You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Affective Disorders 331 (2023) 167–174

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Affective Disorders


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jad

Research paper

Mediating role of personality traits in the association between


multi-dimensional adverse childhood experiences and depressive symptoms
among older adults: A 9-year prospective cohort study
Yanzhi Li a, b, 1, Lu Cheng c, 1, Lan Guo a, b, Liwan Zhu a, b, Hao Zhao a, b, Caiyun Zhang a, b,
Manjun Shen c, Yifeng Liu c, Muhammad Youshay Jawad d, e, Lingjiang Li f, Wanxin Wang a, b, *,
Ciyong Lu a, b, Roger S. McIntyre d, e, g, h
a
Department of Medical Statistics and Epidemiology, School of Public Health, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
b
Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Food, Nutrition and Health, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
c
Department of Psychiatry, Shenzhen Nanshan Center for Chronic Disease Control, Shenzhen, China
d
Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
e
Mood Disorders Psychopharmacology Unit, University Health Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
f
Mental Health Institute of the Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
g
Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
h
Brain and Cognition Discovery Foundation, Toronto, ON, Canada

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Background: To explore the mediating role of personality traits in the correlation between multi-dimensional
Depressive symptoms adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) and depressive symptoms in older adults.
Adverse childhood experiences Methods: This cohort study used data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing, and included 4050 older
Personality traits
adults without depressive symptoms in 2010–2011. Multi-dimensional ACEs were evaluated in 2006–2007.
Mediation
Older adults
Personality traits were assessed using the Midlife Development Inventory in 2010–2011. Depressive symptoms
were measured using the 8-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale during
2012–2019. Cox proportional hazard model was used to explore the associations between ACEs and depressive
symptoms. The package named “mediation” in R was used to test mediating role of personality traits.
Results: ACEs in each dimension significantly increased the risk of depressive symptoms (all P-values < 0.05). The
association of maltreatment (18.18 %) and household dysfunction (19.69 %) with depressive symptoms was
significantly mediated by neuroticism. The correlation between poor parent-child bonding and depressive
symptoms was significantly mediated by neuroticism (19.43 %), conscientiousness (4.84 %), and extroversion
(8.02 %).
Limitations: ACEs were retrospectively assessed based on participants' memories, which may induce recall bias.
Conclusions: Maltreatment and household dysfunction may induce depressive symptoms by increasing neuroti­
cism. Poor parent-child bonding may induce depressive symptoms by increasing neuroticism and reducing
conscientiousness and extraversion. In addition to reducing the occurrence of ACEs, reducing neuroticism of
individuals with maltreatment and household dysfunction in childhood, and reducing neuroticism, and
increasing conscientiousness and extraversion of individuals with poor parent-child bonding in childhood might
help to decrease their risk of depressive symptoms.

Abbreviations: ACEs, Adverse childhood experiences; ELSA, English Longitudinal Study of Ageing; CES-D, Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; HR, Hazard
ratio; CI, Confidence interval.
* Corresponding author at: Department of Medical Statistics and Epidemiology, School of Public Health, Sun Yat-sen University, No. 74 Zhongshan Rd 2,
Guangzhou 510080, China.
E-mail address: wangwx65@mail.sysu.edu.cn (W. Wang).
1
Yanzhi Li and Lu Cheng contributed equally to this work.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.03.067
Received 26 October 2022; Received in revised form 17 March 2023; Accepted 20 March 2023
Available online 22 March 2023
0165-0327/© 2023 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Y. Li et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 331 (2023) 167–174

Fig. 1. The timeline and procedures of this study.


Abbreviations: ACEs, adverse childhood experiences.

1. Introduction extraversion could also mediate the correlation between ACEs and
depressive symptoms in addition to neuroticism (Zhang et al., 2018).
Depression is a serious and debilitating mental disorder affecting Furthermore, the samples of previous longitudinal studies were from
approximately 256 million adults globally, with a prevalence of 5.02 % patients with depressive or anxiety disorders (Hovens et al., 2016;
(Institute of Health Metrics and Evaluation, 2021). Depression is asso­ Spinhoven et al., 2016), which might reduce the generalizability of the
ciated with considerable morbidity and mortality (Cuijpers et al., 2014), results due to selection bias. Studies in non-clinical adults were limited
and leads to high economic costs (JamaBloom et al., 2011). Addition­ to the cross-sectional design (Lee and Song, 2017), so they could not
ally, the World Health Organization has ranked depression as the third determine the temporality between personality traits and depressive
cause of burden of disease worldwide, and has predicted that depression symptoms. Although it is reasonable to assume that personality traits
will rank first by 2030 (World Health Organization, 2008). Thus, it is of precede depressive symptoms in adulthood given that personality traits
great significance to clarify the potential risk factors and relevant usually form early in life and remain generally stable throughout life
mechanisms of depression. At present, compelling evidence has (Clark and Watson, 2008), this does not mean that personality traits
confirmed that adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) increase the risk cannot change a bit in adulthood (Damian et al., 2019; Roberts et al.,
of depressive symptoms in adulthood (Iob et al., 2020a; Kim et al., 2022; 2008). Another limitation is that ACEs in previous studies only included
Yazawa et al., 2022). Overall, individuals with at least one ACE have a abuse (emotional, physical, and sexual) and neglect (emotional and
>2-fold increased risk of developing depression in adulthood (Li et al., physical) (Dagnino et al., 2020; Hovens et al., 2016; Lee and Song, 2017;
2016). Nevertheless, the underlying mechanisms by which ACEs induce Okubo et al., 2017; Spinhoven et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2018). Recent
depressive symptoms in adulthood have not been fully elucidated. studies have incorporated additional adversities to represent other
Personality traits (i.e., neuroticism, conscientiousness, extraversion, important dimensions of ACEs, such as household dysfunction, loss ex­
openness, and agreeableness) are usually formed in childhood under the periences, and poor parent-child bonding (Iob et al., 2020a, 2020b). It is
influence of various social and environmental factors (Clark and Wat­ uncertain whether ACEs in other dimensions can increase the risk of
son, 2008; Shiner, 2006), and continue to develop throughout adult­ depressive symptoms via unfavorable personality traits.
hood, albeit at a slow rate (Damian et al., 2019). Several studies have Therefore, this study aimed to use a 9-year nationally representative
reported that individuals with ACEs are more likely to develop unfa­ cohort study to explore the mediating role of personality traits in the
vorable personality traits, increasing their likelihood of depressive association between multi-dimensional ACEs (i.e., maltreatment,
symptoms in turn (Dagnino et al., 2020; Hovens et al., 2016; Lee and household dysfunction, poor parent-child bonding, and loss experi­
Song, 2017; Okubo et al., 2017; Spinhoven et al., 2016; Zhang et al., ences) and depressive symptoms in non-clinical older adults. This study
2018). However, the results on the mediating effects of specific per­ might clarify some mechanisms by which ACEs induce depressive
sonality traits on the association between ACEs and depressive symp­ symptoms, and provide a theoretical basis for effectively preventing and
toms are inconsistent. For example, Lee et al. found that only improving depressive symptoms.
neuroticism could mediate the effects of ACEs on depressive symptoms
(Lee and Song, 2017). In contrast, Zhang et al. uncovered that

168
Y. Li et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 331 (2023) 167–174

Table 1 scored in reverse. The total scores were calculated separately for each
Different types of adverse childhood experiences before the age of 16 in the Life parental-child bonding, ranging from 0 to 7. Higher scores indicate
History Interview in wave 3 (2006–2007). poorer father/mother-child bonding. According to the previous studies
Dimensions Adverse childhood experiences Exposure (Iob et al., 2020a, 2020b), individuals with the total scores ≥ 3 were
rates (%) considered to have poor father/mother-child bonding. ACEs were
Maltreatment 7.26 divided into four dimensions: maltreatment, household dysfunction,
Serious physical attack or assault 3.84 poor parent-child bonding, and loss experiences, which has been
Sexual assault (including rape or 1.34 confirmed by factor analysis and been widely used in previous studies
harassment)
(Iob et al., 2020a, 2020b). In each dimension, individuals with at least
Physical abuse from parents 3.25
Household 24.43 one ACE were considered to have ACEs (Iob et al., 2020a, 2020b). In
dysfunction Parents drank excessively, took drugs, or 6.08 addition, this study generated a dichotomy indicator of whether ACEs
had mental health problems had been experienced (any ACEs vs. no ACEs).
Parents argued or fought very often 19.70
Parents permanently separated or 6.98
divorced
2.3. Assessment of depressive symptoms
Poor parent-child 24.41
bonding Poor father-child bonding 16.16 In waves 6–9, depressive symptoms were assessed using the 8-item
Poor mother-child bonding 15.84 version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-
Loss experiences 19.28
D) (Radloff, 1977). The scale has good internal consistency in each wave
Parent death 6.38
Separation from mother for more than 15.50 (Cronbach α ≥ 0.95) (Di Gessa and Price, 2022). Participants responded
six months whether they felt depressed, felt that everything was an effort, slept
Foster care or adoption 1.31 restlessly, were happy, felt lonely, enjoyed life, felt sad, and could not
Living in a children's home 1.73 get going in the past week. Each item was rated as 0 (yes) or 1 (no), and
Adverse childhood 49.51
experiences
two items (i.e., “were happy” and “enjoyed life”) were scored in reverse.
The total scores range from 0 to 8, with higher scores suggesting greater
severity of depressive symptoms. A cut-off value of ≥4 was adopted to
2. Methods indicate significant depressive symptoms (Zaninotto et al., 2022), which
is equivalent to the conventional cut-off value of ≥16 on the full 20-item
2.1. Study design and participants CES-D scale (Steffick, 2000).

This study used data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing 2.4. Assessment of personality traits
(ELSA) (www.elsa-project.ac.uk), which has been reported previously in
detail (Steptoe et al., 2013). Briefly, the ELSA is an ongoing, prospective, In wave 5, personality traits were assessed using the Midlife Devel­
and nationally representative cohort of community-dwelling adults ≥ opment Inventory. Personality traits included neuroticism, conscien­
50 years in the UK. This sample represents the general population of tiousness, extraversion, openness, and agreeableness, assessed by four
older adults in the UK, and was not selected based on exposure to ACEs items (i.e., “worrying”, “nervous”, “moody”, and “calm”), five items (i.
or the presence of depressive symptoms (Iob et al., 2020a). The ELSA e., “responsible”, “hardworking”, “thorough”, “organized”, and “care­
was initiated in 2002–2003 (wave 1). Participants were biennially fol­ less”), five items (i.e., “outgoing”, “lively”, “friendly”, “active”, and
lowed up until 2018–2019 (wave 9). The ELSA datasets can be down­ “talkative”), seven items (i.e., “intelligent”, “creative”, “imaginative”,
loaded from the UK Data Service (https://ukdataservice.ac.uk/). Ethical “broad-minded”, “sophisticated”, “curious”, and “adventurous”), and
approval for ELSA was obtained from the London Multicenter Research five items (i.e., “warm”, “helpful”, “softhearted”, “caring”, and “sym­
Ethics Committee (MREC/01/2/91). All participants provided informed pathetic”), respectively (Lachman and Weaver, 1997). Participants used
consent. a Likert scale (1 = not at all; 4 = a lot) to rate how much each adjective
Data on ACEs were collected during the Life History Interview in described themselves. Some items were reversely scored when neces­
wave 3 (2006–2007). Data on personality traits were collected in wave 5 sary. The average value of each subscale was regarded as the score for
(2010–2011). Thus, wave 5 was used as the baseline for this prospective each personality trait. Higher scores represent higher levels of person­
cohort study, and new-onset depressive symptoms were assessed from ality traits (Strickhouser and Sutin, 2021). Cronbach α ranged from 0.67
wave 6 (2012–2013) to wave 9 (2018–2019). Fig. 1 shows the timeline to 0.80, suggesting at least adequate internal consistency (Gale et al.,
and procedures. This study merged data on the Life History Interview in 2015).
wave 3 (7855 participants) and data in wave 5 (10,274 participants). A
total of 6446 individuals participated in the two surveys. The following 2.5. Assessment of potential covariates
participants were excluded: those younger than 50 years at baseline (n
= 91), those with missing data on ACEs (n = 845), personality traits (n Potential covariates included age (continuous), sex (male and fe­
= 471) or depressive symptoms (n = 26) at baseline, those with male), race (White race and others), childhood socioeconomic status,
depressive symptoms (n = 650) at baseline, and those lost to follow-up education level, marital status (married, divorced/separated/widowed,
(n = 313). Ultimately, this study included 4050 participants. Supple­ and never married), adult wealth, employment status (retired,
mentary Fig. S1 presents the inclusion process of participants. employed, and unemployed), smoking status (never, former, and current
smokers), drinking status, and physical activity. All potential covariates
2.2. Assessment of ACEs were assessed in wave 5 (baseline) except for childhood socioeconomic
position. During the Life History Interview in wave 3, information on
As shown in Table 1, different types of ACEs before the age of 16 childhood socioeconomic status was collected, including overcrowding
were investigated during the Life History Interview in wave 3. Except for (1 = number of people/number of bedrooms > 2; 0 = number of people/
father-child bonding and mother-child bonding, participants responded number of bedrooms ≤ 2), number of books in the home (1 = 0–10
whether they had experienced the events in Table 1. Father-child books; 0 = >10 books), father's occupation (1 = manual; 0 = other),
bonding and mother-child bonding were evaluated using the 7-item financial hardship (1 = yes; 0 = no), and father unemployment for >6
Parental Bonding Instrument (Parker et al., 1979). All responses were months (1 = yes; 0 = no). The total scores of the above items were
divided into “agree (=1)” and “disagree (=0)”, and positive items were calculated to represent childhood socioeconomic status. Higher scores

169
Y. Li et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 331 (2023) 167–174

suggest lower childhood socioeconomic status (Iob et al., 2020a, Table 2


2020b). Education level was classified into three groups: university Baseline characteristics by exposure status of ACEs (wave 5, 2010–2011).
degree or equivalent, advanced level (A-level)/higher education below Characteristics Overall (n No ACEs Any ACEs P-value
degree, and no qualifications/ordinary level (O-level) or equivalent = 4050) (n = 2045) (n = 2005)
(Ronaldson et al., 2022). O-level is the first half part of the General Age (years), mean (SD) 67.34 67.28 67.40 0.670
Certificate of Education (GCE). O-level is usually taken up either in the (8.98) (9.03) (8.93)
11th year or between the age group of 14–16. It is the final certification Female, n (%) 2249 1069 1180 <0.001
for secondary school and once a student has completed it, they are (55.53) (52.27) (58.85)
White race, n (%) 3985 2016 1969 0.339
considered to have completed formal education. A-level is the second (98.40) (98.58) (98.20)
part of the GCE. Students between the ages of 16–18 or the 12th or 13th Education level, n (%) 0.175
year of schooling take up this programme. It is a two-year course and is University degree or 1415 738 677 (33.77)
considered as a standard assessment of students in order to determine equivalent (34.94) (36.09)
A-level/higher 325 (8.02) 170 (8.31) 155 (7.73)
their candidacy for college and university applications. Adult wealth
education below degree
was derived from a comprehensive assessment of the economic re­ No qualifications/O- 2310 1137 1173
sources (e.g., financial, housing, and physical wealth) excluding pension level or equivalent (57.04) (55.60) (58.50)
wealth, and was categorized into quintiles (1 = poorest; 5 = richest) Marital status, n (%) 0.008
(Steptoe and Di Gessa, 2021). According to the frequency of drinking in Married 2925 1520 1405
(72.22) (74.33) (70.07)
the past 12 months, drinking status was categorized into three groups: Divorced/separated/ 928 438 490 (24.44)
less than weekly, 1–4 days a week, and 5–7 days a week (Iob et al., widowed (22.02) (21.42)
2020a). Physical activity was assessed by the frequency of participation Never married 173 (4.66) 87 (4.45) 110 (5.49)
in vigorous, moderate, and light physical activities, and was categorized Employment status, n (%) 0.290
Retired 2485 1243 1242
into three groups: vigorous (vigorous activity on a weekly basis), mod­
(61.36) (60.78) (61.95)
erate (moderate activity on a weekly basis), and light (no vigorous or Employed 1213 633 580 (28.93)
moderate activity on a weekly basis) (Iob et al., 2020b). (22.91) (30.95)
Unemployed 197 (4.86) 169 (8.26) 183 (9.13)
Wealth quintiles, n (%)
1 (poorest) 462 187 (9.14) 275 (13.72) <0.001
2.6. Statistical analyses (11.41)
2 677 331 346 (17.26)
The baseline characteristics of participants were presented as mean (16.72) (16.19)
(standard deviation) or number (percentage), as appropriate. Student's t- 3 778 384 394 (19.65)
(19.21) (18.78)
tests were used to compare the mean levels of continuous variables
4 847 442 405 (20.20)
between participants with and without ACEs. Pearson chi-squared tests (20.91) (21.61)
were performed to compare the distribution of categorical variables. 5 (richest) 1286 701 585 (29.18)
Cox proportional hazard model was used to estimate the hazard ratio (31.75) (34.28)
(HR) and 95 % confidence interval (CI) to explore the associations be­ Smoking status, n (%) 0.003
Never smokers 399 (9.85) 185 (9.05) 214 (10.67)
tween ACEs and depressive symptoms. Person-years were measured in Former smokers 2071 1011 1060
months, from the date of interview in wave 5 to the date of new-onset (51.14) (49.44) (52.87)
depressive symptoms, loss to follow-up, or end of follow-up, which­ Current smokers 1580 849 731 (36.46)
ever came first. The proportional hazards assumption was examined by (39.01) (41.52)
Drinking status, n (%)
Schoenfeld tests. No violation of the assumption was identified. To
<0.001
Less than weekly 1509 700 809 (40.35)
assess the effects of potential covariates on the association of ACEs with (37.26) (34.23)
depressive symptoms, we incorporated potential covariates into the 1–4 days a week 1629 875 754 (37.61)
models in turn. Model 1 was unadjusted for any potential covariates. (40.22) (42.79)
Model 2 was adjusted for age, sex, and race at baseline. Model 3 was 5–7 days a week 912 470 442 (22.04)
(22.52) (22.98)
further adjusted for childhood socioeconomic position. Model 4 was Physical activity, n (%) 0.254
additionally adjusted for education level, marital status, employment Light 869 449 420 (20.95)
status, adult wealth, smoking status, drinking status, and physical ac­ (21.46) (21.96)
tivity at baseline. Moderate 1855 951 904 (45.09)
(45.80) (46.50)
The package named “mediation (4.5.0)” in R version 4.0.3 was used
Vigorous 1326 645 681 (33.97)
to perform mediation analyses with each personality trait as a separate (32.74) (31.54)
mediator. According to the procedure described by Tingley (Tingley Childhood socioeconomic 0.55 (0.78) 0.48 (0.73) 0.63 (0.82) <0.001
et al., 2014), we established two models: an exposure-mediator-outcome score, mean (SD)
model (survival regression) and an exposure-mediator model (linear Neuroticism, mean (SD) 2.01 (0.55) 1.97 (0.53) 2.06 (0.57) <0.001
Conscientiousness, mean 3.32 (0.47) 3.34 (0.45) 3.30 (0.48) 0.005
regression). Firstly, depressive symptoms were regressed by ACEs, per­ (SD)
sonality traits, and potential covariates. Secondly, personality traits Extraversion, mean (SD) 3.19 (0.53) 3.20 (0.52) 3.18 (0.54) 0.286
were regressed by ACEs and potential covariates. The scores of person­ Openness, mean (SD) 2.90 (0.53) 2.90 (0.53) 2.90 (0.53) 0.828
ality traits were standardized before being entered into the above two Agreeableness, mean (SD) 3.51 (0.46) 3.51 (0.47) 3.52 (0.46) 0.654
models. Personality traits reaching statistical significance (α = 0.05) in Student's t-tests were used to compare the means of continuous variables.
both models were further investigated. Finally, the results of the Pearson chi-squared tests were performed to compare the distribution of cate­
exposure-mediator-outcome and exposure-mediator models were com­ gorical variables.
bined to calculate the proportion of mediation. Quasi-Bayesian estima­ Abbreviations: ACEs, adverse childhood experiences; SD, standard deviation.
tion with 1000 iterations was used to estimate the P-values of the
mediating effect (Ho et al., 2020).
All statistical analyses were conducted using Stata version 17.0
(StataCorp LLC) and R version 4.0.3 (R Core Team, Vienna, Austria).
Statistical significance was defined as a two-tailed P-value < 0.05.

170
Y. Li et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 331 (2023) 167–174

Table 3
The association of ACEs at baseline (wave 5, 2010–2011) with depressive symptoms during follow up (waves 6–9, 2012–2019).
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

HR (95 % CI) P-value HR (95 % CI) P-value HR (95 % CI) P-value HR (95 % CI) P-value

ACEs
No 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
Yes 1.64 (1.40, 1.91) <0.001 1.56 (1.24, 1.82) <0.001 1.52 (1.30, 1.77) <0.001 1.44 (1.24, 1.69) <0.001
Maltreatment
No 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
Yes 1.32 (1.02, 1.73) 0.036 1.45 (1.11, 1.89) 0.007 1.43 (1.10, 1.87) 0.008 1.39 (1.06, 1.82) 0.017
Household dysfunction
No 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
Yes 1.53 (1.30, 1.79) <0.001 1.60 (1.37, 1.89) <0.001 1.54 (1.30, 1.81) <0.001 1.48 (1.26, 1.75) <0.001
Low parental bonding
No 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
Yes 1.53 (1.30, 1.81) <0.001 1.52 (1.29, 1.80) <0.001 1.49 (1.26, 1.76) <0.001 1.43 (1.20, 1.69) <0.001
Loss experiences
No 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)
Yes 1.50 (1.26, 1.79) <0.001 1.33 (1.12, 1.59) 0.001 1.31 (1.10, 1.56) 0.003 1.25 (1.05, 1.50) 0.012

Model 1: unadjusted for any potential covariates.


Model 2: adjusted for age, sex, and race at baseline.
Model 3: model 2 plus childhood socioeconomic status.
Model 2: model 3 plus education level, marital status, employment status, adult wealth, smoking status, drinking status, and physical activity at baseline.
Abbreviations: ACEs, adverse childhood experiences; HRs, hazard ratios; CIs, confidence intervals.

3. Results extraversion, openness, and agreeableness scores, respectively. ACEs


was positively correlated with neuroticism (β = 0.127, 95 % CI: 0.065,
3.1. Characteristics of participants 0.188), and negatively correlated with conscientiousness (β = − 0.076,
95 % CI: − 0.137, − 0.020) and agreeableness (β = − 0.068, 95 % CI:
As shown in Table 1, 49.51 % of participants had at least one ACE. − 0.128, − 0.008), but not correlated with other personality traits. Both
The percentages of participants with at least one ACE related to maltreatment (supplementary Table S2, β = 0.128, 95 % CI: 0.008,
maltreatment, household dysfunction, poor parent-child bonding, and 0.248) and household dysfunction (supplementary Table S3, β = 0.168,
loss experiences were 7.26 %, 24.43 %, 24.41 %, and 19.28 %, respec­ 95 % CI: 0.096, 0.240) were only positively related to neuroticism. Poor
tively. Table 2 summarizes the baseline characteristics of participants by parent-child bonding was positively associated with neuroticism (β =
exposure status of ACEs. The mean age of participants was 67.34 years 0.153, 95 % CI: 0.081, 0.226), and negatively associated with consci­
(standard deviation: 8.98 years), and females accounted for 55.53 %. entiousness (β = − 0.102, 95 % CI: − 0.173, − 0.030), extraversion (β =
Compared with participants without ACEs, those with at least one ACE − 0.122, 95 % CI: − 0.195, − 0.050), and agreeability (β = − 0.118, 95 %
were more likely to be females, unmarried, and never smokers, had CI: − 0.190, − 0.046), but not associated with openness (supplementary
lower socioeconomic status both in childhood and adulthood, drank less Table S4). Loss experiences were not correlated with any personality
than once a week, and had higher neuroticism, and lower conscien­ traits (supplementary Table S5).
tiousness scores. However, there existed no statistically significant dif­ Fig. 2 displays the standardized path coefficient and mediation
ferences in the distributions of age, race, education level, employment proportion of personality traits with a statistically significant mediation
status, physical activity, extraversion, openness, and agreeableness be­ effect (all P-values < 0.05). The association of ACEs with depressive
tween the two groups. symptoms was mediated by neuroticism and conscientiousness in 15.68
% and 3.24 %, respectively. In each dimension analysis, the correlation
3.2. Association of ACEs with depressive symptoms of maltreatment and household dysfunction with depressive symptoms
was mediated by neuroticism in 18.18 % and 19.69 %, respectively. The
During the median follow-up of 7.92 years (interquartile range: relationship between poor parent-child bonding and depressive symp­
4.17–8.08 years), 675 participants developed depressive symptoms, toms was mediated by neuroticism, conscientiousness, and extroversion
with an incidence density of 25.98 per 1000 person-years. Table 3 shows in 19.43 %, 4.84 %, and 8.02 %, respectively.
the association of ACEs with depressive symptoms in adulthood. After
adjusting for childhood socioeconomic status, baseline age, sex, race, 4. Discussion
education level, marital status, employment status, adult wealth,
smoking status, drinking status, and physical activity, compared with Using nationally representative data from the ELSA, this 9-year
participants without any ACEs, the HR (95 % CI) of those with at least prospective cohort study explored the mediating role of personality
one ACEs was 1.44 (1.24, 1.69). Moreover, compared with participants traits in the effects of multi-dimensional ACEs (i.e., maltreatment,
without ACEs, the full-adjusted HRs (95 % CIs) of those with at least one household dysfunction, poor parent-child bonding, and loss experi­
ACE related to maltreatment, household dysfunction, poor parent-child ences) on the risk of depressive symptoms in non-clinical older adults.
bonding, and loss experiences were 1.39 (1.06, 1.82), 1.48 (1.26, 1.75), Our results indicate that ACEs in each dimension significantly increase
1.43 (1.20, 1.69), and 1.25 (1.05, 1.50), respectively. the risk of depressive symptoms in adulthood, and the mediating role of
personality traits might vary with the dimensions of ACEs. The associ­
ation of maltreatment (18.18 %) and household dysfunction (19.69 %)
3.3. Mediating role of personality traits with depressive symptoms might be mediated by neuroticism. The as­
sociation between poor parent-child bonding and depressive symptoms
As shown in supplementary Table S1, the full-adjusted HRs (95 % might be mediated by neuroticism (19.43 %), conscientiousness (4.84
CIs) of depressive symptoms were 1.58 (1.46, 1.71), 0.85 (0.78, 0.91), %), and extraversion (8.02 %). In contrast, the relationship between loss
0.79 (0.73, 0.85), 0.90 (0.83, 0.97), and 0.99 (0.91, 1.07) for each experiences and depressive symptoms might not be mediated by any
standard deviation increase in neuroticism, conscientiousness,

171
Y. Li et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 331 (2023) 167–174

Fig. 2. The standardized path coefficient and mediation proportion of personality traits with statistically significant mediation effect.
Adjusted confounders included ACEs (only for personality traits), age, sex, race, childhood socioeconomic status, education level, marital status, employment status,
adult wealth, smoking status, drinking status, and physical activity.
*P-value < 0.05; **P-value < 0.01; ***P-value < 0.001.
Abbreviations: ACEs, adverse childhood experiences.

personality traits. This study clarifies some mechanisms by which ACEs source of sample (clinical population vs. non-clinical population). This
lead to depressive symptoms in adulthood, and provides a theoretical 9-year prospective cohort study further provides solid evidence that in
basis for effectively preventing and improving depressive symptoms. the non-clinical adults, only neuroticism at baseline significantly me­
Previous studies have consistently reported that neuroticism medi­ diates the correlation between maltreatment with depressive symptoms
ates the association of maltreatment with depressive symptoms, and during follow-up, but other personality traits might not be significant
openness and agreeableness do not play a mediating role. However, in mediating factors. Besides, this study extends previous findings by
previous studies, the mediating roles of extraversion and conscien­ exploring the mediating role of personality traits in the correlation be­
tiousness remain controversial (Hovens et al., 2016; Lee and Song, 2017; tween ACEs in other dimensions and depressive symptoms. The rela­
Zhang et al., 2018). This disagreement might be attributed to differences tionship between poor parent-child bonding and depressive symptoms
in instruments for evaluating maltreatment, study design (longitudinal was mediated by the most personality traits (i.e., neuroticism, consci­
studies vs. cross-sectional studies), age (adolescents vs. adults), and entiousness, and extraversion). The association of household

172
Y. Li et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 331 (2023) 167–174

dysfunction with depressive symptoms was only mediated by neuroti­ especially in older adults (Hardt and Rutter, 2004). Second, since our
cism. We speculate that compared with ACEs in other dimensions, the sample is restricted to the general elderly population in the UK, the re­
poor parent-child relationship might be a more important factor sults might not be generalized to other ethnic and age groups. Our
affecting the formation of personality traits. Our findings also suggest findings need to be validated in different populations in the future.
that when exploring the other mechanism by which ACEs induce Finally, despite controlling for a large number of potential confounders,
depressive symptoms, the dimensions of ACEs should be considered. we could not exclude residual confounding from unmeasured and un­
Furthermore, Stieger et al. found that personality traits can be changed recognized confounders.
through intervention in a randomized controlled trial of non-clinical In conclusion, maltreatment and household dysfunction in childhood
adults, and pointed out that some intervention measures of personality might increase the risk of depressive symptoms in the elderly by
traits are low-cost and low-threshold (Stieger et al., 2021). Our findings increasing neuroticism. Poor parent-child bonding in childhood might
suggest that neuroticism mediates the effects of ACEs in three di­ induce depressive symptoms in the elderly through increasing neuroti­
mensions on depressive symptoms, and has the highest mediation pro­ cism, and reducing conscientiousness and extraversion. Although loss
portion (approximately 20 %) among personality traits. Thus, experiences in childhood might elevate the risk of depressive symptoms
neuroticism might be an important intervention factor when formu­ in the elderly, personality traits might not play a mediating role. Our
lating measures to prevent depressive symptoms in individuals with findings provide preliminary evidence that in addition to interventions
ACEs. to reducing the occurrence of ACEs, reducing neuroticism of individuals
It is biologically plausible that personality traits mediate the asso­ with maltreatment and household dysfunction in childhood, and
ciation of ACEs with depressive symptoms in adulthood. First, person­ reducing neuroticism, and increasing conscientiousness and extraver­
ality traits usually develop during childhood under the influence of sion of individuals with poor parent-child bonding in childhood might
multiple social and environmental factors (Clark and Watson, 2008; help to decrease their risk of depressive symptoms.
Shiner, 2006). Individuals with ACEs are likely to develop maladaptive
personality traits, and individuals often rely on personality traits to cope Role of the funding source
with stressful events in later life (Zhang et al., 2018). Thus, maladaptive
personality traits might induce depressive symptoms through unrea­ This work was supported by Sanming Project of Medicine in Shenz­
sonable coping styles. For instance, compared with individuals with low hen Nanshan (No. 11).
extraversion, those with high extraversion usually engage in more social
activities, and seek more comfort and help from others, so extraversion CRediT authorship contribution statement
can protect individuals from stress events to a certain extent, and then
reduce their depressive symptoms (Yu and Hu, 2022). On the contrary, Yanzhi Li and Wanxin Wang designed this study. Lu Cheng, Lan Guo,
neuroticism is characterized by low self-esteem, shyness, anxiety, guilt, Liwan Zhu, Hao Zhao, Caiyun Zhang, and Xiuwen Li managed the
tension, emotional, irrational, and moody, so individuals with high literature searches and summaries of previous related work. Yanzhi Li,
neuroticism might have more negative emotions under stressors, which Lu Cheng, and Lan Guo did the statistical analysis. Yanzhi Li wrote the
can induce depressive symptoms (Malouff et al., 2005). Second, per­ first draft of the manuscript. Wanxin Wang, Ciyong Lu, Muhammad
sonality traits can predict life outcomes, such as success in love and work Youshay Jawad, Roger S. McIntyre, Manjun Shen, Yifeng Liu, and
and well-being, which are closely related to depressive symptoms Lingjiang Li reviewed the manuscript.
(Borghans et al., 2008; Soto, 2019). For example, individuals with high
conscientiousness tend to exhibit better academic achievement, job
performance, relationship quality, and physical health, which might Conflict of interest
reduce the incidence of their depressive symptoms in turn (Dudley et al.,
2006; Dumfart and Neubauer, 2016; Hampson et al., 2013; Hill et al., None.
2014; Kern and Friedman, 2008). Finally, high neuroticism, low
conscientiousness, and low extraversion can reduce resilience, and then Acknowledgments
lead to depressive symptoms (Gong et al., 2020). It is worth noting that
the mediation proportion of all personality traits is <20 %, indicating The authors gratefully acknowledge the research team and partici­
that there are other important mediators between ACEs and depressive pants from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), and
symptoms. Previous studies have reported that self-esteem (Kim et al., gratefully acknowledge technical support from the School of Public
2022), emotion regulation (Hopfinger et al., 2016), C-reactive protein Health, Sun Yat-sen University.
(Iob et al., 2020a), and cortisol (Iob et al., 2021) can mediate the as­
sociation of ACEs with depressive symptoms. Therefore, a study Appendix A. Supplementary data
covering multi-dimensional mediators is needed to comprehensively
clarify the mechanism by which ACEs lead to depressive symptoms. Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
This study has several important strengths. First, the ELSA is a large- org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.03.067.
scale and nationally representative study in older adults, and the study
population was not selected based on exposure to ACEs or the presence
References
of depressive symptoms, so the sample of this study is representative of
the general elderly population. Second, a 9-year prospective cohort Borghans, L., Duckworth, A.L., Heckman, J.J., Ter Weel, B., 2008. The economics and
study design can identify the temporality between personality traits and psychology of personality traits. J. Hum. Resour. 43, 972–1059. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.0042-7092.2007.00700.x.
depressive symptoms, overcoming the shortcomings of previous non-
Clark, L.A., Watson, D., 2008. Temperament: an organizing paradigm for trait
clinical sample studies to some extent. Finally, this study included psychology. In: Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, 3rd ed. The Guilford
multi-dimensional ACEs, enabling us to assess the mediating role of Press, New York, NY, US, pp. 265–286.
personality traits in the effects of ACEs in different dimensions on Cuijpers, P., Vogelzangs, N., Twisk, J., Kleiboer, A., Li, J., Penninx, B.W., 2014.
Comprehensive meta-analysis of excess mortality in depression in the general
depressive symptoms. community versus patients with specific illnesses. Am. J. Psychiatry 171, 453–462.
However, this study still has some limitations. First, ACEs were https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13030325.
retrospectively assessed based on participants' memories of childhood Dagnino, P., Ugarte, M.J., Morales, F., González, S., Saralegui, D., Ehrenthal, J.C., 2020.
Risk factors for adult depression: adverse childhood experiences and personality
experiences. Such assessments might be prone to induce recall bias due functioning. Front. Psychol. 11, 594698 https://doi.org/10.3389/
to participants' motivation, cognitive function, and memory bias, fpsyg.2020.594698.

173
Y. Li et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 331 (2023) 167–174

Damian, R.I., Spengler, M., Sutu, A., Roberts, B.W., 2019. Sixteen going on sixty-six: a Li, M., D'Arcy, C., Meng, X., 2016. Maltreatment in childhood substantially increases the
longitudinal study of personality stability and change across 50 years. J. Pers. Soc. risk of adult depression and anxiety in prospective cohort studies: systematic review,
Psychol. 117, 674–695. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000210. meta-analysis, and proportional attributable fractions. Psychol. Med. 46, 717–730.
Di Gessa, G., Price, D., 2022. The impact of shielding during the COVID-19 pandemic on https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291715002743.
mental health: evidence from the english longitudinal study of ageing. Br. J. Malouff, J.M., Thorsteinsson, E.B., Schutte, N.S., 2005. The relationship between the
Psychiatry 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2022.44. five-factor model of personality and symptoms of clinical disorders: a meta-analysis.
Dudley, N.M., Orvis, K.A., Lebiecki, J.E., Cortina, J.M., 2006. A meta-analytic J. Psychopathol. Behav. 27, 101–114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-005-5384-y.
investigation of conscientiousness in the prediction of job performance: examining Okubo, R., Inoue, T., Hashimoto, N., Suzukawa, A., Tanabe, H., Oka, M., Narita, H.,
the intercorrelations and the incremental validity of narrow traits. J. Appl. Psychol. Ito, K., Kako, Y., Kusumi, I., 2017. The mediator effect of personality traits on the
91, 40–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.1.40. relationship between childhood abuse and depressive symptoms in schizophrenia.
Dumfart, B., Neubauer, A.C., 2016. Conscientiousness is the most powerful noncognitive Psychiatry Res. 257, 126–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.06.039.
predictor of school achievement in adolescents. J. Individ. Differ. 37, 8–15. https:// Parker, G., Tupling, H., Brown, L.B., 1979. A parental bonding instrument. Br. J. Med.
doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000182. Psychol. 52, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1979.tb02487.x.
Gale, C.R., Deary, I.J., Wardle, J., Zaninotto, P., Batty, G.D., 2015. Cognitive ability and Radloff, L.S., 1977. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the
personality as predictors of participation in a national colorectal cancer screening general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1, 385–401. https://doi.org/10.1177/
programme: the english longitudinal study of ageing. J. Epidemiol. Community 014662167700100306.
Health 69, 530–535. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2014-204888. Roberts, B.W., Wood, D., Caspi, A., 2008. The development of personality traits in
Gong, Y., Shi, J., Ding, H., Zhang, M., Kang, C., Wang, K., Yu, Y., Wei, J., Wang, S., adulthood. In: Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, pp. 375–398.
Shao, N., Han, J., 2020. Personality traits and depressive symptoms: the moderating Ronaldson, A., Arias de la Torre, J., Bendayan, R., Yadegarfar, M.E., Rhead, R.,
and mediating effects of resilience in chinese adolescents. J. Affect. Disord. 265, Douiri, A., Armstrong, D., Hatch, S., Hotopf, M., Dregan, A., 2022. Physical
611–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.102. multimorbidity, depressive symptoms, and social participation in adults over 50
Hampson, S.E., Edmonds, G.W., Goldberg, L.R., Dubanoski, J.P., Hillier, T.A., 2013. years of age: findings from the english longitudinal study of ageing. Aging Ment.
Childhood conscientiousness relates to objectively measured adult physical health Health 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2021.2017847.
four decades later. Health Psychol. 32, 925–928. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Shiner, R.L., 2006. In: Temperament and Personality in Childhood, Handbook of
a0031655. Personality Development. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, Mahwah, NJ,
Hardt, J., Rutter, M., 2004. Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood US, pp. 213–230.
experiences: review of the evidence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 260–273. Soto, C.J., 2019. How replicable are links between personality traits and consequential
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00218.x. life outcomes? The life outcomes of personality replication project. Psychol. Sci. 30,
Hill, P.L., Nickel, L.B., Roberts, B.W., 2014. Are you in a healthy relationship? Linking 711–727. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619831612.
conscientiousness to health via implementing and immunizing behaviors. J. Pers. 82, Spinhoven, P., Elzinga, B.M., Van Hemert, A.M., de Rooij, M., Penninx, B.W., 2016.
485–492. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12051. Childhood maltreatment, maladaptive personality types and level and course of
Ho, F.K., Celis-Morales, C., Gray, S.R., Petermann-Rocha, F., Lyall, D., Mackay, D., psychological distress: a six-year longitudinal study. J. Affect. Disord. 191, 100–108.
Sattar, N., Minnis, H., Pell, J.P., 2020. Child maltreatment and cardiovascular https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.036.
disease: quantifying mediation pathways using UK biobank. BMC Med. 18, 143. Steffick, D., 2000. Documentation of Affective Functioning Measures in the Health and
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-020-01603-z. Retirement Study, 2022.
Hopfinger, L., Berking, M., Bockting, C.L., Ebert, D.D., 2016. Emotion regulation Steptoe, A., Breeze, E., Banks, J., Nazroo, J., 2013. Cohort profile: the english
mediates the effect of childhood trauma on depression. J. Affect. Disord. 198, longitudinal study of ageing. Int. J. Epidemiol. 42, 1640–1648. https://doi.org/
189–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.03.050. 10.1093/ije/dys168.
Hovens, J.G., Giltay, E.J., Van Hemert, A.M., Penninx, B.W., 2016. Childhood Steptoe, A., Di Gessa, G., 2021. Mental health and social interactions of older people with
maltreatment and the course of depressive and anxiety disorders: the contribution of physical disabilities in England during the COVID-19 pandemic: a longitudinal
personality characteristics. Depress. Anxiety 33, 27–34. https://doi.org/10.1002/ cohort study. Lancet Public Health 6, e365–e373. https://doi.org/10.1016/s2468-
da.22429. 2667(21)00069-4.
Institute of Health Metrics and Evaluation, 2021. Global Health Data Exchange (GHDx). Stieger, M., Flückiger, C., Rüegger, D., Kowatsch, T., Roberts, B.W., Allemand, M., 2021.
http://ghdx.healthdata.org/gbd-results-tool?params=gbd-api-2019-permalink/d78 Changing personality traits with the help of a digital personality change
0dffbe8a381b25e1416884959e88b. (Accessed 5 January 2022). intervention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 118, e2017548118 https://doi.org/
Iob, E., Baldwin, J.R., Plomin, R., Steptoe, A., 2021. Adverse childhood experiences, 10.1073/pnas.2017548118.
daytime salivary cortisol, and depressive symptoms in early adulthood: a Strickhouser, J.E., Sutin, A.R., 2021. Personality, retirement, and cognitive impairment:
longitudinal genetically informed twin study. Transl. Psychiatry 11, 420. https:// moderating and mediating associations. J. Aging Health 33, 187–196. https://doi.
doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01538-w. org/10.1177/0898264320969080.
Iob, E., Lacey, R., Steptoe, A., 2020a. Adverse childhood experiences and depressive Tingley, D., Teppei, H., Mit, Y., Keele, L., Imai, K., 2014. Mediation: R package for causal
symptoms in later life: longitudinal mediation effects of inflammation. Brain Behav. mediation analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 59, 38.
Immun. 90, 97–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2020.07.045. World Health Organization, 2008. The global burden of disease: 2004 update. https://
Iob, E., Lacey, R., Steptoe, A., 2020b. The long-term association of adverse childhood apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/43942/9789241563710_eng.pdf.
experiences with C-reactive protein and hair cortisol: cumulative risk versus Yazawa, A., Shiba, K., Inoue, Y., Okuzono, S.S., Inoue, K., Kondo, N., Kondo, K.,
dimensions of adversity. Brain Behav. Immun. 87, 318–328. https://doi.org/ Kawachi, I., 2022. Early childhood adversity and late-life depressive symptoms:
10.1016/j.bbi.2019.12.019. unpacking mediation and interaction by adult socioeconomic status. Soc. Psychiatry
JamaBloom, D., Cafiero, E., Jané-Llopis, E., Abrahams-Gessel, S., Weiss, J., 2011. The Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 57, 1147–1156. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-022-02241-
Global Economic Burden of Noncommunicable Diseases. World Economic Forum, x.
Geneva. Yu, T., Hu, J., 2022. Extraversion and neuroticism on college freshmen's depressive
Kern, M.L., Friedman, H.S., 2008. Do conscientious individuals live longer? A symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating role of social support.
quantitative review. Health Psychol. 27, 505–512. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278- Front Psychiatry 13, 822699. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.822699.
6133.27.5.505. Zaninotto, P., Iob, E., Demakakos, P., Steptoe, A., 2022. Immediate and longer-term
Kim, Y., Lee, H., Park, A., 2022. Patterns of adverse childhood experiences and changes in the mental health and well-being of older adults in England during the
depressive symptoms: self-esteem as a mediating mechanism. Soc. Psychiatry COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA Psychiatry 79, 151–159. https://doi.org/10.1001/
Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 57, 331–341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-021-02129-2. jamapsychiatry.2021.3749.
Lachman, M.E., Weaver, S.L., 1997. In: The Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI) Zhang, M., Han, J., Shi, J., Ding, H., Wang, K., Kang, C., Gong, J., 2018. Personality traits
Personality Scales: Scale Construction and Scoring, 7. Brandeis University, Waltham, as possible mediators in the relationship between childhood trauma and depressive
MA, pp. 1–9. symptoms in chinese adolescents. J. Psychiatr. Res. 103, 150–155. https://doi.org/
Lee, M.A., Song, R., 2017. Childhood abuse, personality traits, and depressive symptoms 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.05.019.
in adulthood. Child Abuse Negl. 65, 194–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
chiabu.2017.02.009.

174

You might also like