You are on page 1of 6

Cognitive Distortions and Psychiatric Diagnosis in Dually

Diagnosed Adolescents
TRACY KEMPTON, PH.D., VINCENT B. VAN HASSELT, PH.D., OSCAR G. BUKSTEIN, M.D.,
AND JANE A. NULL, B.S.

ABSTRACT
Objective: The purpose of this study was to examine the characteristics and patterns of cognitive distortions among
psychiatrically hospitalized adolescents. Method: Measures of cognitive distortions, depression, and hopelessness
were administered to 135 adolescents on two psychiatric inpatient units. Subjects were grouped according to their Axis
I diagnoses: depression only, conduct disorder only, depression and substance abuse, conduct disorder and substance
abuse, all three diagnoses, and none of the three diagnoses. Results: Multivariate analyses of covariance indicated
that differently diagnosed adolescents exhibited varying levels of cognitive distorting as measured by the Children's
Negative Cognitive Errors Questionnaire (CNCEQ). In particular, adolescents with multiple Axis I diagnoses tended to
score highest. On all but one of four CNCEQ subscales, the depression only group evidenced as much cognitive
distortion as did the group with multiple diagnoses. However, each diagnostic grouping demonstrated its own somewhat
distinct distortions based on CNCEQ subscales. Conclusions: Findings are discussed in terms of the utility of differenti-
ating cognitive styles for subsequent treatment. It is suggested that disparate cognitive interventions could be matched
with adolescents displaying particular problems. J. Am. Acad. Child Ado/esc. Psychiatry, 1994, 33, 2:217-222. Key
Words: cognitive distortions, psychiatric diagnosis, inpatient adolescents.

Over the past three decades, considerable clinical and Gottlib, 1983), such findings have been the impetus
investigative attention has focused on the relationship for initial applications of cognitive therapy with a
between forms of cognitive distortions (e.g., catastroph- wide range of adult disorders including depression,
izing, selective abstraction, overgeneralizing, personaliz- personality disorders, and substance abuse (Beck and
ing) and psychopathology (cf Abramson et al., 1978; Freeman, 1990; Beck et al., 1979; Foreyt and Rath-
Beck, 1967, 1976; Ellis, 1962). In particular, a bur- jen, 1978).
geoning body of research has documented the associa- Although investigative efforts regarding the role of
tion between faulty or irrational cognitions and cognitive distortions in depression and other disorders
depressed mood states (e.g., Krantz and Hammen, in younger groups are at the nascent stage, findings
1979; Nelson and Craighead, 1977). Although the of preliminary studies appear to support this associa-
theoretical basis and nature of this association has tion. For example, in an investigation by Thurber
been questioned (Beidel and Turner, 1986; Coyne and et al. (1990), the tendency for psychiatrically hospital-
ized 12- to 17-year-old adolescents to emit cognitive
Accepted june 2, 1993. errors, as evaluated via the Children's Negative Cogni-
Tracy Kempton is Associate Director, Child Inpatient Unit, and Assistant
tive Errors Questionnaire (CNCEQ), was significantly
Professor of Psychiatry, Kobacker Center, Medical College of Ohio; Dr. Van
Hasselt is Professor of Psychology, and Jane Null is Clinic and Research correlated with scores on the Beck Depression Inven-
Coordinator, Nova University, Ft. Lauderdale, FL; and Dr. Bukstein is tory (Beck et al., 1961) and the Hopelessness Scale
Assistant Professor ofPsychiatry, University ofPittsburgh School ofMedicine. for Children (Kazdin et al., 1986). However, in this
This study was supported in part by the Center for Education and Drug
Abuse Research (CEDAR), ftnded by the National Institute on Drug Abuse study, the possibility of common method variance must
(DA05605). be considered, as the measures completed by parents
Reprint requests to Dr. Van Hasselt, Centerfor Psychological Studies, Nova concerning their children did not correspond with
University, 3301 College Avenue, Fort Lauderdale, FL 33314.
0890-8567/94/3302-0217$03.0010©1994 by the American Academy the CNCEQ.
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Haley et al. (1985) employed the Cognitive Bias

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 33:2 FEBRUARY 1994 217


KEMPTON ET AL.

Questionnaire for Children (CBQC), a child and ado- present among psychiatrically disturbed adolescents.
lescent version of the Cognitive Bias Questionnaire Furthermore, although cognitive techniques are being
(Krantz and Hammen, 1979), with 39 child and adoles- applied toward a number of problem areas, such as
cent inpatients ranging in age from 8 to 16 years (X depression (Lewinsohn et al., 1990; Reynolds and
= 12.8). Seventeen of the subjects were diagnosed Coats, 1986) and conduct disorder (Kendall
with an affective disorder and 22 with a different or et al., 1990), investigative endeavors have yet to deter-
with no Axis I disorder. Results indicated that scores mine whether distortions differ among disparate psychi-
on the depressed distorted (DO) scale of the CBQC atric diagnostic groups. It is possible that adolescents
were positively correlated with (1) psychiatric ratings with distinct diagnoses will evince different types of
of dysthymic symptoms, (2) child-reported symptoms cognitive distortions. If so, forms of cognitive interven-
on the Child Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1981), tions should be tailored to specific populations and
and (3) the child-completed Children's Depression disorders. In light of this hypothesis, an instrument
Scale (Lang and Tisher, 1978). In addition, the affective was chosen that would delineate types of cognitive
group had significantly higher scores on the DO scale, errors in addition to substantiating their existence in
after adjusting for age (which differed between the this population.
two groups). The primary purpose of this study was to provide
The relationship between cognitive distortions and some of the first empirical data concerning cognitive
substance abuse also has been examined. In an investiga- distortions in dually diagnosed adolescents. Further-
tion of78 adolescents (age not provided) in a residential more, a variety of diagnostic categories were examined
drug treatment program, Denoff (1987) used regression to ascertain whether there are specific forms or charac-
analyses to predict frequency of drug use from subscales teristics (type, amount) ofcognitive distortions endemic
of the Rational Behavior Inventory (RBI) (Shorkey to each.
and Whiteman, 1977). Specific beliefs, such as catas-
trophizing ("it is a catastrophe when things are not
the way one wants them"), approval ("it is absolutely METHOD
necessary to be approved of by everyone for everything Subjects
one does"), and blame and punishment ("certain people The present study was part of a comprehensive evaluation of
are bad and should be punished") emerged as the social and emotional adjustment in dually diagnosed psychiatrically
strongest predictors of substance abuse. A second at- hospitalized adolescents. The sample included 135 adolescents (78
females and 57 males). The age range of the subjects was 12 to
tempt to predict substance abuse in the same sample 18 years (X = 15.3). Socioeconomic status (SES), as determined
employed the Perceived Parenting Questionnaire (a by the Hollingshead Socioeconomic Status Index (Hollingshead,
modified version of the Cornell Parental Behavior 1975) ranged from 1 to 5 (X = 1.91), reflecting that most subjects
were from lower socioeconomic levels. Ninety-seven (72%) of the
Scales) (Devereux et aI., 1969) and demographic infor- subjects were white, 34 (25%) were African American, and four
mation, in addition to the RBI (Denoff, 1988). Again, were other. Eighty-five of the participants were admitted to the
several RBI scales significantly predicted frequency of Adolescent Drug Abuse and Psychiatric Treatment (ADAPT) pro-
drug use, with catastrophizing emerging as the strongest gram, and 50 were admitted to the Services for Teens at Risk
(STAR) Program at the University of Pittsburgh School of Medi-
predictor. Although preliminary, these findings suggest cine. ADAPT is a psychiatric inpatient unit for adolescents with
a possible role of irrational beliefs in substance use a history of substance abuse and a comorbid psychiatric disorder.
and related problems in adolescents. STAR is a psychiatric inpatient unit for treatment of depressed or
suicidal adolescents. Length of stay on both units ranges from
The aforementioned studies indicate that some psy- several days to several months, with an average of between 4 and
chiatrically impaired adolescents may present with 6 weeks. Clinical interviews of the adolescents served as the source
cognitive distortions. However, although many investi- of diagnoses. These interviews were completed by a psychiatrist
experienced in the use of semistructured diagnostic interviews.
gators and clinicians have initiated cognitive treatment Additional diagnostic information from an ongoing NIAAA re-
strategies with adolescent groups, assessment research search project supplemented the clinical interviews in reaching
has yet to adequately define the nature of the association final diagnoses for each adolescent in the program.
A total of 158 admissions were considered for participation in this
between cognitive distortions and various types of
study. Twenty-three subjects were eliminated because of inability to
psychiatric disorders. Moreover, there is only a modi- match diagnostic configurations with current study groups or
cum of evidence showing that cognitive distortions are noncompliance with assessment procedures. Chi square and analyses

218 j. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY. 33:2, FEBRUARY 1994


DISTORTIONS IN DUALLY DIAGNOSED ADOLESCENTS

of variance were conducted to determine whether these nonpartici- and substance abuse (DE + SA) (N = 22), conduct
pants differed in any important way from the participants. No
significant differences emerged on race, gender, age, or SES.
disorder and substance abuse (CD + SA) (N = 37),
all three Axis I diagnoses (CD + SA + DE) (N = 15),
Measures and none of these three Axis I diagnoses (ND) (N =
Children's Negative Cognitive Errors Questionnaire (CNCEQ) 21). Diagnoses for subjects in the ND group consisted
(Leitenberg et al., 1986). The CNCEQ is a 24-item self-report
questionnaire that presents vignettes illustrating four different types primarily of adjustment disorder, attention-deficit hy-
of cognitive distortions (selective abstraction, overgeneralization, peractivity disorder, and no Axis I disorder.
catastrophizing, personalization) in three areas (athletic, social, To determine whether groups differed with regard
academic). The respondent ranks a thought following each vignette
on a five-point Likert scale describing how similar the thought is
to relevant demographic characteristics, one-way analy-
to the way they would think in the situation. The Total Subscale, ses of variance were conducted. No significant differ-
as well as the subscales of Overgeneralization, Personalization, ences between groups were found for SES, race, or
Selective Abstraction, and Catastrophizing were used. Test-retest
gender. However, significant differences were found
reliability has been shown to be .69; internal consistency for the
total score is .89 (Leitenberg et al., 1986). between groups for age, F(5,137) = 4.30, P < .01,
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961). The BDl indicating that the DE + SA and the DE only groups,
has been widely used in psychotherapy studies of depression. were significantly older than the SA, CD, and ND
This measure is a 2l-item self-report inventory consisting of four
alternative statements scored on a 0 to 3 basis reflecting the range groups, with the SA + DE group also being significantly
of symptom severity. Split-half reliability for the BDl is .93 (Beck older than the CD + SA + DE group. Means ranged
er al., 1961). Moreover, the BDI has proved to show high test- from 14.73 to 16.26. To ensure that age did not
retest reliability as well as high internal consistency in various
studies with adolescents (Strober et al., 1981a,b). The BDI has
influence group differences, age was used as a covariate
been shown to correlate well with ratings of depression made by in subsequent analyses.
independent clinicians in a number of investigations (Beck et al., Next, because of the correlation shown to exist
1961; Nussbaum et al., 1963). In addition, the BDI discriminates
between cognitive errors and depression and hope-
well between anxiery and depression (Beck, 1978).
Beck Hopelessness Scale (BHS) (Beck et al., 1974). This 20-item lessness, analyses were conducted to ascertain whether
true/false scale taps the specific cognitive distortion of pessimism there were group differences on the BDI and the BHS.
concerning the future. Each item is extracted from affective, motiva- No differences emerged for either scale.
tional, and cognitive aspects of hopelessness. Split-half reliabiliry
for the BHS has been shown to be .93 (Beck et al., 1974). Because the Total subscale of the CNCEQ is the
The BHS is predictive of repeated suicide attempts and suicide sum of the cognitive errors subscales, this subscale
completion. A score of nine or higher in adult suicidal idearors initially was tested using analysis of covariance; signifi-
has been found to be predictive of suicide.
cant findings were obtained, F(5,128) = 2.79 P < .05.
Procedure Consequently, the individual subscales were analyzed
All subjects completed the above-mentioned measures within using a multivariate analysis of covariance; a significant
the first week of admission (usually on day four) as part of multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA)
their comprehensive psychological assessment. Instructions for each
measure were read by a research assistant who was available to emerged, F(20,512) = 1.64, P < .05. Univariate analy-
answer any questions subjects may have had. For subjects with ses of variance indicated that three of the four subscales
low reading levels, questions were read aloud by the research were significant: Catastrophizing, F(5,128) = 2.89, P
assistant, and responses were marked on each form. Completion
< .05, Overgeneralizing, F(5,128) = 2.55, P < .05, and
of the entire battery required between 2 Y2 and 3 hours. As an
incentive for measure completion, subjects advanced from the first Personalizing, F(5,128) = 2.54, P < .05. Differences
(evaluation) phase of a mulririer unitwide level (token economic) between groups approached significance on the Selec-
system to the second level.
tive Abstraction subscale, F(5,128) = 2.06, P < .10.
Planned comparisons were conducted on each
RESULTS
CNCEQ subscale to determine which particular diag-
nostic groups differed from each other. For the Caras-
Subjects were divided into groups on the basis of trophizing and Personalizing subscales, the CD + SA
DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) + DE group significantly differed from each of the
Axis I psychiatric diagnoses. Diagnoses and group other groups, with the exception of the DE group.
composition were as follows: depression (major depres- For the Overgeneralizing subscale, the CD + SA +
sion, dysthymia, or bipolar disorder) only (DE) (N = DE group differed significantly from the CD group
22), conduct disorder only (CD) (N = 18), depression as well as from the ND group. Similarly, the DE group

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 33:2, FEBRUARY 1994 219


KEMPTON ET AL .

differed significantly from the CD and ND groups. of their relationship to a variety of psychiatric disorders.
All differences were significant at the .05 level. For In particular, this effort suggests their relevance beyond
the Selective Abstraction subscale, the CD + SA + DE the most commonly assessed symptoms of psychiat-
group differed significantly from every other group , ric disorders.
despite the overall nonsignificance of this subscale in The most salient finding in this study concerns the
the MANCOVA (see Table 1 for adjusted means of group of adolescents with multiple diagnoses (conduct
each measure). disorder, substance abuse, and depression). For two of
the three significant CNCEQ subscales (Catastrophiz-
ing and Personalizing), this group reported the most
DISCUSSION
cognitive errors. Subjects in this condition also scored
The present investigation provided some of the significantly higher on cognitive errors than did every
first empirical data concerning the nature of cognitive other group on the Selective Abstraction subscale and
distortions in psychiatrically impaired adolescents. higher than every other group except the depressed-
Findings indicated that adolescents who suffer from only subjects on the Catastrophizing and Personalizing
disparate disorders may actually think in differently subscales. Thus, adolescents with problems in multiple
distorted ways. Additionally, adolescents with multiple areas have a greater tendency to cognitively distort
problems are correspondingly more likely to commit than do adolescents who have only single or dual
cognitive errors. This was true although the subjects diagnoses. The exception to this finding , however,
did not report different levels of depression and hope- relates to the depressed-only group. Specifically, on
lessness on self-report measures. One would expect three of the four subscales, the adolescent group with
that adolescents with different diagnoses, particularly all three diagnoses did not differ from the depression-
diagnoses as distinct as conduct disorder and depres- only condition. This finding suggests that the adoles-
sion, would be differentiated by self-reported levels of cents with the single diagnosis of depression were most
depression and hopelessness. Furthermore, one may similar to the subjects who were conduct disordered
question the validity of the diagnostic groupings when and abusing substances in addition to their depression.
no such disparities occur. However, group differences It appears that depression is the crucial diagnosis for
did emerge when the construct of cognitive errors increasing the likelihood of making cognitive errors.
was used as the independent variable. Thus, types of These data are consonant with previous research docu-
cognitive errors made by diagnostic groups appear menting the association between cognitive distortions
to be more sensitive measures of differences between and depression (e.g., Krantz and Hammen, 1979).
adolescents with varying psychiatric problems than For one particular subscale, Overgeneralizing, a
are self-reported levels of depression and hopelessness. slightly different pattern was evident. Here, the multiple
Although a few studies have implicated cognitive distor- diagnosis group differed significantly only from the
tions as potentially important variables in adolescent groups with either none of the diagnoses or conduct
depression (Thurber et al., 1990), the present investiga- disorder only. Furthermore, the depression-only group
tion provides more conclusive evidence as to the nature was also significantly different from the groups with

TABLE 1
Adjusted Means for Each Area of Children 's Negative Cognitive Errors Que stionna ire for Each Diagnostic Group
Carasrrophizing Overgeneralization Personalizing Selective Abstraction

Depr ession (N = 22) 15.36-·b 17.22- 15.87-·b 13.15b


Conduct disorder (N = 18) 12.75b 11.78b 13.51b 13.47 b
Depression and substance abuse (N = 22) 13.n b 14.61 -·b 13.37 b 13.84b
Conduct disorderlsbu stance abuse (N = 37) 12.78b 13.6yb 13.27 b 13.64b
All three diagnoses (N = 15) 18.10- 16.94- 17.62- 17.82-
None of the diagnoses (N = 21) 11.79b 12.67b 11 .50b 12.21 b
p< .05 .05 .05 .10

Note: Means with different superscripts differ significantly from each other at the .05 level.

220 J. AM . AC AD . CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHI ATRY . 33:2. FEBRUARY 1994


DISTORTIONS IN DUALLY DIAGNOSED ADOLESCENTS

none or conduct disorder only diagnoses. Again, the The present investigation also is limited by the use
depression-only group most closely resembled the mul- of a single, inhospital sampling of cognitive errors. If
tiple diagnoses group. However, the dually diagnosed mood and cognitions are indeed related, it would be
adolescents fell between the none and conduct disorder beneficial in future research to gather cognitive data
groups on the one hand, and the depression and over multiple sessions. In addition, various types and
multiple diagnosis groups on the other, and differed degrees of substance abuse were combined in the cur-
significantly from neither. rent sample. It is possible that differences in thinking
For Overgeneralizing and all other subscales, the styles exist between, for example, alcohol, cocaine,
dually diagnosed adolescents did not report higher and heroin abusers. Nevertheless, the present findings
levels of cognitive errors than did the group with none provide preliminary evidence of the potentially im-
or conduct disorder only. Whereas one may not expect portant role of cognitive styles in psychiatrically im-
an increase in cognitive errors among youth with paired adolescents.
conduct disorder and substance abuse, the similar find-
ing with respect to the depression and substance abuse
Clinical Implications
group warrants attention. That is, adolescents who
have concurrent problems with depression and sub- As research has identified the presence of cognitive
stance abuse do not appear to be as prone to cognitive errors and distortions in adolescents, there is a potential
errors as do their peers who are only depressed. Perhaps for the effectiveness of cognitive-based therapies for
the depression in the dually diagnosed group is best adolescents (Kendall, 1991). Although cognitive distor-
characterized as secondary depression. Several investiga- tions have been identified in specific diagnostic groups
tors (e.g. Schuckit, 1983; Weissman et al., 1977) have of adolescents (i.e., those with depression or conduct
examined the phenomenology of depression when it disorder) and research has been conducted on the
is secondary to substance abuse in adults with equivocal efficacy of cognitive therapy applied to these specific
findings. Schuckit (1983) reported few minor differ- diagnostic groups, little consideration has been given
ences between primary and secondary depressives, to cognitive errors in adolescents with comorbid or
whereas Weissman et al. (1977) showed that secondary multiple, coexisting diagnoses who comprise a sizable
depressives were generally less depressed and less symp- proportion of youth in treatment (Bukstein and Van
tomatic on other dimensions. The present findings Hasselt, 1993; in press). The clinical implications of
suggest that the diagnosis of depression in the dually the findings of this study include not only the necessity
diagnosed adolescents in this study may be secondary of considering assessment of cognitive distortions as
to the substance abuse and clinically distinct from part of a comprehensive evaluation of an adolescent
primary depression. Unfortunately, this study did not with one or more psychiatric disorders but also assess-
distinguish primary versus secondary depression. ment of the type of distortion within the context
The existence of cognitive distortions and their evo- of the psychiatric diagnosis. Examination of possible
lution over the course of normal and abnormal develop- differences in cognitive distortions offers the potential
ment is an area that has not been adequately examined. of individualizing cognitive approaches. Perhaps the
Although the adolescents in this study showed varying types or pattern of cognitive distortions displayed by
degrees and types of distorting, it would be useful to be the adolescent may prove to be a better guide to
able to compare their scores with those of a normative treatment selection than are categorical diagnoses.
sample. The closest comparison group available consists The importance of depression as a constituent diag-
of the 8th graders in a sample investigated by Leitenberg nosis of the multiple diagnosed group or alone in the
et al. (1986), where subscale score averages ranged depression only group is manifested by these groups
from 13.12 to 15.13. Thus, it is likely that only the having the most cognitive errors. Therefore, the pres-
DE and the DE + SA + CD groups in the present ence of depression, whether alone or comorbid with
study were distorting at a significantly higher rate than nonaffective diagnoses should direct the clinician to
their normative peers. Clearly, the absence of a normal carefully assess cognitive errors or distortions and con-
control group in this study represents a limitation; sider cognitively based therapies as part of the treatment
more information in this area is needed. plan. On the other hand, dually diagnosed adolescents

j. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY. 33:2. FEBRUARY 1994 221


KEM PT ON ET AL .

with depression and substance abuse may be clinically Denoff MS (1987), Irrational beliefs as predictors of adolescent drug abuse
and runn ing away. J Clin PsychoI 43:412-423
distinct from depressed adolescents with depression Denoff MS (1988), An integrated analysis of the contribution made by
only or those with depression, substance abuse, and irrational beliefs and parental interaction to adolescent drug abuse. Int
j Addict 23:655- 669
conduct disorder. In assessing cognitive errors, the DevereuxE, Bronfenbrenner U, Rodgers R (I 969), Child- rearing in England
identification of significant cognitive errors in the pres- and the United States: a cross-national comparison. J Marr Family
3 1:257- 270
ence of depression, regardless of the coexisting diagno- Ellis A (1962), Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy. New York: L. Stuart
sis, may serve to alert the clinician that this depression Foreyt ]P , Rathjen DP (1978), Cognitive Behavior Therapy: Research and
Application. New York: Plenum Press
may be similar to a pure or primary depression and Haley GMT, Fine SV, Marriage K, Moretti MM , Freeman R] (1985),
perhaps responsive to similar cognitive interventions. Cognitive bias and depression in psychiatrically disturbed children and
Thus, all adolescents presenting with nonaffective psy- adolescents. j Consult Clin PsychoI 53:535-53 7
Hollingshead AF (1975), Four-Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven,
chopathology should not only be assessed for the pres- CT: Yale University Sociology Department
ence of depression bur also for cognitive errors in Kandel D (1975), Sequence and stages in patterns of adolescent drug use.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 32:923- 932
attempting to identify salient (homogenous) subgroups Kazdin AF, Rogers A, Colbus D (1986), The Hopelessness Scale for
for treatment planning. Children : psychometric characteristics and concurrent validity. j Consult
Clin PsychoI 54:241-245
Kendall PC, ed (1991), Child and Adolescent Therapy: Cognitive-Behavioral
Procedures. New York: Guilford Press
REFERENCES Kendall PC, Reber M, McLeer S, Epps ] , Ronan K (1990), Cogn itive-
Abramson LY, Seligman MEP, Teasdale ] (1978), Learned helplessness in behavioral treatment of conduct-disordered children. Cognitive Therapy
hum ans: cririque and reformularion. J Abnorm PsychoI 87:49-74 and Research 14:279-297
American Psychiatric Association (1987), Diagnostic and Statistical Man ual, Kovacs M (1981), Rating scales to assess depression in school-aged children.
3rd Edition- Revised(DSM-III-R). Washington, DC: American Psychi- Acta Paedopsychiatr 46:305- 3 15
atric Association Krantz S, Hamm en C (1979), Assessment of cognitive bias in depression.
Beck AT (1967), Depression: Clinical Experimental and Theoretical Aspects. j Abnorm PsychoI8 8:6 11-6 19
New York: Hoeber Med ical Division, Harper & Row Lang M, Tisher M (1978), Children's Depression Scale. Victoria, Australia:
Beck AT (1976), Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. New York: Australian Council for Educational Research
International Universities Press Leitenberg H, Yooc L, Carroll-Wilson M (1986), Negative cognitive errors in
Beck AT (1978), Depression Inventory. Philadelphia: Center for Cogni- children: questionnaire development, normative data, and comparisons
tive T herapy between children with and without self-reported symptoms of depres-
Beck AT, Freeman AM (1990), Cognitive Therapy of Personality Disorders. sion, low self-esteem, and evaluarion anxiety. j Consult Clin Psychol
New York: Gu ilford Press. 54:528- 536
Beck AT, Rush A] , Shaw BF, Emery G (1979), Cognitive Therapy of Lewisohn PM, Clarke GN , Hops H , Andrews] (1990), Cogn itive-behavioral
Depression, New York: Th e Guilford Press treatment for depressed adolescents. Behau Ther 21:385-4 01
Beck AT , Ward CH , Mendelsohn M, Mock ] E, Erbaugh ] (1961) . An Nelson RE, Craighead WE (1977), Selecrive recall of positive and negative
inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 4:56 1-57 1 feedback, self-control behaviors, and depression. J Abnorm Psycho!
Beck AT, Weissman A, Lester D, Texler L (1974), Th e measurement of 86:379-388
pessimism: the Hopelessness Scale. J Consult Clin PsychoI42:861- 865 Nussbaum K, Wittig BA, Hanlon TE, Kurland AA (1963), Intravenous
Beidel D, Turner S (1986), A critique of the theoretical bases of cognitive- nialamide in the treatment of depressed female patients. ComprPsychia-
behavioral theories and therapy. Clin Psychol Rev 6:177-1 97 try 4:105-1 16
Bukstein OG , Van Hasselt VB (1993), Alcohol and drug abuse. In: Reynolds W , Coats K (1986), A comparison of cognitive-behavioral therapy
Handbook of Behavior Therapy in the Psychiatric Setting, eds, Bellack and relaxation training for the treatment of depression in adolescents.
AS, Hersen M . New York: Plenum Press j Consult Clin Psychology 54:653-6 60
Buksrein OG , Van Hasselt VB (in press). Substance use disorders. In: Ryan N, Puig-Antich ] , Ambrosini P er aI. (1987), The clinical picture of
Handbook of Adolescent Psychopathology: A Guide to Diagnosis and major depression in children and adolescents. Arch Gen Psy chiatry
Treatment, eds, Van Hasselr VB, Hersen M. Boston, MA: Lexington 44:854-860
Books Schuckit M (1983), Alcoholic patients with secondary depression Am J
Carlson GA, Cantwell DP (1980), A survey of depressive symptoms in a Psychiatry 140:711-714
child and adolescent psychiatric populat ion : interview data. JA m Acad Shorkey C, Whit eman V (1977), Development of the Rational Behavior
Child Psychiatry 18:587-599 Inventory: initial validity and reliability. Educational and Psychological
Carlson GA, Garber] (1986), Developmental issues in the classification Measurement 37:527- 533
of depression in children. In: Depression in Young People: Developmental Strober M, Green ] , Carlson G (l981a), Utility of the Beck Depression
and Clinical Perspectives, eds, Rutt er M, Izard CE, Read PB. New York: Inventory in hospitalized adolescents. J Consult PsychoI 49:482-483
Gu ilford Press, pp. 399-434 Strober M, Green], Carlson G (1981b), Reliability of psychiatric diagnosis
Chambers W), Puig-Antich ] , Hirsch M, er al. (1985), Th e assessment of in hospitalized adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry38:14 1- 145
affective disorders in children and adolescents by sernistructural inter- Thurber S, Crow LA, Thurber ]A, Woffington LM (1990), Cognitive
view: test-retest reliability of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and distortions and depression in psychiatrically disturbed adolescent inpa-
Schizophrenia for school-age children, present episode version. Arch tients. J Clin PsychoI46:57-60
Gen Psychiatry 42:696-702 Weissman M, Potten ger M, Kleber H, Ruben H , Williams D, Thompson
Coyne ]C, Gotlib IH (1983), T he role of cognition in depression: a critical WD (1977), Symptom pattern s in primary and secondary deptession.
appraisal. Psychol Bull 94:472-505 Arch Gen Psychiatry 34:854- 862

222 j . AM . AC AD . CHILD ADOL ESC . PSYCH IATR Y, 33:2 . FEBRUARY 19 94

You might also like