predicting word reading and comprehension with
H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation executive function and speed measures across develop-
(Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. ment: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained atten- Psychology: General, 141(3), 470–488.
tion, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying Chung, K. K., Ho, C. S., Chan, D. W., Tsang, S. M., & Lee, S. H.
theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65–94. (2013). Contributions of syntactic awareness to reading
Barnea, A., Lamm, O., Epstein, R., & Pratt, H. (1994). Brain in Chinese-speaking adolescent readers with and
potentials from dyslexic children recorded during without dyslexia. Dyslexia, 19(1), 11–36.
short-term memory tasks. International Journal of Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112
Neuroscience, 74(1–4), 227–237. (1), 155–159.
Bauermeister, J. J., Matos, M., Reina, G., Salas, C. C., Cornwall, A., & Bawden, H. N. (1992). Reading disabilities
Martínez, J. V., Cumba, E., & Barkley, R. A. (2005). and aggression a critical review. Journal of Learning
Comparison of the DSM-IV combined and inattentive Disabilities, 25(5), 281–288.
types of ADHD in a school-based sample of Latino/ Cruz-Rodrigues, C., Barbos, T., Toledo-Piza, C. M. J.,
Hispanic children. Journal of Child Psychology & Miranda, M. C., & Buen, O. F. A. (2014).
Psychiatry, 46(2), 166–179. Neuropsychological characteristics of dyslexic children.
Beneventi, H., Tønnessen, F. E., Ersland, L., & Hugdahl, K. Psicologia: Reflexão E Crítica, 27(3), 539–546.
(2010). Executive working memory processes in dys- Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory
lexia: Behavioral and fMRI evidence. Scandinavian and language comprehension: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Psychology, 51(3), 192–202. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(4), 422–433.
Bental, B., & Tirosh, E. (2007). The relationship between Dawes, E., Leitão, S., Claessen, M., & Nayton, M. (2015). A
attention, executive functions and reading domain abil- profile of working memory ability in poor readers.
ities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and Australian Psychologist, 50(5), 362–371.
reading disorder: A comparative study. Journal of Child De Jong, P. F. (1998). Working memory deficits of reading
Psychology & Psychiatry, 48(5), 455–463. disabled children. Retrieved from http://www.
Bexkens, A., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Tijms, J. sciencedirect.com.ucd.idm.oclc.org/science/article/pii/
(2015). Rapid automatized naming in children with dys- S0022096598924511
lexia: Is inhibitory control involved? Dyslexia: An De Jong, C. G. W., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2006).
International Journal of Research and Practice, 21(3), The role of double dissociation studies in the search
212–234. for candidate endophenotypes for the comorbidity of
Booth, J. N., Boyle, J. M. E., & Kelly, S. W. (2010). Do tasks attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and reading dis-
make a difference? Accounting for heterogeneity of ability. International Journal of Disability, Development
performance of children with reading difficulties on and Education, 53(2), 177–193.
tasks of executive function: Findings from a meta-analy- De Jong, C. G. W., Van De Voorde, S., Roeyers, H.,
sis. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(1), Raymaekers, R., Allen, A. J., Knijff, S., … Sergeant, J. A.
133–176. (2009). Differential effects of atomoxetine on executive
Breznitz, Z. (1997). Effects of accelerated reading rate on functioning and lexical decision in attention-deficit/
memory for text among dyslexic readers. Journal of hyperactivity disorder and reading disorder. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 89(2), 289–297. Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 19(6), 699–707.
Brosnan, M., Demetre, J., Hamill, S., Robson, K., Shepherd, de Lima, R. F., Azoni, C. A. S., & Ciasca, S. M. (2013).
H., & Cody, G. (2002). Executive functioning in adults Attentional and Executive Deficits in Brazialian
746 A. LONERGAN ET AL.

Children with Developmental Dyslexia. Psychology, 4 Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2009). Developmental dis-
(10), 1–6. orders of language learning and cognition. West
De Ribaupierre, A. (2001). Working memory and atten- Sussex: John Wiley & Sons.
tional processes across the lifespan. Lifespan Jeffries, S., & Everatt, J. (2004). Working memory: Its role in
Development of Human Memory, 59–80. dyslexia and other specific learning difficulties. Dyslexia,
Dickersin, K. (1990). The existence of publication bias and 10(3), 196–214.
risk factors for its occurrence. JAMA: The Journal of the Jerman, O., & Lee, H. (2005). Working memory and reading
American Medical Association, 263(10), 1385–1389. disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. In
Donfrancesco, R., Mugnaini, D., & Dell’Uomo, A. (2005). Advances in learning and behavioral disabilities (Vol. 18,
Cognitive impulsivity in specific learning disabilities. pp. 1–31). Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 14(5), 270–275. Kapoula, Z., Lê, T.-T., Bonnet, A., Bourtoire, P., Demule, E.,
Eden, G. F., & Vaidya, C. J. (2008). ADHD and developmen- Fauvel, C., … Yang, Q. (2010). Poor Stroop performances
tal dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, in 15-year-old dyslexic teenagers. Experimental Brain
1145, 316–327. Research, 203(2), 419–425.
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. Kasper, L. J., Alderson, R. M., & Hudec, K. L. (2012).
(1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and Moderators of working memory deficits in children
general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(7),
309–331. 605–617.
Faccioli, C., Peru, A., Rubini, E., & Tassinari, G. (2008). Poor Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is
readers but compelled to read: Stroop effects in devel- (little more than) working-memory capacity?!
opmental dyslexia. Child Neuropsychology, 14(3), 277– Intelligence, 14(4), 389–433.
283. Landerl, K., Fussenegger, B., Moll, K., & Willburger, E. (2009).
Fletcher, J. M., Coulter, W. A., Reschly, D. J., & Vaughn, S. Dyslexia and dyscalculia: Two learning disorders with
(2004). Alternative approaches to the definition and different cognitive profiles. Journal of Experimental
identification of learning disabilities: Some questions Child Psychology, 103(3), 309–324.
and answers. Annals of Dyslexia, 54(2), 304–331. Layes, S., Lalonde, R., Mecheri, S., & Rebaï, M. (2015).
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., Phonological and cognitive reading related skills as pre-
DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive dictors of word reading and reading comprehension
functions are related to intelligence. Psychological among Arabic dyslexic children. Psychology, 6(1), 20–38.
science, 17(2), 172–179. Lewandowska, M., Gane, M., Wlodarczyk, E., Senderski, A.,
Gathercole, S., & Alloway, T. P. (2008). Working memory and McPherson, D. L., Bednarek, D., & Skarzynski, H. (2013).
learning: A practical guide for teachers. London: SAGE. Central auditory processes predict reading abilities of
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (2014). Working memory children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of
and language. Hove: Psychology Press. Hearing Science, 3(2), 30–40.
Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2000). Assessment of Lijffijt, M., Kenemans, J. L., Verbaten, M. N., & van Engeland,
working memory in six-and seven-year-old children. H. (2005). A meta-analytic review of stopping
Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 377–390. performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
Ghelani, K., Sidhu, R., Jain, U., & Tannock, R. (2004). Deficient inhibitory motor control? Journal of Abnormal
Reading comprehension and reading related abil- Psychology, 114(2), 216–222.
ities in adolescents with reading disabilities and Lyon, G., & Krasnegor, N. A. (1996). Attention, memory, and
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dyslexia, 10 executive function. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes
(4), 364–384. Publishing.
Gooch, D., Snowling, M., & Hulme, C. (2011). Time percep- Maehler, C., & Schuchardt, K. (2011). Working memory in
tion, phonological skills and executive function in chil- children with learning disabilities: Rethinking the cri-
dren with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. Journal of terion of discrepancy. International Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(2), 195–203. Disability, Development and Education, 58(1), 5–17.
Göthe, K., Esser, G., Gendt, A., & Kliegl, R. (2012). Working Maehler, C., & Schuchardt, K. (2016). Working memory in
memory in children: Tracing age differences and children with specific learning disorders and/or atten-
special educational needs to parameters of a formal tion deficits. Learning and Individual Differences, 41(6),
model. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 459–476. 514–523.
Helland, T., & Asbjørnsen, A. (2000). Executive functions in Mannuzza, S., Klein, R. G., Bessler, A., Malloy, P., & Hynes, M.
dyslexia. Child Neuropsychology, 6(1), 37–48. E. (1997). Educational and occupational outcome of
Helland, T., & Asbjørnsen, A. (2004). Digit span in dyslexia: hyperactive boys grown up. Journal of the American
Variations according to language comprehension and Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(9), 1222–
mathematics skills. Journal of Clinical and Experimental 1227.
Neuropsychology, 26(1), 31–42. Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock,
Hopewell, S., Clarke, M., & Mallett, S. (2005). Grey literature R. (2005). A meta-analysis of working memory impair-
and systematic reviews. In Publication bias in meta- ments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments (pp. disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
48–72). West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(4), 377–384.
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 747

Martinussen, R., & Tannock, R. (2006). Working memory Pennington, B. F. (2002). The development of psychopathol-
impairments in children with attention-deficit hyperac- ogy: Nature and nurture. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
tivity disorder with and without comorbid language Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit
learning disorders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101(2),
Neuropsychology, 28(7), 1073–1094. 385–413.
Marzocchi, G. M., Oosterlaan, J., Zuddas, A., Cavolina, P., Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O., & Roberts, R. J.,
Geurts, H., Redigolo, D., … Sergeant, J. A. (2008). Jr. (1996). Executive functions and working memory:
Contrasting deficits on executive functions between Theoretical and measurement issues. In G. R. Lyon &
ADHD and reading disabled children. Journal of Child N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive
Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(5), 543–552. function (pp. 327–348). Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes
Maughan, B., & Carroll, J. (2006). Literacy and mental dis- Publishing Co.
orders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 19(4), 350–354. Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions
McGee, R., Brodeur, D., Symons, D., Andrade, B., & Fahie, C. and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child
(2004). Time perception: Does it distinguish ADHD and Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87.
RD children in a clinical sample? Journal of Abnormal Peterson, R. L., Boada, R., McGrath, L. M., Willcutt, E. G.,
Child Psychology, 32(5), 481–490. Olson, R. K., & Pennington, B. F. (2017). Cognitive
McGrath, L. M., Pennington, B. F., Shanahan, M. A., prediction of reading, math, and attention: Shared
Santerre-Lemmon, L. E., Barnard, H. D., Willcutt, E. G., and unique influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
… Olson, R. K. (2011). A multiple deficit model of 50(4), 408–421.
reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity Peterson, R. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2012). Developmental
disorder: Searching for shared cognitive deficits. dyslexia. The Lancet, 379(9830), 1997–2007.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(5), 547– Pickering, S. J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2004). Distinctive
557. working memory profiles in children with special edu-
Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task- cational needs. Educational Psychology, 24(3), 393–408.
switching. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 234–249. Poblano, A., Valadéz-Tepec, T., de Lourdes Arias, M., &
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., García-Pedroza, F. (2000). Phonological and visuo-
Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diver- spatial working memory alterations in dyslexic children.
sity of executive functions and their contributions to Archives of Medical Research, 31(5), 493–496.
complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Poljac, E., Simon, S., Ringlever, L., Kalcik, D., Groen, W. B.,
Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. Buitelaar, J. K., & Bekkering, H. (2010). Impaired task
Moores, E., & Andrade, J. (2000). Ability of dyslexic and switching performance in children with dyslexia but
control teenagers to sustain attention and inhibit not in children with autism. Quarterly Journal of
responses. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, Experimental Psychology, 63(2), 401–416.
12(4), 520–540. Protopapas, A., Archonti, A., & Skaloumbakas, C. (2007).
Moura, O., Simões, M. R., & Pereira, M. (2015). Executive Reading ability is negatively related to stroop interfer-
functioning in children with developmental dyslexia. ence. Cognitive Psychology, 54(3), 251–282.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 28(Suppl 1), 20–41. Purvis, K. L., & Tannock, R. (2000). Phonological processing,
Närhi, V., & Ahonen, T. (1995). Reading disability with or not inhibitory control, differentiates ADHD and reading
without, attention deficit hyperactivity, disorder: Do disability. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
attentional problems, make a difference? Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(4), 485–494.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 11(3), 337–349. Reiter, A., Tucha, O., & Lange, K. W. (2005). Executive func-
Nigg, J. T. (1999). The ADHD response-inhibition deficit as tions in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 11(2), 116–131.
measured by the stop task: Replication with DSM-IV Rucklidge, J. J., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuropsychological
combined type, extension, and qualification. Journal of profiles of adolescents with ADHD: Effects of reading
Abnormal Child Psychology, 27(5), 393–402. difficulties and gender. Journal of Child Psychology and
Nigg, J. T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychiatry, 43(8), 988–1003.
Psychological Bulletin, 127(5), 571–598. Schmid, J. M., Labuhn, A. S., & Hasselhorn, M. (2011).
Nigg, J. T. (2006). What causes ADHD?: Understanding what Response inhibition and its relationship to phonological
goes wrong and why. New York, NY: Guilford Press. processing in children with and without dyslexia.
Nigg, J. T., Hinshaw, S. P., Carte, E. T., & Treuting, J. J. (1998). International Journal of Disability, Development and
Neuropsychological correlates of childhood attention- Education, 58(1), 19–32.
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Explainable by comorbid Schuchardt, K., Maehler, C., & Hasselhorn, M. (2008).
disruptive behavior or reading problems? Journal of Working memory deficits in children with specific learn-
Abnormal Psychology, 107(3), 468–480. ing disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(6), 514–
Palladino, P., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., & Pazzaglia, F. (2001). 523.
Working memory and updating processes in Semrud-Clikeman, M. P. D., Biederman, J. M. D., Sprich-
reading comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 29(2), Buckminster, S. B. A., Lehman, B. K. B. A., Faraone,
344–354. S. V. P. D., & Norman, D. E. D. (1992). Comorbidity
Palmer, S. (2000). Phonological recoding deficit in working between ADDH and learning disability: A review and
memory of dyslexic teenagers. Journal of Research in report in a clinically referred sample. Journal of the
Reading, 23(1), 28–40. American Academy of Child, 31(3), 439–448.
748 A. LONERGAN ET AL.

Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., Huijbregts, S., Scheres, A., & with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonolo-
Oosterlaan, J. (2003). The top and the bottom of gical processes are important. Journal of Experimental
ADHD: A neuropsychological perspective. Neuroscience Child Psychology, 79(3), 294–321.
& Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(7), 583–592. Swanson, H. L., Zheng, X., & Jerman, O. (2009). Working
Sesma, H. W., Mahone, E. M., Levine, T., Eason, S. H., & memory, short-term memory, and reading disabilities
Cutting, L. E. (2009). The contribution of executive skills a selective meta-analysis of the literature. Journal of
to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology, 15 Learning Disabilities, 42(3), 260–287.
(3), 232–246. Tannock, R. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:
Sexton, C. C., Gelhorn, H. L., Bell, J. A., & Classi, P. M. (2012). Advances in cognitive, neurobiological, and genetic
The co-occurrence of reading disorder and ADHD epi- research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39
demiology, treatment, psychosocial impact, and econ- (1), 65–99.
omic burden. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(6), Tannock, R., Martinussen, R., & Frijters, J. (2000). Naming
538–564. speed performance and stimulant effects indicate
Shah, P., & Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working effortful, semantic processing deficits in attention
memory resources for spatial thinking and language deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal
processing: An individual differences approach. Child Psychology, 28(3), 237–252.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(1), 4. Tiffin-Richards, M. C., Hasselhorn, M., Woerner, W.,
Shanahan, M. A., Pennington, B. F., & Yerys, B. E. (2006). Rothenberger, A., & Banaschewski, T. (2008).
Processing speed deficits in attention deficit/hyperac- Phonological short-term memory and central executive
tivity disorder and reading disability. Journal of processing in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(5), 584–601. with/without dyslexia – evidence of cognitive overlap.
Smith, A., Taylor, E., Rogers, J. W., Newman, S., & Rubia, K. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115(2), 227–234.
(2002). Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in Tridas, E. Q. (2007). From abc to ADHD: What parents should
children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology & know about dyslexia and attention problems. Baltimore,
Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 43(4), 529–542. MD: International Dyslexia Association.
Snyder, H. R., Miyake, A., & Hankin, B. L. (2015). Advancing van der Schoot, M., Licht, R., Horsley, T. M., & Sergeant, J. A.
understanding of executive function impairments and (2000). Inhibitory deficits in reading disability depend
psychopathology: Bridging the gap between clinical on subtype: Guessers but not spellers. Child
and cognitive approaches. Psychopathology, 6, 328. Neuropsychology, 6(4), 297–312.
Song, F., Hooper, L., & Loke, Y. (2013). Publication bias: van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2004).
What is it? How do we measure it? How do we avoid Inhibition and shifting in children with learning
it? Open Access Journal of Clinical Trials, 2013(5), 71–81. deficits in arithmetic and reading. Journal of
Song, F., Parekh, S., Hooper, L., Loke, Y. K., Ryder, J., Sutton, A. Experimental Child Psychology, 87(3), 239–266.
J., … Harvey, I. (2010). Dissemination and publication of Vargo, F. E., Grosser, G. S., & Spafford, C. S. (1995). Digit
research findings: An updated review of related biases. span and other WISC–R scores in the diagnosis of dys-
Health Technology Assessment, 14(8), 1–193. lexia in children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80(3
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Sergeant, J. A., Nigg, J., & Willcutt, E. suppl), 1219–1229.
(2008). Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D.
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Nosologic and M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what
diagnostic implications. Child and Adolescent have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17(2), 367–384. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 2–40.
Spironelli, C., Penolazzi, B., & Angrilli, A. (2008). Wang, L. C., Tasi, H. J., & Yang, H. M. (2012). Cognitive
Dysfunctional hemispheric asymmetry of theta and inhibition in students with and without dyslexia and
beta EEG activity during linguistic tasks in developmen- dyscalculia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33
tal dyslexia. Biological Psychology, 77(2), 123–131. (5), 1453–1461.
Stoet, G., & Snyder, L. H. (2007). Extensive practice does not West, T. (1997). In the mind’s eye – visual thinkers, gifted
eliminate human switch costs. Cognitive, Affective, & people with dyslexia and other learning difficulties, com-
Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(3), 192–197. puter images and the irony of creativity. New York, NY:
Swanson, H. L. (2006). Working memory and reading dis- Prometheus Books.
abilities: Both phonological and executive processing Willcutt, E. G., Betjemann, R.S., McGrath L.M., Chhabildas,
deficits are important. N.A., Olson, R.K., Defries, J.C., … Pennington, B. F.
Swanson, H. L., Howard, C. B., & Saez, L. (2006). Do different (2010). Etiology and neuropsychology of comorbidity
components of working memory underlie different sub- between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple-deficit
groups of reading disabilities? Journal of Learning models. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the
Disabilities, 39(3), 252–269. Nervous System & Behavior, 46(10), 1345–1361.
Swanson, H. L., Mink, J., & Bocian, K. M. (1999). Cognitive pro- Willcutt, E. G., DeFries, J. C., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K.,
cessing deficits in poor readers with symptoms of reading Smith, S. D., & Cardon, L. R. (2003). Genetic etiology
disabilities and ADHD: More alike than different? Journal of comorbid reading difficulties and ADHD. In
of Educational Psychology, 91(2), 321–333. Behavioral genetics in a postgenomic era (pp. 227–
Swanson, H. L., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001). Mathematical 246). Washington, DC: American Psychological
problem solving and working memory in children Association.
JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 749

Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & comorbidity between reading disability and attention
Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function deficit hyperactivity disorder: In search of the
theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta- common deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27
analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336–1346. (1), 35–78.
Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Comorbidity of Willcutt, E. G., Petrill, S. A., Wu, S., Boada, R., DeFries, J. C.,
reading disability and attention-deficit/ hyperactivity Olson, R. K., & Pennington, B. F. (2013). Comorbidity
disorder differences by gender and subtype. Journal between reading disability and math disability:
of Learning Disabilities, 33(2), 179–191. Concurrent psychopathology, functional impairment,
Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Boada, R., Ogline, J. S., and neuropsychological functioning. Journal of
Tunick, R. A., Chhabildas, N. A., & Olson, R. K. (2001). A Learning Disabilities, 46(6), 500–516.
comparison of the cognitive deficits in reading disability Willcutt, E. G., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Nigg, J. T., & Sergeant,
and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of J. A. (2008). Recent developments in neuropsychologi-
Abnormal Psychology, 110(1), 157–172. cal models of childhood psychiatric disorders. In T.
Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., Banaschewski & L. A. Rohde (Eds.), Advances in biological
& Hulslander, J. (2005). Neuropsychological analyses of psychiatry (pp. 195–226). Basel: Karger.

You might also like