You are on page 1of 17

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04461-z

ORIGINAL PAPER

Executive Function in High‑Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder:


A Meta‑analysis of fMRI Studies
Zheng Zhang1 · Peng Peng1 · Delong Zhang2

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Abstract
Abnormalities in executive function (EF) are clinical markers for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, the neural
mechanisms underlying abnormal EF in ASD remain unclear. This meta-analysis investigated the construct, abnormali-
ties, and age-related changes of EF in ASD. Thirty-three fMRI studies of inhibition, updating, and switching in individuals
with high-functioning ASD were included (n = 1114; age range 7–57 years). The results revealed that the EF construct in
ASD could be unitary (i.e., common EF) in children/adolescents, but unitary and diverse (i.e., common EF and inhibition)
in adults. Abnormalities in this EF construct were found across development in individuals with ASD in comparison with
typically developing individuals. Implications and recommendations are discussed for EF theory and for practice in ASD.

Keywords  Autism spectrum disorder · Executive function · Unity and diversity · Development · Fronto-parietal areas ·
Meta-analysis

Introduction Until recently, neurocognitive studies have linked ASD


with impairments in widely distributed brain systems.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) consists of a wide range Accordingly, functions that depend on those systems, such
of neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by clinical as sensorimotor processing, theory of mind, attention, and
symptoms that include difficulties in social communication executive function (EF), are compromised in those who have
and interaction; restricted and repetitive patterns of interests ASD (Loukusa et al. 2014; Mosconi et al. 2011; O’Hearn
or activities; and atypical sensitivity to sensory information et al. 2008). Among these functions, EF has been of great
(American Psychiatric Association 2013). Approximately interest in ASD research (De Vries and Geurts 2015; Mos-
70% of individuals with ASD are reported to have normal tert-Kerckhoffs et al. 2015). EF is itself an umbrella term
intelligence (an IQ of 70 or above; California Department that refers to a set of cognitive processes that guide individu-
of Developmental Services 2019). Such individuals are typi- als toward goal-oriented behaviors (Diamond 2013). It has
cally said to have high-functioning ASD (Jarrold and Brock been postulated that EF abnormalities are clinical markers
2004; Scheeren et al. 2013). for ASD (Rødgaard et al. 2019), representing poor func-
tional integration of prefrontal areas and posterior cortical
and subcortical areas (e.g., parietal cortex and striatum; Lan-
Electronic supplementary material  The online version of this
gen et al. 2012; Maximo et al. 2014). Furthermore, evidence
article (https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-020-04461​-z) contains
supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. has supported that EF abnormalities may vary with age but
in general appear to be present both in childhood and adult-
* Zheng Zhang hood (Dawson 2008; Luna et al. 2007; Takarae et al. 2004).
zhengzhang796@gmail.com Despite much research, however, the findings for abnormal
1
Department of Special Education, College of Education, The EF are quite mixed (e.g., Craig et al. 2016; Lawson et al.
University of Texas at Austin, 1912 Speedway, Stop D5000, 2015; Luna et al. 2002). The neural correlates underpinning
Austin, TX 78712, USA abnormal EF in those with ASD remain to be clarified.
2
Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science
of Guangdong Province, School of Psychology, Center
for the Study of Applied Psychology, South China Normal
University, Guangzhou, China

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

The Construct of EF in ASD meta-analysis, conducted by Philip et  al. (2012), has
explored EF abnormalities in those with ASD. Philip et al.
One explanation for the previous inconsistent findings is included 13 EF studies of ASD. Data for between-group
a clear lack of agreement about the EF construct in those effects (ASD vs. TD) on brain activity were selected. The
with ASD (e.g., Baggetta and Alexander 2016; Barkley results showed greater activation in the left middle fron-
2012). The integrative EF model (unity and diversity) tal gyrus in the ASD group, and greater activation in the
decomposes the EF construct into what is common across right middle frontal gyrus, the left inferior parietal lobe,
all EF processes (i.e., common EF) and what is unique to the left insula, the right posterior cingulate gyrus, and the
each specific EF process (Friedman and Miyake 2017). left lentiform nucleus in the TD group. However, Philip
The specific EF processes are correlated, yet separa- et al.’s meta-analysis integrated all EF processes as unitary
ble. They include inhibition (the restriction of prepotent EF and did not seek to ascertain brain activity specific to
actions); updating (constant monitoring and addition or each of the three fundamental EF processes.
deletion of the contents of working memory); and switch-
ing (task or mindset shifting).
Previous neuroimaging studies of ASD have not fully Age‑Related Changes of EF in ASD
considered this EF construct. For example, Hill (2004)
included planning, mental flexibility, and inhibition in Age-related changes are also worthy of consideration when
order to represent EF. Diamond (2013), however, pos- investigating EF in those with ASD. Until recently, the inte-
ited that planning is a higher-level EF process that the grative model of EF has been generally confirmed by behav-
three fundamental EF processes in the integrative model ioral evidence across different age groups (e.g., Duan et al.
work together to build up. Indeed, among the studies that 2010; Ito et al. 2015; Vaughan and Giovanello 2010). How-
have emphasized the fundamental EF processes, most ever, these studies suggest that the pattern of unity and diver-
have focused on only one or two specific processes; few sity in EF may be quite nuanced and that it can vary with
have evaluated EF across all three. For example, O’Hearn development. At younger ages, different EF processes have
et  al. (2008) included only inhibition and updating in been reported as indistinguishable, so that EF in this stage
their investigation of EF, whereas Schmitz et al. (2006) has often been interpreted as a unitary construct (Brydges
explored only inhibition and switching in their attempt to et al. 2014; Wiebe et al. 2011). In later development, how-
understand the specific EF processes in ASD. ever, different EF processes, although they share an overlap-
In addition, among the ASD studies that have focused ping variance (i.e., common EF), become more separable
on a specific EF process, findings are conflicting. For inhi- (Huizinga et al. 2006; Lehto et al. 2003; Senn et al. 2004;
bition, some studies have found significantly increased St Clair-Thompson and Gathercole 2006). Such age-related
brain activation in the inferior frontal gyrus in partici- changes in the EF construct have also been reported in a
pants with ASD in comparison with typically develop- neuroimaging meta-analysis (McKenna et al. 2017). This
ing (TD) controls on go/no-go tasks (Kana et al. 2007; evidence from TD samples indicates that the integrative
Schmitz et al. 2006). But Shafritz et al. (2015) recently model of EF seems to bear a developmental feature.
found that the inferior frontal gyrus was deactivated in Based on neuroscientific evidence from the TD popula-
those with ASD performing two versions of a go/no-go tion, cognitive maturation in EF is believed to be subserved
task (emotional and nonemotional). For updating, Luna by changes in brain activation that continue through child-
et al. (2002) reported deactivation in the prefrontal gyrus hood and begin to reach adult levels in mid-adolescence
and the posterior cingulate gyrus in participants with ASD (Christou et al. 2002; Gogtay et al. 2004). One EF study
in comparison with TD controls. Yet Herrington et al. found that children relied more on the prefrontal areas than
(2015) found increased activation in prefrontal areas in did adults, who relied more on the parietal lobe, indicating a
participants with ASD in comparison with TD controls. transition from the prefrontal areas to the parietal lobe with
As for switching, Schmitz et al. (2006) identified increased increasing age (Houdé et al. 2010). In TD samples, children
activation in the parietal lobe in those with ASD during do not seem to share the completely common neural cor-
task shifting. But Shafritz et al. (2008) provided evidence relates of EF with adults.
to support the opposite view—that reduced activation in In ASD, age-related changes have been reported in the
the parietal lobe was responsible for the underlying abnor- cerebral cortex, limbic structures, and cerebellum (Amaral
malities in switching in those with ASD. et al. 2008). From childhood to adolescence, Amaral et al.
Meta-analysis is an effective way to address these (2008) found an apparent increase in brain size in those
inconsistencies across individual studies (Rothman et al. with ASD in comparison with TD participants, especially
2008). Based on our review, only one neuroimaging in the frontal lobe. Nonetheless, the brain of those with ASD
would result in a normal size by adulthood (Redcay and

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Courchesne 2005). Such age-related differences in the brain were implemented as follows. First, data from different EF
structure of those with ASD reflect distinct brain circuits tasks were separated into a young group (< 18 years of age),
recruited by children and adults (Russo et al. 2007). fMRI an adult group (> 18 years), and a whole group (including
studies of ASD have revealed a more direct link between the young and the adult groups). Convergence of brain activ-
abnormalities in the frontal lobe and EF (Hill 2004). The ity across all EF tasks was extracted to identify the brain pat-
failure of the frontal lobe to follow a normal maturational tern for common EF in each of the three groups. The brain
pattern is likely to have long-term consequences for EF pattern of each EF process was identified by calculating
abnormalities in those with ASD (Luna et al. 2002; Ohnishi convergence of activation pertaining to inhibition, switch-
et al. 2000). ing, or updating, separately, in each of the three groups.
Given the evidence for possible effects of age on the These analyses should reveal the neural construct of EF in
neural mechanisms of EF in those with ASD, many stud- those with ASD. Neural abnormalities in EF were revealed
ies have tended to combine participants across different age by comparing common EF and each of the three EF pro-
groups into a single sample, which cannot reflect age-related cesses between the ASD sample and the TD sample in each
changes in EF abnormalities (Mosconi et al. 2009; Philip of the three groups, separately. By comparing common EF
et al. 2012; Wallace et al. 2016). Although a few neuroim- and each of the three EF processes between the young and
aging studies have focused mainly on a specific age range, the adult groups, age effects on the EF construct and the EF
findings are mixed. Specifically, EF abnormalities in those abnormalities in those with ASD were further investigated.
with ASD (including abnormalities in inhibition, updating, We decided to use a cut-off point of 18 years for groups
and switching) have been found in both childhood and adult- because both the behavioral and the neuroimaging literature
hood (Chantiluke et al. 2015; Daly et al. 2014; Gooskens suggest that EF processes including inhibition, updating, and
et al. 2019; Takarae et al. 2007; Vogan et al. 2014; Yerys switching across the development of ASD would be mature
et al. 2015). In contrast, some studies have argued that some in mid-adolescence (Chantiluke et al. 2015; Happé and Frith
EF processes such as those assessed in delayed match-to- 2006; van den Bergh et al. 2014; Yerys et al. 2015) or in
sample tasks and tasks with simple rules are intact in early early adulthood (Lever et al. 2015; Rosenthal et al. 2013;
development (Dawson et al. 2002; Liss et al. 2001). Other Solomon et al. 2014).
studies have also found intact aspects of updating in adult-
hood (Edgin and Pennington 2005). Such discrepancies
across studies may be due to the late maturation of brain Methods
areas including the frontal gyrus that support EF during
development (Gogtay et al. 2004). Given this context, these Studies Search and Selection Criteria
mixed findings of age-related EF abnormalities in those with
ASD are difficult to interpret. Studies for this meta-analysis were identified as follows.
First, a computer search of ERIC, MEDLINE, PsycARTI-
CLES, and PsycINFO databases for studies was conducted
Research Aims from the earliest possible start date through February 2019.
Then, unpublished studies were searched through the Dis-
Since the construct of EF in those with ASD has not been sertation and Masters Abstract indexes in ProQuest Disserta-
established and age-related changes have not been clarified, tions and Theses. The search terms for full-text search com-
EF abnormalities associated with age should be further bined with AND operators were (a) “working memory” OR
investigated in those with ASD. The purpose of the present flexib* OR inhibit* OR execut* OR shift* OR switch* OR
meta-analysis is to identify the neural construct of EF and updat* OR refresh* OR atten*; (b) autis* OR Asperger OR
explore the neural mechanisms underlying abnormal EF, “pervasive developmental” OR PDD OR ASD; (c) fMRI OR
including specific EF processes, in individuals with ASD “functional magnetic resonance.” Next, we hand-searched
across different age groups. citations in prior relevant reviews. Finally, we completed
For this meta-analysis of data from neuroimaging stud- forward and backward searches by reviewing the reference
ies, we used coordinate-based activation likelihood estima- lists of the included studies and searching all the studies that
tion (ALE). fMRI studies that assessed brain activity in cited the included studies in order to identify any that might
high-functioning ASD during EF task performance were have been missed in the former search phases.
included. Specific EF processes of interest included inhibi- The initial search revealed 18,633 studies. Titles and
tion, updating, and switching (Friedman and Miyake 2017). abstracts were then reviewed for removing irrelevant stud-
Data from the contrasts focusing on the within-group effects ies. After 547 duplicate studies and 17,470 irrelevant stud-
in the ASD sample and the TD sample and the between- ies were excluded, 616 studies remained; these were further
group effects (ASD vs. TD) were selected. Data analyses reviewed according to the following criteria:

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Criterion 1 noting that SVC analyses should potentially be included if


peaks in the areas were statistically thresholded as in the rest
Studies that employed an fMRI technique to explore human of the brain (Müller et al. 2018).
brain activity related to the review topic were included. A total of 33 studies met all criteria and were identified
Studies that employed other techniques (e.g., EEG, PET, as eligible for inclusion in this meta-analysis; 30 of the stud-
DTI, sMRI) were excluded. ies were peer-reviewed articles, and three were dissertations
(see Tables S1–S4). Figure 1 shows the PRISMA flow chart
for the search process. Overall, the 33 studies recruited both
Criterion 2
participants with high-functioning ASD and TD controls.
The ASD group and the TD group in each of the 33 stud-
Studies recruiting both participants with ASD and TD con-
ies were matched for age, mean IQ values, sex, or handed-
trols were included. Data from participants with ASD were
ness. The 33 studies measured “cool” EF, which operates in
included even if data from the TD controls were not acces-
more affectively neutral contexts (i.e., a task that “does not
sible. Similarly, data from TD participants were included
involve obvious rewards or punishers”), unlike “hot” EF,
even if data from participants with ASD were not accessible.
which operates in “motivationally and emotionally signifi-
Studies that recruited participants with ASD and participants
cant high-stakes situations” (Zelazo and Carlson 2012, p.
with other disorders (e.g., ADHD, Down syndrome, learning
355). In addition, 32 of the studies employed a threshold of
disorders) were included if data from participants with ASD
p < 0.05 to correct multiple comparisons; one study used a
were accessible. Studies that recruited a single participant
threshold of p < 0.001 (Shafritz et al. 2015).
were excluded. Additionally, data related to after-training,
(post)-intervention, sleep deprivation, or pharmacological
Inclusion Coding
manipulation were excluded, whereas data from participants
with ASD and TD participants without experimental manip-
A consensus coding schema was created for extracting
ulations were included if separately available.
information from the 33 studies, including several charac-
teristics: authors, publication dates, demographics, sample
Criterion 3 sizes, ASD diagnostic instruments, IQ tests and mean scores,
match types for the ASD and the TD groups, EF types,
Studies reporting activation and deactivation data from experimental tasks, experimental contrasts, main findings,
subtractions between target conditions and lower cognitive and coordinate space (see Tables S1–S4). All studies were
control, sensorimotor control, or resting-baseline conditions coded by the first author and a trained graduate student. The
were included, and comparisons between participants with interrater reliability of the averages across characteristics
ASD and TD controls were included. Data from correla- was 0.97. Any nuanced coding was discussed by referring
tions between brain areas and other predictors (e.g., ASD to the original studies in order to reach consensus. If char-
diagnostic scores, IQ scores, task reaction times) and from acteristics of interest were not reported, researchers were
functional connectivity analyses, independent component contacted by e-mail.
analysis, or principal component analysis, were excluded.
Data from conjunction analysis or common analysis were Analytic Strategy
excluded. Data from studies that investigated only within-
or between-group effects due to sex, handedness, and age Four types of contrasts from the 33 studies were selected.
were excluded, because those data did not purely reflect the Activation and deactivation data were extracted separately.
critical processes of EF. Contrast 1 examined brain activity in participants with ASD
when they performed EF tasks compared with control con-
ditions, such as resting-state, fixation cross, motor response
Criterion 4
tasks, and lower cognitive tasks. Contrast 2 examined brain
activity in TD participants when they performed the same
Studies that reported results in whole-brain analyses as coor-
EF tasks compared with the same control conditions. Con-
dinates in standard reference space (Talairach or Montreal
trast 3 investigated greater activity in participants with ASD
Neurological Institute) were included. Studies that reported
than in TD participants during the same EF tasks. Contrast
significant effects only in region of interest (ROI), hid-
4 focused on greater activity in TD participants than in par-
den ROI, or small volume correction (SVC) analysis were
ticipants with ASD during the same EF tasks.
excluded. The inclusion of heterogeneous ROI or SVC
It is possible that a single study might have multi-
analyses would lead to inflated significance for brain areas
ple contrasts of the same type. Because ALE analyses
overrepresented in ROI/SVC analyses. However, it is worth
treat contrasts of the same type within a study as distinct

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Fig. 1  PRISMA flow chart of the studies search process

experiments, this might bias findings of convergence ALE Meta‑analyses


across studies, leading to within-experiment effects (Tur-
keltaub et al. 2002). To minimize such effects, we imple- We performed ALE meta-analyses using BrainMap Gin-
mented the ALE analyses by combining multiple contrasts gerALE script (version 3.0) based on the MATLAB plat-
of the same type within a study into a single experiment form. Activation and deactivation data were analyzed
(Müller et al. 2018). Nonetheless, these contrasts could be separately. Coordinates of brain areas reported in Talairach
treated as distinct experiments if they were independent stereotactic space were transformed into the Montreal Neu-
of each other (i.e., if they resulted from nonoverlapping rological Institute reference system using the Lancaster
samples). transform (Lancaster et al. 2007). For each experimental
The data from each type of contrast were then grouped contrast, the ALE meta-analysis used the foci and the num-
into different ALE analyses, in which activation and deac- ber of participants in each study as input.
tivation data were grouped separately: (1) common EF ALE is an important algorithm for coordinate-based
analyses (convergence of brain activity across tasks tap- meta-analyses. It is used for a quantitative evaluation of
ping inhibition, updating, and switching), and (2) analy- the reliability of spatial patterns of activation foci across
ses of each EF process (convergence of brain activity in independently conducted neuroimaging studies. Spatial
tasks tapping only inhibition, updating, or switching). uncertainty can influence the distribution, including
Next, these different ALE analyses were further sepa- between-subject and between-laboratory variances (Eick-
rated into three groups: young (< 18  years old), adult hoff et al. 2009). The number of participants in each study
(> 18 years old), and whole (including both young and can adjust the spatial uncertainty of each focus. The study
adult groups). The numbers of experiments, foci, and with more participants endows a larger likelihood of true
participants included in each group are summarized in activation to the reported foci. During the ALE meta-
Table 1. analysis, the included foci are modeled as the centers

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 1  Number of studies, foci, and subjects in the present meta-analysis


Whole group (Young & Adult) Age group
Young Adult
EF process Experiments Foci Participants Experiments Foci Participants Experiments Foci Participants

ASD group (Contrast 1)


 Inhibition 10 141 141 3 63 47 7 78 94
 Updating 5 63 93 2 21 46 3 42 47
 Switching 4 25 54 1 7 20 3 18 34
TD group (Contrast 2)
 Inhibition 11 180 202 4 76 103 7 104 99
 Updating 4 61 72 2 22 47 2 39 25
 Switching 3 30 37 1 14 19 2 16 18
ASD > TD (Contrast 3)
 Inhibition 11 37 333 2 3 56 9 34 277
 Updating 2 18 53 1 15 31 1 3 22
 Switching 3 10 89 2 8 67 1 2 22
TD > ASD (Contrast 4)
 Inhibition 11 89 383 3 47 136 8 42 247
 Updating 5 45 189 3 19 143 2 26 46
 Switching 1 9 12 0 0 0 1 9 12

of a 3D Gaussian probability distribution in stereotactic Second‑Level Analyses


space. The probability of activation occurring at a given
voxel is calculated. Then the probability estimate for the After obtaining the ALE maps for the ASD sample (Contrast
entire voxel volume is calculated using a permutation- 1) and the TD sample (Contrast 2), we performed second-
testing framework to assess the extent to which the spa- level analyses—contrast analyses to examine group differ-
tial locations of activation foci are correlated across each ences in convergence. Second-level analyses compare two
conducted study. A final ALE map would be generated first-level analyses, examining significant differences in
with a p value for each voxel that indicates that the brain convergence between two ALE maps, by subtracting one
voxel is more active than would be expected by chance ALE map from the other. The second-level analyses were
alone. To assess the statistical significance of the ALE computed only for Contrasts 1 and 2.
scores, the p values were cut off at a threshold of p < 0.05 Specifically, for common EF, the contrast analyses
with a cluster-level family-wise error (cFWE) correction between the ASD sample and the TD sample were con-
(cluster-forming threshold at voxel level: p < 0.001), a ducted. Then, the contrast analyses between the young group
permutation testing with 10,000 iterations, and a cluster and the adult group were conducted for the ASD sample
extent threshold of 50 m­ m3 and transformed into Z scores and the TD sample separately. Next, the contrast analyses
for display (Eickhoff et al. 2016). between the ASD sample and the TD sample were conducted
for the young group and the adult group separately. These
second-level analyses were also conducted for each of the
First‑Level Analyses three EF processes separately.

First-level analyses were conducted on common EF and


each EF process in each of the three groups (the whole Results
group, the young group, and the adult group) separately.
First-level analyses describe clusters that pass the applied The 33 studies included in this meta-analysis comprised
threshold for significant convergence of activation across data from 1114 participants (age range 7–57 years) and
these groups of studies. The first-level analyses were reported 708 activation foci observed in 70 experimental
computed for the four types of contrasts separately. contrasts (see Table 1) and 21 deactivation foci observed in

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

five experimental contrasts. Among the deactivation data,


there were three experiments and 10 foci for inhibition (one
experiment and three foci in Contrast 1; two experiments
and seven foci in Contrast 2), and two experiments and 11
foci for updating (one experiment and six foci in Contrast
1, one experiment and five foci in Contrast 2). There were
no experiments with switching. The ALE results revealed
no convergence of deactivation in the first-level analyses or
differences in convergence in the second-level analyses using
deactivation data. For the activation data, we summarize the
ALE results in Tables S5–S8 and Figs. 2, 3, 4 and 5 describe
the details below.

Common EF

First‑Level Analyses

For Contrast 1 (the ASD sample), the first-level analyses


for common EF included 19 experiments with 229 foci
(see Table 1). In the whole group (19 experiments, 229
foci), analysis demonstrated convergence of activation in
the fronto-parietal areas, with the largest cluster in the
left inferior frontal gyrus, extending to the right inferior

Fig. 3  Brain patterns showing differences in convergence for com-


mon EF by comparing the ASD sample and the TD sample. a The
whole group. b The young group. c The adult group. IFG inferior
frontal gyrus, IPG inferior parietal gyrus, SMA supplementary motor
area, ACG​anterior cingulate gyrus, MFG middle frontal gyrus

frontal gyrus, the left precentral gyrus, the right superior


medial frontal gyrus, and the left inferior parietal gyrus.
In the young group (six experiments, 91 foci), analysis
revealed convergence of activation in the left inferior fron-
tal gyrus. In the adult group (13 experiments, 138 foci),
analysis revealed convergence of activation in the left
inferior frontal gyrus and the left inferior parietal gyrus
(Table S5; Fig. 2).
For Contrast 2 (the TD sample), the first-level analyses
for common EF included 18 experiments with 271 foci (see
Table 1). In the whole group (18 experiments, 271 foci),
convergence of activation was found only in the right cere-
brum, with the largest cluster in the anterior cingulate gyrus,
as well as the inferior frontal gyrus and the middle frontal
gyrus. In the young group (7 experiments, 112 foci), con-
vergence of activation was found in the right inferior frontal
gyri and the right anterior cingulate gyrus. In the adult group
(11 experiments, 159 foci), convergence of activation was
Fig. 2  Brain patterns showing convergence of activation for common found in the right inferior frontal gyrus and the left supple-
EF within the ASD sample and the TD sample, respectively. a The mentary motor area (Table S6; Fig. 2).
whole group. b The young group. c The adult group. IFG inferior
For Contrast 3 (16 experiments, 65 foci) and 4 (17 experi-
frontal gyrus, IPG inferior parietal gyrus, SFGmed superior medial
frontal gyrus, SMA supplementary motor area, ACG​ anterior cingu- ments, 143 foci; see Table 1), no convergence of activation
late gyrus, PreCG precentral gyrus, MFG middle frontal gyrus was revealed for common EF.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Fig. 4  Brain patterns showing convergence of activation for inhibi- Fig. 5  Brain patterns showing differences in convergence for inhibi-
tion within the ASD sample and the TD sample, respectively. a The tion by comparing the ASD sample and the TD sample. a The whole
whole group. b The young group. c The adult group. IFG inferior group. b The young group. c The adult group. IFG inferior frontal
frontal gyrus, ACG​ anterior cingulate gyrus, SFG superior frontal gyrus, SMA supplementary motor area, ACG​anterior cingulate gyrus,
gyrus, MFG middle frontal gyrus MFG middle frontal gyrus, PreCG precentral gyrus

Second‑Level Analyses the TD sample compared with the ASD sample (Table S8;
Fig. 3).
Comparing common EF between the ASD sample (Contrast Within the ASD sample (Contrast 1), comparing common
1) and the TD sample (Contrast 2) in the whole group, a dif- EF between the young and the adult groups, there was no
ference in convergence was found in the left inferior parietal difference in convergence for the young or the adult group
gyrus for the ASD sample compared with the TD sample. compared with each other. Within the TD sample (Contrast
In the young group, no difference in convergence was found 2), comparing common EF between the two age groups,
for the ASD sample. In the adult group, a difference in con- there was no difference in convergence for the young group,
vergence was found in the left inferior parietal gyrus for the whereas there were differences in convergence in the right
ASD sample (Table S7; Fig. 3). sub-lobar matter, the right inferior frontal gyrus, and the
In contrast, comparing common EF between the ASD right medial frontal gyrus for the adult group.
sample (Contrast 1) and the TD sample (Contrast 2) in the
whole group, differences in convergence were found in
the right inferior frontal gyrus, the right anterior cingulate The Three EF Processes
gyrus, the left supplementary motor area, and the right mid-
dle frontal gyrus for the TD sample compared with the ASD First‑Level Analyses for Contrast 1 (the ASD Sample)
sample. In the young group, differences in convergence were
found in the right inferior frontal gyrus and the right anterior The first-level analyses included 10 experiments with inhibi-
cingulate gyrus for the TD sample compared with the ASD tion (141 foci), four experiments with switching (25 foci),
sample. In the adult group, differences in convergence were and five experiments with updating (63 foci; see Table 1).
found in the right inferior frontal gyrus, the right anterior
cingulate gyrus, and the left supplementary motor area for

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Inhibition First‑Level Analyses for Contrasts 3 and 4

In the whole group, the ALE results demonstrated con- For Contrast 3 (16 experiments, 65 foci) and Contrast 4
vergence of activation in the bilateral inferior frontal gyri (17 experiments, 143 foci; see Table 1), no convergence of
and the right anterior cingulate gyrus. No convergence of activation was revealed for any of the three EF processes.
activation was found in the young group, but convergence
of activation was found in the bilateral inferior frontal gyri
in the adult group (Table S5; Fig. 4). Second‑Level Analyses

The detailed ALE results of second-level analyses are


Updating summarized in Tables S7–S8 and Fig. 5.

The ALE results revealed convergence of activation in the


left middle occipital gyrus in the young group, but there Inhibition
was no convergence of activation in the whole group or
the adult group (Table S5). Comparing inhibition between the ASD sample (Contrast
1) and the TD sample (Contrast 2) in the whole group,
the ALE results revealed a difference in convergence in
Switching
the left inferior frontal gyrus for the ASD sample com-
pared with the TD sample. In the young group, no dif-
No convergence of activation was revealed in any of the
ference in convergence was revealed for the ASD sample
three groups (Table S5).
compared with the TD sample. In the adult group, a dif-
ference in convergence in the left inferior frontal gyrus
First‑Level Analyses for Contrast 2 (the TD Sample) was revealed for the ASD sample compared with the TD
sample (Table S7; Fig. 5).
The first-level analyses for each EF process included 11 In contrast, comparing inhibition between the ASD sam-
experiments with inhibition (180 foci), three experiments ple (Contrast 1) and the TD sample (Contrast 2) in the whole
with switching (30 foci), and seven experiments with group, the ALE results revealed differences in convergence
updating (61 foci; see Table 1). in the right anterior cingulate gyrus, the right precentral
gyrus, and the left supplementary motor area for the TD
sample compared with the ASD sample. In the young group,
Inhibition no difference in convergence was revealed for the TD sample
compared with the ASD sample. In the adult group, a differ-
In the whole group, the ALE results revealed convergence ence in convergence in the right middle frontal gyrus was
of activation only in the right cerebrum, including the infe- revealed for the TD sample compared with the ASD sample
rior frontal gyrus, the anterior cingulate gyrus, and the (Table S8).
middle frontal gyrus. No convergence of activation was Within the ASD sample (Contrast 1), comparing inhibi-
found in the young group, whereas the results revealed tion between the young group and the adult group, the ALE
convergence of activation in the right cerebrum in the results revealed a difference in convergence in the left infe-
adult group, including the inferior frontal gyri, the anterior rior frontal gyrus for the adult group, whereas no difference
cingulate gyrus, and the superior frontal gyrus (Table S6; in convergence was found for the young group. Within the
Fig. 4). TD sample (Contrast 2), there was no difference in conver-
gence for the young or the adult group in comparison with
each other.
Updating and Switching

No convergence of activation was found for updating or Updating and Switching


switching in any of the three groups (Table S6).
The ALE results did not reveal any difference in convergence
for the ASD sample or the TD sample compared with each
other in the whole, young, or adult groups (Tables S7–S8).
Additionally, the second-level analyses did not reveal any
difference in convergence when the young group and the

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

adult group were compared within the ASD sample (Con- Within the ASD sample, we also identified brain patterns
trast 1) or the TD sample (Contrast 2), separately. of activation for both the young group (in the left inferior
frontal gyrus) and the adult group (in the left inferior frontal
Contributions of the Included Studies to Specific gyrus and the left inferior parietal gyrus). In many EF stud-
Brain Regional Effects ies, the prefrontal areas are well known to be primary brain
areas that support general EF processing (inhibition, updat-
Post-hoc analyses were conducted to provide more detailed ing, switching) in both young individuals and adults in TD
information about the revealed brain areas of convergence. samples (Brázdil et al. 2007; Wang et al. 2010). The present
In the Supplemental Material, we present a list of the stud- findings may indicate that the prefrontal areas also play an
ies that contributed to the specific regional effects, and most important role in individuals with ASD across ages. How-
of the results were stable and not likely to be have been ever, we did not find differences in convergence between the
driven by only a small portion of studies. Specifically, for two age groups for the common EF within the ASD sample.
the first-level analyses within the ASD or the TD sample, Therefore, we suggest that the neural correlates for common
half of the studies or more included in a specific analysis EF in those with ASD may be relatively stable from child-
contributed to the effect in each brain area. For example, 10 hood to adulthood.
studies of inhibition were included in the first-level analysis
for revealing convergence of activation in inhibition. The Inhibition
results showed that five of the 10 studies contributed to the
effect in the right inferior frontal gyrus and six of the 10 Within the ASD sample, we identified a brain pattern of
studies contributed to the effect in the left inferior frontal activation for inhibition in the whole group, mainly in the
gyrus. Less convergence was found in the second-level con- bilateral inferior frontal gyri and the right anterior cingulate
trast analyses, where less than half of the included studies gyrus. In TD samples, the right inferior frontal gyrus has
in a specific analysis contributed to the effect in each brain been found critical for inhibitory control across go/no-go
area, especially for the brain areas in the ASD sample when (Chiu and Egner 2015), stop-signal (Aron 2011), Stroop
compared with the TD sample (only one or two studies). (Kim et al. 2011), and antisaccade (Fitzgerald et al. 2008)
tasks. The right anterior cingulate gyrus has been found
to play a central role in several functions associated with
response inhibition, conflict monitoring, and error detection
Discussion (Botvinick et al. 2004). Findings are similar in ASD studies
(Chan et al. 2011; Kana et al. 2007; Vara et al. 2014). Given
The present meta-analysis is the first to comprehensively this evidence, the right inferior frontal gyrus and the right
explore the construct of EF and the first to investigate EF anterior cingulate gyrus could be the key areas for the inhibi-
abnormalities in individuals with high-functioning ASD tory function in individuals with and without ASD.
across ages. This study provides updated information about Further, within the ASD sample, we did not find any con-
the construct EF and abnormalities of EF associated with vergence of activation in the young group, but we identi-
age in those with ASD on a neural level. fied a brain pattern (in the bilateral inferior frontal gyri) in
adults. In TD samples, previous studies have often failed
The EF Construct and Age‑Related Changes in ASD to find an inhibition factor in young individuals (Karr et al.
2018; McKenna et al. 2017; van der Sluis et al. 2007). With
Common EF increasing age, the process of inhibition emerges (Brydges
et al. 2014; Lerner and Lonigan 2014). Combined with this
Within the ASD sample, we identified a brain pattern of evidence, our findings may indicate that the inhibition-spe-
activation for common EF in the whole group, mainly in cific process may not be observable in individuals with ASD
the bilateral inferior frontal gyri, the left precentral gyrus, until adulthood, in line with TD individuals.
the right superior medial frontal gyrus, and the left inferior
parietal gyrus, consistent with previous ASD studies (Lynch Updating and Switching
et al. 2017; Schmitz et al. 2006). These fronto-parietal areas
were found to be commonly activated across EF tasks in the Within the ASD sample, for updating, we found the conver-
TD samples (Church et al. 2017; Nee et al. 2012; Niendam gence of activation only in the left middle occipital gyrus
et al. 2012; Yaple and Arsalidou 2018). Given the present in the young group. The occipital areas are known to be
findings, the fronto-parietal areas may be the core areas that selective for visual-based functions, such as mirror actions
subserve performance during the common EF processing in (Rizzolatti and Craighero 2004), visual imagery processes
both TD and ASD samples. (Jung and Haier 2007), and face processing (Dalton et al.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

2005). ASD studies have proposed that activation in the 2009), indicating the use of different cognitive strategies to
occipital areas may reflect a tendency to rely primarily on successfully solve EF tasks. These findings, together with
visual features and visual attention during cognitive tasks ours, indicate that abnormalities in common EF may be
(Koshino et al. 2007; Mottron et al. 2006; Vogan et al. 2014). associated with abnormal attention control in those with
Given this evidence, our findings may imply that young indi- ASD.
viduals with ASD may utilize visuoperceptual strategies to In contrast, compared with the ASD sample, the TD sam-
perform updating tasks. Hence, the activation in the left mid- ple showed differences in convergence for common EF in the
dle occipital gyrus could not be considered specialized for right inferior frontal gyrus, the right middle frontal gyrus,
updating as a brain pattern in young individuals with ASD. the right anterior cingulate gyrus, and the left supplemen-
In addition, we did not find any convergence of activation tary motor area. Activation in these brain areas is essential
for updating or switching across the three groups. That is, we for performance during EF tasks (Bonini et al. 2009; Wang
failed to identify significant brain patterns for updating or et al. 2010), as well as in supporting sensorimotor function
switching. However, this result should be treated with sub- (Mosconi and Sweeney 2015). It has been reported that sen-
stantial caution due to underpowered analyses of restricted sorimotor deficits are the primary features of ASD (Mosconi
data in our meta-analysis (five experiments and 63 foci for and Sweeney 2015). Abnormalities in these areas under-
updating, four experiments and 25 foci for switching). Alter- lying sensorimotor deficits have been associated with EF
natively, these null results could reflect the heterogeneity in abnormalities (Bock and Girgenrath 2006; Perry et al. 2007;
findings across the included studies of ASD, despite con- Sheridan and Hausdorff 2007) and have even accounted for
sistent task paradigms used for examining updating (n-back them in part (Hyder et al. 1997). Since our findings indicate
tasks) and switching (set-switching or rule-switching tasks) that the ASD sample could be less likely to recruit these sen-
in those studies. Future research, possibly with larger sorimotor areas than would the TD sample during EF tasks,
samples, should continue to explore whether the specific we hypothesize that sensorimotor deficits in those with ASD
processes of updating and switching exist in ASD across may affect these brain areas and thus lead to abnormalities
development, as well as whether the updating or switching in EF (Rødgaard et al. 2019).
measures for the TD population apply to ASD in different In our Introduction, we noted a neuroimaging meta-analy-
age groups. sis (Philip et al. 2012) that also recruited data from between-
In sum, our findings demonstrate that for the ASD sam- group effects (ASD vs. TD) to explore EF abnormalities in
ple, there was a brain pattern of activation for common those with ASD. However, those results show some limita-
EF (in the left inferior frontal gyrus) among children and tions, such as the inclusion of inaccurate data in the analyses
adolescents, and there were brain patterns for common EF and the adopting of an inadequate correction (false discovery
(in part of the fronto-parietal areas) and inhibition (in the rate) to threshold the ALE values. By addressing these limi-
bilateral inferior frontal gyri) in adults. These findings are, tations, we have confirmed some of Philips et al.’s (2012)
to some extent, in line with the body of behavioral literature findings and have extended them by revealing a difference in
on EF development suggesting age-related changes in the EF convergence in the left supplementary motor area for the TD
construct in TD samples (Best and Miller 2010; Karr et al. sample in comparison with the ASD sample. This brain area
2018; Lee et al. 2013; Rottschy et al. 2012). We therefore is often revealed in EF processes in TD individuals (Hazel-
suggest a unitary model of EF (common EF) in children and tine et al. 2000; Scherf et al. 2006; Smolker et al. 2015),
adolescents and a unitary yet diverse model of EF in adults especially in the inhibition of motor behavior (Dinomais
(common EF and inhibition) for ASD individuals. et al. 2009). ASD studies have found abnormal activation
in the left supplementary motor area during EF tasks (Ishii-
The EF Abnormalities and Age‑Related Changes Takahashi et al. 2014; Smith et al. 2004). Combining these
in ASD findings, we propose that the left supplementary motor area
plays an important role in EF abnormalities in ASD.
Common EF Regarding age effects, within the ASD sample, we did not
find differences in convergence between the young and the
Based on the whole group, the ASD sample showed a dif- adult groups for common EF. However, across samples, we
ference in convergence in the left inferior parietal gyrus found a difference in convergence for common EF in the TD
for common EF in comparison with the TD sample. The sample as opposed to the ASD sample in the right inferior
parietal lobe has been found important for spatial atten- frontal gyrus across the young and the adult groups. This
tion and spatial coordinate transformations in TD samples may suggest that the abnormal mechanisms underlying com-
(Merriam et al. 2003). Abnormal parietal activation could mon EF in those with ASD are relatively stable across young
occur with the effort to maintain attention during EF tasks and adult groups. These findings are supported by behavioral
in those with ASD (Fatemi et al. 2002; McAlonan et al. evidence that, overall, EF abnormalities are present from

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

childhood through adulthood (Dawson 2008; Oznoff et al. meta-analysis or the reliability of the updating and switching
2004; Zelazo and Müller 2002). measures for ASD.
In sum, for common EF, our results show that compared
Inhibition with TD controls, individuals with ASD recruited the left
inferior parietal gyrus but were less likely to recruit the sen-
Based on the whole group, we found a difference in conver- sorimotor areas to perform EF tasks. For inhibition, the left
gence in the left inferior frontal gyrus in those with ASD inferior frontal gyrus could be the core abnormality under-
in comparison with the TD sample. This is consistent with mining inhibition abilities in individuals with ASD. In addi-
previous findings demonstrating that abnormalities in the tion, there were no differences in convergence between the
prefrontal areas undermined inhibitory ability in those with ASD sample and the TD sample for updating or switching.
ASD (Dumontheil et al. 2008; Kana et al. 2007; Schmitz Thus, EF abnormalities in those with ASD may manifest
et al. 2006). The abnormal activation could be due to inef- early in common EF, but with increasing age, they may
ficient effort or neuronal network recruitment in ASD (Rubia manifest in common EF as well as inhibition.
et al. 2006). Of note, in the present study, this difference in
convergence in the inferior frontal gyrus resided only in the Limitations
left hemisphere. On the contrary, in TD samples, the inhibi-
tion process was found to be mediated by the predominantly Some limitations to our meta-analysis should be considered.
right prefrontal areas (Rubia et al. 2006). Thus, this finding To investigate the construct of EF in those with ASD, we
may point toward abnormally left lateralization in individu- conducted an extensive ALE meta-analysis that was robust
als with ASD during inhibition (Paus et al. 1999). (McKenna et al. 2017). In the first-level analyses, conver-
It is also likely that this difference in convergence in the gence of activation for common EF and each of the three
left inferior frontal gyrus may reflect language deficits (e.g., specific processes was found in the young group, the adult
phonological deficits, semantics deficits, and communication group, and the whole group separately. Then, in the second-
disorders) in those with ASD (De Fossé et al. 2004; Tager- level analyses, brain areas commonly activated for each spe-
Flusberg and Joseph 2003). Language deficits are salient cific process compared with common EF was examined for
symptoms for explaining restrictive and repetitive behaviors the three groups separately. Since ALE analyses pool data
in individuals with ASD (Solomon et al. 2014). Previous across studies, they are very likely to include the same study
ASD studies have linked language deficits to abnormalities in common EF and specific process maps, which would lead
in the left inferior frontal gyrus (Hodge et al. 2010; Knaus to a bias in significant convergence. We therefore prevented
et al. 2008; Sharda et al. 2016). Within this context, our any individual study included in two first-level maps from
finding may suggest a unitary site in the left inferior frontal being compared in the second-level analyses. For exam-
gyrus that underlies both language and inhibition deficits in ple, to reveal inhibition-specific activation, the ALE map
those with ASD. for inhibition was compared with that for “common EF”
Regarding age effects, within the ASD sample, we found (updating and switching), which was defined as including
a difference in convergence in the left inferior frontal gyrus all EF processes except inhibition. Corresponding analyses
in the adult group compared with the young group, which were also administered for updating and switching. How-
could indicate that adults with ASD were more likely to ever, due to the small number of qualified studies, some of
recruit this brain area to solve inhibition tasks. On the other these analyses could not be conducted, because at least two
hand, comparing ASD and TD samples, we found a differ- studies must be included in each ALE analysis (Eickhoff
ence in convergence in the right inferior frontal gyrus in et al. 2016). Further, the second-level analyses did not yield
TD adults but not in young individuals. These results, taken any process-specific activation. Thus, our findings on the
together, reveal a lack of significant difference in inhibition construct of EF in those with ASD across ages may have
between children/adolescents with and without ASD, but in been underpowered. Future research should consider recruit-
adults, this difference was becoming pronounced. Thus, we ing sufficient data to clarify the construct of EF in those with
suggest that for those with ASD, abnormalities in inhibitory ASD using this approach.
function may not be manifest until adulthood. Second, it is important to acknowledge issues with the sta-
tistical approach used in this study. ALE, as well as many other
Updating and Switching coordinate-based neuroimaging meta-analytic approaches,
examines spatial convergence of brain effects across individual
The ALE results showed no age-related differences for neuroimaging studies in the null space of random spatial con-
updating and switching within the ASD sample or between vergence (Rottschy et al. 2012). This implies that significant
the two samples (ASD vs. TD). As we have discussed ALE effects should be interpreted as reflecting brain areas
above, these results could owe to limited data in the present commonly associated with a cognitive process across studies

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

but not as increases or decreases in activation. Similarly, the Conclusions and Implications for Future ASD
contrasts between coordinate-based meta-analyses reveal dif- Studies
ferences in convergence of brain activation between groups but
not convergence of activation in differences between groups. With all these limitations in mind, the present meta-anal-
Since image-based meta-analyses and effect-size signed dif- ysis of neuroimaging data does suggest that the EF con-
ferential mapping do not own these limitations (Radua et al. struct for ASD is unitary in the early stage but unitary and
2012; Salimi-Khorshidi et al. 2009), they provide possibilities partially diverse in later development. Even when com-
for researchers to further discuss EF abnormalities (patterns of pared with TD controls, those with ASD show relatively
increases vs. decreases) in ASD. stable EF abnormalities across development. However, the
Third, we originally aimed to study a wide range of ASD, fractionation of EF abnormalities into specific EF pro-
including high-functioning and other types of ASD. However, cesses is partially supported (mainly in inhibition).
the 33 EF studies included in this meta-analysis focused only These findings may have implications for theory and
on high-functioning ASD. Thus, whether our findings would practice. Theoretically, our findings provide some evi-
generalize across the whole spectrum of individuals with ASD dence that the integrative EF model (Friedman and Miyake
(e.g., individuals with “typical” autism—those with learn- 2017) is partially applicable to those with ASD. Specifi-
ing disabilities and developmental language delay) remains cally, the EF construct and EF abnormalities have a uni-
unresolved. tary and diverse nature in those with ASD, showing more
Fourth, we planned to divide the age groups into several unitary early but becoming more separable with age.
age groups, including children (< 12 years old), adolescents Practically, our findings show that in addition to using
(> 12 & < 18 years old), young adults (> 18 & < 45 years old), behavioral measures, evaluations of neural changes in EF-
middle-aged adults (> 45 & < 65 years old), and older adults related brain areas may provide evidence for the effective-
(< 65 years old). However, owing to the small number of neu- ness of EF interventions for individuals with ASD. Our
roscience EF studies conducted in children or adolescents, findings could not allow any conclusions about increased
we combined children and adolescents into a young group. In or decreased activation in brain areas. Thus, future
addition, no study was conducted in older adults. The exist- research could test what activation patterns (increases vs.
ing brain imaging studies are not well suited to address the decreases) represent training-induced changes in ASD.
important question of whether there are differential brain Brain activity changes in these areas could be indices to
effects across children, adolescents, young adults, middle-aged assess improvements in EF abilities and even the magni-
adults, and older adults. More neuroscience studies associated tude of transfer-related effects on clinical symptoms in
with age would allow a closer look at the neural correlates of those with ASD after EF intervention.
EF processes in ASD across development. Moreover, the establishment of the role of EF in those
Finally, behavioral research suggests that the measurement with ASD in our meta-analysis sheds light for interven-
of EF may make it difficult to explain age-related features of tions in this population. We suggest that any interven-
the EF construct. That is, it is widely acknowledged that EF tion directly targeting the improvement of EF abilities
measures suffer from validity issues such that any tasks tap- by using EF tasks for training may need to consider the
ping a specific EF process often measure other EF processes developmental construct of EF in those with ASD. That
as well. Such validity problems in EF measurement are par- is, early EF interventions may need to consider improv-
ticularly salient among young children, possibly because these ing general EF abilities (common EF), and types of EF
EF tasks are often cognitively demanding (e.g., to perform training tasks may not necessarily moderate intervention
a switching task, besides using switching resources, children outcomes. In contrast, for EF interventions in adults with
often actively use more working memory/updating and inhibi- ASD, researchers might consider programs that not only
tion resources to remember/use switching rules than do adults; consist of training different EF processes but also a spe-
Jacob and Parkinson 2015). Our findings of unclear age effects cific EF process (e.g., inhibition) associated with the tar-
on some EF processes in those with ASD should also be inter- geted individual’s EF behavior.
preted with caution with respect to the validity of EF measures With respect to methodology, all 33 studies included
in young children. in this meta-analysis reported results of whole-brain
analyses. Our search for studies excluded 13 studies that
reported significant effects only in ROI analyses. Of these
13 studies, most defined the inferior frontal gyrus and
anterior cingulate gyrus as ROIs. The left supplementary
motor area and the left inferior parietal gyrus revealed in
our present study were discussed less in the studies using

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

the ROI analyses. Additional research on brain activity California Department of Developmental Services. (2019). Fact
and functional connectivity should enable more extensive book: Fiscal year 2017–2018, Sixteenth edition. Retrieved
August 30, 2019, from https​://www3.dds.ca.gov/Facts​Stats​/
examination of the functional roles of these brain areas in docs/DDS_FactB​ook_2019.pdf
the neural basis of EF abnormalities in those with ASD. Chan, A. S., Han, Y. M., Leung, W. W. M., Leung, C., Wong, V. C.,
& Cheung, M. C. (2011). Abnormalities in the anterior cingu-
late cortex associated with attentional and inhibitory control
Author Contributions  ZZ conceived of the study, participated in the deficits: A neurophysiological study on children with autism
coordination of the study, performed literature search, data extraction, spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
coding, and statistical analysis, participated in the interpretation of the 5, 254–266.
data, and drafted the manuscript; PP participated in the coordination of Chantiluke, K., Barrett, N., Giampietro, V., Santosh, P., Brammer, M.,
the study, contributed to the interpretation of the data, and helped draft Simmons, A., et al. (2015). Inverse fluoxetine effects on inhibi-
the manuscript; DZ helped draft the manuscript. All authors have read tory brain activation in non-comorbid boys with ADHD and with
and approved the final manuscript. ASD. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 232, 2071–2082.
Chiu, Y. C., & Egner, T. (2015). Inhibition-induced forgetting: When
more control leads to less memory. Psychological Science, 26,
Funding  No funding was received for this study. 27–38.
Christou, C., Spanoudis, G., Demetriou, A., & Platsidou, M. (Eds.).
Compliance with Ethical Standards  (2002). The development of mental processing: Efficiency, work-
ing memory, and thinking. Boston, MA: Blackwell.
Conflict of interest  The authors declare no conflicts of interest. Church, J. A., Bunge, S. A., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2017).
Preparatory engagement of cognitive control networks increases
Ethical Approval  All procedures in this meta-analysis were in accord- late in childhood. Cerebral Cortex, 27, 2139–2153.
ance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national Craig, F., Margari, F., Legrottaglie, A. R., Palumbi, R., De Giambat-
research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its tista, C., & Margari, L. (2016). A review of executive function
later amendments or comparable ethical standards. deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyper-
activation disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12,
1191–1202.
Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gerns-
bacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and
the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuro-
References science, 8, 519–526.
Daly, E., Ecker, C., Hallahan, B., Deeley, Q., Craig, M., Murphy, C.,
Amaral, D. G., Schumann, C. M., & Nordahl, C. W. (2008). Neuro- et al. (2014). Response inhibition and serotonin in autism: A
anatomy of autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 31, 137–145. functional MRI study using acute tryptophan depletion. Brain,
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical 137, 2600–2610.
manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, and
Psychiatric Association. the prevention of autism spectrum disorder. Development and
Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: Psychopathology, 20, 775–803.
Developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Dawson, E., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (2002). Motivated reasoning
Biological Psychiatry, 69, e55–e68. and performance on the Wason selection task. Personality and
Baggetta, P., & Alexander, P. A. (2016). Conceptualization and opera- Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1379–1387.
tionalization of executive function. Mind, Brain, and Education, De Fossé, L., Hodge, S. M., Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., Caviness, V.
10, 10–33. S., Jr., McGrath, L., et al. (2004). Language-association cortex
Barkley, R. A. (2012). Executive functions: What they are, how they asymmetry in autism and specific language impairment. Annals
work, and why they evolved. New York: Guilford Press. of Neurology, 56, 757–766.
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on De Vries, M., & Geurts, H. (2015). Influence of autism traits and exec-
executive function. Child Development, 81, 1641–1660. utive functioning on quality of life in children with an autism
Bock, O., & Girgenrath, M. (2006). Relationship between sensorimotor spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
adaptation and cognitive functions in younger and older subjects. orders, 45, 2734–2743.
Experimental Brain Research, 169, 400–406. Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychol-
Bonini, L., Rozzi, S., Serventi, F. U., Simone, L., Ferrari, P. F., & ogy, 64, 135–168.
Fogassi, L. (2009). Ventral premotor and inferior parietal cortices Dinomais, M., Minassian, A. T., Tuilier, T., Delion, M., Wilke, M.,
make distinct contribution to action organization and intention N’Guyen, S., et al. (2009). Functional MRI comparison of pas-
understanding. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1372–1385. sive and active movement: Possible inhibitory role of supplemen-
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict moni- tary motor area. NeuroReport, 20, 1351–1355.
toring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cogni- Duan, X., Wei, S., Wang, G., & Shi, J. (2010). The relationship between
tive Sciences, 8, 539–546. executive functions and intelligence on 11-to 12-year-old chil-
Brázdil, M., Mikl, M., Mareček, R., Krupa, P., & Rektor, I. (2007). dren. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 52, 419–431.
Effective connectivity in target stimulus processing: A dynamic Dumontheil, I., Burgess, P. W., & Blakemore, S. J. (2008). Develop-
causal modeling study of visual oddball task. NeuroImage, 35, ment of rostral prefrontal cortex and cognitive and behavioural
827–835. disorders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 50,
Brydges, C. R., Fox, A. M., Reid, C. L., & Anderson, M. (2014). The 168–181.
differentiation of executive functions in middle and late child- Edgin, J. O., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Spatial cognition in autism
hood: A longitudinal latent-variable analysis. Intelligence, 47, spectrum disorders: Superior, impaired, or just intact? Journal of
34–43. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 729–745.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Grefkes, C., Wang, L. E., Zilles, K., & attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. NeuroImage:
Fox, P. T. (2009). Coordinate-based activation likelihood esti- Clinical, 4, 53–63.
mation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: A random-effects Jacob, R., & Parkinson, J. (2015). The potential for school-based
approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. interventions that target executive function to improve academic
Human Brain Mapping, 30, 2907–2926. achievement: A review. Review of Educational Research, 85,
Eickhoff, S. B., Nichols, T. E., Laird, A. R., Hoffstaedter, F., Amunts, 512–552.
K., Fox, P. T., et al. (2016). Behavior, sensitivity, and power of Jarrold, C., & Brock, J. (2004). To match or not to match? Methodo-
activation likelihood estimation characterized by massive empiri- logical issues in autism-related research. Journal of Autism and
cal simulation. NeuroImage, 137, 70–85. Developmental Disorders, 34, 81–86.
Fatemi, S. H., Halt, A. R., Stary, J. M., Kanodia, R., Schulz, S. C., & Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2007). The parieto-frontal integration the-
Realmuto, G. R. (2002). Glutamic acid decarboxylase 65 and ory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence.
67 kDa proteins are reduced in autistic parietal and cerebellar Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 135–154.
cortices. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 805–810. Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2007). Inhibi-
Fitzgerald, K. D., Zbrozek, C. D., Welsh, R. C., Britton, J. C., Liber- tory control in high-functioning autism: Decreased activation and
zon, I., & Taylor, S. F. (2008). Pilot study of response inhibition underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biological Psychiatry,
and error processing in the posterior medial prefrontal cortex 62, 198–206.
in healthy youth. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Karr, J. E., Areshenkoff, C. N., Rast, P., Hofer, S. M., Iverson, G.
49, 986–994. L., & Garcia-Barrera, M. A. (2018). The unity and diversity of
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of execu- executive functions: A systematic review and re-analysis of latent
tive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive variable studies. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 1147–1185.
structure. Cortex, 86, 186–204. Kim, C., Kroger, J. K., & Kim, J. (2011). A functional dissociation
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., of conflict processing within anterior cingulate cortex. Human
Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human corti- Brain Mapping, 32, 304–312.
cal development during childhood through early adulthood. Pro- Knaus, T. A., Silver, A. M., Lindgren, K. A., Hadjikhani, N., & Tager-
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 8174–8179. Flusberg, H. (2008). fMRI activation during a language task in
Gooskens, B., Bos, D. J., Mensen, V. T., Shook, D. A., Bruchhage, adolescents with ASD. Journal of the International Neuropsy-
M. M., Naaijen, J., et al. (2019). No evidence of differences in chological Society, 14, 967–979.
cognitive control in children with autism spectrum disorder or Koshino, H., Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N.
obsessive-compulsive disorder: An fMRI study. Developmental J., & Just, M. A. (2007). fMRI investigation of working memory
Cognitive Neuroscience, 36, 100602. for faces in autism: Visual coding and underconnectivity with
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail- frontal areas. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 289–300.
focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Lancaster, J. L., Tordesillas-Gutiérrez, D., Martinez, M., Salinas,
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25. F., Evans, A., Zilles, K., et al. (2007). Bias between MNI and
Hazeltine, E., Poldrack, R., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2000). Neural activation Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain tem-
during response competition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, plate. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 1194–1205.
12(Supplement 2), 118–129. Langen, M., Leemans, A., Johnston, P., Ecker, C., Daly, E., Murphy, C.
Herrington, J. D., Riley, M. E., Grupe, D. W., & Schultz, R. T. (2015). M., et al. (2012). Fronto-striatal circuitry and inhibitory control
Successful face recognition is associated with increased pre- in autism: Findings from diffusion tensor imaging tractography.
frontal cortex activation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Cortex, 48, 183–193.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 902–910. Lawson, R. A., Papadakis, A. A., Higginson, C. I., Barnett, J. E., Wills,
Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive M. C., Strang, J. F., et al. (2015). Everyday executive function
Sciences, 8, 26–32. impairments predict comorbid psychopathology in autism spec-
Hodge, S. M., Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., Caviness, V. S., Howard, J., trum and attention deficit hyperactivation disorders. Neuropsy-
McGrath, L., et al. (2010). Cerebellum, language, and cognition chology, 29, 445–453.
in autism and specific language impairment. Journal of Autism Lee, K., Bull, R., & Ho, R. M. (2013). Developmental changes in exec-
and Developmental Disorders, 40, 300–316. utive functioning. Child Development, 84, 1933–1953.
Houdé, O., Rossi, S., Lubin, A., & Joliot, M. (2010). Mapping numeri- Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimen-
cal processing, reading, and executive functions in the develop- sions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British
ing brain: An fMRI meta-analysis of 52 studies including 842 Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59–80.
children. Developmental Science, 13, 876–885. Lerner, M. D., & Lonigan, C. J. (2014). Executive function among
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age- preschool children: Unitary versus distinct abilities. Journal of
related change in executive function: Developmental trends and Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36, 626–639.
a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017–2036. Lever, A. G., Werkle-Bergner, M., Brandmaier, A. M., Ridderink-
Hyder, F., Phelps, E. A., Wiggins, C. J., Labar, K. S., Blamire, A. M., & hof, K. R., & Geurts, H. M. (2015). Atypical working memory
Shulman, R. G. (1997). “Willed action”: A functional MRI study decline across the adult lifespan in autism spectrum disorder?
of the human prefrontal cortex during a sensorimotor task. Pro- Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124, 1014–1026.
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 6989–6994. Liss, M., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., Morris, R., et al.
Ito, T. A., Friedman, N. P., Bartholow, B. D., Correll, J., Loersch, C., (2001). Executive functioning in high-functioning children with
Altamirano, L. J., et al. (2015). Toward a comprehensive under- autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and
standing of executive cognitive function in implicit racial bias. Allied Disciplines, 42, 261–270.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 187–218. Loukusa, S., Mäkinen, L., Kuusikko-Gauffin, S., Ebeling, H., &
Ishii-Takahashi, A., Takizawa, R., Nishimura, Y., Kawakubo, Y., Kuwa- Moilanen, I. (2014). Theory of mind and emotion recognition
bara, H., Matsubayashi, J., et al. (2014). Prefrontal activation skills in children with specific language impairment, autism
during inhibitory control measured by near-infrared spectros- spectrum disorder and typical development: Group differences
copy for differentiating between autism spectrum disorders and and connection to knowledge of grammatical morphology,

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

word-finding abilities and verbal working memory. Interna- Ozonoff, S., Cook, I., Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R. M., Klin,
tional Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49, A., et al. (2004). Performance on Cambridge Neuropsycho-
498–507. logical Test Automated Battery subtests sensitive to frontal
Luna, B., Doll, S. K., Hegedus, S. J., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, J. A. lobe function in people with autistic disorder: Evidence from
(2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological the Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism network.
Psychiatry, 61, 474–481. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 139–150.
Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., Garver, K. E., Lazar, N. A., Thulborn, K. R., Paus, T., Zijdenbos, A., Worsley, K., Collins, D. L., Blumenthal,
Eddy, W. F., et al. (2002). Neocortical system abnormalities in J., Giedd, J. N., et al. (1999). Structural maturation of neural
autism: An fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology, pathways in children and adolescents: In vivo study. Science,
59, 834–840. 283, 1908–1911.
Lynch, C. J., Breeden, A. L., You, X., Ludlum, R., Gaillard, W. D., Perry, W., Minassian, A., Lopez, B., Maron, L., & Lincoln, A.
Kenworthy, L., et al. (2017). Executive dysfunction in autism (2007). Sensorimotor gating deficits in adults with autism.
spectrum disorder is associated with a failure to modulate fron- Biological Psychiatry, 61, 482–486.
toparietal-insular hub architecture. Biological Psychiatry: Cogni- Philip, R. C., Dauvermann, M. R., Whalley, H. C., Baynham, K.,
tive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2, 537–545. Lawrie, S. M., & Stanfield, A. C. (2012). A systematic review
Maximo, J. O., Cadena, E. J., & Kana, R. K. (2014). The implications and meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spec-
of brain connectivity in the neuropsychology of autism. Neu- trum disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36,
ropsychology Review, 24, 16–31. 901–942.
McAlonan, G. M., Cheung, V., Chua, S. E., Oosterlaan, J., Hung, S.-F., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., Phillips, M. L., El-Hage, W., Kronhaus,
Tang, C.-P., et al. (2009). Age-related grey matter volume cor- D. M., Cardoner, N., et al. (2012). A new meta-analytic method
relates of response inhibition and shifting in attention-deficit for neuroimaging studies that combines reported peak coordi-
hyperactivity disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 194, nates and statistical parametric maps. European Psychiatry,
123–129. 27, 605–611.
McKenna, R., Rushe, T., & Woodcock, K. A. (2017). Informing the Redcay, E., & Courchesne, E. (2005). When is the brain enlarged in
structure of executive function in children: A meta-analysis of autism? A meta-analysis of all brain size reports. Biological
functional neuroimaging data. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, Psychiatry, 58, 1–9.
11, 154. Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system.
Merriam, E. P., Genovese, C. R., & Colby, C. L. (2003). Spatial updat- Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.
ing in human parietal cortex. Neuron, 39, 361–373. Rødgaard, E.-M., Jensen, K., Vergnes, J.-N., Soulières, I., & Mot-
Mosconi, M. W., Kay, M., D’Cruz, A. M., Seidenfeld, A., Guter, S., tron, L. (2019). Temporal changes in effect sizes of studies
Stanford, L. D., et al. (2009). Impaired inhibitory control is asso- comparing individuals with and without autism: A meta-anal-
ciated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum ysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76, 1124–1132.
disorders. Psychological Medicine, 39, 1559–1566. Rosenthal, M., Wallace, G. L., Lawson, R., Wills, M. C., Dixon, E.,
Mosconi, M. W., & Sweeney, J. A. (2015). Sensorimotor dysfunctions Yerys, B. E., et al. (2013). Impairments in real-world execu-
as primary features of autism spectrum disorders. Science China tive function increase from childhood to adolescence in autism
Life Sciences, 58, 1016–1023. spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology, 27, 13–18.
Mosconi, M. W., Takarae, Y., & Sweeney, J. A. (2011). Motor impair- Rothman, K. J., Greenland, S., & Lash, T. L. (2008). Modern epide-
ments and dyspraxia in autism. In D. G. Amaral, G. Dawson, & miology. Philadelphia, PA: Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott
D. H. Geschwind (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders (pp. 355– Williams & Wilkins.
380). New York: Oxford University Press. Rottschy, C., Langner, R., Dogan, I., Reetz, K., Laird, A. R., Schulz,
Mostert-Kerckhoffs, M. A., Staal, W. G., Houben, R. H., & de Jonge, J. B., et al. (2012). Modelling neural correlates of working
M. V. (2015). Stop and change: Inhibition and flexibility skills memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 60,
are related to repetitive behavior in children and young adults 830–846.
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Devel- Rubia, K., Smith, A. B., Woolley, J., Nosarti, C., Heyman, I., Tay-
opmental Disorders, 45, 3148–3158. lor, E., et al. (2006). Progressive increase of frontostriatal
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). brain activation from childhood to adulthood during event-
Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight related tasks of cognitive control. Human Brain Mapping, 27,
principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Develop- 973–993.
mental Disorders, 36, 27–43. Russo, N., Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P. D., &
Müller, V. I., Cieslik, E. C., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Radua, J., Mataix- Burack, J. A. (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among
Cols, D., et al. (2018). Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta- persons with autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience.
analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 84, 151–161. Brain and Cognition, 65, 77–86.
Nee, D. E., Brown, J. W., Askren, M. K., Berman, M. G., Demiralp, E., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Smith, S. M., Keltner, J. R., Wager, T. D., &
Krawitz, A., et al. (2012). A meta-analysis of executive compo- Nichols, T. E. (2009). Meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: A
nents of working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 264–282. comparison of image-based and coordinate-based pooling of
Niendam, T. A., Laird, A. R., Ray, K. L., Dean, Y. M., Glahn, D. C., studies. NeuroImage, 45, 810–823.
& Carter, C. S. (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superor- Scheeren, A. M., de Rosnay, M., Koot, H. M., & Begeer, S. (2013).
dinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive Rethinking theory of mind in high-functioning autism spec-
functions. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, trum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54,
12, 241–268. 628–635.
O’Hearn, K., Asato, M., Ordaz, S., & Luna, B. (2008). Neurodevelop- Scherf, K. S., Sweeney, J. A., & Luna, B. (2006). Brain basis of devel-
ment and executive function in autism. Development and Psy- opmental change in visuospatial working memory. Journal of
chopathology, 20, 1103–1132. Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1045–1058.
Ohnishi, T., Matsuda, H., Hashimoto, T., Kunihiro, T., Nishikawa, M., Schmitz, N., Rubia, K., Daly, E., Smith, A., Williams, S., & Murphy,
Uema, T., et al. (2000). Abnormal regional cerebral blood flow D. G. (2006). Neural correlates of executive function in autistic
in childhood autism. Brain, 123, 1838–1844. spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 7–16.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Senn, T. E., Espy, K. A., & Kaufmann, P. M. (2004). Using path analy- van den Bergh, S. F., Scheeren, A. M., Begeer, S., Koot, H. M., &
sis to understand executive function organization in preschool Geurts, H. M. (2014). Age related differences of executive func-
children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 445–464. tioning problems in everyday life of children and adolescents
Shafritz, K. M., Bregman, J. D., Ikuta, T., & Szeszko, P. R. (2015). in the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Neural systems mediating decision-making and response inhi- Disorders, 44, 1959–1971.
bition for social and nonsocial stimuli in autism. Progress in van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive
Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 60, functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading,
112–120. and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35, 427–449.
Shafritz, K. M., Dichter, G. S., Baranek, G. T., & Belger, A. (2008). Vara, A. S., Pang, E. W., Doyle-Thomas, K. A., Vidal, J., Taylor, M. J.,
The neural circuitry mediating shifts in behavioral response and & Anagnostou, E. (2014). Is inhibitory control a “no-go” in ado-
cognitive set in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 974–980. lescents with autism spectrum disorder? Molecular Autism, 5, 6.
Sharda, M., Khundrakpam, B. S., Evans, A. C., & Singh, N. C. (2016). Vaughan, L., & Giovanello, K. (2010). Executive function in daily life:
Disruption of structural covariance networks for language in Age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental
autism is modulated by verbal ability. Brain Structure and Func- activities of daily living. Psychology and Aging, 25, 343–355.
tion, 221, 1017–1032. Vogan, V. M., Morgan, B. R., Lee, W., Powell, T. L., Smith, M. L., &
Sheridan, P. L., & Hausdorff, J. M. (2007). The role of higher-level Taylor, M. J. (2014). The neural correlates of visuo-spatial work-
cognitive function in gait: Executive dysfunction contributes to ing memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: Effects of
fall risk in Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cogni- cognitive load. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6, 19.
tive Disorders, 24, 125–137. Wallace, G. L., Budgett, J., & Charlton, R. A. (2016). Aging and autism
Smith, A. B., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., & Rubia, K. (2004). Neural spectrum disorder: Evidence from the broad autism phenotype.
correlates of switching set as measured in fast, event-related Autism Research, 9, 1294–1303.
functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping, Wang, L., Liu, X., Guise, K. G., Knight, R. T., Ghajar, J., & Fan,
21, 247–256. J. (2010). Effective connectivity of the frontoparietal network
Smolker, H. R., Depue, B. E., Reineberg, A. E., Orr, J. M., & Banich, during attentional control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
M. T. (2015). Individual differences in regional prefrontal gray 22, 543–553.
matter morphometry and fractional anisotropy are associated Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A., Chevalier, N., &
with different constructs of executive function. Brain Structure Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-
and Function, 220, 1291–1306. olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 436–452.
Solomon, M., Yoon, J. H., Ragland, J. D., Niendam, T. A., Lesh, T. A., Yaple, Z., & Arsalidou, M. (2018). N-back working memory task:
Fairbrother, W., et al. (2014). The development of the neural sub- Meta-analysis of normative fMRI studies with children. Child
strates of cognitive control in adolescents with autism spectrum Development, 89, 2010–2022.
disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 76, 412–421. Yerys, B. E., Antezana, L., Weinblatt, R., Jankowski, K. F., Strang, J.,
St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive func- Vaidya, C. J., et al. (2015). Neural correlates of set-shifting in
tions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, children with autism. Autism Research, 8, 386–397.
and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy- Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function
chology, 59, 745–759. in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive Development Perspectives, 6, 354–360.
phenotypes in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and
Society of London: Series B Biological Sciences, 358, 303–314. atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook
Takarae, Y., Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (2004). Ocu- of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Malden, MA:
lomotor abnormalities parallel cerebellar histopathology in Blackwell Publishing.
autism. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,
75, 1359–1361. Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
Takarae, Y., Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Atypi- jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
cal involvement of frontostriatal systems during sensorimotor
control in autism. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 156,
117–127.
Turkeltaub, P. E., Eden, G. F., Jones, K. M., & Zeffiro, T. A. (2002).
Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word
reading: method and validation. NeuroImage, 16, 765–780.

13

You might also like