You are on page 1of 12

Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychoneuroendocrinology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psyneuen

Psychobiological foundations of coping and emotion regulation: Links to


maltreatment and depression in a racially diverse, economically
disadvantaged sample of adolescent girls
Jason José Bendezú a, c, *, Elizabeth D. Handley b, Jody T. Manly b, Sheree L. Toth b,
Dante Cicchetti a
a
The Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota, USA
b
Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester, USA
c
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Adolescent risk for depression and passive or active suicidal ideation (PASI) involves disturbance across multiple
Maltreatment systems (e.g., arousal regulatory, affective valence, neurocognitive). Exposure to maltreatment while growing up
Cortisol as a child or teenager may potentiate this risk by noxiously impacting these systems. However, research
C-reactive protein
exploring how coordinated disturbance across these systems (i.e., profiles) might be uniquely linked to
Adolescent
depressogenic function, and how past maltreatment contributes to such disturbance, is lacking. Utilizing a
Cognitive reappraisal
racially diverse, economically disadvantaged sample of adolescent girls, this person-centered study identified
psychobiological profiles and linked them to maltreatment histories, as well as current depressive symptoms and
PASI. Girls (N = 237, Mage=13.98, SD=0.85) who were non-depressed/non-maltreated (15.1%), depressed/non-
maltreated (40.5%), or depressed/maltreated (44.4%) provided morning saliva samples, completed question­
naires, a clinical interview, and a neurocognitive battery. Latent profile analysis of girls’ morning cortisol:C-
reactive protein ratio, positive and negative affect levels, and attentional set-shifting ability revealed four pro­
files. Relative to Normative (66.6%), girls exhibiting a Pro-inflammatory Affective Disturbance (13.1%), Severe
Affective Disturbance (10.1%), or Hypercortisol Affective Neurocognitive Disturbance (n = 24, 10.1%) profile re­
ported exposure to a greater number of maltreatment subtypes while growing up. Girls exhibiting these dysre­
gulated profiles were also more likely (relative to Normative) to report current depressive symptoms (all three
profiles) and PASI (only Pro-inflammatory Affective Disturbance and Hypercortisol Affective Neurocognitive Distur­
bance). Of note, girls’ cognitive reappraisal utilization moderated profile membership–depression linkages
(depressive symptoms, but not PASI). A synthesis of the findings is presented alongside implications for person-
centered tailoring of intervention efforts.

1. Introduction (Congressional Black Caucus, 2019; NIMH NOT-MH-21–1 202 020),


who are three times more likely to report depression-related sequela
Adolescence is characterized by elevated rates of depressive symp­ than Black boys (Fitzpatrick et al., 2008). Utilizing a racially diverse,
toms and related symptomatology (e.g., passive of active suicidal idea­ economically disadvantaged sample of adolescent girls, the current
tion, PASI; Nock et al., 2013). This may especially be the case for girls, study aimed towards (a) a more nuanced understanding of the multi­
given their increased sensitivity to relational stressors (Hankin et al., system etiology of girls’ risk for depressive symptoms and PASI, (b)
2007) and developmental differences in arousal regulatory, affective, those relational stressors that contribute to such etiology, and (c) to
and neurocognitive function (Beauchaine et al., 2019; Shields et al., identify putative psychosocial buffers to be leveraged in the design of
2017; Slavich and Sacher, 2019). Recent reports have pointed to a more effective interventions.
precipitous rise in depression among Black girls in particular

* Correspondence to: Department of Psychology, S463 Elliott Hall, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA.
E-mail address: bende369@umn.edu (J.J. Bendezú).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105826
Received 10 February 2022; Received in revised form 2 June 2022; Accepted 3 June 2022
Available online 8 June 2022
0306-4530/© 2022 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

1.1. Psychobiological Function and Depressive Symptomatology attentional control, set shifting) may highlight foundations that avail
adolescents the use of more sophisticated regulatory strategies (e.g.,
Despite conceptualizations of adolescent risk for depression as cognitive reappraisal, McRae et al., 2010). However, such a depiction is
involving coordinated disturbance across arousal regulatory, affective, likely to be incomplete without concomitantly considering affective and
and neurocognitive systems (Cha et al., 2019; Glenn et al., 2017), arousal regulatory processes also requisite for efficacious coping and
empirical studies have yet to provide an integrative understanding of emotion regulation.
how risk coalesces across these levels of analysis (Cicchetti and Dawson, In healthy individuals, affective experience such as moderate nega­
2002). Researchers have cited an over-reliance on single system ap­ tive affect helps orient a person to a source of stress (e.g., stressor, in­
proaches as a contributor to this empirical ambiguity (Hostinar et al., ternal response to a stressor) and prompts the need to take action (Selye,
2021), examining systems in isolation or analyzing between-group dif­ 1936). Arousal regulatory processes such as well-orchestrated immu­
ferences in the outcome of interest (e.g., MDD vs. No MDD) across sys­ no-endocrine function (e.g., healthy cortisol suppression of inflamma­
tems. These studies have been useful in pointing to elevated negative tion) mobilize physiological resources needed to take action without the
affect and emotionality (Enns et al., 2003; Fergusson et al., 2000), low onset of sickness behaviors that might impede such action (Slavich and
hedonic capacity (Auerbach et al., 2015), executive function deficits (e. Irwin, 2014). Executive functions such as attentional control and
g., poor attentional control and set shifting ability; Wilkinson and cognitive flexibility support coping efforts insofar as they help a person
Goodyer, 2006), and dysregulated immuno-endocrine functioning (e.g., shift attention away from a negative affect-inducing stressor or think
low cortisol:C-reactive protein (CRP) ratio reflective of pro-inflammatory differently about a stressor in a way that induces positive affect
state, Suarez and Sundy, 2017; high cortisol:CRP ratio reflective of (Eisenberg and Zhou, 2016). For maltreated individuals, dysregulation
hypercortisolemia, Landau et al., 2021) as potential etiological mecha­ across these multiple systems may increase risk for depression by
nisms. Here, we propose that a person-centered approach (Cicchetti and making it difficult to effectively cope with stress or regulate emotions.
Rogosch, 1996; Warmingham et al., 2019) that spans these levels may For example, high negative affect and low positive affect may contribute
provide additional nuanced detail of how coordinated psychobiological to attentional biases that interfere with accurate stressor detection
risk manifests within adolescent girls. (Peckham et al., 2010). Excessive cortisol production that suppresses
immune system function (e.g., cellular immunity) may result in an
1.2. Psychobiological function and maltreatment history immunocompromised state that hampers ability to manage stressors
(Shields et al., 2017). Insufficient cortisol production relative to in­
The caregiver-youth relationship is one context in which some chil­ flammatory activity may promote fatigue and withdrawal behavior in
dren experience acute and chronic forms of relational stress. Child place of active coping (Slavich and Irwin, 2014). Inability to shift one’s
maltreatment, specifically (i.e., severely adverse childhood experiences attention or perspective may contribute to perseveration on a stressor
such as emotional, sexual, and physical abuse, as well as physical and and limit cognitive access to alternate coping options (Uddin, 2021).
emotional neglect; Cicchetti and Toth, 2016), is known to potentiate risk Whether profiles of psychobiological function may be more (e.g.,
for depressive symptoms (Humphreys et al., 2020) and PASI (Duprey healthy) or less (e.g., unhealthy, linked to maltreatment) amenable to
et al., 2021). Rates of child maltreatment are approximately twice as the use of specific strategies and, thereby, decrease or increase depres­
high for Black relative to White youth (Sedlak et al., 2010), with studies sogenic function (Bendezú et al., 2021a, 2021b) is not yet known.
pointing to systemic biases in reporting systems as contributors (e.g., Identification of such profiles may inform the tailoring of treatment
suspected maltreatment for Black youth is more likely to be reported, efforts to girls’ psychobiological strengths and weaknesses.
proceed to investigation, and be substantiated; Detlaff & Boyd, 2020).
Also, Black youth with a history of maltreatment are more than five 1.4. The current study: aims and hypotheses
times as likely to report suicidality relative to those without (Fitzpatrick
et al., 2008). Evidence suggests that one salient manner by which past The current study had the following aims and hypotheses. Aim 1.
maltreatment confers the risk is via insults sustained to youth psycho­ Explore within-person patterns (i.e., profiles) of immuno-endocrine (i.e.,
biological function. A history of maltreatment is known to contribute to cortisol:CRP ratio1), affective (i.e., self-reported positive (PA) and
dysregulated hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and immune negative (NA) affect), and neurocognitive (i.e., attentional set shifting)
system processes (Coelho et al., 2014; Doom et al., 2013), affect dys­ function in a racially diverse, economically disadvantaged sample of
regulation (Courtney-Seidler et al., 2014; Crowell et al., 2009), and adolescent girls. Based on existing maltreatment, depression, and
impaired neurocognitive function (Cowell et al., 2015; Valentino et al., emotion regulation research in the developmental psychobiology liter­
2009). However, similar to the depression etiology literature, much of ature,2 we expected to identify two profiles: Normative (e.g., moderate
this work has examined these systems in isolation as well as by cortisol:CRP ratio, low NA, high PA, high attentional set shifting),
comparing youth with and without a history of maltreatment. As such, Multisystem Disturbance (e.g., high or low cortisol:CRP ratio, high NA,
whether past maltreatment exposure contributes to heterogeneity in
coordinated psychobiological function is not yet known.
1
Use of the cortisol:CRP ratio is relatively new and consensus on what a low
1.3. Psychobiological foundations of coping and emotion regulation versus high ratio represents has not yet been reached (Landau et al., 2021).
However, we argue that including the cortisol:CRP ratio in a person-centered
From a coping and emotion regulation perspective, the manner by framework such as ours has the potential to provide additional clarity insofar
which past maltreatment contributes to emerging risk for psychopa­ as profiles of psychobiological function allow the cortisol:CRP ratio to connect
thology may be through adolescents’ compromised ability to effica­ with affective and neurocognitive function within persons, perhaps in ways that
ciously mange stressors or their involuntary stress responses to them (e. might compliment or extend current interpretation.
2
g., emotional reactivity, rumination). Indeed, evidence increasingly Our focus on these specific indices was also informed by evidence of their
association with all three of our focal study variables: maltreatment (e.g., affect,
suggests that maltreatment exposure may contribute to this risk by
Courtney-Seidler et al., 2014; attentional set-shifting, Gould et al., 2012;
noxiously affecting the psychobiological substrates that support coping
cortisol and C-reactive protein, Doom et al., 2013; Ehrlich et al., 2021),
and emotion regulation and, thus, constrain efficacious strategy use depressogenic function (e.g., affect, Enns et al., 2003; attentional set-shifting,
(Gruhn and Compas, 2020). As noted by Cicchetti and Bendezú (in Wilkinson and Goodyer, 2006; cortisol and C-reactive protein, Landau et al.,
press) and others (Compas et al., 2017), a more nuanced understanding 2021), cognitive reappraisal (e.g., affect, Andreotti et al., 2013; attentional
of the psychobiological foundations of coping and emotion regulation is set-shifting, Eisenberg and Zhou, 2016; cortisol and C-reactive protein; Johnson
needed. Attention to specific aspects of executive function (e.g., et al., 2019).

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

low PA, low attentional set shifting). However, we also explored the different rooms.
possibility of additional profiles that previous research may have over­
looked due to reliance on single-system approaches and predetermined 2.3. Primary measures
patterns. Aim 2. Examine maltreatment history as a correlate of profile
membership. We expected the likelihood of membership in Multisystem 2.3.1. Immuno-endocrine regulation
Disturbance (relative to Normative) to increase with a history of expo­ Girls collected saliva samples on two consecutive weekdays via
sure to a greater number of maltreatment subtypes. Aim 3. Examine Trident stimulated passive drool immediately upon waking
profile membership to depression linkages and cognitive reappraisal as a (Mwake_time=7:43 am, SD=2:12), circumventing morning cortisol waking
moderator of those linkages. We expected girls with Multisystem response (Susman et al., 2007) and morning routine (e.g., food or drink
Disturbance (relative to Normative) profiles to report greater current consumption, tooth brushing, smoking) data contamination. The deci­
depressive symptoms and PASI. We expected reappraisal to moderate sion to utilize morning cortisol and CRP was motivated by a desire to
profile membership to depression linkages, such that reappraisal would keep methodologically consistent with the only study to date that has
be associated with lower depressive symptoms and decreased likelihood examined the cortisol:CRP ratio with saliva in relation to depression in
of PASI for girls with Normative profiles, and not associated (i.e., stably an adolescent sample (Landau et al., 2021). Girls stored samples in their
elevated) or positively associated with depressive symptoms and PASI (i. home refrigerators they could be collected by study staff and stored in a
e., stably high likelihood) for girls with Multisystem Disturbance medical grade ultra-low temperature freezer (− 40◦ C). Samples were
profiles. shipped on dry ice to Salimetrics Laboratories (State College, PA) for
assay. Salivary cortisol (µg/dl) and C-reactive protein (CRP) (pg/mL)
2. Method were assayed using an enzyme immunoassay kit (Salimetrics, State
College, PA). The average intra- and inter-assay coefficient of variation
2.1. Participants for cortisol and CRP were < 5.0%, 10.0% and 10.0%, 15.0%, respec­
tively. Biomarker levels on each consecutive weekday were averaged to
Adolescent girls (N = 237, Mage=13.98, SD=0.85) were recruited create single cortisol (Cortave) and CRP (CRPave) indices.
from an urban setting in upstate New York to participate in a random­
ized control trial of a depression intervention. We utilized data from the 2.3.2. Affect
larger study’s baseline assessment. Participants consisted of three Girls reported on the frequency of certain feelings and emotions in
different groups: girls with subsyndromal symptoms of depression or the past two weeks using the Positive and Negative Affect Scale for
who met criteria for clinical depression with (MDD/MAL; 44.4%) and Children (PANAS-C; Laurent et al., 1999). Positive affect (PA) and
without (MDD/NO MAL; 40.5%) a history of maltreatment, and controls negative affect (NA) scores were computed by averaging across 12 PA
who were non-depressed and non-maltreated (NO MDD/NO MAL; items (Interested, Excited, Happy, Strong, Energetic, Calm, Cheerful,
15.1%). Most girls were Black (62.4%), followed by White (23.2%), Active, Proud, Joyful, Delighted, Lively; α = 0.92) and 15 NA items (Sad,
multiracial (3.8%), Asian (0.4%), American Indian or Alaskan Native Frightened, Ashamed, Upset, Nervous, Guilty, Scared, Miserable, Jittery,
(0.4%), and Other (6.8%). Also, 12.2% of girls were Latina. Average Afraid, Lonely, Mad, Disgusted, Blue, Gloomy; α = 0.89). Higher scores
annual household income was $28 353 (SD=$12 734). reflect greater frequency of experienced PA and NA.

2.2. Procedures 2.3.3. Neurocognitive function


Girls were administered the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test
All study procedures were approved by the University’s Institutional Automated Battery (CANTAB Eclipse, Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge,
Review Board (IRB). As described in detail elsewhere (MASKED et al., UK). CANTAB tests several neurocognitive processes and executive
2018), informed consent and assent were obtained from parents and functions. We utilized the Intradimensional–Extradimensional (IDED)
girls, respectively. By design, all families participating in the study were Set Shifting module, which tests capacity for attentional set formation,
required to be eligible for Temporary Assistance to Needy Families and the maintenance, shifting, and flexibility of attention. The module
(TANF). Efforts to recruit girls with a history of maltreatment included consists of nine stages. Participants complete a stage after providing six
collaborations with University Medical Center’s pediatric social consecutive correct responses (i.e., establishing an attentional set).
workers, local schools and mental health organizations, and a liaison in Stage 1 consists of simple discrimination (SD), where participants must
the Department of Human Services (DHS) who examined Child Protec­ correctly choose one of two stimuli of same dimension (e.g. “color-filled
tive Services (CPS) reports for histories of maltreatment. Collaborators shapes”). Stage 2 consists of simple reversal (SR), where contingencies
at these various institutions contacted potentially eligible families. change and the previously correct stimuli is now incorrect. Stage 3
Interested parents signed a release allowing their contact information to consists of compound discrimination (C_D), where the second dimension
be shared with study staff. Nonmaltreating families were also recruited is introduced (e.g., “white line”) as a distractor and the participant must
from a pool of participants similarly eligible for TANF. Restricting in­ continue selecting the previously correct stimuli. At Stage 4, stimulus
come enabled examination of variability in maltreatment-related phe­ complexity increases as stimuli from the second dimension become
nomena without the potentially overriding contributions of superimposed on the stimuli from the first dimension. Stage 5 consists of
socioeconomic status (Sedlak et al., 2010). To ensure the integrity of the compound discrimination reversal (CDR), where the stimulus from the
data (e.g., variation due to reading ability and literacy), interviews and first dimension with a superimposed stimulus from the second dimen­
questionnaires were read to participants, who read along and marked sion that was previously incorrect now becomes the correct choice.
their own answers. Interviewers were graduate students of clinical Stage 6 consists of an intra-dimensional shift (IDS), where participants
psychology and post-bac research assistants. In addition to coursework are presented with new versions of the stimuli from the two dimensions.
in cultural competency as part of their clinical training, graduate stu­ Participants must continue to correctly select the stimuli from the first
dents received training on cultural sensitivity via our standard research dimension. Stage 7 consists of an intra-dimensional shift reversal (IDR),
protocol given our population, as did post-bac research assistants. where participants must correctly select the previously incorrect stimuli
Questionnaires were checked by interviewers to ensure that items were from the first dimension. Stage 8 consists of an extra-dimensional shift
not skipped and that answers marked by participants were matched to (EDS), where participants are again presented with new versions of the
the appropriate item. Neuropsychological tests were nonverbal, stimuli from the two dimensions. Here, participants must shift attention
administered with touchscreen technology, with instructions read to from the first dimension to the second dimension, choosing the correct
participants. Girls and their mothers were interviewed concurrently in stimuli from the second dimension rather than attempting to choose one

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

of the two stimuli from the now incorrect first dimension. Stage 9 con­ 2.4. Covariates
sists of an extra-dimensional shift reversal (EDR), where participants
must shift the focus of their selection back to stimuli from the previously Covariates include chronological age, pubertal status, income to
incorrect first dimension. The module terminates when a participant needs ratio (INR), body mass index (BMI), medication use, and morning
completes all nine stages or when the participant fails to establish an saliva sample time. Though chronological age is confounded with pu­
attentional set on any given stage after 50 trials. We utilized number of bertal status, it also affects HPA and immune system function in ways
stages completed as an index of attentional set-shifting ability (M=8.21, not directly accounted for by reproductive hormones (e.g., regulatory
SD=1.21, Min=2.00, Max=9.00). gland size and structure; Linton and Dorshkind, 2004). Chronological
age was, thus, controlled for in Aims 2 and 3 analyses. Girls completed
2.3.4. Maltreatment history the Pubertal Development Scale (Petersen et al., 1988), which consists of
Girls completed the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; Bern­ five Likert type items (1 =no development, 4 =development seems
stein et al., 2003), a 28-item measure assessing girls’ experiences of complete) about physical development, including body hair, skin
maltreatment, abuse, and neglect as a child and teenager while growing changes, growth spurt, breast development and menarche (dichoto­
up. Girls rated a series of statements about specific maltreatment ex­ mous). A mean score (M=3.35, SD=0.48) was computed across items (α
periences on a 5-point scale (1 =never true, 5 =very often true). = 0.62). Given that poverty has been linked to immuno-endocrine
Maltreatment subtypes include emotional abuse (e.g., “People in my dysregulation (for review, see Jensen et al., 2017) and executive func­
family called me things like stupid, lazy, or ugly.”), physical abuse (e.g., tion deficits (for review, see Blair and Raver, 2016), we controlled for
“People in my family hit me so hard that it left me with bruises or INR in Aims 2 and 3 analyses. INR is an index of household income
marks.”), sexual abuse (e.g., “Someone tried to touch me in a sexual way, relative to national poverty line norms (adjusted for number of family
or tried to make me touch them.”), emotional neglect (e.g., “I thought members residing in the household). Middle-income family INR ranges
that my parents wished I had never been born.”), and physical neglect (e. between 2.0 and 4.0 while an INR of 1.0 is indicative of poverty (Duncan
g., “I did not have enough to eat.”). Subscales demonstrated good in­ et al., 1993; Evans and Marcynyszyn, 2004). Though the sample was low
ternal consistency (α = 0.71–0.96). Following Walker et al. (1999), income, an examination of INR descriptive statistics suggested that there
severity ratings for each subtype were dichotomized to indicate whether was variability with respect economic disadvantage to be considered
adolescents reported clinically significant maltreatment: emotional (M=1.13, SD=0.49). Though 45.4% of the sample lived at or below the
abuse (nyes=63), physical abuse (nyes=55), sexual abuse (nyes=30), poverty line, 44.0% of families lived above the poverty line, with 10.6%
emotional neglect (nyes=28), physical neglect (nyes=34). A count vari­ of families approaching or meeting middle income status cutoffs. As
able was created by summing the number of maltreatment subtypes obesity is also associated with a number of our study variables (e.g.,
experienced. inflammation, executive function; Spyridaki et al., 2016; Yang et al.,
2018), we controlled for BMI in Aims 2 and 3 analyses. Girls’ height and
2.3.5. Cognitive reappraisal weight were obtained during their study visit. Age-specific BMI per­
Girls reported on their regulatory strategy use using the Emotion centiles calculated following Center for Disease Control (CDC) guide­
Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross and John, 2003). The scale in­ lines (M=81.93, SD=20.27). Medications known to influence
cludes 10 Likert-type items (1 =Strongly Disagree, 4 =Strongly Agree). immune-endocrine function or saliva collection were noted (e.g., corti­
Six items assess cognitive reappraisal (e.g., “I control my feelings about costeroids, anxiolytics, mood stabilizers, selective serotonin reuptake
things by changing the way I think about them.”, α = 0.74) and four inhibitors, birth control; Granger et al., 2009). A dichotomous variable
items assess expressive suppression (e.g., “I control my feelings by not was created for girls who were (yes=1) and were not (no=0) taking such
showing them.”, α = 0.67). Researchers have observed that certain medications (nyes=47). Because wake time can impact morning cortisol
children (e.g., stress-affected, emotional and behavioral difficulties, levels (Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2003), we controlled for girls’ waking
girls) tend toward greater overall endorsement of strategy utilization saliva sample time in Aims 2 and 3 analyses.
items and have, thus, recommended the use of ratio scores to account for
such potential differences in item endorsement base rates (Connor-­ 2.5. Data preparation
Smith et al., 2000; Hilt et al., 2010). As such, we divided the cognitive
reappraisal score (i.e., average of cognitive reappraisal items) by the 2.5.1. Cortisol:CRP ratio
sum of all scale item ratings. The resulting ratio score reflects the pre­ As per Suarez and Sundy (2017), we divided Cortave by CRPave and
dominance of cognitive reappraisal utilized as a strategy for managing the resulting ratio was log10 transformed for latent profile analysis. No
stressors and difficult emotions. outliers (+/- 3 SDs from the grand mean) were noted in the transformed
cortisol:CRP ratio variable.
2.3.6. Depressive symptoms
Girls reported on current depressive symptoms using the Beck 2.5.2. IDED set-shifting
Depression Inventory for Youth (BDI-Y; Beck et al., 2005). The scale A ceiling effect emerged for IDED Set Shifting – stages completed,
includes 20 items (0 =Never, 3 =Always). Raw total scores (α = 0.93) which has been observed in other studies of adolescent mood disorders
were converted to T-scores to ensure scale rating integrity. (e.g., Dickstein et al., 2007; Dickstein et al., 2016). In all, 61.2% of girls
completed Stage 9, 7.5% completed only up to Stage 8 (i.e., did not
2.3.7. Passive or active suicidal ideation (PASI) complete Stage 9), 29.3% completed only up to Stage 7 (i.e., did not
Girls and a parent reported on the frequency of girls’ current complete Stage 8 or 9), 0.7% completed only up to Stage 4, and 1.4%
thoughts of death (e.g., “Sometime children who get upset or feel bad, completed only up to Stage 2. Following Leeson et al. (2009) as well as
wish they were dead or feel they’d be better off dead.”) and suicide (e.g., others (Barnett et al., 2005; Jazbec et al., 2007), a “pass/fail” variable
“Sometimes children who get upset or feel bad think about dying or even was created (0 = module not completed, 1 = module completed). Given
killing themselves.”) via the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and that the majority of participants who did not complete the IDED module
Schizophrenia-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS–PL; Kaufman had difficulty completing Stages 7 and 8 which consists of the
et al., 1997). Consensus was established across interviews and the extra-dimensional (ED) shift, our dichotomized IDED variable can be
summary score was used. PASI (nyes=55) was indicated by a threshold (i. thought to reflect attentional set-shifting proficiency and inefficiency
e., “recurrent”) summary score of the frequency of thoughts of death or with respect to ED set-shifting ability in particular. This dichotomous
suicide. variable was modeled as a categorical indicator in our latent profile
analyses.

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

2.6. Overview of analyses In our final model, profile membership by cognitive reappraisal (grand
mean centered) interactive effects were examined.
2.6.1. Aim 1
To identify subgroups of girls with unique psychobiological profiles, 2.6.4. Post-hoc analyses
latent profile analysis (LPA) via MPLUS (Version 8; Muthén & Muthén, To shed light on ways that a multisystem, person-centered approach
1998–2017) was used and classified girls based on the extent to which provided unique information relative to conventional methods, we
they exhibited similar within-person patterns of psychobiological func­ compared girls with and without maltreatment histories on our study
tion across our four indicators of interest: cortisol:CRP ratio, negative variables of interest. Specifically, we conducted t-tests comparing these
affect, positive affect, IDED set shifting. Model specification began with two groups on our psychobiological indicators, cognitive reappraisal,
a one-class solution and additional, more complex solutions (e.g., and depressive functioning.
two-class, three-class) were evaluated across a series of model fit sta­
tistics: Akaike information criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1973), Bayesian in­ 3. Results
formation criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978), adjusted Bayesian
information criterion (adjusted BIC; Sclove, 1987). Simulations studies Results are organized by study aim. Descriptive statistics and
have demonstrated BIC and Adjusted BIC to be conservative fit statistics bivariate correlations for key study variables are presented in Table 1.
for determining superior class specification (Magidson and Vermunt, Cortisol levels across days were positively correlated, as were CRP
2004; Yang, 2006), with lower values indicating the most optimal so­ levels. With the exception of positive and negative affect, which were
lution. The entropy statistic was also utilized during model specification inversely correlated, no significant between-person correlations across
(Celeux and Soromenho, 1996), with values approaching 1 indicative of our psychobiological indicators of interest emerged, further supporting
a superior solution. Though we expected to identify at least two profiles, our aim of examining within-person profiles of psychobiological func­
we allowed both theory and model fit indices to guide us to the most tioning. Negative affect was positively correlated with number of
informative solution (e.g., 3-profile, 4-profile; Masyn, 2013; Tein et al., maltreatment subtypes, depressive symptoms, and PASI. Positive affect
2013). At present, there are no firm guidelines on determining the was not associated with number of maltreatment subtypes, but nega­
appropriateness of subgroup size in LPA. However, LPA methodologists tively correlated with depressive symptoms and PASI. Cortisol, CRP, and
have previously suggested that subgroup sizes should not be smaller IDED set shifting were not correlated with number of maltreatment
than 5% of the overall sample size (Weller et al., 2020). Once specified, subtypes, depressive symptoms, or PASI. Cognitive reappraisal was
girls were categorized into subgroups based on the highest posterior positively correlated with positive affect and negatively correlated with
probability of class membership. After categorization, distinction ana­ negative affect, number of maltreatment subtypes and depressive
lyses comparing mean indicator levels across systems and subgroups symptoms. Depressive symptoms were positively correlated with PASI.
helped to characterize the profiles and informed our labeling
conventions. 3.1. Aim 1: psychobiological foundation profiles

2.6.2. Aim 2 LPA model specification supported a four-profile solution (Table S1,
Multinomial logistic regression was used to examine number of see supplementary materials). Model fit indices did not uniformly settle
maltreatment subtypes as a correlate of profile membership. Simulation on a single solution. As recommended (Masyn, 2013; Tein et al., 2013),
studies have shown that examining correlates of categorical profile we utilized both theory and model fit indices as conceptual and statis­
membership (i.e., assigning participants to subgroups based on the tical guide posts and determined that the four profile solution was most
highest posterior probability of class membership) is suitable for latent informative. Profile specific means and standard deviations for each
profile analysis solutions with an entropy greater than .80 (Clark and continuous indicator in each profile are presented in Table S2 (see
Muth´en, 2009). Study covariates (e.g., age, pubertal status, INR, BMI, supplementary materials). Proportions are reported for binary in­
medication use, and morning saliva sample time) and number of dicators in each profile (e.g., proportion of participants completing the
maltreatment subtypes were examined in a single model. IDED set-shifting module). To aid interpretation of the cortisol:CRP
ratio, raw and standardized cortisol and CRP levels for each profile are
2.6.3. Aim 3 presented in Table S3 (see supplementary materials). Profile specific
Three regression models were used to examine a) profile member­ deviations from the sample mean or probability for continuous and bi­
ship to depressive symptoms (multiple linear regression) and PASI (lo­ nary indicators, respectively, in each profile are graphically depicted in
gistic regression) linkages, and b) cognitive reappraisal as a moderator Fig. 1. The Normative profile was consistent with expectation, the largest
of those linkages. As a more stringent test of the predictive capacity of of all groups (n = 15 866.6%), and characterized by moderate cortisol:
our identified profiles, the number of maltreatment subtypes girls CRP ratio levels, low negative affect levels, high positive affect levels,
experienced was also included as a covariate in addition to our estab­ and moderate probability of completing the IDED set-shifting module.
lished set of study covariates.3 In two initial models, profile membership Three smaller subgroups emerged whose profiles reflected various
and cognitive reappraisal were examined independently as predictors. within-person patterns of psychobiological dysregulation. The Pro-in­
flammatory Affective Disturbance profile (n = 31, 13.1%) was character­
ized by the lowest cortisol:CRP ratio levels in the sample, high negative
affect levels, low positive affect levels, but also the highest probability of
3
Aim 3 analyses included a small group of non-depressed, non-maltreated completing the IDED set-shifting module in the sample. The Severe Af­
girls (15.1%). These girls were included in our Aim 1 and 2 analyses given our fective Disturbance subgroup (n = 24, 10.1%) was characterized by
interest in identifying potentially well-regulated and dysregulated psychobio­ moderate cortisol:CRP ratio levels, the highest negative affect and
logical profiles and linking those profiles to maltreatment history. While lowest positive affect levels in the sample, and moderate probability of
including number of maltreatment subtypes as covariate in models predicting completing the IDED set-shifting module. The Hypercortisol Affective
depression poses certain methodological issues when including this small group
Neurocognitive Disturbance (n = 24, 10.1%) subgroup was characterized
of girls (i.e., conflation by design as they were both non-depressed and non-
by the highest cortisol:CRP ratio levels in the sample, high negative
maltreated), our aim was less focused on predicting depression via maltreat­
ment and more focused on examining the contribution of our identified profiles affect levels, moderate positive affect levels, and the lowest probability
to depression beyond that accounted for by maltreatment. Also, Aim 3 analyses of completing the IDED set-shifting module in the sample.
conducted with this group removed returned similar results and did not alter
conclusions. Thus, this group was retained in our Aim 3 analyses.

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

Table 1
Descriptives and Correlations for Key Study Variables.
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11.

1. Cortisol (µg/dl) – day 1 —


2. Cortisol (µg/dl) – day 2 0.43 * —
3. CRP (pg/mL) – day 1 -0.11 -0.07 —
4. CRP (pg/mL) – day 2 -0.11 -0.07 0.60 * —
5. Negative affect -0.01 0.04 -0.04 -0.08 —
6. Positive affect 0.01 0.04 -0.06 -0.04 -0.28 * —
7. IDED set-shiftinga -.11 -0.06 0.08 0.05 -0.04 -0.08 —
8. Maltreatment subtypes -0.03 0.08 -0.05 -0.07 0.36 * -0.17 0.02 —
9. Cognitive reappraisal 0.04 0.03 -0.07 -0.11 -0.19 * 0.35 * -0.04 -0.28 * —
10. Depressive symptoms 0.06 0.02 -0.05 -0.02 0.67 * -0.37 * -0.08 0.37 * -0.35 * —
11. PASIa .11 0.02 0.01 -0.03 0.39 * -0.21 * -0.14 0.19 * -0.17 0.45 * —
Means (Proportions) 0.33 0.30 6419.07 6613.28 1.90 3.11 (0.61) 0.91 0.55 48.94 (0.23)
SD 0.24 0.23 15,104.7 17,629.2 0.68 0.68 0.49 1.28 0.08 12.06 0.42
Min 0.01 0.02 102.60 135.11 1.00 1.08 0.00 0.00 0.25 34.00 0.00
Max 1.49 1.29 115,322.7 181,512.4 4.00 5.00 1.00 5.00 0.78 100.00 1.00

Note. CRP = C-reactive protein; IDED = Intra Dimensional-Extra Dimensional, PASI = Passive or active suicidal ideation. IDED set-shifting coded 0 for “module not
completed” and 1 for “module completed.” PASI coded 0 for “not indicated” and 1 for “indicated.”
a
= Spearman’s Rho
*p < .05.

Fig. 1. Profile specific deviations from the sample mean for psychobiological indices.

3.2. Aim 2: maltreatment history correlates of psychobiological


Table 2
foundation profiles
Odds Ratios (95% Lower CI, 95% Upper CI) for Multinomial Logistic Regressions
Predicting Profile Membership.
Parameter estimates for our multinomial logistic regression models
Comparison Pro-inflammatory Severe Hypercortisol
are shown in Table 2. Medication use and saliva sample time were not
Profile Affective Affective Affective
associated with our dependent variables in Aim 2 or 3 analyses and Disturbance Disturbance Neurocognitive
were, thus, removed in the interest of model parsimony.4 The Normative Disturbance
profile (i.e., largest, healthy) was used as the reference profile. Our co­ Age 1.08 (0.65, 1.78) 0.91 (0.49, 1.23 (0.70, 2.16)
variate model was not significant. Neither age, pubertal status, INR, or 1.69)
BMI were associated with profile membership. Our maltreatment model Pubertal status 0.77 (0.33, 1.76) 1.10 (0.37, 0.86 (0.33, 2.27)
was significant. As hypothesized, relative to Normative, girls exhibiting 3.25)
Income-to-needs 0.99 (0.43, 2.31) 1.98 (0.75, 1.15 (0.46, 2.89)
any of the three dysregulated profiles were more likely to have experi­
ratio 5.22)
enced a greater number of maltreatment subtypes while growing up. Body mass index 1.00 (0.98, 1.02) 1.01 (0.98, 1.00 (0.97, 1.02)
1.04)
3.3. Aim 3: psychobiological foundation profile to depressive function Number of 1.44 * (1.04, 2.00) 2.06 * (1.47, 1.66 * (1.18, 2.34)
linkages maltreatment 2.89)
subtypes
Х2 (df) 27.80 (15)
3.3.1. Depressive symptoms Nagelkerke’s R2 .14
Parameter estimates for our linear regression models predicting
Note. CI = confidence interval. The Normative profile served as the reference
group.
* p < .05.
4
Similar to the findings controlling for medication use more generally,
neither corticosteroid (nyes=31) or birth control (nyes=10) use were associated
with our dependent variables in Aim 2 or 3 analyses. They were, thus, omitted
from Aim 2 and 3 analyses in the interest of model parsimony.

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

Table 3 depressive symptoms. Cognitive reappraisal was negatively associated


Parameter Estimates for a Nested Taxonomy of Multiple Linear Regression with depressive symptoms. A significant interaction between subgroup
Models Predicting Depressive Symptoms. membership and cognitive reappraisal emerged (Fig. 2). For Normative
Profile Cognitive Profile × and Pro-inflammatory Affective Disturbance girls, cognitive reappraisal
Reappraisal Cognitive was not associated with depressive symptoms (i.e., stably low). How­
Reappraisal ever, for girls exhibiting the Severe Affective Disturbance or Hypercortisol
Independent B SE B SE B SE Affective Neurocognitive Disturbance profiles, cognitive reappraisal was
Variables negatively associated with depressive symptoms.
Intercept 32.33 * 11.06 43.08 * 11.27 30.48 * 10.73
Age 0.62 0.78 0.76 0.76 0.62 0.75 3.3.2. PASI
Pubertal status -0.67 1.34 -0.41 1.31 0.30 1.30 Parameter estimates for logistic regression models predicting PASI
Income-to-needs 1.03 1.29 1.62 1.28 1.23 1.26
ratio
are shown in Table 4. Age, pubertal status, and INR were not associated
Body mass index 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.02 0.03 with PASI. However, BMI and number of maltreatment subtypes were
Number of 2.52 * 0.54 2.29 * 0.53 2.10 * 0.53 associated with increased likelihood of PASI. Relative to Normative, girls
maltreatment exhibiting the Pro-inflammatory Affective Disturbance or Hypercortisol
subtypes
Affective Neurocognitive Disturbance profiles (but not Severe Affective
Profilea
Pro-inflammatory 8.55 * 1.89 8.04 * 1.85 8.59 * 1.84 Disturbance) were more likely to report PASI.
Affective Reappraisal was not associated with PASI. No significant profile
Disturbance membership by cognitive reappraisal interaction emerged.
Severe Affective 16.47 * 2.27 16.30 * 2.22 16.42 * 2.17
Disturbance
Hypercortisol 13.74 * 2.11 13.51 * 2.06 13.39 * 2.02 3.4. Post-hoc analyses
Affective
Neurocognitive
A comparison of girls with and without maltreatment histories on
Disturbance
Cognitive -25.17 * 7.56 our psychobiological indicators of interest, cognitive reappraisal,
reappraisal depressive symptoms, and PASI is shown in Table S4 (see supplementary
Normative -9.90 9.65 materials). Differences between girls with and without maltreatment
× cognitive
histories emerged on negative affect, positive affect, depressive symp­
reappraisal
Pro-inflammatory 2.54 21.97 toms, and PASI. No other significant differences emerged.
Affective
Disturbance 4. Discussion
× cognitive
reappraisal
Severe Affective -50.54 * 20.94 Utilizing a racially diverse, economically disadvantaged sample of
Disturbance adolescent girls, the current study identified distinct profiles of psy­
× cognitive chobiological function that were meaningfully associated with girls’
reappraisal past maltreatment exposure and current depressogenic functioning. Our
Hypercortisol -63.59 * 23.59
Affective
Neurocognitive Table 4
Disturbance Parameter estimates for a nested taxonomy of logistic regression models pre­
× cognitive dicting passive or active suicidal ideation.
reappraisal
Profile Cognitive Profile ×
a
= Normative served as the reference profile. Reappraisal Cognitive
* p < .05. Reappraisal

Independent Variables B SE B SE B SE
current depressive symptoms are shown in Table 3. Neither age, pu­ Intercept -4.87 3.36 -3.81 3.48 -5.69 3.52
bertal status, INR, or BMI were associated with depressive symptoms. Age 0.03 0.23 0.05 0.23 0.05 0.26
Number of maltreatment subtypes was positively associated with Pubertal status 0.08 0.39 0.11 0.39 0.12 0.41
depressive symptoms. Relative to Normative, girls exhibiting any of the Income-to-needs ratio -0.20 0.40 -0.14 0.40 -0.22 0.41
Body mass index 0.03 * 0.01 0.03 * 0.01 0.03 * 0.01
three dysregulated profiles were more likely to report elevated
Number of maltreatment 0.50 * 0.14 0.48 * 0.14 0.45 * 0.15
subtypes
Profilea
Pro-inflammatory Affective 1.40 * 0.49 1.36 * 0.49 1.32 * 0.52
Disturbance
Severe Affective Disturbance 0.98 0.57 0.95 0.58 0.88 0.66
Hypercortisol Affective 1.19 * 0.56 1.14 * 0.56 1.12 0.60
Neurocognitive Disturbance
Cognitive reappraisal -2.78 2.31
Normative × cognitive 3.39 3.42
reappraisal
Pro-inflammatory Affective -10.70 6.49
Disturbance × cognitive
reappraisal
Severe Affective Disturbance -15.16 7.94
× cognitive reappraisal
Hypercortisol Affective -9.38 7.80
Neurocognitive Disturbance
× cognitive reappraisal
Fig. 2. Moderation effects of cognitive reappraisal on profile membership to
a
girls’ depressive symptoms. Effects plotted at - 1 SD and + 1 SD for illustra­ = Normative served as the reference profile.
tive purposes. * p < .05.

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

adoption of a person-centered (Cicchetti and Rogosch, 1996), multiple monocytes, macrophages) become less sensitive to cortisol’s anti-
levels of analysis (Cicchetti and Dawson, 2002) approach helped illus­ inflammatory signaling properties (Slavich and Irwin, 2014).
trate varied ways in which coordinated disturbance across the Pro-inflammatory states can also contribute to depressive states
immuno-endocrine, affective valence, neurocognitive systems manifest (Anacker et al., 2011; Dantzer, 2001), which is consistent with the
within adolescents (Cha et al., 2019; Glenn et al., 2017). Specifically, observation that these girls also exhibited high negative affect and low
latent profile analysis (LPA) of girls’ morning cortisol:CRP ratios, posi­ positive affect levels. Notably, our simultaneous attention to neuro­
tive affect levels, negative affect levels, and attentional set shifting ca­ cognitive systems revealed a potential psychobiological protective fac­
pacities revealed four profiles. These profiles connected with tor: better than average attentional set shifting skills. The capacity to
maltreatment and maladjustment in ways that potentially point to effortfully focus attention on task-related demands and intentionally
person-specific psychobiological mechanisms the may underlie mal­ shift this focus in service of goal-directed behavior is thought to be a core
treated girls’ risk for depression during adolescence. Interestingly, component for the modulation of emotion (e.g., felt sadness; Eisenberg
cognitive reappraisal to depression linkages varied across profiles, and Zhou, 2016), perhaps reflected in these girls relatively mild eleva­
highlighting various sources of psychobiological resilience that might tions in negative affect. That Pro-inflammatory Affective Disturbance girls
mitigate such risk by differentially supporting the use of this potentially exhibited better than average attentional set-shifting capacity is also
efficacious strategy. Our findings provide nuanced detail of the psy­ consonant with claims about the adaptive function of chronic
chobiological foundations of coping and emotion regulation (Compas stress-related attenuated cortisol release patterns, which are thought to
et al., 2017), with implications for tailoring of prevention and inter­ protect adolescents’ developing brains (e.g., hippocampus, prefrontal
vention efforts targeting the emergence of depression in maltreated cortex) against the potential neurotoxic effects of cortisol overexposure
adolescent girls. (Fries et al., 2005; Gunnar and Vazquez, 2001).
Our study identified four profiles, one reflecting healthy psychobi­ Girls exhibiting the Severe Affective Disturbance profile displayed
ological function and three reflecting varied forms of psychobiological psychobiological function characterized by disruptions primarily of the
dysregulation. Negative affect disruptions were common across all three affective valence system. Both low positive affect and high negative
dysregulated profiles, whereas disturbance across other systems studied affect have conceptual and empirical links to risk for depression
were not. Negative affect may, therefore, fail to differentiate among (Chorpita and Daleiden, 2002; Olino et al., 2011). However, our
profiles of psychobiological dysregulation, consistent with the argument concomitant consideration of both neurocognitive and
that negative affectivity is a non-specific feature of internalizing diffi­ immuno-endocrine functioning uncovered a set of psychobiological
culties more broadly (Kotov et al., 2017). Person-centered, multilevel protective factors parallel to these girls’ affective disruptions. In addi­
studies of adolescent psychobiological function have also shown tion to exhibiting attentional set-shifting abilities similar to that
elevated negative affect to be a common feature across identified dys­ observed in Normative, girls exhibiting the Severe Affective Disturbance
regulated profiles (Bendezú et al., 2021a, Bendezú et al., 2021b; Koss profile displayed a moderate ratio of morning cortisol to CRP levels.
et al., 2017). Our study suggests that concurrent attention to positive Such close correspondence between morning cortisol and CRP levels is
affect, immuno-endocrine, and neurocognitive function may provide a believed to index homeostatic equilibrium between the HPA and im­
more nuanced and comprehensive understanding of the heterogeneity mune system (Landau et al., 2021). Such countervailing effects are
involved in adolescent girls’ psychobiological risk for depression. believed to support healthy stressor management, where by cortisol
To our knowledge, Landau and colleagues (2021) is the only study to mobilizes metabolic resources needed to efficaciously respond to a
date that has examined the morning cortisol:CRP ratio using saliva in stressor while corresponding inflammatory processes facilitate recovery
relation depression in adolescents. No studies have examined the ratio in (i.e., physical healing, limit infection) from stress exposure (Slavich and
a racially diverse, economically disadvantaged sample of adolescent Irwin, 2014). Indeed, homeostatic immuno-endocrine equilibrium has
girls. Our multi-system, person-centered study helped to address this gap been implicated as a biological buffer of girls’ risk for depression during
in the literature. In so doing, our results also help to clarify inconsistent the adolescent transition (for review, see Slavich and Sacher, 2019).
findings from studies using variable-centered methods (e.g., linear Relative to Normative, Hypercortisol Affective Neurocognitive Distur­
regression) to examine the cortisol:CRP ratio as it pertains to internal­ bance girls exhibited disrupted functioning across multiple (but not all)
izing symptomatology. Specifically, both high and low cortisol:CRP ra­ systems. First, these girls exhibited relatively higher morning cortisol:
tios were observed in our dysregulated profiles. Lower ratios have been CRP ratios. Elevated cortisol:CRP ratios are thought to indicate the
associated with depression in females and stress-induced negative affect overproduction of cortisol to CRP (Guilliams and Edwards, 2010), a
in depressed adults (Suarez et al., 2015; Suarez and Sundy, 2017). pattern often attributed to chronic stress-related glucocorticoid immuno­
Higher ratios have been associated with anxiety in females and suppression whereby excessive cortisol via its anti-inflammatory
first-onset depression in at-risk adolescents (Landau et al., 2021; Suarez signaling properties unduly suppresses immune system function (e.g.,
et al., 2015). In agreement with Landau and colleagues, our cellular immunity). The resulting immunocompromised state can in­
person-centered findings suggest the possibility that both lower and crease risk for psychopathology (e.g., depression, Slavich and Irwin,
higher ratios within the same study and sample may reflect qualitatively 2014). That Hypercortisol Affective Neurocognitive Disturbance girls also
distinct forms of immuno-endocrine dysregulation and, thus, index risk. displayed the lowest attentional set-shifting abilities is further consistent
If so, conclusions drawn from variable-centered methods (e.g., linear with this claim, as excessive HPA activation and cortisol overexposure
regression) that average across potential subgroups at both ends of the noxiously impact neurobiological circuits that support healthy executive
cortisol:CRP ratio distribution may be erroneous (e.g., concluding that function (Shansky and Lipps, 2013). These girls, however, also displayed
higher cortisol:CRP ratios weakly confer risk for internalizing problems normative levels of positive affect, consistent with a prior
when in actuality both higher and lower cortisol:CRP ratios moderately person-centered, multisystem study of depressed adolescent girls’ stress
or strongly confer risk). responsivity and its identified Hyperresponsive profile (J.J. Bendezú,
Calhoun et al., 2021; J. J. Bendezú, Calhoun et al., 2021). Positive
4.1. Psychobiological foundation profiles emotionality can be a source of resilient function (Curtis and Cicchetti,
2007) and buffer of negative emotions and related sequelae (Fre­
Relative to Normative, girls exhibiting the Pro-inflammatory Affective drickson, 2004), which may explain these girls’ relatively mild eleva­
Disturbance profile presented with lower cortisol:CRP ratios. For these tions of negative affect.
girls, lower cortisol:CRP ratios may reflect the inadequate release of
cortisol relative to CRP, a pattern often attributed to chronic stress-
related glucocorticoid resistance whereby immune cells (e.g.,

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

4.2. Links to maltreatment history affect levels may constrain the efficacious use of certain coping skills
(Compas, 2009). However, if their moderate cortisol:CRP ratios and
The validity of our profiles was supported through their connections normative attentional set shifting skills indicate the healthy suppression
with maltreatment. Relative to Normative, girls exhibiting profiles of inflammatory processes by the HPA, which together support adaptive
thought to reflect psychobiological dysregulation reported having been functional changes in brain regions and circuitry (e.g., amygdala, pre­
exposed to a greater number of maltreatment subtypes while growing up frontal cortex) implicated in executive function and self-regulation
as a child and teenager. Exposure to maltreatment has been indepen­ (Shields et al., 2017), then it is possible that these girls’
dently linked to youth affective valence disturbance (e.g., elevated immuno-endocrine and neurocognitive strengths support the use of
negative affect, emotional lability; Courtney-Seidler et al., 2014; Cro­ cognitive reappraisal for managing depressive symptoms in a way that
well et al., 2009), which findings from our post-hoc conventional ana­ circumvents their affective predispositions. Similarly, Hypercortisol Af­
lyses are consistent with. However, by spanning multiple levels of fective Neurocognitive Disturbance girls may rely on normative positive
analysis to identify qualitatively unique within-person profiles of coor­ affect levels to support their use of cognitive reappraisal. Indeed, posi­
dinated psychobiological function, our person-centered approach tive emotionality promotes flexible thinking and adaptive coping with
strengthens inference about how immuno-endocrine and neurocognitive negative emotions (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2000; Fredrickson and
disruptions that have been previously independently linked to Branigan, 2005). This strength might circumvent a possible neuro­
maltreatment (e.g., elevated and blunted cortisol levels; Cicchetti et al., cognitive proclivity towards perseveration (i.e., being “stuck” on stimuli
2010; Doom et al., 2013; elevated CRP and pro-inflammatory cytokine from previously established dimension the ED stages of the IDED
levels; Coelho et al., 2014; Ehrlich et al., 2021; compromised memory set-shifting module; Henry and Bettenay, 2010; Yerys and Munakata,
and executive functioning; Cowell et al., 2015; Valentino et al., 2009) 2006) that would otherwise interfere with reappraisal utilization. In
may interact with one another within maltreated girls. each case, knowledge about girls’ psychobiological profiles may help
In so doing, our person-centered, multisystem approach illustrated clinicians leverage clients’ strengths in service of teaching this
profile-specific immuno-endocrine (e.g., Severe Affective Disturbance) multi-faceted strategy (e.g., harnessing positive affect to identify the
and neurocognitive (e.g., Pro-inflammatory Affective Disturbance) “silver lining,” harnessing executive function to brainstorm numerous
strengths that may potentially offset the contributions of past reinterpretations, select the most optimal solution, and shift to a
maltreatment exposure to profile-specific weaknesses (e.g., affective different solution in case the first fails) in ways that may circumvent
difficulties common across dysregulated profiles). This finding is note­ these girls’ weaknesses.
worthy, insofar as it would be reasonable to hypothesize from the
summarized maltreatment research focusing on systems in isolation that 4.4. Links to passive or active suicidal ideation (PASI)
maltreatment would be associated with profiles characterized by
disturbance across all systems (e.g., Multisystem Disturbance). Our study, Findings obtained when PASI was the outcome of interest com­
however, suggests that psychobiological dysregulation as it relates to plemented but also diverged from our depressive symptoms findings.
maltreatment exposure is far more nuanced and complex. For example, Relative to Normative, girls exhibiting the Pro-inflammatory Affective
while neurocognitive impairment and maltreatment history linkages are Disturbance or Hypercortisol Affective Neurocognitive Disturbance profiles
well-documented (Cowell et al., 2015; Gould et al., 2012), our (but not Severe Affective Disturbance) were more likely to report PASI.
concomitant consideration of affective valence in the Hypercortisol Af­ This finding is consistent with recent conceptualizations of adolescent
fective Neurocognitive Disturbance profile pointed to positive affect as girls’ risk for suicidality as failure of the arousal regulatory, affective
potential undocumented strength. These findings highlight the utility of valence, and neurocognitive systems (Beauchaine et al., 2019; Crowell
our multisystem approach. As we discuss later, identification of these et al., 2009). However, our study extends the literature by empirically
profiles has the potential to inform tailored care approaches to inter­ illustrating how coordinated disturbance across such systems may
vention that might better capitalize on and circumvent maltreated girls’ manifest within adolescent girls to confer such risk. Given that only
profile-specific strengths and weaknesses. Pro-inflammatory Affective Disturbance or Hypercortisol Affective Neuro­
cognitive Disturbance girls were more likely to report PASI, it is possible
4.3. Links to depressive symptoms that these profiles reflect more pathological states of psychobiological
dysregulation, perhaps due to disruptions across multiple as opposed to
Our identified profiles were also linked to depressogenic function in a single (e.g., affect) system (e.g., Severe Affective Disturbance).
expected ways. Relative to Normative, girls exhibiting profiles reflecting Contrary to expectation, cognitive reappraisal was not significantly
psychobiological dysregulation were more likely to report current associated with PASI and this association did not vary by girls’ psy­
depressive symptoms, even after adjusting for past maltreatment expo­ chobiological functioning. This finding runs counter to evidence sug­
sure. When examined in isolation, depression has been independently gesting that the tendency to use cognitive reappraisal in response to
linked high negative affect (Enns et al., 2003; Fergusson et al., 2000), difficult emotions helps reduce risk for suicidal ideation (Forkmann
low positive affect (Auerbach et al., 2015), executive function diffi­ et al., 2014; Ong and Thompson, 2019), and that girls’ ability to do so
culties (Wilkinson and Goodyer, 2006), and dysregulated may depend on their psychobiological foundations (Compas et al.,
immuno-endocrine function (e.g., Landau et al., 2021; Suarez and 2017). It is possible that unexamined individual differences in exposure
Sundy, 2017). Our investigation is one the first to provide a more to recent stressful life events may have contributed to our nonsignificant
nuanced, comprehensive depiction of the varied ways these systems findings (e.g., Boyes et al., 2016; Duprey et al., 2021; Franz et al., 2021).
might interact in an etiological sense to confer risk for depression in From a stress sensitization perspective (Hammen et al., 2000), girls with
adolescent girls. maltreatment histories and resulting dysregulated psychobiological
Depressive symptom levels also varied across profiles by girls’ use of function may be most prone to suicidal ideation when they are also
cognitive reappraisal, a finding that extends the psychobiological unduly exposed to recent life stressors (Heim and Nemeroff, 2001). For
foundations of coping and emotion regulation literature (Compas et al., Black youth, experiences with racial/ethnic discrimination may be one
2017). While Normative and Pro-inflammatory Affective Disturbance girls specific form of contextualized life stress that has been understudied in
displayed mild depressive symptom levels irrespective of their cognitive this respect. Black preadolescent youth are unduly exposed to racial
reappraisal utilization, cognitive reappraisal was negatively related to discrimination (Argabright et al., 2021), with experience sampling
depressive symptoms for Severe Affective Disturbance and Hypercortisol studies suggesting that these youth encounter on average five experi­
Affective Neurocognitive Disturbance girls. Regarding the Severe Affective ences per day (English et al., 2020). If so, their maltreatment-related
Disturbance profile, these girls’ high negative affect and low positive heightened sensitivity to ongoing racial discrimination may also have

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

implications for how efficacious their use of coping and emotion regu­ during which maltreatment initially occurred) and chronicity (i.e.,
lation strategies might be in managing their depressogenic mood and number of developmental periods during which maltreatment occurred)
thought patterns (Gruhn and Compas, 2020). Some studies have sug­ examined. Future studies focusing on these temporal aspects may find
gested that cognitive reappraisal and related strategies (e.g., positive that profiles reflecting greater degrees of psychobiological dysregulation
thinking) may “backfire” for minoritized youth when attempting to may not only be linked to greater number of subtypes experienced, but
manage depressotypic thoughts and feelings in the face of racial/ethnic also earlier timing and more chronic exposure over the course of youths’
discrimination, due to (a) limited positive reinterpretations about development. Our cross-sectional design precludes causal inference
experienced racial transgressions and threats in offensive contexts, as about linkages among child maltreatment, psychobiological func­
well as (b) inadvertent internalization of the message that depressotypic tioning, and depressogenic outcomes (Kraemer et al., 1997). Future
thoughts about discrimination are the problem to be resolved rather research incorporating longitudinal designs may be better situated to
than discrimination itself (Perez and Soto, 2011; Perzow et al., 2021; test whether identified profiles function as psychobiological mecha­
Wadsworth et al., 2020). Research is needed to understand whether girls nisms of risk involved in past maltreatment exposure (i.e., antecedent)
with maltreatment-linked dysregulated psychobiological profiles ability to future depressogenic function (i.e., outcome) linkages.
to utilize such strategies for effectively managing PASI depends on
minoritized stress exposure. Funding sources

4.5. Limitations and future directions Manuscript preparation was supported by NIMH Grant T32
MH015755 awarded to Dr. Dante Cicchetti, NIDA Grant T32 DA050560
The lack of evidence with respect to one of our focal anticipated awarded to Drs. Monica Luciana and Scott Vrieze, NICHD Grant P50-
outcomes is also noteworthy. Though our findings provided support for HD096698 awarded to Drs. Dante Cicchetti and Sheree Toth, and NIMH
our anticipated Normative profile, we did not find any evidence of a Grant R01-MH091070 awarded to Drs. Sheree L. Toth, Dante Cicchetti,
Multisystem Disturbance profile. Future studies utilizing larger sample and Jody Manly.
sizes and recruitment strategies that target more severe clinical pre­
sentations (e.g., inpatient) may be needed in order to identify this pro­
Declarations of interest
file. Nevertheless, our identified profiles, those which provided close but
incomplete approximations of Multisystem Disturbance, illustrated
None.
unique patterns of psychobiological strengths and weaknesses that
pointed to person-specific sources of clinically informative resilience.
Our study also had several limitations that point to directions for Appendix A. Supporting information
future research. Our findings are limited to a racially diverse, econom­
ically disadvantaged sample of adolescent girls, some of whom were Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the
depressed and/or maltreated, who were not treatment-seeking. Future online version at doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105826.
research is needed to determine whether our findings generalize to other
genders, non-maltreated, and non-clinical populations of adolescents. References
Indeed, given that our sample was economically disadvantaged and
Akaike, H., 1973. Maximum likelihood identification of Gaussian autoregressive moving
poverty has been linked pro-inflammatory profiles (for review, see average models. Biometrika 60 (2), 255–265.
Jensen et al., 2017), our Pro-inflammatory Affective Disturbance profile Anacker, C., Zunszain, P.A., Carvalho, L.A., Pariante, C.M., 2011. The glucocorticoid
may be unique to this demographic aspect. Though our sample size was receptor: pivot of depression and of antidepressant treatment?
Psychoneuroendocrinology 36 (3), 415–425.
similar to other notable person-centered studies of psychobiological Andreotti, C., Thigpen, J.E., Dunn, M.J., Watson, K., Potts, J., Reising, M.M., Compas, B.
function (Del Giudice et al., 2012; Koss et al., 2017; Turpyn et al., 2015), E., 2013. Cognitive reappraisal and secondary control coping: associations with
it was small for our approach. Though common in LPA, unequal sub­ working memory, positive and negative affect, and symptoms of anxiety/depression.
Anxiety. Stress Coping 26 (1), 20–35.
group sizes emerged and may have reduced power to detect effects in Argabright, S.T., Visoki, E., Moore, T.M., Ryan, D.T., DiDomenico, G.E., Njoroge, W.F.,.
our maltreatment and depression analyses and precluded examination & Barzilay, R. (2021). Association between discrimination stress and suicidality in
of moderation effects by race. Thus, cautious interpretation and repli­ preadolescent children. medRxiv.
Auerbach, R.P., Millner, A.J., Stewart, J.G., Esposito, E.C., 2015. Identifying differences
cation in studies with larger sample sizes is warranted, moderation
between depressed adolescent suicide ideators and attempters. J. Affect. Disord. 186,
findings from which may strengthen inference about whether effects are 127–133.
most pronounced for Black, economically disadvantaged adolescent Barnett, J.H., Sahakian, B.J., Werners, U., Hill, K.E., Brazil, R., Gallagher, O., Jones, P.B.,
girls in particular. Though we controlled for girls’ waking saliva sample 2005. Visuospatial learning and executive function are independently impaired in
first-episode psychosis. Psychol. Med. 35 (7), 1031–1041.
time and use of pain medications (e.g., Aspirin), morning Beauchaine, T.P., Hinshaw, S.P., Bridge, J.A., 2019. Nonsuicidal self-injury and suicidal
immuno-endocrine levels can be impacted by variables which were behaviors in girls: The case for targeted prevention in preadolescence. Clin. Psychol.
unmeasured in our study (e.g., sleep quality, sunlight, chronic stress). Sci. 7 (4), 643–667.
Beck, J.S., Beck, A.T., Jolly, J.B., Steer, R.A., 2005. Beck Youth Inventories, second ed.
Thus, replication in studies that account for these factors is warranted. Harcourt Assessment Inc, San Antonio, TX.
Cortisol levels are increased when chewing gum compared to unstimu­ Bendezú, J.J., Calhoun, C.D., Patterson, M.W., Findley, A., Rudolph, K.D., Hastings, P.,
lated passive drool, whereas salivary CRP levels are not. Although Prinstein, M.J. (2021). Adolescent girls’ stress responses as prospective predictors of
self-injurious thoughts and behaviors: A person-centered, multilevel study.
confounds related to collection method were constant across partici­ Development and Psychopathology, 1–21.
pants, this pattern of effects from gum chewing may have led to slightly Bendezú, J.J., Howland, M., Thai, M., Marceau, K., Shirtcliff, E.A., Hastings, P.D., Klimes-
higher cortisol levels. Studies using passive drool methods may find Dougan, B., 2021. Adolescent cortisol and DHEA responses to stress as prospective
predictors of emotional and behavioral difficulties: a person-centered approach.
different cortisol:CRP ratio distributions among identified profiles. Our Psychoneuroendocrinology 132, 105365.
study relied on retrospective self-reports of child maltreatment, which Bernstein, D.P., Stein, J.A., Newcomb, M.D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T.,
can be potentially impacted by several biases (e.g., forgetting, subse­ Zule, W., 2003. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the
Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abus. Negl. 27 (2), 169–190.
quent life-events; Hardt and Rutter, 2004; Tajima et al., 2004). We also
Blair, C., Raver, C.C., 2016. Poverty, Stress, and Brain Development: New Directions for
focused on the specific number of subtypes girls experienced over the Prevention and Intervention. In: Academic Pediatrics, 16, pp. S30–S36.
course of their childhood and early adolescence. Child maltreatment, Boyes, M.E., Hasking, P.A., Martin, G., 2016. Adverse life experience and psychological
however, is multidimensional (Manly et al., 2001), and maltreatment to distress in adolescence: Moderating and mediating effects of emotion regulation and
rumination. Stress and Health 32 (4), 402–410.
psychobiological dysregulation linkages in our study may have been Celeux, G., Soromenho, G., 1996. An entropy criterion for assessing the number of
better explained were differences in timing (i.e., developmental period clusters in a mixture model. J. Classif. 13 (2), 195–212.

10

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

Cha, C.B., Wilson, K.M., Tezanos, K.M., DiVasto, K.A., Tolchin, G.K., 2019. Cognition and Fergusson, D.M., Woodward, L.J., Horwood, L.J., 2000. Risk factors and life processes
self-injurious thoughts and behaviors: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. associated with the onset of suicidal behaviour during adolescence and early
Clin. Psychol. Rev. 69, 97–111. adulthood. Psychol. Med. 30 (1), 23–39.
Chorpita, B.F., Daleiden, E.L., 2002. Tripartite dimensions of emotion in a child clinical Fitzpatrick, K.M., Piko, B.F., Miller, E., 2008. Suicide ideation and attempts among low-
sample: measurement strategies and implications for clinical utility. J. Consult. Clin. income African American adolescents. Suicide Life-Threat. Behav. 38 (5), 552–563.
Psychol. 70 (5), 1150. Folkman, S., Moskowitz, J.T., 2000. Stress, positive emotion, and coping. Curr. Dir.
Cicchetti, D., & Bendezú, J.J. (in press). Early adversity and the development of coping. Psychol. Sci. 9 (4), 115–118.
In E. A. Skinner & M. J. Zimmer-Gembeck (Eds.), The handbook of the development Forkmann, T., Scherer, A., Bocker, M., Pawelzik, M., Gauggel, S., Glaesmer, H., 2014.
of coping. Cambridge University Press. The relation of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression to suicidal ideation
Cicchetti, D., Dawson, G., 2002. Multiple levels of analysis. Dev. Psychopathol. 14 (3), and suicidal desire. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior 44 (5), 524–536.
417–420. Franz, P.J., Kleiman, E.M., Nock, M.K., 2021. Reappraisal and suppression each
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F.A., 1996. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental moderate the association between stress and suicidal ideation: preliminary evidence
psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 8 (4), 597–600. from a daily diary study. Cogn. Ther. Res. 1–8.
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S., 2016. Child maltreatment and developmental psychopathology: a Fredrickson, B.L., 2004. The broaden–and–build theory of positive emotions. Philos.
multilevel perspective. In: Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Developmental Psychopathology, (3rd Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 359 (1449), 1367–1377.
ed.,, Vol. 3. Wiley, pp. 457–512. Fredrickson, B.L., Branigan, C., 2005. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F.A., Gunnar, M.R., Toth, S.L., 2010. The differential impacts of and thought-action repertoires. Cogn. Emot. 19 (3), 313–332.
early physical and sexual abuse and internalizing problems on daytime cortisol Fries, E., Hesse, J., Hellhammer, J., Hellhammer, D.H., 2005. A new view on
rhythm in school-aged children. Child Dev. 81 (1), 252–269. hypocortisolism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30 (10), 1010–1016.
Clark, S. & Muth´en, B. (2009). Relating latent class analysis results to variables not Glenn, C.R., Cha, C.B., Kleiman, E.M., Nock, M.K., 2017. Understanding suicide risk
included in the analysis. 〈https://www.statmodel.com/download/relatinglca.pdf〉. within the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework: Insights, challenges, and
Coelho, R., Viola, T.W., Walss-Bass, C., Brietzke, E., Grassi-Oliveira, R., 2014. Childhood future research considerations. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 5 (3), 568–592.
maltreatment and inflammatory markers: a systematic review. Acta Psychiatr. Gould, F., Clarke, J., Heim, C., Harvey, P.D., Majer, M., Nemeroff, C.B., 2012. The effects
Scand. 129 (3), 180–192. of child abuse and neglect on cognitive functioning in adulthood. J. Psychiatr. Res.
Compas, B.E., 2009. Coping, regulation, and development during childhood and 46 (4), 500–506.
adolescence. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 124, 87–99. Granger, D.A., Hibel, L.C., Fortunato, C.K., Kapelewski, C.H., 2009. Medication effects on
Compas, B.E., Jaser, S.S., Bettis, A.H., Watson, K.H., Gruhn, M.A., Dunbar, J.P., salivary cortisol: tactics and strategy to minimize impact in behavioral and
Thigpen, J.C., 2017. Coping, emotion regulation, and psychopathology in childhood developmental science. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34 (10), 1437–1448.
and adolescence: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychol. Bull. 143 (9), 939. Gross, J.J., John, O.P., 2003. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:
Connor-Smith, J.K., Compas, B.E., Wadsworth, M.E., Thomsen, A.H., Saltzman, H., 2000. implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 85
Responses to stress in adolescence: measurement of coping and involuntary stress (2), 348.
responses. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68 (6), 976. Gruhn, M.A., Compas, B.E., 2020. Effects of maltreatment on coping and emotion
Courtney-Seidler, E.A., Burns, K., Zilber, I., Miller, A.L., 2014. Adolescent suicide and regulation in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analytic review. Child Abus. Negl.
self-injury: Deepening the understanding of the biosocial theory and applying 103, 104446.
dialectical behavior therapy. Int. J. Behav. Consult. Ther. 9, 35. Guilliams, T.G., Edwards, L., 2010. Chronic stress and the HPA axis. Standard 9 (2),
Cowell, R.A., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F.A., Toth, S.L., 2015. Childhood maltreatment and 1–12.
its effect on neurocognitive functioning: timing and chronicity matter. Dev. Gunnar, M.R., Vazquez, D.M., 2001. Low cortisol and a flattening of expected daytime
Psychopathol. 27 (2), 521–533. rhythm: potential indices of risk in human development. Dev. Psychopathol. 13 (3),
Crowell, S.E., Beauchaine, T.P., Linehan, M.M., 2009. A biosocial developmental model 515–538.
of borderline personality: Elaborating and extending Linehan’s theory. Psychol. Bull. Hammen, C., Henry, R., Daley, S.E., 2000. Depression and sensitization to stressors
135 (3), 495. among young women as a function of childhood adversity. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.
Curtis, W.J., Cicchetti, D., 2007. Emotion and resilience: a multilevel investigation of 68 (5), 782.
hemispheric electroencephalogram asymmetry and emotion regulation in maltreated Hankin, B.L., Mermelstein, R., Roesch, L., 2007. Sex differences in adolescent depression:
and nonmaltreated children. Dev. Psychopathol. 19 (3), 811–840. stress exposure and reactivity models. Child Dev. 78 (1), 279–295.
Dantzer, R., 2001. Cytokine-induced sickness behavior: mechanisms and implications. Hardt, J., Rutter, M., 2004. Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 933 (1), 222–234. experiences: review of the evidence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45 (2), 260–273.
Del Giudice, M., Hinnant, J.B., Ellis, B.J., El-Sheikh, M., 2012. Adaptive patterns of stress Heim, C., Nemeroff, C.B., 2001. The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of
responsivity: a preliminary investigation. Dev. Psychol. 48 (3), 775. mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biol. Psychiatry 49 (12),
Dettlaff, A.J., Boyd, R., 2020. Racial disproportionality and disparities in the child 1023–1039.
welfare system: why do they exist, and what can be done to address them? Ann. Am. Henry, L.A., Bettenay, C., 2010. The assessment of executive functioning in children.
Acad. Political Soc. Sci. 692 (1), 253–274. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 15 (2), 110–119.
Dickstein, D.P., Axelson, D., Weissman, A.B., Yen, S., Hunt, J.I., Goldstein, B.I., Keller, M. Hilt, L.M., McLaughlin, K.A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., 2010. Examination of the response
B., 2016. Cognitive flexibility and performance in children and adolescents with styles theory in a community sample of young adolescents. J. Abnorm. Child
threshold and sub-threshold bipolar disorder. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 25 (6), Psychol. 38 (4), 545–556.
625–638. Hostinar, C.E., Swartz, J.R., Alen, N.V., Guyer, A.E., Hastings, P.D., 2021. Childhood
Dickstein, D.P., Nelson, E.E., McCLURE, E.B., Grimley, M.E., Knopf, L., Brotman, M.A., adversity as a transdiagnostic risk factor for psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal
Leibenluft, E., 2007. Cognitive flexibility in phenotypes of pediatric bipolar disorder. Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000619.
J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 46 (3), 341–355. Humphreys, K.L., LeMoult, J., Wear, J.G., Piersiak, H.A., Lee, A., Gotlib, I.H., 2020. Child
Doom, J.R., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F.A., Dackis, M.N., 2013. Child maltreatment and maltreatment and depression: a meta-analysis of studies using the Childhood Trauma
gender interactions as predictors of differential neuroendocrine profiles. Questionnaire. Child Abus. Negl. 102, 104361.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 38 (8), 1442–1454. Jazbec, S., Pantelis, C., Robbins, T., Weickert, T., Weinberger, D.R., Goldberg, T.E., 2007.
Duncan, G.J., Smeeding, T.M., Rodgers, W., 1993. W(h) ither the middle class? A Intra-dimensional/extra-dimensional set-shifting performance in schizophrenia:
dynamic view. In: Papadimitriou, D.P., Wolfe, W.D. (Eds.), Poverty and prosperity in impact of distractors. Schizophr. Res. 89 (1–3), 339–349.
the USA in the late twentieth century. Palgrave Macmillan, London, pp. 240–274. Jensen, S.K., Berens, A.E., Nelson 3rd, C.A., 2017. Effects of poverty on interacting
Duprey, E.B., Handley, E.D., Manly, J.T., Cicchetti, D., Toth, S.L., 2021. Child biological systems underlying child development. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 1 (3),
maltreatment, recent stressful life events, and suicide ideation: a test of the stress 225–239.
sensitivity hypothesis. Child Abus. Negl. 113, 104926. Johnson, A.E., Perry, N.B., Hostinar, C.E., Gunnar, M.R., 2019. Cognitive–affective
Ehrlich, K.B., Miller, G.E., Rogosch, F.A., Cicchetti, D., 2021. Maltreatment exposure strategies and cortisol stress reactivity in children and adolescents: normative
across childhood and low-grade inflammation: considerations of exposure type, development and effects of early life stress. Dev. Psychobiol. 61 (7), 999–1013.
timing, and sex differences. Dev. Psychobiol. 63 (3), 529–537. Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U.M.A., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., Ryan, N., 1997.
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., 2016. Conceptions of executive functioning and regulation: Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present
when and to what degree do they overlap? In: Griffin, J.A., Freund, L.S., and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J. Am. Acad.
McCardle, P. (Eds.), Executive function in preschool age children: Integrating Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36 (7), 980–988.
measurement, neurodevelopment and translational research. American Koss, K.J., Cummings, E.M., Davies, P.T., Cicchetti, D., 2017. Patterns of adolescent
Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp. 115–136. regulatory responses during family conflict and Turpyn, C. C., Chaplin, T. M., Cook,
English, D., Lambert, S.F., Tynes, B.M., Bowleg, L., Zea, M.C., Howard, L.C., 2020. Daily E. C., & Martelli, A. M. (2015). A person-centered approach to adolescent emotion
multidimensional racial discrimination among Black US American adolescents. regulation: Associations with psychopathology and parenting. Journal of
J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 66, 101068. Experimental Child Psychology, 136, 1-16. mental health trajectories. J. Res. Adolesc.
Enns, M.W., Cox, B.J., Inayatulla, M., 2003. Personality predictors of outcome for 27 (1), 229–245.
adolescents hospitalized for suicidal ideation. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Kotov, R., Krueger, R.F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T.M., Althoff, R.R., Bagby, R.M.,
42 (6), 720–727. Eaton, N.R., 2017. The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): a
Evans, G.W., Marcynyszyn, L.A., 2004. Environmental justice, cumulative environmental dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 126 (4), 454.
risk, and health among low-and middle-income children in upstate New York. Am. J. Kraemer, H.C., Kazdin, A.E., Offord, D.R., Kessler, R.C., Jensen, P.S., Kupfer, D.J., 1997.
Public Health 94 (11), 1942–1944. Coming to terms with the terms of risk. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 54 (4), 337–343.

11

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J.J. Bendezú et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 143 (2022) 105826

Kudielka, B.M., Kirschbaum, C., 2003. Awakening cortisol responses are influenced by Shansky, R.M., Lipps, J., 2013. Stress-induced cognitive dysfunction: hormone-
health status and awakening time but not by menstrual cycle phase. neurotransmitter interactions in the prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 123.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 28 (1), 35–47. Shields, G.S., Moons, W.G., Slavich, G.M., 2017. Inflammation, self-regulation, and
Landau, E.R., Raniti, M.B., Blake, M., Waloszek, J.M., Blake, L., Simmons, J.G.,. & Byrne, health: an immunologic model of self-regulatory failure. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12
M.L. (2021). The ratio of morning cortisol to CRP prospectively predicts first-onset (4), 588–612.
depression in at-risk adolescents. Social Science & Medicine, 114098. Slavich, G.M., Irwin, M.R., 2014. From stress to inflammation and major depressive
Laurent, J., Catanzaro, S.J., Joiner Jr., T.E., Rudolph, K.D., Potter, K.I., Lambert, S., disorder: a social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychol. Bull. 140 (3),
Gathright, T., 1999. A measure of positive and negative affect for children: scale 774–815.
development and preliminary validation. Psychol. Assess. 11 (3), 326. Slavich, G.M., Sacher, J., 2019. Stress, sex hormones, inflammation, and major
Leeson, V.C., Robbins, T.W., Matheson, E., Hutton, S.B., Ron, M.A., Barnes, T.R., depressive disorder: extending Social Signal Transduction Theory of Depression to
Joyce, E.M., 2009. Discrimination learning, reversal, and set-shifting in first-episode account for sex differences in mood disorders. Psychopharmacology 236 (10),
schizophrenia: stability over six years and specific associations with medication type 3063–3079.
and disorganization syndrome. Biol. Psychiatry 66 (6), 586–593. Spyridaki, E.C., Avgoustinaki, P.D., Margioris, A.N., 2016. Obesity, inflammation and
Linton, P.J., Dorshkind, K., 2004. Age-related changes in lymphocyte development and cognition. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 9, 169–175.
function. Nat. Immunol. 5 (2), 133–139. Suarez, E.C., Sundy, J.S., 2017. The cortisol: C-reactive protein ratio and negative affect
Magidson, J., Vermunt, J.K., 2004. Latent class models. The Sage handbook of reactivity in depressed adults. Health Psychol. 36 (9), 852.
quantitative methodology for the social sciences. Sage, pp. 175–198. Susman, E.J., Dockray, S., Schiefelbein, V.L., Herwehe, S., Heaton, J.A., Dorn, L.D., 2007.
Manly, J.T., Kim, J.E., Rogosch, F.A., Cicchetti, D., 2001. Dimensions of child Morningness/eveningness, morning-to-afternoon cortisol ratio, and antisocial
maltreatment and children’s adjustment: Contributions of developmental timing and behavior problems during puberty. Developmental Psychology 43 (4), 811.
subtype. Dev. Psychopathol. 13 (4), 759–782. Tajima, E.A., Herrenkohl, T.I., Huang, B., Whitney, S.D., 2004. Measuring child
Masyn, K.E., 2013. Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. In: Little, T.D. (Ed.), maltreatment: a comparison of prospective parent reports and retrospective
The Oxford handbook of quantitative methods, 2. Oxford University Press, p. 551. adolescent reports. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 74 (4), 424–435.
McRae, K., Hughes, B., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J.D., Gross, J.J., Ochsner, K.N., 2010. The Tein, J.Y., Coxe, S., Cham, H., 2013. Statistical power to detect the correct number of
neural bases of distraction and reappraisal. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22 (2), 248–262. classes in latent profile analysis. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 20 (4),
Muthen, B., Muthen, L., 1998. Mplus user’s guide. Author, Los Angeles. 640–657.
Nock, M.K., Green, J.G., Hwang, I., McLaughlin, K.A., Sampson, N.A., Zaslavsky, A.M., Turpyn, C.C., Chaplin, T.M., Cook, E.C., Martelli, A.M., 2015. A person-centered
Kessler, R.C., 2013. Prevalence, correlates, and treatment of lifetime suicidal approach to adolescent emotion regulation: associations with psychopathology and
behavior among adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey parenting. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 136, 1–16.
Replication Adolescent Supplement. JAMA Psychiatry 70 (3), 300–310. Uddin, L.Q., 2021. Cognitive and behavioural flexibility: neural mechanisms and clinical
Olino, T.M., Lopez-Duran, N.L., Kovacs, M., George, C.J., Gentzler, A.L., Shaw, D.S., considerations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 22 (3), 167–179.
2011. Developmental trajectories of positive and negative affect in children at high Valentino, K., Toth, S.L., Cicchetti, D., 2009. Autobiographical memory functioning
and low familial risk for depressive disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52 (7), among abused, neglected, and nonmaltreated children: The overgeneral memory
792–799. effect. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50 (8), 1029–1038.
Ong, E., Thompson, C., 2019. The importance of coping and emotion regulation in the Wadsworth, M.E., McDonald, A., Joos, C.M., Ahlkvist, J.A., Perzow, S.E., Tilghman-
occurrence of suicidal behavior. Psychol. Rep. 122 (4), 1192–1210. Osborne, E.M., Brelsford, G.M., 2020. Reducing the biological and psychological
Peckham, A.D., McHugh, R.K., Otto, M.W., 2010. A meta-analysis of the magnitude of toxicity of poverty-related stress: Initial efficacy of the Ba SICS intervention for early
biased attention in depression. Depress Anxiety 27 (12), 1135–1142. adolescents. Am. J. Community Psychol. 65 (3–4), 305–319.
Perez, C.R., Soto, J.A., 2011. Cognitive reappraisal in the context of oppression: Walker, E.A., Unutzer, J., Rutter, C., Gelfand, A., Saunders, K., VonKorff, M., Katon, W.,
implications for psychological functioning. Emotion 11 (3), 675. 1999. Costs of health care use by women HMO members with a history of childhood
Perzow, S.E., Bray, B.C., Wadsworth, M.E., Young, J.F., Hankin, B.L., 2021. Individual abuse and neglect. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 56 (7), 609–613.
differences in adolescent coping: Comparing a community sample and a low-ses Warmingham, J.M., Handley, E.D., Rogosch, F.A., Manly, J.T., Cicchetti, D., 2019.
sample to understand coping in context. J. Youth Adolesc. 50 (4), 693–710. Identifying maltreatment subgroups with patterns of maltreatment subtype and
Petersen, A.C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., Boxer, A., 1988. A self-report measure of chronicity: a latent class analysis approach. Child Abus. Negl. 87, 28–39.
pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. J. Youth Adolesc. 17 (2), Weller, B.E., Bowen, N.K., Faubert, S.J., 2020. Latent class analysis: a guide to best
117–133. practice. J. Black Psychol. 46 (4), 287–311.
Schwarz, G., 1978. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 461–464. Wilkinson, P.O., Goodyer, I.M., 2006. Attention difficulties and mood-related ruminative
Sclove, S.L., 1987. Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in response style in adolescents with unipolar depression. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry
multivariate analysis. Psychometrika 52 (3), 333–343. 47 (12), 1284–1291.
Sedlak, A., McPherson, K.S., Das, B., Westat, Inc, 2010. Supplementary analyses of race Yang, Y., Shields, G.S., Guo, C., Liu, Y., 2018. Executive function performance in obesity
differences in child maltreatment rates in the NIS-4. Westat, Incorporated,, and overweight individuals: a meta-analysis and review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
Rockville, MD. 84, 225–244.
Selye, H., 1936. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature 138 (3479), Yerys, B.E., Munakata, Y., 2006. When labels hurt but novelty helps: children’s
32-32. perseveration and flexibility in a card-sorting task. Child Dev. 77 (6), 1589–1607.

12

Downloaded for Fakultas Kedokteran Universitas Hasanuddin (meetingresidenpsikiatriunhas@gmail.com) at Hasanuddin University from ClinicalKey.com
by Elsevier on February 20, 2023. For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2023. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

You might also like