You are on page 1of 6

Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 239–244

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Associations between subtypes of social withdrawal and emotional


eating during emerging adulthood
Rebecca G. Etkin ⁎, Julie C. Bowker, Matthew D. Scalco
University at Buffalo, SUNY, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: While the psychosocial difficulties associated with one specific type of social withdrawal, shyness, have been
Received 19 October 2015 extensively studied, less is known about the correlates of other subtypes, such as preference-for-solitude. Of
Received in revised form 14 March 2016 the existing studies on withdrawal subtypes, few focus on the emerging adulthood developmental period, and
Accepted 18 March 2016
none have examined possible physical health-related correlates and associated mechanisms. This study
Available online xxxx
considered whether two withdrawal subtypes (shyness, preference-for-solitude) are associated with emotional
Keywords:
eating vis-à-vis internalizing problems during emerging adulthood. Participants included 643 emerging adults
Shyness (283 males; Mage = 19.61) who completed measures of withdrawal subtypes, emotional eating, and internalizing
Preference-for-solitude problems (depression, loneliness, social anxiety). Path models revealed that the associations between both
Emotional eating shyness and preference-for-solitude and emotional eating were explained, in part, by depression and social
Internalizing problems anxiety. Findings suggest that withdrawing from peers during emerging adulthood, due to fear or preferences-
Emerging adulthood for-solitude, may have significant consequences for both physical health outcomes vis-à-vis psychological
difficulties.
© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction during this developmental stage is underscored by findings linking


emotional eating with numerous physical health problems, including
It is well-known that social withdrawal (i.e., a behavioral tendency obesity and eating disorder symptomatology, which often persist into
to engage solitary behavior in the company of familiar and unfamiliar later adulthood. Mediation models in which the withdrawal subtypes
peers) during childhood, adolescence, and emerging adulthood is a lead to emotional eating vis-à-vis internalizing problems are also
strong risk factor for psychosocial maladjustment (i.e., depression, evaluated in light of theory and research suggesting that emotional
anxiety, peer victimization; Rubin, Coplan, & Bowker, 2009). Little is eating often serves as a (non-adaptive) means of regulating or coping
known, however, about the physical health-related concomitants of with unpleasant or threatening emotions such as loneliness and sadness
social withdrawal. There is some evidence that youth who are rejected (Macht, 2008). Understanding if and why withdrawn emerging adults
and excluded by their peers (and as a consequence, spend considerable are at risk for engaging in maladaptive eating behaviors has the
time alone) are at risk for the development and maintenance of obesity- potential to extend etiological risk models of social withdrawal, which
and physical health-related difficulties (Salvy, de la Haye, Bowker, & to date, focus exclusively on social, social-cognitive, and psychological
Hermans, 2012). When alone and away from peers, such youth appear outcomes (Rubin et al., 2009).
to overeat and “miss out” on opportunities for physical activity with
peers. These recent findings raise the intriguing possibility that youth 1.1. Social withdrawal subtypes and adjustment difficulties
who are socially withdrawn and actively avoid their peers, not because
of peer difficulties but due to shyness or a preference-for-solitude, may Most developmental research has focused on withdrawn youth who
also be at risk for obesity-related health difficulties, including emotional are shy (Rubin et al., 2009); these youth want to be with their peers but
eating (i.e., eating in response to negative emotions; Bruch, 1973). are too fearful or anxious to do so. More recent work, however, has
In this investigation, we consider whether two types of withdrawal broadened to consider two additional withdrawal subtypes, unsociabil-
(shyness, preference-for-solitude) are related to emotional eating during ity and avoidance. Whereas unsociable youth have non-fearful prefer-
emerging adulthood (18–25 years). Investigators have not yet evaluat- ences for solitude, avoidant youth withdraw from peers because they
ed these associations but the importance of studying emotional eating actively dislike the company of others (Rubin et al., 2009). It is argued
that these two latter types of withdrawal may be best studied together
⁎ Corresponding author at: University at Buffalo, The State University of New York,
and as reflecting a preference-for-solitude, as they both involve an
Department of Psychology, 306 Park Hall, Buffalo, NY 14260, USA affinity for being alone (Wang, Rubin, Laursen, Booth-LaForce, & Rose-
E-mail address: retkin@buffalo.edu (R.G. Etkin). Krasnor, 2013). Whether unsociability and avoidance are examined

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.059
0191-8869/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
240 R.G. Etkin et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 239–244

independently or jointly (i.e., preference-for-solitude), growing emotional eating has been implicated as a correlate and precursor to se-
evidence indicates that these constructs are distinct from (albeit associ- rious mental- and physical-health problems including depression, binge
ated with) shyness (e.g. Bowker & Raja, 2011; Nelson, 2013; Wang et al., eating disorder, and obesity (e.g. Braet et al., 2008; Ganley, 1989; Stice,
2013). However, the extant research suggests that the risks are greater Presnell, & Spangler, 2002).
for shyness than preference-for-solitude, at least during childhood and Given the reasons why individuals emotionally eat, there is some
early adolescence (Rubin et al., 2009). reason to expect direct and indirect associations between emotional
One notable limitation of recent research on withdrawal subtypes is eating and shyness and preference-for-solitude. Because emotional
that most prior research has focused on childhood and early adoles- eating and overeating are more likely to occur when individuals are
cence (10–14 years) to the neglect of withdrawal during emerging alone (e.g., Ganley, 1989), it is possible that youth who are both shy
adulthood. However, certain types of withdrawal, such as shyness, and prefer solitude, who, by definition, spend considerable time alone,
may be especially problematic during emerging adulthood because are at risk for emotional eating. Yet, indirect effects are also plausible.
such behaviors likely interfere with the successful completion of Shy emerging adults experience significant levels of internalizing
stage-salient developmental tasks, such as forming intimate relation- distress (in the form of anxiety, depression, and loneliness; Nelson
ships, going to college, and obtaining employment (e.g., Bowker, et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2013) and also difficulties with emotion
Nelson, Markovic, & Luster, 2014). Indeed, Nelson (2013) found that regulation (unlike youth who prefer solitude; Nelson, 2013), all of
shy (and also avoidant) emerging adults endorsed higher levels of a which are associated with emotional eating (e.g., Macht, 2008). For
number of different psychosocial problems (e.g., depression, relation- these reasons, we hypothesized that the linkage between shyness and
ship difficulties, emotion dysregulation) relative to non-withdrawn emotional eating might be especially strong, and that shy youth may
emerging adults. Unsociable emerging adults reported only elevated emotionally eat because they have difficulty regulating the internalizing
levels of depression. However, Wang et al. (2013) found that shyness, feelings they experience. As there is some evidence that preference-for-
but not preference-for-solitude, was associated with internalizing diffi- solitude is associated with depressive feelings (Nelson, 2013), we
culties (e.g., depression, anxiety) and emotion dysregulation during late hypothesized that this specific type of emotional distress may also
adolescence (which partially overlaps with emerging adulthood). To account for associations with emotional eating. The present study is
our knowledge, these are the only two studies that have compared the first, to our knowledge, to evaluate whether internalizing problems
the concomitants of withdrawal subtypes beyond the late childhood/ account for linkages between these withdrawal subtypes and emotional
early adolescent years, with inconclusive results. Thus, additional eating.
research is clearly needed to clarify the degree of risk associated with
different types of withdrawal during emerging adulthood and 1.3. Study summary
determine whether shyness also carries the greatest risk at this time.
As noted previously, another limitation of the aforementioned The present study extends past research by examining if withdrawal
studies is that the withdrawal subtypes were considered only in relation subtypes during emerging adulthood are associated with emotional
to psychosocial outcomes. However, there is some indication that eating, and whether these associations are mediated by internalizing
shyness is also related to negative physical health outcomes in adoles- difficulties (depression, loneliness, social anxiety). Main effects were
cence and emerging adulthood. For instance, Page, Hammermeister, expected for both shyness and preference-for-solitude, and mediation
Scanlan, and Allen (1996) found shyness to predict frequent TV viewing, models were expected to be supported for shyness by way of all three
which is a risk factor for obesity-related problems such as inactivity and internalizing problems considered, but only supported for preference-
overeating. Miller, Schmidt, and Vaillancourt (2008) found significant for-solitude vis-à-vis depression. All hypotheses were tentative, howev-
associations between shyness and eating disorder symptoms in a sam- er, due to the lack of past research, and with those published studies
ple of female undergraduate students. Aside from these studies, noth- yielding conflicting findings. To test the unique effects of actively
ing, to our knowledge, is known about the physical concomitants of avoiding peers, peer isolation was controlled. Gender differences were
actively withdrawing from and avoiding peers due to fearful and non- also explored but no specific predictions were made, as emotional
fearful motivations. eating is typically more common among females (e.g., Braet et al.,
2008) but evidence is mixed regarding the frequency and costs of
1.2. Emotional eating and social withdrawal subtypes withdrawal for males and females during emerging adulthood (e.g.
Nelson, 2013; Nelson et al., 2008). Finally, given significant relations
In this study, we evaluate whether shyness and preference-for- between emotional eating and weight gain in several studies (e.g.
solitude are related to emotional eating. Theories of emotional eating Ganley, 1989; Stice et al., 2002), BMI was considered as a covariate of
posit that many individuals eat not due to hunger, but in an attempt emotional eating.
to distract from, cope with, or regulate negative or threatening emotion-
al states or experiences (Bruch, 1973; Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991; 2. Method
Macht, 2008). Recent research offers compelling evidence for these
theories; for instance, Evers, Marijn Stok, and de Ridder (2010) found 2.1. Participants and procedure
that non-adaptive emotion regulation strategies, including cognitive
suppression, led to increased eating following a negative mood induc- Participants were 643 undergraduate students (283 males; Mage =
tion. Raspopow et al. (2013) and Spoor et al. (2007) found that coping 19.61 years, SD = 1.25) enrolled in an introductory psychology course
strategies typically conceptualized as non-adaptive due to associations at a large public university in the northeast United States. Approximate-
with psychological difficulties (avoidance-distraction coping, emotion- ly 41% of participants self-identified as Caucasian, 41% North-East or
focused coping) mediate associations between social and emotional South Asian, 8% Black/African-American, 4% Hispanic/Latino, and 6% as
problems (unsupportive social interactions, negative affect) and emo- “other.” Participants attended one 45-min laboratory visit conducted
tional eating in clinical and non-clinical samples. These authors argue by trained research assistants. Written consent was obtained from all
that certain individuals resort to emotional eating when they are unable participants, and participants were informed that they were free to
to successfully regulate or escape their emotions (due to a lack of adap- withdraw from the study at any time. During the laboratory visit, partic-
tive strategies, such as problem-focused coping), and come to associate ipants completed a series of self-report measures administered using
emotional eating with reductions in their unpleasant feelings SurveyMonkey.com, and their height and weight were measured. All
(Raspopow et al., 2013; Spoor et al., 2007). Despite positive expectan- participants were awarded course credit for their participation. Study
cies that eating with help regulate emotions and facilitate coping, procedures were approved by the university Institutional Review Board.
R.G. Etkin et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 239–244 241

2.2. Measures 3. Results

2.2.1. Social withdrawal subtypes 3.1. Examining the factor structure and psychometric properties of the child
Participants completed the 21-item revised Child Social Preference social preference scale-revised
Scale (Bowker & Raja, 2011) by indicating how much they were like
each item on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = “Not at all” to 5 = “A lot”). An exploratory factor analysis was conducted for the Child Social
For the present study, items were revised to be appropriate for emerg- Preference Scale-Revised items. The scree plot suggested that a
ing adulthood samples (e.g., “I'd like to play with other kids, but I'm four-factor solution was most appropriate; also, only four factors had ei-
sometimes nervous to” was revised to read “I'd like to hang out with genvalues over 1.0. Principal-axis factoring with an oblique (promax)
others, but I'm sometimes nervous to”). In addition, two items each rotation initially yielded four factors. However, two unsociability items
descriptive of avoidance (e.g., “I often try to spend time alone because were dropped because they loaded highly (N.30) on more than one fac-
I don't like to be with others”), shyness (e.g., “I often watch people tor (“I don't have a strong need to be with others,” “I rarely initiate being
hanging out, but I don't try to join in”), and unsociability (e.g., “I don't with others because I don't mind spending time alone”). After dropping
have a strong preference for being alone or with others”) were added these items, principal-axis factoring with oblique (promax) rotation
in an attempt to improve reliability of the factors reported by Bowker yielded four factors, but two additional unsociability items were
and Raja (2011). Given these revisions, the 21 items were subjected to dropped because of high cross-loadings (“I don't really mind spending
exploratory factor analyses, with results reported below. time alone”, “I don't have a strong preference for being alone or with
others”). A final factor analysis yielded three factors, which we refer to
as preference-for-solitude (7-items; α = .85), shyness (6-items; α =
2.2.2. Depression .88), and isolation (4-items; α = .87). The shyness factor accounted
The Beck Depression Inventory-II (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) was for 40.55% of the total variance, the preference-for-solitude factor
used to measure depressive symptoms. This measure includes accounted for 10.49% of the total variance, and the isolation factor
20-items (one item assessing suicidal ideation was omitted) assessed accounted for 5.13% of the total variance (56.17% altogether). There
on a 4-point scale (e.g., ranging from 1 = “I do not feel sad” to 4 = “I were no cross-loadings higher than .26 for the 17 items, and factor load-
am so sad and unhappy that I can't stand it”). Internal consistency for ings ranged from to .38 to .91. Factor correlations based on the promax
this measure was very good (α = .89). rotation were .50 (preference-for-solitude and isolation), .54 (shyness
and preference-for-solitude), and .70 (isolation and shyness). Final fac-
tor items and loadings are presented in Table 1. Descriptive statistics
2.2.3. Loneliness
and correlations are presented in Table 2.
The UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell, Peplau, & Ferguson, 1978) was
utilized to assess subjective feelings of loneliness. Participants indicated
3.2. Results of path models testing the indirect effects of social withdrawal
how often they felt similarly to the state described by each item using a
subtypes on emotional eating
4-point scale (1 = “I never feel this way” to 5 = “I often feel this
way”). Sample items include “I have nobody to talk to” and “I cannot
Mplus version 6.12 (Muthén & Muthén, 2010) was used to estimate
tolerate being so alone.” Internal consistency for this scale was excellent
a path model in which shyness and preference were related to emotion-
(α = .96).
al eating vis-à-vis three related but distinct types of internalizing diffi-
culties, namely, loneliness, social anxiety, and depression (see Fig. 1).
All independent variables and mediators were allowed to covary. Peer
2.2.4. Social anxiety
isolation was included as a control variable, and was allowed to covary
Anxiety, specifically relating to social interactions, was measured
with all predictors and mediators and emotional eating. In addition, BMI
using the Social Interaction and Anxiety Scale (Mattick & Clarke,
was included as a control variable but was only allowed to predict emo-
1998). For each of the 20 items, participants rated the degree to which
tional eating and was not allowed to covary with any other independent
they felt each statement was true or characteristic of them (e.g., “I be-
variable or mediator. Given that the BMI variable was skewed and
come tense if I have to talk about myself or my feelings”) with answer
kurtotic, indirect effects were tested using the delta method and robust
choices ranging from 0 = “Not at all” to 4 = “Extremely.” Reliability
for this scale was very good (α = .89).
Table 1
Results of principal axis factor analyses of the child social preference scale-revised.

2.2.5. Emotional eating Factor


Participants completed the Emotional Eating subscale (13-items) of Items (abbreviated) Shyness Pref. Isolation
the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (Van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, & solitude
Defares, 1986). Frequency of the desire to eat in response to negative
Desire to be with others, but nervous to .85 −.08 .04
emotions was assessed using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = “never” to Turn down hanging out because too shy .84 .00 −.15
5 = “very often”). Sample items include “Do you have a desire to eat Like to hang out but too nervous .82 −.02 −.06
when you are bored and restless?” and “Do you have a desire to eat Like to ask others to hang out, but afraid .75 −.13 .15
when you are feeling lonely?” Internal consistency was excellent Watch people but don't try to join in .55 .13 .12
Stand near others without joining in .38 .13 .26
(α = .95). Choose to spend time alone, don't like others −.01 .76 .12
Don't like others, prefer to be alone .10 .73 −.01
Like spending time alone more than others .15 .71 −.14
2.2.6. Body mass index (BMI) Try to spend time alone because don't like others −.09 .69 .21
Participants' weight and height were measured in order to calculate When given the choice prefer others (R) −.14 .63 −.15
Happiest when with others (R) −.09 .61 −.13
BMI. Participants were asked to remove excess clothing and shoes be-
Try to avoid spending time with others .15 .56 .16
fore measurements were taken. Height was measured to the nearest Sometimes others don't want me to hang out −.11 .01 .91
one-eighth inch using a wall-mounted stadiometer (Doran Scales, Inc., Wish I could spend time with others −.04 −.08 .87
DS1100 Mechanical Height Rod), and weight was measured with a dig- Others don't want to be with me .06 −.11 .85
ital scale to the nearest 0.2 lbs. Body mass index (BMI) was determined I′d like to hang out but often excluded .24 −.05 .55

by dividing an individual's weight by height ((lbs/in2) ∗ 703). Primary factor loadings are in boldface.
242 R.G. Etkin et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 239–244

Table 2
Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations among study variables.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Shyness .50⁎⁎⁎ .66⁎⁎⁎ .20⁎⁎⁎ .32⁎⁎⁎ .46⁎⁎⁎ .75⁎⁎⁎ −.03 −.04


2. Pref. solitude .41⁎⁎ .12⁎⁎ .29⁎⁎⁎ .35⁎⁎⁎ .46⁎⁎⁎ .09⁎ −.03
3. Peer isolation .15⁎⁎⁎ .31⁎⁎⁎ .53⁎⁎⁎ .50⁎⁎⁎ −.04 .03
4. Emotional eating .26⁎⁎⁎ .17⁎⁎⁎ .25⁎⁎⁎ .01 −.02
5. Depression .48⁎⁎⁎ .43⁎⁎⁎ −.02 −.05
6. Loneliness .53⁎⁎⁎ −.03 −.04
7. Social anxiety −.05 −.08
8. Age −.05
9. BMI
M 2.26 2.22 1.80 2.31 1.48 1.93 1.68 19.61 23.51
SD 0.96 0.72 0.85 0.90 0.43 0.70 0.78 1.25 4.19
⁎ p b .05.
⁎⁎ p b .01.
⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.

maximum likelihood estimators (MLR) were used. Using bootstrapping model did not significantly differ from the constrained model (Sattorra–
(5000 resamples), the model yielded nearly identical results. Bentler Scaled χ2 (15) = 20.7, p = 1.00). Thus, the model parameters
The model demonstrated good fit to the data, χ2 (3) = 14.07, p = did not significantly differ by gender.
.02; RMSEA = 0.05; CFI/TLI = 0.99/0.96; SRMR = 0.02. Both shyness
and preference-for-solitude were uniquely associated with social anxi- 4. Discussion
ety, depression, and loneliness. Paths were also significant from social
anxiety to emotional eating and from depression to emotional eating The mediation results from this investigation are novel and offer
but not between loneliness and emotional eating (see Fig. 1). new evidence of the risks associated with shyness during emerging
There was a significant indirect effect of shyness on emotional eating adulthood as well as possible risk mechanisms (that could be targeted
(β = 0.15, SE = 0.05, p b .01) through social anxiety (β = 0.10, SE = in interventions). As shyness tends to be associated with poor emotion
0.04, p b .05) and depression (β = 0.04, SE = 0.01, p b .01), but not lone- regulation and ineffective coping tendencies (Eisenberg, Fabes, &
liness (β = −0.01, SE = 0.02, p = .80). Similarly, there was a significant Murphy, 1995), shy youth may turn to eating as a way of coping with
indirect effect of preference-for-solitude on emotional eating (β = 0.06, the depression and anxiety they experience. Furthermore, given that
SE = 0.02, p b .001). However, only depression emerged as a significant shy emerging adults tend to have relationships that are lower in quality
mediator of this pathway (β = 0.04, SE = 0.01, p b .01); the pathway and intimacy (Nelson, 2013; Nelson et al., 2008), they may have difficul-
through social anxiety (β = 0.02, SE = 0.01, p = .06) was marginally ty engaging in more direct and problem-focused coping strategies, such
significant, and the pathway through loneliness was not significant as seeking social support, when experiencing these emotional prob-
(β = − 0.00, SE = 0.01, p = 0.80). Direct effects of both shyness and lems. Importantly, the current findings are the first to suggest that the
preference-for-solitude on emotional eating were not significant, psychological difficulties experienced by shy youth may, in turn, place
suggesting the presence of full mediation. them at risk for problems of a different ilk (e.g., physical health-
Finally, to assess for gender differences, two multiple group models related problems). Unexpectedly, however, the pathway between
were examined, one in which paths were allowed to be freely estimated shyness and emotional eating was not mediated by loneliness, despite
across gender and another in which paths were constrained to be the unique associations between shyness and loneliness. Although loneli-
same across gender. The nested test indicated that the freely estimated ness is a common trigger for emotional eating (e.g., Ganley, 1989), it

Fig. 1. Path model of shyness and preference-for-solitude predicting emotional eating vis-à-vis social anxiety, depression, and loneliness. Note. Covariates were left out of the diagram for
ease of presentation; *p b .05, **p b .01, ***p b .001.
R.G. Etkin et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 239–244 243

may be that shy emerging adults engage in alternative behaviors to cope in clinical samples (Davies, Bekker, & Roosen, 2011). Thus, it is plausible,
with feelings of loneliness, such as distracting themselves by engaging given the present findings, that many withdrawn emerging adults may
in solitary-constructive activities that are more social in nature benefit from similar interventions or prevention efforts, regardless of
(e.g., connecting with others online). their BMI. Withdrawn emerging adults may also benefit from education
Perhaps most noteworthy, however, were the findings pertaining to about emotional eating (e.g., that emotional eating leads to increases in
preference-for-solitude. First, the significant and unique associations negative feelings, which in turn, can lead to further food consumption).
between preference-for-solitude and social anxiety and loneliness Learning about these cycles may motivate youth to engage in different
were novel and provide additional evidence that preference-for- strategies less likely to exacerbate emotional and health problems.
solitude may not be entirely “benign” during emerging adulthood In conclusion, the present study takes a step toward expanding
(Nelson, 2013). Moreover, both types of withdrawal were related to existing models of social withdrawal by revealing that that emerging
all indices of psychological difficulties, raising question as to whether adults with fearful and non-fearful motivations of withdrawal are at-
shyness continues to carry greater risk beyond the early adolescent risk for psychological difficulties, which may place them at further risk
years. But why might preference-for-solitude be particularly detrimen- for engaging in unhealthy eating behaviors. Continuing to investigate
tal during the college years? There is some indication that having a the types of risks associated with withdrawal subtypes across develop-
preference-for-solitude leads to a lack of social initiation (Bowker & ment is essential to informing ways in which to intervene and prevent
Raja, 2011); this in turn might interfere with establishing and further withdrawal and health problems.
maintaining new relationships, particularly in an unfamiliar college
environment, and the development of psychological distress. References
Interestingly, the results further indicated that preference-for-
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II.
solitude was related to emotional eating vis-à-vis depression, but not San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
social anxiety or loneliness. It has been suggested that youth with Bowker, J. C., & Raja, R. (2011). Social withdrawal subtypes during early adolescence in
preferences-for-solitude may engage in social interactions when they India. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 201–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s10802-010-9461-7.
feel it will be rewarding (Bowker & Raja, 2011) and thus it is possible Bowker, J. C., Nelson, L. J., Markovic, A., & Luster, S. (2014). Social withdrawal during ad-
that they might seek support or reassurance from peers when feeling olescence and emerging adulthood. In R. J. Coplan, & J. C. Bowker (Eds.), The handbook
lonely or socially anxious. However, when experiencing depressive of solitude: Psychological perspectives on social isolation, social withdrawal, and Being
alone. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
symptoms, such youth may lack the energy or motivation to engage in
Braet, C., Claus, L., Goossens, L., Moens, E., Van Vlierberghe, L., & Soetens, B. (2008). Differ-
alternate strategies or activities to alleviate their negative feelings and ences in eating style between overweight and normal-weight youngsters. Journal of
instead turn to eating. There is also evidence that depressive feelings Health Psychology, 13, 733–743. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1359105308093850.
Bruch, H. (1973). Eating disorders. New York: Basic Books, Inc., Publishers.
are more likely to be associated with greater eating urges than high-
Davies, M. M., Bekker, M. J., & Roosen, M. A. (2011). The role of coping and general
arousal emotions such as fear or anxiety, which commonly inhibit psychopathology in the prediction of treatment outcome in eating disorders. Eating
food consumption (Macht, 2008). Given these associations, youth who Disorders: The Journal of Treatment & Prevention, 19, 246–258. http://dx.doi.org/10.
prefer to be alone may come to expect eating to alleviate their feelings 1080/10640266.2011.566147.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1995). Relations of shyness and low sociability
of depression only (e.g., Stice et al., 2002). to regulation and emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
Several limitations should be noted. First, the present study is cross- 505–517. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.505.
sectional. Future studies should be longitudinal to best evaluate the Evers, C., Marijn Stok, F., & de Ridder, D. D. (2010). Feeding your feelings: Emotion
regulation strategies and emotional eating. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
mediation results found in this study as well as the direction of effects. 36, 792–804. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167210371383.
Second, all measures included in the present study were self-report Ganley, R. M. (1989). Emotion and eating in obesity: A review of the literature.
(with the exception of BMI), and thus reporter-bias and shared- International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8, 343–361. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098-
108X(198905)8:3b343::AID-EAT2260080310N3.0.CO;2-C.
method variance are possible limitations. Self-report measures are Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness.
considered the “gold standard” for assessing internalizing problems, Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86.
motivations for social withdrawal, and emotional eating. However, Macht, M. (2008). How emotions affect eating: A five-way model. Appetite, 50, 1–11.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002.
experimental and observational assessments of emotional eating have
Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of social
been utilized (e.g., Evers et al., 2010). phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
The present study provides new evidence regarding the risks associ- 36, 455–470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-6.
Miller, J. L., Schmidt, L. A., & Vaillancourt, T. (2008). Shyness, sociability, and eating prob-
ated with two types of social withdrawal during emerging adulthood.
lems in a non-clinical sample of female undergraduates. Eating Behaviors, 9, 352–359.
However, one study of this age range, that utilized the same measure http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.01.001.
as we did, found evidence for three withdrawal subtypes (shyness, Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2010). 1998–2010 mplus user's guide. Muthén and Muthén.
unsociability, avoidance; Nelson, 2013). And, the aforementioned Nelson, L. J. (2013). Going it alone: Comparing subtypes of withdrawal on indices of
adjustment and maladjustment in emerging adulthood. Social Development, 22,
Wang et al. (2013) study used a different measure that did not allow 522–538. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2012.00671.x.
for the distinction between avoidance and unsociability. As such, it is Nelson, L. J., Padilla-Walker, L. M., Badger, S., Barry, C. M., Carroll, J. S., & Madsen, S. D.
clear that additional research on the measurement and factor structure (2008). Associations between shyness and internalizing behaviors, externalizing
behaviors, and relationships during emerging adulthood. Journal of Youth and
of social withdrawal during emerging adulthood is needed. Although Adolescence, 37, 605–615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-007-9203-5.
gender differences were not found in the path analyses, a t-test revealed Nguyen-Rodriguez, S. T., Chou, C., Unger, J. B., & Spruijt-Metz, D. (2008). BMI as a moder-
that, consistent with past research, the mean level of emotional eating ator of perceived stress and emotional eating in adolescents. Eating Behaviors, 9,
238–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.09.001.
was significantly higher for females than males, t(637) = − 4.49, Page, R. M., Hammermeister, J., Scanlan, A., & Allen, O. (1996). Psychosocial and
p b .001; thus, researchers should consider that associations with health-related characteristics of adolescent television viewers. Child Study Journal,
other health-related outcomes not considered herein may differ for 26, 319–331.
Raspopow, K., Matheson, K., Abizaid, A., & Anisman, H. (2013). Unsupportive social
males and females.
interactions influence emotional eating behaviors. The role of coping styles as
Despite some empirical and theoretical associations between mediators. Appetite, 62, 143–149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.031.
emotional eating and BMI, a handful of recent studies (including the Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., & Bowker, J. C. (2009). Social withdrawal in childhood. Annual
Review of Psychology, 60, 141–171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.
present study) have found nonsignificant associations in non-clinical
110707.163642.
samples (e.g., Nguyen-Rodriguez, Chou, Unger, & Spruijt-Metz, 2008), Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Ferguson, M. L. (1978). Developing a measure of loneliness.
suggesting that normal weight and otherwise healthy individuals may Journal of Personality Assessment, 42, 290–294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
engage in emotional eating to regulate their emotions. Initial evidence s15327752jpa4203_11.
Salvy, S., de la Haye, K., Bowker, J. C., & Hermans, R. J. (2012). Influence of peers and
suggests that interventions promoting adaptive coping strategies, such friends on children's and adolescents' eating and activity behaviors. Physiology &
as support-seeking, are effective in reducing emotional eating behavior Behavior, 106, 369–378. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.022.
244 R.G. Etkin et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 239–244

Spoor, S. P., Bekker, M. J., Van Strien, T., & van Heck, G. L. (2007). Relations between neg- Wang, J. M., Rubin, K. H., Laursen, B., Booth-LaForce, C., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2013). Prefer-
ative affect, coping, and emotional eating. Appetite, 48, 368–376. http://dx.doi.org/10. ence-for-solitude and adjustment difficulties in early and late adolescence. Journal of
1016/j.appet.2006.10.005. Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 42, 834–842. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Stice, E., Presnell, K., & Spangler, D. (2002). Risk factors for binge eating onset in adoles- 15374416.2013.794700.
cent girls: A 2-year prospective investigation. Health Psychology, 21, 131–138.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.21.2.131.
Van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E., Bergers, G., & Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch eating behavior
questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating
behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295–315.

You might also like