You are on page 1of 9

American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 67:369-377 (1996)

New Phenotype Definition of Attention Deficit


Hyperactivity Disorder in Relatives for
Genetic Analyses
Sharon Milberger, Stephen V.Faraone, Joseph Biederman, Marcia Testa, and Ming T. Tsuang
Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital (S.M., S.V.F., J.B.), Harvard Institute of
Psychiatric Epidemiology and Genetics (S.V.F., M.T.T.) and Harvard Schools of Medicine and Public Health
(S.M., S.V.F., J.B., M.T., M.T.T.), Harvard University, Boston, Massachusetts

The goal of the present investigation was to 1990; Stewart et al., 1980; Welner et al., 19771. Al-
create a phenotype definition in relatives of though familial transmission may be of genetic or envi-
probands that reflects a more genetic form ronmental origin, it is believed that genes play an im-
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder portant role in the familial transmission of ADHD
(ADHD). Logistic regression was applied to based on data from half-sibling, twin, adoption, and
the first-degree relatives of ADHD and nor- segregation analysis studies [Cantwell, 1975; Deutsch
mal control probands to create a quantitative et al., 1990; Faraone and Santangelo, 1992; Goodman
phenotype that combined information across and Stevenson, 1989a,b; Lopez, 1965; Morrison and
psychiatric, cognitive, and demographic do- Stewart, 1973; Rutter et al., 1963; Safer, 1973; Torger-
mains. Models were run separately in moth- sen and Kringlen, 1978; Willerman, 19733. If a single
ers, fathers, sisters, and brothers. Although major gene does exist, misclassification could make it
there was some overlap between the vari- difficult, if not impossible, t o detect with linkage or as-
ables retained in each model, no two models sociation studies.
had exactly the same variables. Our results In this investigation we were interested in classify-
suggest that the use of fitted logits may be ing ADHD into categories that discriminated between
valuable as a potential index of caseness. genetic and nongenetic subtypes of the disorder. This is
Since different characteristics were in- what Tsuang et al. [1993]referred to as “psychiatric ge-
cluded in different groups of relatives, our netic nosology.” This approach does not suggest that
results suggest that gender and generation such a nosology will be useful for clinicians or that it
may moderate the expression of ADHD. should replace DSM-111-R. What it does imply is that
0 1996 Wiley-Liss, Inc. psychiatric genetics need not rely on diagnostic con-
structs for other purposes [Tsuang et al., 19931. A psy-
KEY WORDS: psychiatric morbidity, famil- chiatric genetic nosology needs to address the key mea-
ial transmission, logistic re- surement issues in psychiatric genetics. Of primary
gression concern is the group of false-positives. This refers to a
group of subjects who are classified as having ADHD
but are incorrectly classified as having a genetic sub-
INTRODUCTION form of the illness (i.e., subjects who have the pheno-
type but not the genotype). These false-positives are of
Family studies of attention deficit hyperactivity dis- critical concern in genetic studies since they may lead
order (ADHD) have found that the relatives of ADHD to reduced statistical power and attenuated evidence
children are at high risk for ADHD [Biederman et al., for genetic linkage [Chen et al., 1992; Tsuang et al.,
1990, 1991a,b; Cantwell, 1972; Faraone et al., 1992; 19931.False-negatives (i.e., subjects who do not express
Faraone and Santangelo, 1992; Lahey et al., 1989; the phenotype but have the genotype) are also of con-
Mannuzza and Gittelman, 1984; Morrison, 1980; Mor- cern in genetic analyses, but to a lesser extent since
rison and Stewart, 1971; Schachar and Wachsmuth, they do not have the same impact on statistical power.
Typically, the phenotype definition for ADHD has
been based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders (DSM) classification system, which
Received for publication August 2, 1995; revision received
December 2, 1995.
treats diagnoses as dichotomous traits [American Psy-
chiatric Association, 1987; Tsuang et al., 19931. In
Address reprint requests to Dr. Joseph Biederman, Pediatric
Psychopharmacology Unit, ACC-725, Massachusetts General many cases, however, such a dichotomy comes about af-
Hospital, 15 Parkman Street, Boston, MA 02114. ter examining several variables on an individual, and
0 1996 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
370 Milberger et al.
then applying a rule as t o who is affected and who is un- create a quantitative phenotype definition in relatives
affected. This dichotomization process can yield consid- that combined information across psychiatric, cogni-
erable loss of information and misclassification. Ott tive, and demographic domains.
[1991] developed an approach that addressed these is-
sues. His scheme is based on a measure of certainty SUBJECTS AND METHODS
that an individual is truly affected and assigns weights Subjects
according to the probability that the subject is affected. We analyzed data from a family-genetic study of
These weights can be the predictions made by mathe- ADHD that has been described previously [Biederman
matical models of disease expression [Tsuang et al., et al., 19921. Briefly, this study selected two groups of
19931. In a linkage analysis, these weights can be used index children: 140 ADHD probands and 120 normal
to quantify the penetrance of each genotype for diag- controls. These groups had 454 and 368 first-degree
noses made with varying degrees of certainty [Ott, biological relatives, respectively. The number of moth-
19911. ers, fathers, sisters, and brothers in each of these
Therefore, differences in demographic and clinical groups is delineated in Table I. We sampled families
features in relatives of ADHD probands and relatives of through Caucasian, non-Hispanic male probands between
normal controls may be combined to create Ott’s mea- ages 6-17 years. Potential probands were excluded if
sure of certainty that would weight the contribution of they had been adopted, or if their nuclear family was
individual relatives in a linkage analysis by the chance not available for study. We also excluded probands if
that they have the genetic form of ADHD. If any one of they had major sensorimotor handicaps (e.g., paralysis,
these characteristics were examined individually it deafness, or blindness), psychosis, autism, or a full-scale
might not be highly predictive, whereas the combina- I& 4 0 . Subjects from the lowest socioeconomic class
tion of a number of characteristics into a composite pro- (SES VI) [Hollingshead, 19751 were excluded t o mini-
file might yield considerable predictive ability [Blacker mize the potential confounding of social adversity. All
et al., 1993; Blacker and Tsuang, 19931. ADHD probands met DSM-111-R diagnostic criteria for
Specific characteristics that may be of particular use current ADHD at time of clinical referral. Two inde-
in the new phenotype definition in relatives are gender pendent sources provided the index children, as de-
and comorbidity status. Gender is relevant in that it scribed elsewhere [Biederman et al., 19921. A three-
may moderate the phenotypic expression of ADHD. For stage ascertainment procedure was used to select all
example, males may be more hyperactive and impul- probands regardless of their source of referral [i.e.,
sive while females may be more inattentive and have Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) or Harvard
more internalizing problems. This gender difference is Community Health Plan (HCHP)]. In each case, sam-
evidenced by ADHD being 6-9 times more prevalent in pling was based on consecutive referrals.
males than females [Anderson et al., 1987; Bird et al., For purposes of this investigation, only those parents
1988; Safer and Krager, 19881. who were interviewed directly about themselves were
Comorbidity status may be another important char- included in the analysis. This led to the exclusion of 42
acteristic to examine in the new phenotype definition, (16%)fathers and 2 (1%)mothers. Moreover, relatives
since it is believed that ADHD with various comorbid with incomplete data were also excluded. Six (2%)
psychiatric disorders represents a group of conditions mothers, 10 (4%)fathers, 17 (11%) sisters, and 22 (14%)
with different etiological risk factors rather than a sin- brothers were excluded for this reason. Thus, data on
gle homogeneous clinical entity. More specifically, it is 252 (97%) mothers, 207 (80%) fathers, 131 (89%) sis-
believed that ADHD with conduct disorder may repre- ters, and 133 (86%)brothers were used in the analysis.
sent a distinct genetic subtype ofADHD [Faraone et al., Table I provides a breakdown of the number of avail-
19911,that ADHD and major depression share common able subjects by whether they were related to an ADHD
familial etiologic factors [Biederman et al., 1991b1, and proband or to a normal control proband.
that ADHD and anxiety disorders are etiologically in-
dependent [Biederman et al., 1991al. Procedures
Modeled after recent work by Blacker and Tsuang All diagnostic assessments were made using DSM-
[19931 and by Faraone et al. [19951, the present work 111-R-based structured interviews. Psychiatric assess-
uses logistic regression to discriminate relatives of ments of probands and siblings were made with the
ADHD probands from relatives of normal controls. As Kiddie SADS-E (EpidemiologicVersion) [Orvaschel and
discussed in Subjects and Methods, this allowed us t o Puig-Antich, 19871. Diagnoses were based on indepen-

~ ~
TABLE I. Number of First-Dem-ee Biological Relatives of Probands
~ ~

Relatives of ADHD probands Relatives of normal control probands


Type of Total number Number included Total number Number included
relative (N = 454) (% of total) (N = 368) (% of total)
Mothers 140 136 (97%) 120 116 (97%)
Fathers 140 111(79%) 119 96 (81%)
Sisters 81 68 (84%) 67 63 (94%)
Brothers 93 78 (84%) 62 55 (89%)
New Phenotype Definition of ADHD 371

dent interviews with mothers and direct interviews of (WRAT-R) [Jastak and Jastak, 19851 and the Gilmore
probands and siblings, except for children below age 12 Oral Reading Test [Gilmore and Gilmore, 19681. Intel-
years, who were not directly interviewed. Diagnostic lectual functioning was assessed with the vocabulary,
assessments of parents were based on direct interviews block design, digit span, and digit symbol subtests of
with each parent, using the Structured Clinical Inter- the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised
view for DSM-111-R (SCID) [Spitzer and Williams, 19861. (WISC-R) [Wechsler, 19741. A total cognitive score was
All assessments were made by raters who were blind created by standardizing each of the above-mentioned
to proband diagnosis (ADHD or control) and ascertain- cognitive variables and summing them. This cognitive
ment site [MGH or Health Maintenance Organization score was then itself standardized to increase the in-
(HMO)]. Mothers’ interviews about their children were terpretability of this measure.
sequenced after the direct interview with the mother
about herself had been completed. Different raters con- Data Analysis
ducted the direct interviews of siblings and the indirect Univariate comparisons were conducted to assess the
interviews with mothers about their children. Inter- ability of individual variables to discriminate relatives
view data were collected on all siblings in both ADHD of ADHD probands from relatives of normal controls.
and control families. All parents signed a written con- Chi-square tests were used for categorical variables,
sent form for participation in the study. two sample t-tests were used for approximately normal
Interviews were conducted by five raters with under- continuous or ordinal variables, and Wilcoxon tests
graduate degrees in psychology who had been trained were used for continuous or ordinal variables with
to high levels of interrater reliability. Kappa coeffi- skewed distributions.
cients of agreement were computed between raters and In an attempt to define a phenotype of ADHD that is
three experienced, board-certified child and adult psy- highly prevalent among relatives of ADHD probands
chiatrists who listened to audiotaped interviews made but rare in the relatives of normal controls, logistic re-
by the raters. A kappa of 1.0 was obtained for ADHD gression was applied to the first-degree relatives of
(95% confidence interval, 0.8-1.0). ADHD and normal control probands. Let P denote the
Diagnoses were considered positive if, based on in- probability of being a first-degree relative of an ADHD
terview results, DSM-111-R criteria were unequivocally proband. The logarithm of the odds, log[P/(l - PI], is
met. All diagnostic uncertainties were resolved by a called the logit of P. Logistic regression models the logit
committee of four board-certified child and adult psy- as a linear function of the explanatory variables, which
chiatrists who were blind t o the subject’s ascertain- included the symptom scores of various psychiatric dis-
ment group, ascertainment site, all data collected from orders, the cognitive summary functioning score, so-
other family members, and all nondiagnostic data (e.g., cioeconomic status, and past and current global assess-
cognitive functioning). Diagnoses presented for review ment of functioning scores. All explanatory variables
were considered positive only if a consensus was were submitted to a forward stepwise logistic regres-
achieved that criteria were met to a degree that would sion using a significance level t o enter or stay in the
be considered clinically meaningful. For children older model of 0.05. At each step, the improvement in X2-test
than 12 years, symptom data from direct and indirect was used to check whether the variable entered at that
interviews were combined by considering a symptom step significantly improved the log-likelihood.Parame-
positive if it were endorsed in either interview. Chil- ter estimates were obtained using maximum likeli-
dren below age 12 years were not directly interviewed hood. After the last step, the hypothesis that the logis-
for a number of reasons: they have limited language ca-
tic model fit the data adequately was tested with the
pabilities, are often unable to map events over time, Hosmer-Lemeshowtest [Hosmer and Lemeshow, 19891.
and have limited abilities with abstract concepts. Given Once the final logistic regression models were deter-
these shortcomings, there is a real issue as to whether
the child’s self-perceptions, memories, emotions, and mined in each group of relatives, the equivalence of
reported behavior can be reliably assessed through self- these models was tested. This was done by using iden-
report. Studies on the use of interview techniques tical variables in each of the four models (a variable
among children under age 12 years show that they only was included if it was retained in any of the stepwise
understand between 28-38% of the questions [Breton logistic regression models). The log-likelihoods from
et al., 19953, that their replies are not reliable [Achen- these four individual models were then summed and
bach et al., 19871, and that their parents tend to compared with the log-likelihood from the model
be more reliable informants [Edelbrock et al., 1986; looking a t all relatives a t once. The difference in log-
Gutterman et al., 19871. likelihoods was multiplied by two and evaluated using
In parents and siblings, symptom scores were cre- a X2-test.
ated corresponding to ADHD and the various psy- After estimates of the coefficients were obtained, an
chiatric disorders that are frequently comorbid with estimate of the probability of being a relative of an
ADHD (e.g., conduct disorder, major depression, and ADHD proband was calculated for each relative in the
anxiety disorders). That is, for each of these diagnostic study as follows:
eOoat(i))l
categories, the number of symptoms endorsed was
summed to give an index of severity, instead of using P = [1 + e(logit(P))
1
the dichotomous DSM-111-R classification system.
Academic achievement was assessed with the Arith- These predicted probabilities provided a quantitative
metic Subtest of the Wide Range Achievement Test index of how likely a subject was t o be a relative of an
372 Milberger et al.
ADHD proband based on the relative’s profile of char- two models looking a t siblings (i.e., a proband could
acteristics as retained in the logistic regression model. have had two or more same-sex siblings). Since our pri-
An important issue to consider when working with mary aim was the separation of relatives of ADHD and
family data is that family members are not statistically normal control probands rather than hypothesis-
independent of one another due to shared cultural testing, we ignored this lack of independence in the
andor genetic factors. Although this dependency is not models using sisters and brothers. However, the mag-
likely t o have a large effect on parameter estimates it nitude of this problem should not be great, since the
can influence statistical testing. Dependency between number of brothers and sisters in these families was
subjects results in decreased variability which, in turn, not large. More specifically, in those families with
can yield underestimates of significance levels (i.e., brothers, 84% had only 1brother, 13%had 2 brothers,
smaller P values). To control for this, four different lo- and 3%had 3 brothers. Similarly, in those families that
gistic models were run for each type of relative of had sisters, 80% had only 1 sister, 17% had 2 sisters,
probands: one for mothers, one for fathers, one for sis- and 3% had 3 sisters.
ters, and one for brothers. This attempt to eliminate de-
pendency also helped address the issues of gender and RESULTS
generational effects, which may have considerable im- Table I1 presents the associations between group sta-
pact on ADHD family data. However, it was still possi- tus (i.e., being a relative of an ADHD proband vs. a rel-
ble that there were some related family members in the ative of a normal control proband) and each of the

TABLE IIa. Univariate Analyses in Mothers (N = 252)


Mothers of ADHD Mothers of normal
probands (N = 136) controls (N = 116) P value
Extra help in school 25% (34) 9% (11) 0.002
Repeated grade in school 20% (27) 5%(6) 0.0006
Special class in school 1%(1) 0% ( 0 ) 1.0
Mean t SD Mean t SD
ADHD symptom score 2.4 -+ 3.5 0.72 t 1.5 0.0001
Conduct disorder symptom score 0.45 f 0.86 0.20 2 0.51 0.005
Major depression symptom score 3.6 f 3.3 2.0 t 2.9 0.0001
Separation anxiety symptom score 0.50 f 1.2 0.36 t 0.77 0.10
Panic disorder symptom score 1.5 ? 1.5 0.43 t 1.4 0.0002
Antisocial personality disorder 1.6 f 1.1 1.3 t 0.63 0.006
symptom score
Generalized anxiety disorder 2.3 -+ 3.8 0.5 t 1.9 0.0001
symptom score
Cognitive summary score -1.2 t 3.7 0.65 t 2.9 0.0001
Current global assessment of 69.3 t 10.1 75.9 t 7.0 0.0001
functioning score
Past global assessment of 57.9 t 13.2 66.9 t 11.1 0.0001
functioning score
Socioeconomic status 1.8 f 0.83 1.5 2 0.67 0.0007
TABLE IIb. Univariate Analyses in Fathers (N = 207)
Fathers of ADHD Fathers of normal
Drobands (N = 111) controls (N = 96) P value
Extra help in school 26% (29) 15% (14) 0.06
Repeated grade in school 23% (25) 21% (22) 0.87
Special class in school 3%(3) 2% (2) 1.0
Mean t SD Mean t SD
ADHD symptom score 3.8 t 3.8 1.7 f 2.4 0.0001
Conduct disorder symptom score 1.2 t 1.5 0.76 t 1.5 0.006
Major depression symptom score 3.0 t 2.9 1.8 t 2.6 0.003
Separation anxiety symptom score 0.39 t 0.75 0.19 ? 0.53 0.005
Panic disorder symptom score 0.52 2 1.7 0.44 t 1.5 0.60
Antisocial personality disorder 2.1 t 1.3 1.6 t 0.93 0.005
symptom score
Generalized anxiety disorder 1.4 t 3.2 0.98 t 2.2 0.71
symptom score
Cognitive summary score -0.29 f 4.7 1.3 f 3.7 0.01
Current global assessment of 68.2 t 10.2 72.6 t 8.0 0.001
functioning score
Past global assessment of 56.5 ? 12.5 62.9 t 11.9 0.0001
functioning score
Socioeconomic status 1.6 t 0.79 1.4 t 0.59 0.06
New Phenotype Definition of ADHD 373
TABLE IIc. Univariate Analyses in Sisters (N = 131)
Sisters of ADHD Sisters of normal
probands (N = 68) controls (N = 63) P value
Extra help in school 35% (24) 22% (14) 0.12
Repeated grade in school 0% (0) 0% (0) NIA
Special class in school 3%(2) 10%(8) 0.17
Mean 2 SD Mean ? SD
ADHD symptom score 2.9 t 3.8 1.5 2 2.6 0.04
Agoraphobia symptom score 0.21 ? 0.59 0.13 ? 0.42 0.66
Conduct disorder symptom score 0.59 ? 1.2 0.22 ? 0.63 0.03
Major depression symptom score 2.1 t 3.1 2.2 t 3.0 0.87
Separation anxiety symptom score 1.1t 1.5 0.78 t 1.0 0.23
Simple phobia symptom score 1.8 ? 2.0 1.2 ? 1.8 0.06
Social phobia symptom score 1.3 t 1.9 0.71 ? 1.4 0.08
Overanxious symptom score 1.6 t 1.9 1.4 ? 1.8 0.32
Panic disorder symptom score 0.53 ? 1.5 0.55 ? 1.6 0.71
Cognitive summary score -0.29 t 3.3 0.44 2 3.0 0.19
Current global assessment of 70.3 2 11.0 72.3 ? 8.7 0.22
functioning score
Past global assessment of 63.8 2 12.5 66.3 2 11.4 0.19
functioning score
Socioeconomic status 1.7 t 0.80 1.5 2 0.70 0.23
TABLE IId. Univariate Analyses in Brothers (N = 133)
Brothers of ADHD Brothers of normal
probands (N = 78) controls (N = 55) P value
Extra help in school 41% (32) 29% (16) 0.20
Repeated grade in school 1%(1) 0% (0) 0.59
Special class in school 10% (8) 11% (6) 0.96
Mean ? SD Mean 2 SD
ADHD symptom score 3.9 ? 4.2 2.3 t 2.9 0.05
Agoraphobia symptom score 0.16 2 0.51 0.22 ? 0.41 0.11
Conduct disorder symptom score 1.0 ? 1.5 0.55 ? 1.2 0.03
Major depression symptom score 2.1 ? 3.0 1.4 ? 2.4 0.33
Separation anxiety symptom score 1.2 2 1.5 0.69 ? 0.79 0.08
Simple phobia symptom score 1.4 t 1.8 1.12 1.7 0.40
Social phobia symptom score 0.78 ? 1.6 0.58 ? 1.2 0.57
Overanxious symptom score 1.6 ? 1.6 0.95 ? 1.3 0.01
Panic disorder symptom score 0.51 t 1.8 0.27 2 1.2 0.31
Cognitive summary score -0.96 ? 3.7 1.2 2 3.8 0.003
Current global assessment of 67.4 ? 10.6 70.1 ? 8.4 0.07
functioning score
Past global assessment of 60.6 t 11.8 66.0 2 10.6 0.006
functioning score
Socioeconomic status 1.8 2 0.84 1.4 ? 0.63 0.002

explanatory variables t h a t was entered into the step- and past global assessment of functioning scores, and
wise logistic regression models. Mothers, fathers, sis- lower socioeconomic status compared with fathers of
ters, and brothers were examined separately (Table normal controls.
IIa-d, respectively). A number of significant differences Only two significant differences were found between
were seen between mothers of ADHD probands and sisters of ADHD probands and sisters of normal con-
mothers of normal controls (Table IIa). Compared with trols (Table IIc). Sisters of ADHD probands had higher
mothers of normal controls, mothers of ADHD ADHD and conduct disorder symptom scores than sis-
probands had higher rates of extra help and repeated ters of normal controls.
grades in school; they also had higher ADHD, conduct A number of significant differences were seen be-
disorder, major depression, panic disorder, antisocial tween brothers of ADHD probands and brothers of nor-
personality disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder mal controls (Table IId). Brothers of ADHD probands
symptom scores, lower current global assessment of had higher ADHD, conduct disorder, and overanxious
functioning, past global assessment of functioning, cog- disorder symptom scores, lower cognitive and past glo-
nitive, and socioeconomic status scores. bal assessment of functioning scores, and lower socio-
A number of significant differences were also found economic status than brothers of normal controls.
between fathers of ADHD probands and fathers of nor- Table I11 presents the final logistic regression models
mal control probands (Table IIb). The fathers of ADHD in the relatives. Models were run separately in moth-
probands had higher ADHD, conduct disorder, major ers, fathers, sisters, and brothers (Table IIIa-d, respec-
depression, separation anxiety, and antisocial person- tively). Although there was some overlap between the
ality disorder symptom scores, lower cognitive, current variables that were retained in each model, no two
374 Milberger et al.
TABLE 111. Logistic Regression Models in Relatives
a. In mothers*
Variable Odds ratio Standard error Z P value
ADHD symptom score 1.21 0.09 2.5 0.012
Cognitive summary score 0.87 0.04 -2.9 0.003
Current global assessment of functioning score 0.95 0.02 -2.5 0.013
Generalized anxiety disorder symptom score 1.17 0.08 2.3 0.022
* Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness of fit test: x2 = 274.72, df = 244, P = 0.10.
b. In fathers*
Variable Odds ratio Standard error Z P value
ADHD symptom score 1.20 0.06 3.5 0.001
Major depression symptom score 1.13 0.06 2.2 0.030
Socioeconomic status 1.54 0.34 2.0 0.047
* Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness of fit test: x2 = 118.40, df = 111, P = 0.30.
c. In sisters*
Variable Odds ratio Standard error Z P value
ADHD symptom score 1.14 0.07 2.2 0.030
* Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness of fit test: $ = 20.96, df = 13, P = 0.07.

d. In brothers"
Variable Odds ratio Standard error Z P value
Cognitive summary score 0.86 0.04 -2.9 0.004
Past global assessment of functioning score 0.96 0.02 -2.4 0.018
* Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness of fit test: x2 = 132.99, df = 130, P = 0.41.

models had exactly the same variables. For example, was not seen in any other relative profiles. Similar to
the logistic regression model discriminating mothers of the model in mothers, the cognitive summary score
ADHD probands from mothers of normal controls was also a n important characteristic of the profile in
showed that the ADHD symptom score (i.e., number of brothers.
ADHD symptoms that were endorsed), the cognitive The equivalence of these four logistic regression mod-
summary score, the current global assessment of func- els was evaluated by comparing the sum of the likeli-
tioning score, and the generalized anxiety disorder hoods from the four separate models and the likelihood
symptom score were the variables in the profile t h a t from the model examining all relatives a t once. The re-
yielded the greatest discriminating ability (Table IIIa). sults of this analysis yielded a x2 = 25.2 on 21 degrees
Similarly, the logistic regression model in fathers also of freedom ( d f )( P > 0.05).
showed that the ADHD symptom score was a n impor-
tant characteristic in the profile that discriminated fa- DISCUSSION
thers of ADHD probands from fathers of normal control Using logistic regression models, we identified a
probands (Table IIIb). In contrast, the model in fathers quantitative phenotype in relatives that consists of
showed that the major depression symptom score and measures across psychiatric, cognitive, and psychoso-
socioeconomic status were also part of the discriminat- cia1 domains. I t is possible that psychiatric genetics
ing profile, while the cognitive summary score, the cur- may be better served by diagnostic constructs created
rent global assessment of functioning score, and the for that particular intent than by existing diagnostic
generalized anxiety disorder symptom score were not. criteria that currently exist for broader purposes in
The logistic regression model in sisters was similar to clinical settings. The finding t h a t so many of the uni-
the models seen in both parent groups in that the variate analyses were significant, especially in parents,
ADHD symptom score was a n important characteristic is consistent with what we expected: that these fea-
in discriminating group status, In contrast to the mod- tures may reflect the phenomenology of a n ADHD gene.
els in parents, the profile that yielded the greatest dis- However, there is a lot of redundancy among these fea-
criminating ability in the sisters consisted of only t h a t tures and therefore when they were entered simultane-
one variable (Table IIIc). The logistic regression model ously into a logistic regression model only some of them
in the brothers differed from the models in all the other were retained.
relative groups in that the ADHD symptom score was The combination of these variables in a quantitative
not part of the profile discriminating group status logistic regression model yielded a discriminating ge-
(Table IIId). Moreover, the profile in brothers included netic phenotype for mothers of children with ADHD
the past global assessment of functioning score, which that included four variables: the ADHD symptom score,
New Phenotype Definition of ADHD 375
the cognitive summary score, the current global assess- affected by environmental factors such as perinatal
ment of functioning score, and the generalized anxiety complications while females (i.e., sisters) may repre-
symptom score. In contrast, the combination of these sent a more genetic group. Additional study is needed
variables yielded a genetic phenotype for the fathers in order to fully evaluate this hypothesis.
that included three variables: the ADHD symptom While our findings in the four groups of relatives sug-
score, the major depression symptom score, and socio- gest that we were successful in creating a quantitative
economic status. That cognitive scores were a significant phenotype in adults (i.e., mothers and fathers), our ap-
discriminator in mothers but not in fathers is not sur- proach was not as useful in children (i.e., sisters and
prising, since impaired cognitive functioning is more brothers). The fact that our approach did not improve
prevalent in males than females. This may be related to the ability to discriminate among children is not sur-
the tenet that males are more prone t o central nervous prising, since the diagnostic criteria have been de-
system insults [Hynd and Semrud-Clikeman, 1989a,b; signed for this age group and, in general, have been
O'Callaghan et al., 19921.Analogously, it is not surpris- shown to be useful. While we believe our new quantita-
ing that the total number of major depression symp- tive phenotype in adults may be useful in the context of
toms (summary score) was a significant discriminator genetic analyses, we do not imply that such a nosology
in fathers but not in mothers, since major depression is will be useful for clinicians or that it should replace
estimated to be twice as prevalent in females than in DSM-111-R.
males. The finding that socioeconomic status (SES) is Our work must be interpreted in the context of its
retained in the father model may be indicative that methodological limitations. An issue in this study is re-
SES is a correlate of the higher rates of psychopathol- call bias, which may be especially problematic in the as-
ogy in fathers of ADHD probands. Alternatively, it sessment of childhood diagnoses in adult relatives,
could be indicative that SES is itself a causal factor. since it requires them to recall experiences from child-
Further work is needed t o determine the influence of hood. For example, the diagnosis of ADHD requires an
SES. The differences between mothers and fathers onset of symptoms by age 7 years. Moreover, another
highlight the importance of considering demographic limitation of our work is that the use of maternal re-
factors such as gender and SES in the definition of fa- ports to make diagnoses of children may have led to
milial phenotypes. those diagnoses being influenced by maternal psy-
It is difficult to interpret the findings from the logis- chopathology. However, incorporation of maternal re-
tic regression model in sisters, since the model did not ports is standard clinical practice, since children are
fit the data well (Hosmer-Lemeshow Goodness of Fit considered to be poor reporters of their symptoms
Test x2 = 20.96, df = 13, P = 0.07). This poor fit sug- [Achenbach et al., 19871.
gests that the DSM-111-R categorical approach is supe- Another potential limitation in clinical studies is
rior in looking a t the phenotype in sisters of probands Berkson's bias, i.e., the sample may include a mislead-
ingly high proportion of subjects with multiple diag-
with ADHD.
noses just because referral will have been influenced by
The combination of variables in brothers of children the occurrence of each separate condition [Berkson,
with ADHD yielded a genetic phenotype that included 19461. However, high levels of comorbidity have been
the following two variables: cognitive functioning sum- observed in both clinical [Biederman et al., 1987,
mary score, and past global assessment of functioning 1991a,b, 19921and epidemiological samples [Anderson
score. The finding that the number of ADHD symptoms et al., 1987; Angold and Costello, 1993; Bird et al.,
in boys was not part of the profile discriminating among 19881, suggesting that the high levels of comorbidity
brothers is a t first glance puzzling. Although a signifi- seen in clinical samples are not solely an artifact of
cant difference in ADHD symptom score was observed Berkson's bias. Similarly, since 50% of the referrals to
between brothers of ADHD probands and brothers of the Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit a t Massachu-
normal control probands on a univariate level (odds ra- setts General Hospital have never been evaluated or
tio = 1.13, P = 0.021, the ADHD symptom score did not treated, this sample does not have the severity bias
add any discriminating ability to the profile. This is that might be associated with a tertiary care facility.
probably due, in part, to the fact that ADHD was so Since we did not control for age, and since the original
prevalent in brothers of normal controls (9%). More- sample of probands in this investigation came from
over, the ADHD symptom score was significantly corre- clinical referrals, we do not know to what degree these
lated with the past global assessment of functioning findings will generalize to nonreferred populations in
score (Spearman correlation coefficient = -0.52, P = the community.
0.0001) and slightly correlated with cognitive function- An additional potential shortcoming is that not all
ing summary score (Spearman correlation coefficient = relatives were included in the analysis due to indirect
-0.16, P = 0.07). interviewing (mostly of fathers) or inadequate data.
The finding that the discriminating variables in the The most extreme situation was the case of fathers of
brothers were reflective of socioeconomic status is par- ADHD probands, where only 79% were available for in-
ticularly interesting. Since these variables could be as- clusion in the analysis. However, as shown in Table I,
sociated with all types of psychopathology they may not no differences were noted in availability of subjects,
necessarily be useful in developing a discriminating ge- whether they were a relative of an ADHD proband or a
netic taxonomy for ADHD. The absence of SES differ- relative of a normal control.
ences in the sisters is consistent with existing litera- Another pitfall of this investigation is that we were
ture suggesting that males (i.e.)brothers) may be more not able to determine whether the different predictive
376 Milberger et al.
models in the four groups of relatives were significantly Angold A, Costello E J (1993): Depressive comorbidity in children and
adolescents: Empirical, theoretical and methodological issues. Am
different from one another. Although our statistical test J Psychiatry 150:1779-1791.
comparing the four models did not reveal statistically American Psychiatric Association (1987): “DSM-111-R.” Washington,
significant differences, this could be due to insufficient DC: American Psychiatric Association.
power. Therefore, our conclusions can only be con- Berkson J (1946): Limitations of the application of fourfold table
sidered tentative. Further studies of larger size are analysis to hospital data. Biometric Bulletin 2:47-52.
needed before more definitive conclusions can be made. Biederman J, Munir K, Knee D (1987): Conduct and oppositional dis-
A further limitation of this investigation is that our order in clinically referred children with attention deficit disorder:
data can only demonstrate a familial association be- A controlled family study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 26:
724-727.
tween the profile of characteristics in the relatives and Biederman J, Faraone SV, Keenan K, Knee D, Tsuang MT (1990):
ADHD in the proband. We infer that it is reasonable to Family-genetic and psychosocial risk factors in DSM-I11 attention
use the familial form of ADHD as a proxy for the ge- deficit disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29:52&533.
netic form, since it is believed that genetic factors me- Biederman J , Faraone SV, Keenan K, Steingard R, Tsuang MT
diate the familial transmission of ADHD. However, (1991a): Familial association between attention deficit disorder
studies of twin or adoption samples are needed to ver- and anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry 148:251-256.
ify that genetic factors account for the familial trans- Biederman J , Faraone SV, Keenan K, Tsuang MT (1991b): Evidence
of familial association between attention deficit disorder and ma-
mission of the profile of characteristics seen in the rel- jor affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 48:633-642.
atives. Since we do not yet know the nature of true Biederman J , Faraone SV, Keenan K, Benjamin J, Krifcher B, Moore
genetic cases, familial aggregation may be a reasonable C, Sprich S, Ugaglia K, Jellinek MS, Steingard R, Spencer T, Nor-
standard for the development of phenotype definitions man D, Kolodny R, Kraus I, Perrin J, Keller MB, Tsuang MT
(1992): Further evidence for family-genetic risk factors in atten-
for use in genetic analyses [Blacker et al., 19931. tion deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Patterns of comorbid-
Another limitation is that by extending our pheno- ity in probands and relatives in psychiatrically and pediatrically
type definition t o include anxiety in mothers and major referred samples. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:728-738.
depression in fathers, it is possible that the false- Bird HR, Canino G , Rubio-Stipec M, Gould MS, Ribera J, Sesman M,
positive rate could have been increased. Moreover, the Woodbury M, Huertas-Goldman S, Pagan A, Sanchez-Lacay A,
Moscoso M (1988): Estimates of the prevalence of childhood
models used may be incorrect or based on erroneous as- maladjustment in a community survey in Puerto Rico. Arch Gen
sumptions. The model assumes that an ADHD geno- Psychiatry 45:1120-1126.
type exists. Even if differences between relatives of Blacker D, Tsuang MT (1993): Unipolar relatives in bipolar pedigrees:
ADHD probands and relatives of controls are found, Are they bipolar? Psychiatr Genet 35-16,
other more complex genetic and environmental factors Blacker D, Lavori PW, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT (1993): Unipolar rel-
may be responsible for them [Blacker and Tsuang, atives in bipolar pedigrees: A search for indicators of underlying
bipolarity. Am J Med Genet (Neuropsychiatr Genet) 48:192-199.
19931. However, a phenotype definition based on these
Breton J, Bergeron L, Valla J, Lepine S, Houde L, Gaudet N (1995):
differences should still help eludicate features that sug- Do children aged 9 through 11years understand the DISC Version
gest familial transmission, and might be of benefit in 2.25 questions? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 34:946-956.
weighting the contribution of individual relatives in a Cantwell DP (1972): Psychiatric illness in the families of hyperactive
linkage analysis. children. Arch Gen Psychiatry 27:414-417.
Despite these limitations, our results suggest that Cantwell DP (1975): Genetics of hyperactivity. J Child Psychol Psy-
the use of fitted logits (or predicted probabilities) may chiatry 16:261-264.
be valuable as a potential index of caseness for linkage Chen WJ, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT (1992): Linkage studies of schizo-
studies. Since different characteristics were included in phrenia: A simulation study of statistical power. Genet Epidemiol
9:123-139.
the different groups of relatives, our results are consis- Deutsch CK, Matthysse S, Swanson JM, Farkas LG (1990): Genetic la-
tent with an existing body of literature [Anderson et al., tent structure analysis of dysmorphology in attention deficit disor-
1987; Bird et al., 1988; Safer and Krager, 19881,and in- der. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29:189-194.
dicate that gender and generation (parent vs. sibling) Edelbrock C, Costello M, Dulcan M, Conover NC, Kala R (1986): Parent-
may moderate expression of ADHD. Therefore, different child agreement on child psychiatric symptoms assessed via struc-
phenotype definitions for each group of relatives may tured interview. J Child Psychol Psychiat 27:181-199.
be necessary to delineate this variable expressivity. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Keenan K, Tsuang MT (1991): Separation
of DSM-I11 attention deficit disorder and conduct disorder: Evidence
from a family-genetic study of American child psychiatric patients.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Psychol Med 21:109-121.
This work was supported, in part, by United States Faraone SV, Biederman J , Chen WJ, Krifcher B, Keenan K, Moore C,
Sprich S, Tsuang M (1992): Segregation analysis of attention
Public Health Service Grant (National Institute of deficit hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for single gene transmis-
Mental Health) grant R01 MH-41314-01A2 (to J.B.). sion. Psychiatr Genet 2:257-275.
We thank Cheryl Ouellette, Janice Jones, and Mary Faraone SV, Santangelo S (1992):Methods in genetic epidemiology. In
Hatch for their contribution to this work. Fava M, Rosenbaum JF (eds): “Research Designs and Methods in
Psychiatry.” Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp 93-118.
Faraone SV, Seidman U,Kremen WS, Pepple JR, Lyons MJ, Tsuang
REFERENCES MT (1995): Neuropsychological functioning among the non-
Achenbach TM, McConaughy SH, Howell CT (1987): Childladolescent psychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients: A diagnostic efficiency
behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant analysis. J Abnormal Psychology 104:28&304.
correlations for situational specificity. Psychol Bull 101:213-232. Gilmore JV,Gilmore EC (1968): “Gilmore Oral Reading Test.” New
Anderson JC, Williams S, McGee R, Silva PA (1987): DSM-I11 disor- York Harcourt, Brace &World, Inc., pp 1-29.
ders in preadolescent children: Prevalence in a large sample from Goodman R, Stevenson J (1989a): A twin study of hyperactivity I:
the general population. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4459-76. An examination of hyperactivity scores and categories derived
New Phenotype Definition of ADHD 377
from Rutter teacher and parent questionnaires. J Child Psychol ter obstetric complications and their association with early onset
Psychiatry 30:671-689. of illness: A controlled study. Br Med [Clin Res1305:1256-1259.
Goodman R, Stevenson J (1989b):A twin study of hyperactivity 11:The Orvaschel H, Puig-Antich J (1987): “Schedule for AfTective Disorder
aetiological role of genes, family relationships and perinatal adver- and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Epidemiologic 4th
sity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 30591-709. Version.” Ft. Lauderdale: Nova University, Center for Psychological
Gutterman EM, O’Brien JD, Young G (1987): Structured diagnostic Study.
interviews for children and adolescents: Current status and future Ott J (1991):Genetic linkage analysis under uncertain disease definition.
directions. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 23:621-630. In Cloninger CR, Begleiter H (eds): “Banbury Report 33: Genetics
Hollingshead AB (1975): “Four Factor Index of Social Status.” New and Biology of Alcoholism.” Cold Spring Harbor, New York Cold
Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, pp 327-331.
Haven: Yale University, Department of Sociology.
Rutter M, Korn S, Birch HG (1963): Genetic and environmental
Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S (1989): “Applied Logistic Regression.” New factors in the development of “primary reaction patterns.” Br J SOC
York John Wiley, pp 1-307. Clin Psychol 2:161-173.
Hynd G, Semrud-Clikeman M (1989a): Dyslexia and brain morphol- Safer D J (1973): A familial factor in minimal brain dysfunction. Behav
ogy. Psychol Bull 106:447-482. Genet 3:175-186.
Hynd G, Semrud-Clikeman M (198913): Dyslexia and neurodevelop- Safer DJ, Krager JM (1988): A survey of medication treatment for
mental pathology: Relationships to cognition, intelligence and hyperactivelinattentive students. JAMA 260:225&2258.
reading skill acquisition. J Learn Disabil22:204-216.
Schachar R, Wachsmuth R (1990): Hyperactivity and parental psy-
Jastak JF, Jastak S (1985): “The Wide Range Achievement Test- chopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 31:381-392.
Revised.” Wilmington, Delaware: Jastak Associates.
Spitzer E, Williams JBW (1986): “The Structured Clinical Interview
Lahey BB, Russo MF, Walker J L (1989):Personality characteristics of for DSM-111-R.”New York New York State Psychiatric Institute.
the mothers of children with disruptive behavior disorders. J Consult
Clin Psychol 57512-515. Stewart MA, de Blois CS, Cummings C (1980): Psychiatric disorder in
the parents of hyperactive boys and those with conduct disorder.
Lopez RE (1965): Hyperactivity in twins. Can Psychiatr Assoc J 10: J Child Psychol Psychiatry 21:283-292.
421426.
Torgersen AM, Kringlen E (1978): Genetic aspects of temperamental
Mannuzza S, Gittelman R (1984): The adolescent outcome of hyper- differences in infants. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 17:433444.
active girls. Psychiatry Res 13:19-29.
Tsuang MT, Faraone SV, Lyons M J (1993): Identification of the pheno-
Morrison J (1980): Adult psychiatric disorders in parents of hyper- type in psychiatric genetics. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
active children. Am J Psychiatry 137:825-827. 682:l-12.
Morrison JR, Stewart MA (1971): A family study of the hyperactive Wechsler D (1974): “Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
child syndrome. Biol Psychiatry 3:189-195. Children-Revised.” New York Psychological Corporation.
Morrison JR, Stewart MA (1973): The psychiatric status of the legal Welner Z, Welner A, Stewart M, Palkes H, Wish E (1977): A controlled
families of adopted hyperactive children. Arch Gen Psychiatry 28: study of siblings of hyperactive children. J Nerv Ment Dis 165:
888-891. 110-117.
OCallaghan E, Gibson T, Colohan HA, Buckley P, Walshe DG, Larkin Willerman L (1973): Activity level and hyperactivity in twins. Child
C, Waddington J L (1992): Risk of schizophrenia in adults born af- Dev 44:28%293.

You might also like