You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Environmental Psychology 44 (2015) 119e125

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Environmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

The value-belief-norm theory, personal norms and sustainable travel


mode choice in urban areas
Hans Brende Lind a, Trond Nordfjærn b, Stig Halvard Jørgensen c, Torbjørn Rundmo a, *
a
Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, 7491 Trondheim, Norway
b
NTNU Social Research Studio Apertura, 7491 Trondheim, Norway
c
Department of Geography, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, 7491 Trondheim, Norway

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: To examine determinants of environmentally significant transport behaviour could contribute to the
Received 25 August 2014 development of efforts aimed to influence people's travel mode choice in a sustainable manner. Travel
Received in revised form mode choices are especially interesting in urban areas where sustainable public transportation is
26 May 2015
available. The aim of this study is to investigate whether the value-belief-norm theory can explain re-
Accepted 2 June 2015
Available online 30 July 2015
ported travel mode change in the Norwegian urban population. The results are based on a self-
completion questionnaire survey carried out among representative samples in six urban areas in Nor-
way (n ¼ 1043). Using structural equation modelling, values and beliefs explained 58 per cent of the
Keywords:
Travel mode choice
variance in personal norms. Three groups of transport mode users were identified: Frequent users of
Urban areas public transportation, frequent users of a car, and a group of people frequently walking or using bicycle.
Value-belief-norm theory Both personal norms and situational factors were found to be significant predictors of reported travel
Situational factors mode choice.
© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction significant behaviour due to its potential impact on the environ-


ment (Stern, 2000). However, there is no clear-cut definition of
A reduction in the number of journeys made by car and other what constitutes environmentally friendly travel mode choice.
motorised private travel modes are important for reducing traffic Virtually all travel modes involve the use of some amount of energy,
accidents, air pollution and other negative environmental effects in materials and land use. The current study assumes that the choice
urban areas. Globally, emissions from motor vehicles contribute of public transportation and active transportation (walking and
substantially to the release of greenhouse gas and other pollutants. cycling) can be defined as sustainable compared to car use in terms
Within the European Union, road transport generates approxi- of environmental impact. In urban areas the public more often has
mately twenty per cent of the total CO2 emissions (Commission of the option to choose between public transportation and private
the European Communities, 2007). Increases in motor vehicle travel modes compared to the public in rural areas. Consequently,
density have resulted in a total increase of 25 per cent in CO2 values and normative factors may to a larger extent influence on
emissions between 1990 and 2004. Motor vehicle density is espe- travel mode choice in urban areas and therefore it is especially
cially a problem in urban areas and the population density is relevant to examine the associations between such factors and
currently increasing. Transport accidents and injuries occur travel choices use in the urban public.
frequently in urban areas and they are partly related to population
density and expanding mobility needs. If the current level of pri-
ority given to countermeasures remains stable, the relative number 1.1. Value-belief-norm theory of environmentalism
of environmentally negative effects may be expected to increase in
the near future. According to (Stern and Dietz, 1994; Stern, Dietz, Abel,
Travel mode choices can be conceived as an environmentally Guagnano, & Kalof, 1999, 2000) choosing environmentally
friendly modes of transportation can be based on a sense of moral
obligation to act in a sustainable fashion. Stern's value-belief-norm
* Corresponding author. theory of environmentalism (VBN theory; Stern, 2000; Stern, et. al.,
E-mail address: torbjorn.rundmo@svt.ntnu.no (T. Rundmo). 1999) provides a framework for investigating normative factors

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.06.001
0272-4944/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
120 H.B. Lind et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 44 (2015) 119e125

that promote sustainable attitudes and behaviour. The theory in- attitudes and behaviour related to transport mode choice. De
tegrates value theory (Schwartz, 1992), the new environmental Groot and Steg (2009) found that awareness of the environ-
paradigm perspective (NEP; Dunlap, Van Liere, Mertig, & Jones, mental consequences of car choice and feelings of responsibility
2000), and norm-activation theory (NAM; Schwartz, 1977). It sug- for these consequences were related to a sense of moral obligation
gests a chain of variables, from values and general environmental to reduce car use, which in turn was associated with acceptance of
concern to specific beliefs about the negative consequences of transport policies increasing the cost associated with car use.
certain actions and the individuals' ability and responsibility to Eriksson, Garvill and Nordlund (2006, 2008) found that accept-
avert these negative consequences, which in turn activates sus- ability of different transport policy measures was related to sus-
tainable personal norms for behaviour. tainable orientation, problem awareness and personal norms.
Schwartz (1992, p. 21) has defined a value as “a desirable Results based on data from five European countries showed that
transsituational goal varying in importance, which serves as a the VBN theory predicted an acceptable percentage of the
guiding principle in the life of a person or other social entity”. acceptability of road pricing, as well as the intention to reduce
Values are thought to influence behaviour indirectly, through own car use when such a policy are implemented (De Groot &
directing attention towards value congruent information and Steg, 2008). Comparing variables from the theory of planned
influencing the perception of this information. Based on Schwartz behaviour (Ajzen, 1985) and norm activation theory, Abrahmse
(1992) value survey, De Groot and Steg (2007, 2008) have devel- et al. (2009) showed that car choice for commuting was chiefly
oped an instrument that measures value types believed to be explained by perceived behavioural control and attitudes, while
particularly important precursors of environmental beliefs and the intention to reduce car use was explained by personal norms.
behaviours. The instrument includes biospheric, egoistic and The VBN theory was also found to be suitable in Argentina, thus
altruistic value orientations. It has later been expanded to also indicating that the theory is not culture specific (Jakovcevic &
include hedonistic values (Steg, Perlaviciute, van der Werff, & Steg, 2013). In addition to testing the model in a Norwegian
Lurvink, 2014). The new environmental paradigm (NEP) empha- context, it is also interesting to investigate the associations be-
sises beliefs in the limits of growth, the necessity of balancing tween personal norms and travel mode choice controlling for
economic growth with environmental protection, and the need to situational factors.
preserve the balance of nature (Dunlap & Van Liere, 1978; Dunlap
et al., 2000; Dunlap, 2008). The paradigm encompasses general
beliefs about the environment, awareness of environmental prob-
lems and acknowledgement of the need for sustainable develop- 1.3. Situational and contextual factors
ment. Condoning such beliefs has been found to be associated with
environmental beliefs and attitudes (Pierce, Dalton, & Zaitsev, 1999; Certain situational or contextual factors can constrain or
Stern, Dietz, & Guagnano, 1995). Several studies carried out during promote sustainable behaviour (e.g. Black et al., 1985; Tanner,
two decades have given evidence for the applicability of the NEP 1999). Relevant when considering choice of transport among
scale (see Dunlap et al., 2000). According to norm-activation theory commuters are costs and availability of different travel modes.
(Schwartz, 1977), pro-social behaviour, e.g. the choice of environ- Previous work has also included such situational factors when
mentally friendly travel modes, should occur as a response to examining the VBN-model. The costs associated with car
personal norms about such behaviour. The theory argues that be- ownership could constitute a strong constrain on the choice of
liefs about the environment influences the individuals' awareness transport mode, especially for individuals with low income. In
of the consequences (AC) related to specific behaviours and beliefs addition, large distances from home to work or to the nearest
about the ability to avert the noxious consequences of these be- point of public transportation can make the choice of public
haviours (ascription of responsibility, AR). Several studies have transportation difficult. Limtanakool, Dijst, and Schwanen (2006)
given support to the idea that norm-activation may be important showed that among medium- and longer-distance commuters,
for a variety of sustainable behaviours (Stern, Dietz, & Kalof, 1993; demographic characteristics and availability of transport modes
Guagnano, 1995; see also Stern et al., 1999). Beliefs focussing on were important factors in determining transport mode choice.
general environmental conditions and specific sustainable behav- They found that males relied more on private car than females,
iours have to be distinguished (Black, Stern, & Elworth, 1985; Stern higher education was associated with the choice of public
et al., 1999; Nordlund & Garvill, 2003). According to Steg, transportation, and that car availability had a strong negative
Dreijerink, and Abrahamse (2005) the predictive power of Value- influence on the likelihood of using public transportation. In one
Belief-Norm models enhance when AC and AR predict specific of the studies which adjusted for situational factors, Collin and
behaviour. These specific beliefs lead to activation of personal Chambers (2005) investigated the relative contribution of
norms and personal norms may influence various types of sus- values, beliefs and situational factors on commuter-transport
tainable behaviours (Stern, 2000; see also Stern et al., 1999). It also mode choice. They found that egoistic beliefs, costs and avail-
determines whether the individual feels morally obliged to act in ability were related to transport mode choice. Neither biospheric
environmentally friendly ways (Steg, 2005). nor altruistic values were found to be significantly associated
with transport mode choice when controlling for other variables.
1.2. VBN-theory and pro-envionmental behaviour e empirical External constraints may therefore play a role in determining
studies travel mode choice in addition to values, beliefs and personal
norms.
Several studies have given support to the applicability of the The main aim of the current study was to investigate the VBN-
value-belief-norm theory to predict various types of specific model applying structural equation modelling and to examine the
sustainable behaviours (Steg & Vlek, 2009). A study conducted relative importance of situational and psychological factors in
among 112 residents of the Dutch city of Groeningen showed that explaining travel mode choice among a sample of the Norwegian
the model explained acceptability of energy policies to reduce the urban public. The specific aims were: (1) to analyse predictors of
emission of CO2 by households (Steg et al., 2005). According to De personal norms, and (2) to examine the association between per-
Groot and Steg (2008), the fit of the VBN theory based model to sonal norms and travel mode choice, controlling for situational
the data was acceptable. VBN theory has been applied to predict factors.
H.B. Lind et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 44 (2015) 119e125 121

2. Method week, and how often they used each mode of transport, on a 6-
point scale from zero to five or more days per week. Transport
2.1. Procedure modes included in the study were train, tram, underground, bus,
automobile, bicycle and walking. The respondents were also asked
The results of the current study are based on a self-completion about the distance from place of residence to workplace (in kilo-
questionnaire survey carried out in year 2013 in a random repre- metres) and the walking distance from place of residence to the
sentative sample of the urban Norwegian population. The sample nearest point of public transportation. The questionnaire also
was obtained electronically by a firm with access to the Norwegian contained information about demographic characteristics of the
population registry. The six largest and most urbanised regions of respondents.
Norway were selected. Respondents living in these six regions were
randomly selected. The sample was restricted to individuals who 2.4. Statistical analysis
were 18 years or above. The urban regions included residents of the
central Oslo region in the urbanized south-eastern area of Norway, Expectation maximisation was used to deal with missing data.
of the municipalities of Skien and Porsgrunn area, of the central Cronbach's a were calculated to examine scale reliability and in-
Trondheim region in the mid-Norway, of the central Stavanger area, ternal consistency. To analyse associations between the scales,
of the central Bergen region in the south western part of the Pearson's r correlation coefficients were estimated. Confirmatory
country, and the municipality of Tromsø in northern Norway (see factor analysis (CFA) was used to assess the fit of the dimensional
Nordfjærn, Lind, Şimşekog lu, Jørgensen, & Rundmo, 2014 for structure of value orientation, NEP and specific beliefs and norms.
details). Structural equation modelling (SEM) using the covariance matrix
with maximum likelihood (ML) estimation was used to examine
2.2. Sample the structural relationship between the variables as proposed by
VBN theory. In the prediction model presented in the results' sec-
A total of 1043 individuals participated in the study (18% tion only the results of the structural model are shown. The fit
response rate). The sample consisted of 463 males (44%) and 579 between the model and data is assessed with the c2/df-ratio, Root
females (56%). The average age was 41.4 years (SD ¼ 12.1) and Means Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), as well tests of
ranged from 18 to 74 years. About 4% of the respondents had adaptability, i.e. Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and standardised root
completed basic education, 19% had completed vocational sec- mean squared residual (SRMR). Hierarchical and k-means cluster
ondary education, 13% had completed general secondary education, analyses were applied to identify travel mode use cluster groups.
22% had achieved a bachelor degree or equivalent, and 43% had Hierarchical cluster analysis was carried out to identify the ideal
achieved a masters' degree or equivalent. In general, there were no number of clusters. When the number of clusters was identified by
significant differences in demographic characteristics between the hierarchical cluster analysis, a k-means iterative partitioning
six subsamples. According to Nordfjærn et al. (2014) relatively low method was used. This made it possible to find the best cluster
response rates are common in transport population studies (see solution. Logistic block regression was used to predict travel mode
also Castanier, Paran, & Delhomme, 2012; Moan, 2013). In a random group belongingness on the basis of contextual and demographic
sample the research topic is salient only for a small part of the factors, values and beliefs and personal norms. Two models were
public. tested and investigated. The first aimed to predict choice of public
versus private travel modes and the second model tested use of
2.3. Measures active transportation (walking and cycling) versus private car.

Value orientation was assessed using a measure developed by 3. Results


De Groot and Steg (2007, 2008) (see also Steg et al., 2014). The scale
includes four items measuring biospheric values, four items 3.1. Reliability and fit-indices of measures
measuring altruistic values, three items measuring hedonic values
and five items measuring egoistic values. In the same manner as Descriptive statistics, reliability and internal consistency for the
described by Schwartz (1992), the respondents were asked to rate value dimensions, NEP, AC, AR and PN are shown in Table 1. All
the importance of each value item as “a guiding principle” in their scales showed initially satisfactory reliability and internal consis-
lives on a 9-point scale ranging from “extremely important” (7) to tency, with exception of the NEP-scale where one item was initially
“opposed to my principles” (1). removed due to low inter-item reliability.
General environmental beliefs were measured using 13 selected The value measure initially showed unsatisfactory fit-indices (c2/
items from the revised new environmental paradigm scale (NEP) df ¼ 7.29, CFI ¼ 0.92, RMSEA ¼ 0.078, SRMR ¼ 0.048). Inspecting the
(Dunlap, 2008; Dunlap et al., 2000). Responses were recorded on a residual covariances suggested that the ‘wealth’-item cross-loaded
5-point scale ranging from “highly disagree” (1) to “highly agree” on several dimensions, and was thus removed. The revised model
(5). Items measuring awareness of consequences (AC), ascription of showed acceptable fit-indices (c2/df ¼ 4.87, CFI ¼ 0.95,
responsibility (AR) and personal norms (PN) were partly adapted RMSEA ¼ 0.061, SRMR ¼ 0.048), and was used in further analyses.
from previous studies (De Groot & Steg, 2007; Abrahmse et al., The 12 remaining items of the NEP-scale showed acceptable fit (c2/
2009) and partly developed for this study in order to be relevant df ¼ 4.28, CFI ¼ 0.94, RMSEA ¼ 0.056, SRMR ¼ 0.040). Finally,
for Norwegian transport users. AR and PN were measured using modelling AC, AR and PN as three latent factors showed satisfactory
five items each, while AC was measured using six items. The re- fit for all included indices (c2/df ¼ 3.83, CFI ¼ 0.95, RMSEA ¼ 0.052,
spondents were asked to indicate their agreement with each SRMR ¼ 0.047).
statement on a 7-point Likert-type scale, ranging from “totally
disagree” (1) to “totally agree” (7). Table 1 shows the associations 3.2. Predictors of personal norms (the value-belief-norm theory)
between the indices of value orientation, The New Environmental
Paradigm, dimensions of norm activation, and personal norms. In accordance with previous studies, the initial model was
To measure transport use, respondents were asked to indicate specified with the hedonic (HED), biospheric (BIO), egoistic (EGO)
which modes of transport they used for work travels in an ordinary and altruistic (ALT) value orientations predicting the NEP, which in
122 H.B. Lind et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 44 (2015) 119e125

Table 1
Means, standard deviations (SD) Cronbach' alphas and pariwise correlation coefficients.

Mean SD Cronbach's a PN AC AR NEP EGO BIO

Personal norms (PN) 2.95 1.08 0.70


Awareness of consequences (AC) 3.23 1.19 0.80 0.74
Ascription of responsibility (AR) 5.16 1.14 0.79 0.69 0.51
New environmental paradigm (NEP) 3.57 0.49 0.77 0.69 0.66 0.73
Egoistic values (EGO) 2.51 1.37 0.77 ¡0.21 ¡0.22 ¡0.23 ¡0.34
Altruistic values (ALT) 5.06 1.39 0.81 0.47 0.32 0.47 0.44 0.12
Biospheric values (BIO) 4.55 1.51 0.88 0.62 0.43 0.52 0.66 ¡0.07 0.78
Hedonic values (HED) 4.28 1.36 0.78 ¡0.19 0.07 ¡0.09 0.10 0.46 0.21 0.12

Significant coefficients in bold (p < .05).

turn was allowed to predict AC and AR. AC was allowed to predict


AR and PN, while AR was allowed to predict PN. The initial model
showed acceptable fit indices (c2 ¼ 2806.26, df ¼ 873, c2/df ¼ 3.26,
CFI ¼ 0.90, RMSEA ¼ 0.046, SRMR ¼ 0.062). However, modification
indices suggested adding paths from BIO to PN, from HED to PN,
and from ALT to AR. The paths from ALT to NEP and from HED to
NEP were removed, as they were insignificant at the 95 per cent-
level. Additionally, when ALT and BIO were regressed on by the
same variable, it caused instability in the estimates, probably due to
multicollinearity. The final model can be seen in Fig. 1. Overall, the
fit indices of the model were within acceptable limits
(c2 ¼ 2609.32, df ¼ 871, c2/df ¼ 3.00, CFI ¼ 0.90, RMSEA ¼ 0.044,
SRMR ¼ 0.054). BIO (b ¼ 0.59) and EGO (b ¼ 0.25) was related to
NEP, explaining a total of 42 per cent of the variance. AC was pre-
dicted by NEP (b ¼ 0.55), with an explained variance of 30%. NEP
(b ¼ 0.45) and AC (b ¼ .21) explained 45 percent of the variance in
AR. Finally, 58 per cent of the variance in PN was explained by AC
(b ¼ 0.37), BIO (b ¼ 0.30), AR (b ¼ 0.28) and HED (b ¼ 0.15). All
regression estimates in the final model were significant at the 95
per cent-level. Fig. 2. Travel mode cluster groups (z-scores).

3.4. Predictors of transport group membership


3.3. Travel mode cluster groups
Applying these clusters as dependent variables, hierarchical
Cluster analysis was carried out to construct groups of re-
logistic regression was used to predict group membership (see
spondents based on the items in the questionnaire on z-scores
Table 2). In two separate analyses the car using-group was con-
regarding how often they used various modes of urban transport
trasted with the public transport-using group and active
(bus, train, tram, underground, car, walking, and bicycle) to and
transportation-group. The predictors were added in three blocks.
from place work or education. Using the Ward method, the hier-
Socio-demographic variables, walking distance to nearest public
archical cluster analysis indicated a discontinuity in the change of
transport and the distance between home and workplace or place
the coefficient between the third and fourth cluster, thus three
of education were entered in the first block. Distance to nearest
clusters were used in the subsequent k-means cluster analysis. As
public transport was not included in the model predicting mem-
can be seen in Fig. 2, the first cluster was characterised by using
bership to the cycling/walking-group as it was not relevant for
public modes of transportation, the second cluster by mainly using
these groups. PN was added in the second block, while AC, AR,
active transportation (walking or cycling), and the third cluster by
egoistic, altruistic, biospheric and hedonic values were added in the
using private car.
final block.
Due to the high observed correlations between values, beliefs
and personal norms, some degree of multicollinearity was ex-
pected. Though none of the variables showed variance inflation
factors above the usual recommended limit of 5e10, instability in
the estimates of the coefficients, inflated standard errors and high
average value inflation factors (VIF) were observed. NEP was found
to have a VIF of 4.4, and seemed to be responsible for the high
average VIF. It was therefore deemed necessary to exclude this
variable from the analysis.

3.4.1. Car use versus public transportation


The first block in the model predicting membership to either the
car use-group or the public transport use-group significantly pre-
dicted group membership (c2 ¼ 70.98, p < .001). Age (OR ¼ 0.97,
Fig. 1. Predictors of personal norms (results of SEM-analysis). p < .01), income (OR ¼ 0.69, p < .001), and distance from home to
H.B. Lind et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 44 (2015) 119e125 123

Table 2
Predictors of travel mode cluster group belongingness.

Car group vs.public transport group Car group vs. active transport group

B SEB eB B SEB eB

Block 1:
Intercept 1.12 0.50 0.71 0.43
Age 0.03** 0.01 0.97 0.01 0.01 0.99
Gender (ref. female) 0.49** 0.23 1.62 0.21 0.18 0.81
Education 0.13 0.09 1.14 0.28*** 0.08 1.32
Income 0.38*** 0.11 0.69 0.21 0.10 0.81
Distance home-work (min.) 0.00 0.01 1.00 0.13*** 0.02 0.88
Distance home-public transport (min.) 0.07*** 0.02 0.93
Model c2 70.89*** 117.30***
Cox & Snell's R2 0.12 0.16
Negelkerke's R2 0.17 0.21
Block 2:
Intercept 1.51 0.053 1.03
Age 0.03** 0.01 0.97 0.01 0.45 0.99
Gender (ref. female) 0.32 0.23 1.38 0.36 0.01 0.70
Education 0.07 0.09 1.07 0.21** 0.19 0.23
Income 0.35** 0.12 1.42 0.14 0.08 1.15
Distance home-work (min.) 0.00 0.01 1.00 0.11** 0.02 1.12
Distance home-public transport (min.) 0.07*** 0.02 1.07
Personal norms 0.68*** 0.13 1.97 0.61*** 0.10 1.84
Model x2 104.90*** 146.30***
Block x2 33.95*** 39.05***
Cox & Snell's R2 0.17 0.21
Negelkerke's R2 0.24 0.28
Block 3:
Intercept 1.14 0.58 1.10 0.49
Age 0.03* 0.01 0.97 0.02 0.01 0.99
Gender (ref. female) 0.41 0.25 1.51 0.26 0.21 0.77
Education 0.12 0.19 1.12 0.19 0.09 1.21
Income 0.41** 0.13 0.67 0.13 0.11 0.88
Distance home-work (min.) 0.00 0.01 1.00 0.11*** 0.12 0.90
Distance home-public transport (min.) 0.07*** 0.02 0.93 e e
Personal norms 1.29*** 0.24 3.65 0.60** 0.21 1.81
Awareness of consequences 0.12 0.24 0.89 0.60** 0.22 1.83
Ascription of responsibility 0.59* 0.29 0.55 0.53*** 0.26 0.59
Egoistic values 0.13 0.14 0.88 0.13 0.11 0.88
Altruistic values 0.09 0.16 0.91 0.03 0.15 1.03
Biospheric values 0.28 0.16 0.76 0.18 0.15 0.84
Hedonic values 0.27 0.13 1.32 0.05 0.11 1.05
Model X2 120.50*** 177.80***
Block x2 15.56* 21.49***
Cox & Snell's R2 0.20 0.23
Negelkerke's R2 0.28 0.31

nearest point of public transport were negatively related to being in kilometre increase in distance between home and workplace
the public transport-using group (OR ¼ 0.93, p < .001). Females decreased the chance of being in the active transport group by an
were more likely to be in the public transport-using group than odds ratio of 0.89 (p < .001). Age, gender and income were not
males (OR ¼ 1.63, p < .01). Education and distance from home to found to be significant. The model was significantly improved by
work were not significant predictors (see Table 2). Adding PN adding PN (Model c2 ¼ 156.31, p < .001, Block c2 ¼ 39.05, p < .001).
significantly improved the model (Model c2 ¼ 104.93, p < .001, A standard deviation difference in personal norms increased the
Block c2 ¼ 33.95, p < .001), and PN significantly predicted group chance of being in the group using active transportation with an
membership (OR ¼ 1.97, p < .001). Adding the remaining variables odds ratio of 1.84 (p < .001). The block with the remaining variables
further added to the explained variance, however only to a small from VBN theory added to the explained variance (Model
extent (Model c2 ¼ 120.49, p < .001, Block c2 ¼ 15.56, p < .05). c2 ¼ 177.80, p < .001, Block c2 ¼ 21.49, p < .001). AC was positively
Contrary to predictions, AR was a significant negative predictor associated, (OR ¼ 1.83, p < .01) and AR was negatively associated
(OR ¼ 0.55, p < .05) and hedonic value orientation was a significant (OR ¼ 0.59, p < .01) with being in the walking/cycling group. None
positive predictor (OR ¼ 1.32, p < .05) of membership in the public of the value dimensions showed significant direct effects on group
transport-using group. The remaining variables failed to reach membership.
significance.

4. Discussion
3.4.2. Car use versus walking and cycling
Similar results were attained when predicting membership The current results suggest that the value-belief-norm (VBN)
either in the car-using group or in the group walking or cycling. The theory (Stern, 2000; Stern et al., 1999) is successful in explaining
first block significantly predicted group membership (c2 ¼ 117.26, travel mode choice in a Norwegian urban public when controlling
p < .001). Higher level of education was positively associated with for situational factors. This is in accordance with Steg, Dreijerink
being in the walking/cycling group (OR ¼ 1.32, p < .001). A one and Abrahamse (2006). When personal norms were added to the
124 H.B. Lind et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 44 (2015) 119e125

model the percentage of explained variance significantly increased. specific factors reflecting strong feelings of moral obligation to
However, the increase in percentage of explained variance was sustainable travel mode choices and high ascription of re-
modest. The results also showed that contextual factors were sponsibility contributed to the choice of public transportation,
important. while low scores on these factors were more frequent among re-
spondents belonging to the group who most often choose private
4.1. Factors associated with personal norms motorised travel modes, and especially their own car for all sorts of
travels. Somewhat unexpected, however, ascription of re-
The first model aimed to examine whether or not values pre- sponsibility was negatively associated with using public trans-
dicted general environmental beliefs and specific beliefs about the portation, i.e. car users felt more responsible for averting the
consequences of car choice and how these factors were related to negative consequences of car use on the environment. It may be
personal norms. As shown the model fitted to the data. Almost 60 that those who most frequently choose public transportation did
per cent of the variance in personal norms was explained by the not feel the responsibility of negative consequences of car use
variables in the model. As expected, values (Schwartz, 1992) because they have already taken the responsibility through
significantly predicted environmental beliefs as measured by the behavioural action. However, frequent car users may feel respon-
new environmental paradigm (NEP) (Dunlap & Van Liere, 1978; sible for these negative consequences, but not act on these beliefs.
Dunlap et al., 2000; Dunlap, 2008). The most important predictor Car users also scored lower on hedonic values compared to those
of NEP was biospheric values. The higher the score on such values, who most frequently choose public transportation. It is harder to
the more positive was the respondents' score on NEP. In addition, explain this association and it may be that this result is spurious.
egoistic values were negatively associated with the NEP-score. The same results emerged in the second analysis when those
Thus, it can be concluded that such values are associated with be- who frequently were walking and cycling were compared to the
liefs of limits to growth, the need of balancing economic growth group of frequent users of private motorised travel modes. How-
with environmental protection and the necessity to preserve the ever, distance from place of residence to place of work and annual
balance of nature. In addition, biospheric and hedonistic values income were also significant predictors in the second analysis.
were significant predictors of personal norms. When the distance is too large, walking and cycling becomes an
Somewhat unexpectedly hedonic and altruistic values were not unavailable option. Those who most frequently walked and used a
associated with NEP beliefs. Steg et al. (2005) also found that bicycle also had a higher annual income when compared to
biospheric values were more important for sustainable behaviour frequent users of private motorised travel modes. This may be
than altruistic values. Therefore, they argue that it is relevant to caused by more knowledge and a higher priority of their own
make a distinction between these two types of values. The current health than frequent car users, and consequently, they more often
study's results also show similar results. When controlling for choose health promoting travel modes. Another possible explana-
biospheric values, altruistic values did not predict NEP. This could tion is that property is more expensive in and near the city centres,
be because altruistic values are less important in determining and the majority of workplaces are localised in such areas. There-
environmental beliefs. This result may be due to strong multi- fore, individuals with high income may have the option to settle
collinearity between altruistic and biospheric values. The results near their workplace.
showed that biosperic values are important for environmental When the groups of frequent users of private and public trans-
beliefs, which are strongly associated with ascription of conse- portation were compared, income and gender were also significant
quences and awareness of consequences. Together, these factors predictors of transport group belongingness. The annual income
may be important for forming sustainable personal norms. was less in the group of frequent private travel mode users and
As shown the VBN model significantly predicted personal males were in majority in this group, whereas females more often
norms. All the associations shown in Fig. 1 were significant at the used public transportation. This is in accordance with studies car-
0.95 per cent-level. As can be seen by the size of the b-values an ried out previously where frequent users of their own car were
acceptable percentage of variance was explained by the predictor more likely to be males, elderly and to hold low education, while
variables in the SEM model. Two of the dimensions of personal frequent users of public transportation most often were females,
norms were found to be indirectly associated with personal norms young people and well-educated individuals (Rundmo, Sigurdson,
mediated by the NEP. This is not quite in accordance with the VBN & Roche-Cerasi, 2011; Rundmo, Nordfjærn, Iversen, Oltedal, &
model. Direct effects that are not hypothesised in the original VBN Jørgensen, 2011). Campaigns aimed to promote public transport
model could be considered as minor deviations from the theoretical mode choice may benefit by targeting people with high income and
model and do not indicate that it is something wrong with the male gender.
theoretical model. The reasons for these differences in the results
compared to the model's hypothesised associations may difficult to 4.3. Methodological issues
identify. The observed differences between the model and the re-
sults of the current study may be due to the fact that the core aim The results of the present research were based on self-reports.
was to identify specific factors associated with travel mode choices This includes psychological measures as well as behavioural mea-
and not sustainable reported behaviour in general. Accordingly, the sures. This dependence only on self-reports be regarded as a
results of the logistic regression analysis also showed that other weakness of the study. Kormos and Gifford (2014) showed in a
factors than hypothesised by the VBN model may be relevant for meta-analysis aimed at examining the association between self-
travel mode choices. However, these results do not challenge the reported and objective measures of sustainable behaviour that 79
important basic assumption of the VBN model. per cent of the variance remained unexplained despite effect sizes
indicating moderate to strong associations between the two types
4.2. Personal norms, situational factors and travel mode choice of measures. However, it may be argued that sustainable behaviour
and travel mode behaviour are quite different. Self reports on
An aim of the study was to examine the relative importance of sustainable behaviours may to a larger extent be associated with
situational factors and personal norms in predicting travel mode values and norms and, therefore, be more influenced by social
choice. The results also showed that personal norms predicted desirability compared to asking people about their transport mode
sustainable travel mode choice. In the first analysis behaviour- choice. More research is needed to draw more decisive conclusions
H.B. Lind et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 44 (2015) 119e125 125

about this. Psychological measures used in the questionnaire of the management measures: The importance of problem awareness, personal norm,
freedom, and fairness. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 26, 15e26.
current study were based on previously validated measurement
Eriksson, L., Garvill, J., & Nordlund, A. M. (2008). Acceptability of single and com-
instruments. Additional reliability tests and fit statistics from SEM- bined transport policy measures: the importance of environmental policy
analysis also showed to be at a satisfactory level. specific beliefs. Transportation Research Part A, 42, 1117e1128.
This study has shown that sustainable behaviour in use of Guagnano, G. A. (1995). Locus of control. Altruism and agentic disposition. Popu-
lation and Environment, 17, 63e77.
environmentally friendly travel modes may be enhanced by Iversen, H. H., Rundmo, T., & Klempe, H. (2005). A comparison of the effects of a
focussing on biospheric values, increasing sustainable beliefs and behaviour modification program and a traffic safety campaign on risk attitudes
general awareness of the responsibility for environmental prob- and behaviour amongst Norwegian adolescents. European Psychologist, 10,
25e38.
lems caused by the use of private motorised travel modes in urban Jakovcevic, A., & Steg, L. (2013). Sustainable transportation in Argentina: values,
areas, and strengthening personal norms for the choice of envi- beliefs, norms and car use reduction. Transportation Research Part F. Traffic
ronmentally friendly travel modes. These results may indicate that Psychology and Behavior, 20, 70e79.
Kormos, C., & Gifford, R. (2014). The validity of self-report measures of pro-
countermeasures to reduce the use of private motorised travel environmental behavior: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Environmental Psy-
modes should focus on these values and norms. chology, 40, 359e371.
The results of the model tests have shown that norms, values Limtanakool, N., Dijst, M., & Schwanen, T. (2006). The influence of socioeconomic
characteristics, land use and travel time considerations on mode choice for
and believes were associated with travel mode choice, and prob- medium- and longer-distance trips. Journal of Transport Geography, 14, 327e341.
ably also mode use. However, it is no by necessity the same vari- Moan, I. S. (2013). Whether or not to ride with an intoxicated driver: predicting
ables that are significantly associated in a prediction model that are intentions using an extended version of the theory of planned behaviour.
Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 20, 193e205.
most suited at explaining how behavioural change most efficient € Jørgensen, S. H., & Rundmo, T. (2014).
lu, O.,
Nordfjærn, T., Lind, H. B., Şimşekog
could be reached. Consequently, future research should also look Perceived thresholds for transport mode change and tolerance of push measures in
into the role of other aspects that could be relevant, e.g. resistance an urban Norwegian public. Trondheim, Norway: Norwegian University of Sci-
to change and habits. How an attitude campaigns is carried out has ence and Technology, Department of Psychology.
Nordlund, A. M., & Garvill, J. (2003). Effects of values, problem awareness, and
been found to be important for how to avoid resistance to change personal norm on willingness to reduce personal car use. Journal of Environ-
(Iversen, Klempe, & Rundmo, 2005). mental Psychology, 23, 339e347.
Pierce, J. C., Dalton, R. J., & Zaitsev, A. (1999). Public perceptions of environmental
conditions. In R. J. Dalton, P. Garb, N. P. Lovrich, J. C. Pierce, & J. M. Whitley (Eds.),
References Critical masses: citizens, nuclear weapons production, and environmental
destruction in the United States and Russia (pp. 97e129). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Abrahmse, W., Steg, L., Gifford, R., & Vlek, C. (2009). Factors influencing car use for Press.
commuting and the intention to reduce it: a question of self-interest or mo- Rundmo, T., Foss Sigurdson, J., & Roche-Cerasi, I. (2011a). Travel mode use, trans-
rality? Transportation Research Part F, 12, 317e324. portation priorities and risk. In T. Rundmo (Ed.), Perception of transport risk and
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intention to action: a theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl, & urban travel mode use. Trondheim: Rotunde Publ. no. 92.
J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action control: from cognition to behaviour (pp. 11e39). Rundmo, T., Nordfjærn, T., Iversen, H. H., Oltedal, S., & Jørgensen, S. H. (2011b). The
Berlin: Springer. role of risk perception in transportation mode use. Safety Science, 49, 226e235.
Black, J. S., Stern, P. C., & Elworth, J. T. (1985). Personal and contextual influences on Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences and altruism. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
household energy adaptations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 3e21. Advances in experimental social psychology, 10 pp. 221e279). N.Y: Academic
Castanier, C., Paran, F., & Delhomme, P. (2012). Risk of crashing with a tram: per- Press.
ceptions of pedestrians, cyclists, and motorists. Transportation Research Part F: Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical
Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 15, 387e394. advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social
Collin, C. M., & Chambers, S. M. (2005). Psychological and situational influences on Psychology, 25, 1e65.
commuter-transport mode Choice. Environment and Behavior, 37, 640e661. Steg, L. (2005). Car use: lust and must. Instrumental, symbolic and affective motives
Commission of the European Communities. (2007). Results of the review of the for car use. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 39, 147e162.
community strategy to reduce CO2 emissions from passenger cars and light- Steg, L., Dreijerink, L., & Abrahamse, W. (2005). Factors influencing the acceptability
commercial vehicles. COM, 19 Final, 7 February 2007, Brussels. of energy policies: a test of VBN theory. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25,
De Groot, J. I. M., & Steg, L. (2007). Value orientation and environmental beliefs in 415e425.
five countries: validity of an instrument to measure egoistic, altruistic and Steg, L., Perlaviciute, G., van der Werff, E., & Lurvink, J. (2014). The significance of
biospheric value orientations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 318e332. hedonic values for environmentally relevant attitudes, preferences, and actions.
De Groot, J. I. M., & Steg, L. (2008). Value orientation to explain environmental Environment and Behavior, 46, 163e192.
attitudes and beliefs: how to measure egoistic, altruistic and biospheric value Stern, P. C. (2000). Towards a coherent theory of environmentally significant
orientation. Environment and Behavior, 40, 330e354. behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 407e424.
De Groot, J. I. M., & Steg, L. (2009). Morality and prosocial behaviour: the role of Stern, P. C., & Dietz, T. (1994). The value basis of environmental concern. Journal of
awareness, responsibility, and norms in the norm activation model. The Journal Social Issues, 50, 65e84.
of Social Psychology, 149, 425e449. Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G. A., & Kalof, L. (1999). A value-belief-norm
Dunlap, R. E. (2008). The new environmental paradigm scale: from marginality to theory of support for social movements: the case of environmentalism.
worldwide use. The Journal of Environmental Education, 40, 3e18. Research in Human Ecology, 6, 81e97.
Dunlap, R. E., & Van Liere, K. D. (1978). The ‘new environmental paradigm.’ A Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., & Guagnano, G. A. (1995). The new ecological paradigm in
proposed measuring instrument and preliminary results. Journal of Environ- social-psychological context. Environment and Behavior, 27, 723e743.
mental Education, 9, 10e19. Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., & Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientation, gender, and environmental
Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G., & Jones, R. E. (2000). Measuring concern. Environment and Behavior, 25, 322e348.
endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: a revised NEP scale. Journal of Tanner, C. (1999). Constraints on environmental behaviour. Journal of Environmental
Social Issues, 56, 425e442. Psychology, 19, 145e215.
Eriksson, L., Garvill, J., & Nordlun, A. M. (2006). Acceptability of travel demand

You might also like