You are on page 1of 10

A Synthesis of Occupational Behavior

and Sensory Integration Concepts in


Theory and Practice, Part 1.
Theoretical Foundations
(pediatric practice, play development, general systems theory)

Wendy Mack Jerry Ernest Lindquist L. Diane Parham

This series of two artIcles presents therapy. We observed that the dif- We considered play the key link
a model of play development for ferences between the two approaches between sensory integration and
use m pediatric occupational ther- seemed to lead many in the profes- occupational behavior approaches.
apy. Proposmg to unify the theo- sion to consider them mutually ex- Most behavioral scientists would
retIcal approaches of sensory inte- clusive frames of reference. In-depth agree that, al though the com plexity
gration and occupational study of basic concepts, however, of play renders it difficult to define,
behavior, the model uses playas revealed them to be more comple- a basic characteristic of play is that it
the unifying link between these mentary than contradictory. This is intrinsically motivated behavior
two apparently different paper represents an effort to syn- that is experienced as pleasurable.
approaches. This first article thesize sensory integration and oc- A more concise definition is not
reviews the major concepts of sen- cupational behavior concepts into a attempted here. Rather, Reilly's
sory integratzon and occupational model of play development upon view (1) of playas a multidimen-
behavior, In addition to dIscussing which occupational therapy prac- sional sys tem for adaptation to the
the differences and similarities tice with handicapped children can environment is accepted. Her view
between the two. General systems be based. is consistent with Vandenberg's
concepts are used as a framework approach to playas an integral part
upon which a model of play of the process underlying tool use
development is constructed. Three and social and motor development
hierarehicallevels of play are Jerry Ernest Lindquist, M.A., (3).
described-sensorimotor, construc- OTR, is an occupational therapist A general systems approach was
tive, and social-with each broken at the Speech and Language found useful for organizing the
down mto several developmental Development Center, Buena Park, complexities of play development
steps. Californza. in this study. Discussion of play
development is limited to the early
he purpose of this paper is to Wendy Mack, OTR, is an occupa- years of life, but many of the con-
T present a perspective that tional therapist at the Speech and cepts presented could be applied to
unifies two seemingly different Language Development Center, the understanding of human behav-
approaches to occupational therapy Buena Park, Callfornza. ior throughout the life span.
with children: the occupational This paper is divided into two
behavior view of play espoused by L. Diane Parham, M. 1., OTR, 1S a sections. Part I aims to synthesize
Mary Reilly (J), and the sensory doctoral student In the (;raduate theoretical concepts from both oc-
integration theory of A. Jean Ayres School of Education at the Unz- cupational behavior and sensory
(2). The conception of these two versity of California, Los Angeles, integration approaches. After a re-
approaches as a unified whole was and is an occupational therapist at view of the two approaches and a
developed by the authors while the Ayres Clinic, Torrance, discussion of their differences and
graduate students of occupational California. similarities, a model of play devel-

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 365


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Table 1
Differences between Occupational Behavior/Play and Sensory Integration
Approach with Children

opment is presented, incorporating Occupational Behavior Sensory Integration


concepts from both. In Part 2, the Theoretical Developmental psychology, Neurophysiology,
interrelationships between sensory Base sociology, anthropology (1) neurophysiology (2)
integration and play are explored Research Conceptual model building: Empirical data collection:
as they relate to clinical practice. Emphasis exploratory (1, 7-15) experimental, correlational
Implications for understanding (16-21 )

dysfunction are discussed, and s'..Ig- Assessment Qualitative: history taking, Quantitative:
gestions for assessment and treat- Techiques description, observation (7, 10, standardized evaluation
12-15) (22,23)
ment are made, drawing from the
literature in both sensory integra- Treatment Traditional: toys, games, arts Nontraditional: equipment and
and crafts (9-13) activities to control sensory
tion and occupational behavior
input (2, 4)
frames of reference.
Table 2
Review of Sensory Integration Similarities between Occupational Behavior/Play and Sensory Integration Approaches
and Occupational Behavior with Children
Approaches
Sensory Integration. Sensory ir:te- Theory Hierarchical Development (1, 2)

grative theory and practice are con- Adaptive response to a "just-right" challenge (2, 4, 12,24)
cerned with the relationship be- Practice
Intrinsic motivation/self-direction (2,4,10). Play is the process
tween fundamental sensorimotor (1,4, 13)
capacities and more complex func-
Outcome Competence (1,4)
tional areas, such as academic
learning (2). Therapists involved in
sensory integration practice address work-play phenomenon in human plete listings. For example, thera-
in a specific client population a life should be considered the foun- pists who use an occupational be-
very basic level of human function- dation for practice and research in havior approach may use stan-
that of sensory processing and neu- occupational therapy. The term oc- dardized evaluations when appro-
ral organization-to enhance cupational behavior referred to the priate, but the literature has stressed
clients' abilities to effectively func- developmental continuum of play history taking and description of
tion in their daily life environments. and work within an individual life observations as primary methods
Because effective environmental span (5, 6). In building a knowledge for assessment. The sensory integra-
interactions are dependent on an base for this perspective, Reilly and tion literature, on the other hand,
accurate interpretation of a specific her associates contributed explora- has largely focused on the use of
environment, sensory integration tory studies relating play to learn- standardized tests, although thera-
practice focuses on the individual's ing skills, rules, and roles for life pists practicing from this perspec-
a bili ties to organ ize and deri ve pre- tasks (e.g., I). Play is viewed as the ti ve also use his tory -taking and
cise information from basic senscry primary activity of the young child, clinical observations in assessment.
inputs arriving from the body and and prerequisite to competence in These differences seem to create a
from the external environment. occupational roles throughout life. polarity between the two ap-
Sensory integrative development is Comparing the Two Approaches. proaches. Occupational behavior is
theorized to occur primarily in early Sensory integration and occupa- very broad in scope, integrating
play experiences (4); thus therapy tional behavior/play approaches information from diverse fields of
strives to create a playful environ- appear to have taken quite different inquiry in order to understand
ment within the context of specific paths in developing theory, re- human problems. At the opposite
activities to enhance sensory inte- search, assessment techniques, and pole, sensory integration focuses on
gration and adaptive capacities. treatment methods. Table 1 sum- one aspect of human development,
Occupational Behavior. The oc- marizes the most prominent differ- that which is mediated by central
cupational behavior frame of refer- ences between the two approaches. nervous system functioning. In
ence is directl y concerned wi th pIa y. The features identified under each doing so, it gains precision in
Drawing from the profession's his- approach are intended to represent measuring and describing the par-
torical roots, Rei!! y wrote that the areas of emphasis rather than com- ticular types of human problems it

366 June 1982, Volume 36, Number 6


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
stressed during the child's first year
Figure 1 of life. Yet. constructive aspects of
Hierarchy of play development
play, as well as social aspects, par-
ticularly in the form of caregiver-
child interactions, certainly exist at
COMPETENCE
this early time. As one level comes
to predominate over another, the
emphasis of the child's play gradu-
all y changes. W hen each level oper-
ates effectively, the result is a child
competent in interacting with the
en vironmen t.
I SOCIAL I General systems theory (CST)
provides a structure upon which to
CONSTRUCTIVE build the model of play develop-
menlo In choosing a G T approach
to integrate theoretical aspects of
SENSORY MOTOR sensory integration and occupa-
tional behavior, the authors fol-
lowed a path first paved by Reilly
(1), Kielhofner, Burke (27, 28), and
others. Tradi tiona II y, science has
tended to view the world as the sum
addresses, bu t loses breadth in its guiding therapy, for importance is of its parts. In contrast to the rigor-
scope of application. placed on the child's inner drive ously empirical approaches of clas-
In spite of the apparent polarity, toward mastery. Play, then, is the sical science, GST views phenomena
the (w approaches need not be process through which therapeutic as wholes in their environments,
considered m u tuall y excl usi ve. goals are achieved The ultimate rather than studying carefully iso-
Strong similari ties exist in concepts goal of therapy-competence in lated parts (29, 30) CST is designed
central to theory, practice, and goals daily life activities-is also shared to describe, explain, and eventually
of thera y (see able 2). These sug- by both perspectives. predict the interactions of a large
gest that, r<lther than being diamet- number of variables, and as such
ricall y opposed, the two approaches Play Development appears to offer new avenues for
might be seen as complementary. Play development is described in understanding human beings and
Both acknowledge concepts of hier- this model as a hierarchy of three their interactions with and within
archical development as basic to levels: sensorimotor play, construc- their environments.
theory underlying the process of tive play, and social play. Figure I Open systems are characterized
therapy. In practice, both ap- depicts this basic hierarchy, drawn by the import-export activities they
proaches deem the therapist re- from writers in occupational ther- conduct with their environments.
sponsible for structuring adaptive apy (25, 26). Each level of the The open system continually takes
behavior from the child. Thus, the hierarchy emphasizes separate in something from its environment
potency of the environment's influ- aspects of the child's play expe- and gives out something to its envi-
ence on development is confirmed rietlce. Sensorimotor play lays the romnent, all the while maintaining
by both. But from neith r perspec- groundwork for the emergence of its structure amidst this flow. These
tive does therapy rely solely on constructive play. Social play relies system-environment interactions
environmental manipulation. The upon an adequate development of are best understood by using the
child's initiative and active involve- sensorimotor and constructive concepts of input, throughput,
ment are critical to the therapeu- aspects of play. I t is im pOltant to output, and feedback. As this model
tic process. From both perspectives, note, however, that all types of play is one of an open system, it is
the intrinsic motivation or self- co-ex is tat each level of the mode 1. assumed that there are interactions
direction of the child is primary in For example, sensorimotor play is occurring between the sy)LClil (child)

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 367


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Figure 2
Expanded hierarchical model of play development

i
Feedback (
~
I
I
Cultural Internalization Soclodramatlc
norms
... of social roles role play; Games
with rules
iii
'uo i

~
(J)
I Feedback I
I
People Learning ... Social
rules of people participation

------------- -
--t---
I Feedback I
--- ----------

~ I
Objects ..
...
Symbolization
Imaginative
fantasy play

i
Feedback

Ql
.~
~ I
'ti
2 Objects ... Imagery
... Problem solving
iii and 1001 use
c:

i
o
U

Feedback

~ I
Objects
... Learning Combining and
rules of objects
"'"I sequencing

-- -- - --------
-t------
Feedback
....
- ----- - --

~ I
Sensation
Learning
rules of motion
..
... Motor skills

r
Feedback ....
~ I
Sensation .
... Perception .
... Adaptive
response
>.
o
III
c:
Ql
(J)
i
I Feedback I

~ eNS organizallon,
I
Adaptive
Sensation
... Including sensory reflextive
integration response

368 June 1982, Volume 36, Number 6

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


and the environment at each level of distinct levels of child's play are 2. Higher levels depend upon
the model. These interactions are depicted, each broken down into lower levels. The adequate devel-
depicted by successive feedback several steps; but it must be remem- opment of sensorimotor play con-
loops. At each level, there are inputs, bered that the boundaries of the tributes to good constructive play.
throughputs, and outputs, part of model are artificial ones allowing This, in turn, forms the basis for the
which are fed back into the system explanation and discussion of the child's social play. The model sug-
to facilitate growth, development, phenomena. In reality, play devel- gests that the child who is deficient
and hence, a change toward increas- opment probably occms in many in early sensorimotor experience
ing complexity. By definition, input small, gradual steps, rather than in will not be as adept in constructive
is energy, material, or information these large steps. Nor are the levels and social aspects of playas the
arising from the external environ- of play exclusive of one another, as child who experiences adequate
ment or from internal sensations. the model might suggest. Rather, it sensorimotor play.
Throughput refers to processes is proposed here that aspects of all 3. Higher levels direct and orga-
within the system that transform levels occur throughout devel- nize the lower levels. As the hierarchy
input to output. At lower levels, opment; the emphasis changes as is ascended and the focus of the
throughpu t is more of a neuro- one level comes to predominate over child's play changes in complexity,
physiological process, but as the another. Nevenheless, all type of the lower levels con inue to be
hierarchy is ascended and the com- play may occur simultaneously at refined while the higher levels pre-
plexity increases, throughput be- each level of the model. For exam- dominate. For example, the empha-
comes more of a cognitive and, ple, the young child sp nds the sis of the 6-year-old's play is clearly
eventually, a social process. Output early part of life engaged predomi- social in nature, yet the refinement
is conceptualized as a force from the nantly in sensorimotor play. There of constructive as well as sensori-
system that acts on the environ- cenainly may also be constructive motor skills continues to occur
ment. Again, nergy, information, and social aspects to play at this within the context of this social
or material can act as output; thi level, but the emphasis is on the play.
includes behavior in the case of the deve lopment of sensori motor ski lis. 4. A change at one level affects all
human syst m. Feedback is defined In a gradual proc ss, the emphasis other levels. The child whose early
as that pan of the output that returns changes to focus on ollstructive sensorimotor experience is lacking
to the system as input, serving to and, later, social aspects of play. may find constructive and social
modify throughput processes and According to CST, most of the play affected c swell. Converseiy,
influence future output toward more complex systems one encoun- treatment experiences dir eted
higher levels of organi7ation (31, ters in natur are hierarchically toward changf's at one level of play
32). organized. Within this hierarchical may result in changes at other levels.
Figure 2 shows all e. panded ver- structure, intt'lTclated subsystems It bears re-empha'izing that
sion of the basic model. Within interact dynamically with one competence is gE'nerated at each
each level of the play hierarchy, the another. A categorization of these level of the model. This model
authors have identj(ied two or three intera tions has been offered by attempts to integrate theoretical
feedback loops that highlight the Feibleman (33). Because this model aspects of playas viewed by both
developmental process. Research of play development is haracter- occupational behavior and sensory
from oc upational therapy and ized by its hierar hi al organiza- integra tion practitioners. The child
psychology was u ed in identifying tion. Feiblernan 's laws of hierarchic whos sensory integrative functions
the specific inputs, throughputs, lev 1 are pertinent in this case: are intact, who freely explores the
and outputs occurring in each feed- I. The compl xity increases at constru tive aspects of the environ-
back loop. Later discussion of this higher levels. The child's social ment, and who successfully expe-
model will begin at the s nsorimo· play is considerably more complex riences the social spheres of the
LOr level with the first feedback loop han earlier play. In social play, world through the vehicle of play
(bollom of Figure 2) and progress interactions occur with more parts will become a competent actor in
up the hierarchy. of the environment, throughput the environment. Com pet ncy is a
Mod Is ultimately diston the processes are increasingly more product of evolution, development,
phenomena they a ttempt to explain. intricate, and resulting behaviors and learning, and is critical for
This model is no exception. Three are certainly more complc,'. adaptation.

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 369


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Robert W. White defined com- of innate adaptive responses in the This process of recei ving sensory
petence as effective interaction with form of reflexes (4). Thus, in the input, integrating it, and respond-
the environment: "To be competent first feedback loop of sensorimotor ing adaptively to it continues
means to be sufficient or adequate play, sensation from the body and throughout the life span. Because of
to meet the demands of a situation the environment as input is received the sensory feedback generated dur-
or task." (34, p 273) and organized by the infant's CNS ing the execution of an adaptive
Ayres suggested that the child as throughput. This process results response, the organization and in-
who is able to integrate sensory in a reflexive adaptive response as tegration processes as well as the
input effectively experiences the output. The adaptive response is an following adaptive responses be-
end-products of self-esteem, self- indication that the CNS is organiz- come increasingly complex. With
control, and self-confidence (4), all ing the input effectively. It must be this increasing complexity, sensa-
crucial aspects of the competent remembered that even at this early tions are organized into perceptions
child Reilly (35) viewed compe- stage, sensory feedback from the and meaning is given to the input.
tency as a biological and social reflexi ve response acts to modify Perception is defined as the inter-
phenomenon: biological because it these innate mechanisms, resulting pretation of sensation based on one's
is a human behavior that becomes in more complex behavior. The experience. For example, the in-
increasingly complex with thedevel- child's active exploration of the fant's sense of sight is not very well
opment of the child; social because environment is a primary facilitator organized at birth. With continued
it is gained through successful in- of this process. contact, the infant learns to recog-
teractions with others in the e~vi­ Of particular importance in the nize the primary caregiver's face,
ronment. She wrote that it is the processes of organization and inte- that particular set of visual stimuli,
openness of the system-the con- gra tion a t this level are sensa tions as significant. With continued ex-
stant flow of energy and informa- arising from the infant's tactile perience, the infant interprets the
tion via feedback loops-that allows receptors, proprioceptors, and ves- sensations and one or' his or her ear-
adaptation, purpose, and goal seek- tibular receptors. These senses have liest perceptions is "mommy's face."
ing. She further suggested that the begun their development in utero This describes the second feedback
laws of processing behavior via this and, in some respects, are fairly well loop of the sensorimotor level. Sen-
feedback arrangement may offer new organized at birth. Adaptive re- sation continues as input. Inter-
avenues for understanding com- sponses to sensations of gentle body preted in the light of experience,
petency. movement, of touch, and from sensation becomes perception, the
muscles and joints are essential to throughput. Adaptive responses
the increasing organiza tion of the continue as output, but behavior is
Sensorimotor Play CNS. Without the integration that no longer as reflex bound as it was
The first feedback loop of the model occurs in this process, adequate earlier. It is at this level that sen-
of play development concerns sen- development would be impossible sorimotor play begins to emerge,
sorimotor play, defined as intrinsi- in later stages (4). involving exploration of sensation
cally motivated, pleasurable activ- The adaptive response is a suc- and movement.
ity involving exploration of sensa- cessful response to an environmen- It must be re-emphasized that,
tion and movement. In the first year tal demand. This process is not a even though the young child spends
of life, the child is especially a sen- passive one; rather, the child plays the early part of life engaged pri-
sorimotor being, acting in response an active role in seeking environ- marily in sensorimotor play, early
to sensation arising from the body mental interactions that encourage social play also occurs at this level.
and the environment. As the child more com plex ada pti ve beha vior. It As the child and caregiver become
organizes sensation and responds is this "intentionali ty" of the child, engaged in earl y social in teractions,
appropriately to it, the central ner- or the active role played in seeking these may infl uence sensorimotor
vous system (CNS) becomes more environmental interactions that development at this level. If deficits
organized. This is the process of facilitates the emergence of adap- exist in early caregiver-child inter-
sensory in tegra tion, the organiza- tive responses of greater complex- actions, sensorimotor play, in turn,
tion and interpretation of sensajon ity. This constructive exploration may be adversely affected. If, on the
for an adaptive response (4). Hered- (4) is a cri tical aspect of the ada ptive other hand, the caregiver presents
ity provides the infan t wi th a variety response. an adequate model of playfulness,

370 June 1982, Volume 36, Number 6

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


including appropriate social inter- actions reqUlnng no motor plan- frequency. During the first year of
actions, sensorimotor play may be ning or conscious attention. They life, most of the infant's play with
enhanced. are habitual actions, for the child objects is sensorimotor and focuses
An early perception developing has mastered them and no longer on action and sensation. The first
at this time is the infant's body needs to di rect a tten tion to their actions with objects are gross and
scheme. This is the perception of performance. Examples of motor do not vary much from one object to
one's body parts, their relationships, skills could inel ude drinking from a another. If given an object, the
and how they move. It requires the cup, walking down steps, and rid- infant is apt to bang it or mouth it.
continual integration arising from ing a tricycle. Reiterated, sensation But as motor skills and perception
movement, from touch, and from arising from the practice of non- are refined, the properties of objects
muscles and joints. This body habi tual motor actions assists in the are explored in actions. The infant
scheme contains information about learning of rules of motion that may reach for an object and manip-
each body part: its size, its weight, allow for the development of motor ulate it while carefully examining
how it feels, and how it moves (4). skills. it. Actions with an object begin to
As a result of this developing body In summary, sensorimotor play show that the infant is aware of its
scheme, an output that emerges at allows the child to achieve a number properties (36). The child is learn-
this level is a beginning form of of things, not the least of which ing che rules of objects. Robinson
imitation. The infant is now able to includes a more organized brain. defines rules of objects as knowl-
reproduce familiar body move- The infant's experience allows sen- edge about the properties of objects
ments. With further development, sa tion to be processed into percep- (24). When the infant "knows the
the child is able to reproduce unfa- tion. Perception includes the devel- rules" about an object, he or she can
miliar gestures (36). opment of a body scheme and anticipate what an object will do
Some of the sensa tions arising praxis, both of which contribute to when it is acted upon. Learning the
from this new-found ability to imi- the development of imitation. rules of objects is a critical process
tate act as feedback to modify and Finally, rules of motion are mas- for beginning constructive play
refine the infant's body scheme. As tered, thus allowing the emergence beca use the properties of objects
this happens, the infant begins to be of automatic motor skills. Sensori- must be known before objects can
able to conceive of and organize a motor experiences continue to de- be combined effectively. Early con-
sequence of unfamiliar actions. velop and contribute to competence structive play begins when the child
This is praxis, or motor planning, as play progresses into constructive spoEtaneously and intentionally
as defined by Ayres (4). The adap- and social levels of development. com;Jines objects in simple ways-
tive motor response that emerges as for example, by putting one object
output at this level is then a non- inside another or by placing one
habitual action. The development Consttuclive Play block on top of another. This first
of motor planning permits play The second major level of pIa y in feedback loop of constructive play,
development to progress to the final this hierarchical model is that of then, involves objects and materials
feedback loop of the sensorimotor constructive play. Constructive play as i:l.put, and learning rules of
level. As the child repeats nonha- is defined as play that involves the objects as throughput; the output is
bitual movements, the resulting combination and/or sequencing of combining and sequencing objects
sensation is processed into knowl- objects in time and space. Most the- in play. Feedback arising from these
edge about how the body moves oris ts and resea rchers of pIa y devel- aCtivities helps the child continue
through space. Robinson suggested opment agree tha t this kind of pIa y to enlarge the repertoire of rules of
that the infan t is developing rules of begins to predominate during the motion and objects. Feedback also
motion (24). These are defined as second year of life (36-39). It con- stimulates the development of cog-
mental representations of how one's tinues to develop as an important nitive processes.
body moves through space. With part of play throughout the pre- Imagery is one such cognitive
the repetition of movements, the school years. process that, in turn, influences
child learns the sensorimotor rules The beginnings of constructive constructive play (40). Imagery is
about how the bod y operates. Even- play occur during sensorimotor the ability to visualize objects and
tuall y, some of these movemen ts play. Becoming more mobile, the events mentally. A I-year-old child
become automatic motor skills, or child contacts objeCts with greater is using imagery when he or she

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 371

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


combines objects that have been puts of perception, rules of motion, the child's play during the pre-
seen together as a set before. For and rules of objects in order to school years, both as a solitary acti v-
example, the child may put a spoon achieve success. The ability to visu- i ty and as a vehicle for the begin-
in a toy cup and then pretend to eat alize relationships between objects ning of social play with peers.
from it. Such pretend play using makes the child more likel y to use
objects starts as single actions, and tools and attempt to solve prob- Social Play
gradually increases in complexity lems. These actions, in turn, pro- Social play emerges somewhere in
as the child links actions in to longer vide feedback that leads to the re- the third year of life. It is defined
sequences. The work of Lowe has finement of fine motor skills, per- here as play activities involving
shown that during the second year ception, and cognitive functions. physical and/or verbal interactions
of life, pretend play with toys not By the time the second birthday is with peers. Eckerman, Whatley, and
only increases in complexity of reached, the child is beginning to Kutz described increases in social
sequences, but also shifts from use real symbols in play. In symbol- play throughout infancy until, at
actions centered on the self to ac- iza tion, one thing is used to rep- a bou t 2 years of age, social play pre-
tions centered on a doll. Seque:l.ces resent something else not present. A dominates over solitary play (41).
also evolve from single actions to a symbol may be an action, object, Before this age, social interactions
series of actions in logical temporal mental image, or word (40). The have primarily involved the child
order (37). last feedback loop in constructive and another adult, usually the care-
Imagery also underlies the devel- play, according to this model of giver. An important product of these
opment of deferred imitation dur- play development, involves the new early child-adult interactions is the
ing the second year of life. In de- throughput of symbolization. beginning development of social
ferred imita tion, the child can retain Imaginative fantasy play is the out- reci proci ty as the child learns to
a visual image of another's actions put when symbols are used in com- playa more active role in social
and imitate itatanother time. Now plex sequences. Often this may in- interactions (42). The development
the children are often onlookers of volve pretending an object is present of reciprocity may be related to the
others' activities and may later when it is not, or substituting one development of rules of people, de-
imitate when the model is no longer object for another while enacting a scribed by Robinson (24) and de-
present (36). dramatic sequence. An example fined here as knowledge about the
When the child's cognitive abili- might be a child preparing a tea behaviors, and meanings of behav-
ties develop to the extent that new party for dolls. By now, the child iors, of others. The infant initially
relationships between objects are is able to focus attention for longer does not have the appropriate rules
perceived, imagery becomes a criti- periods of time, and play shows of people to guide the social in terac-
cal process in the development of evidence of more elaborate spatio- tion. In this situation, the adult
problem-solving and tool-use skills. temporal sequencing. To an out- plays the major role in providing
This brings us to the next feedback side observer the child's play appears direction and meaning to the en-
loop in constructive play develop- more organized and mature (36,37). counter. Such child-adult play se-
ment: the input (objects) is pro- To summarize, constructive play quences often have well-defined
cessed by using imagery to produce develops out of sensorimotor play structures. For example, the game
observa ble problem -sol ving and as the child learns rules of objects of peek-a-boo has a predictable se-
tool-use behaviors in play. At first, and begins to combine and sequence quence of interaction. Initially, the
the child uses an overt trial-and- them, first in single concrete acts, adult acts to provide the internal
error approach to solve problems. and later in longer elaborate play structure to the game. As the game
During the second year of life, the sequences. The development of is repeated in this same sequence,
process of imagery develops to the imagery expands the play repertoire the child learns what behaviors are
extent that the child can visualize by contributing to deferred imita- likely to occur and what an appro-
attempting different solutions be- tion, as well as to problem solving priate response might be (42). Now
fore actually trying one. Simple and tool use. By the second birth- the child becomes a more active par-
tool use also emerges; for example day, the child is able to symbolize tici pan t in social interaction. In
using a stick to obtain an out-of- and engages in truly imaginative this way, early child-adult interac-
reach object (36). In these situations fantasy play. Constructive play will tions help the child to begin to
the child relies on earlier through- continue to be an important part of interpret others' behaviors and to

372 June 1982, Volume 36, Number 6

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


predict how others will respond to children in sociodramatic play. an equal player in this social world.
them. Since there now is the begin- Sociodramatic play is defined here Thus, we see at this level of play the
ning ability to derive social mean- as group play around a role-playing expansion of competence through
ing from behavior, the child can situation when each member takes the development of effeeti ve social
initiate and participate in social on a differen t role, such as "pIa ying skills, which are essential to the role
in teractions. house." The way in which a child of preschooler and which will con-
At this social level of play in the plays a role is based on internal tinue to be important throughout
model, the child expands on the expectations, or the child's own the life span.
rules of people with reference to understanding of that role. In addi-
pIa y wi th peers. As social interac- tion, feedback from peers about the Summary
tions occur mainly around senso- accuracy of role enactment may be This paper has presented overviews
rimotor play and play with objects, received that will further modify of sensory integration and occupa-
adequate development of these specific internal role expectations. tional behavior approaches, in addi-
lower levels of play is essential for Ca rvey (43) descri bes severa I tion to comparing and contrasting
the proper emergence of social play. values of sociodramatic play. These the two, to provide the background
The primary input of the first include a more flexible approach to for a synthesis of these approaches
feedback loop is the existence of situations and greater cooperative within a model of play develop-
people, especially peers, with whom behavior and role-taking skills. ment. Differences between sensory
to interact Interactions with peers Rosen (44) facilitated sociodramatic integration and occupational be-
here result in the formation, and play in preschool children, demon- havior were found in the nature of
further refinement, of rules of peo- strating significant improvements assessmen t tools used, the areas of
ple. These may involve imitation of in cooperative group problem-solv- literature from which underlying
others' behaviors and language as ing tasks as well as in role-taking concepts have been drawn, research
vehicles for communication in play. skills. emphasis, and specific treatment
Social participation around a Another more complex output at proced ures used.
common play activi ty becomes pos- this level is the engagement in These differences highlighted the
sible as the child learns how partic- games with rules. These can be apparent incongruity of the two
ular behaviors may affect others, defined as institutionalized play approaches; however, similarities
and wha t the beha viors of others activities that contain specific and central to occupational therapy
mean. As rules of people are refined, pu blicl y defi ned rule structures goals and practice were identified
the child gains a greater under- (eg, jacks, hop-scotch). The child that make possible the unification
standing of the possibilities of peer is able to engage in games with of sensory integration and occupa-
interactions and is able to engage in rules as a result of expanding abili- tional behavior approaches. These
more complex coopera ti ve social ties for cooperation within a social similari ties inel ude the concept of
groups. group, for planning and construct- hierarchical development basic to
At a higher level of social pIa y lies ing longer action seq I nces in pIa y, each approach and identified as a
the input of cultural norms. These and for greater self-control for con- cri tical concept within the model of
are formed from the child's observa- straining actions wi thin a rule struc- play development. In the area of
tions of others engaged in cultur- ture. Sutton-Smith (45) described a practice, therapists' slructuring of
ally relevant role behaviors-for developmental progression of games the "just right" challenge, acknowl-
example, watching D3ddy shave or with rules that enhances the child's edgment of the child's self-direction
a doctor perform routine duties. conception of social actions, social toward competent behavior, and the
From these immedia te social expe- relations, and role-taking skills. use of playas the process for devel-
riences, the child develops internal Through social play and games, opment of adaptive behavior are
role expectations about behaviors then, the child is able to develop critical commonalities. Finally, the
and attitudes characterizing these social skills expected of the age-role therapeutic goal of competent per-
roles; for example, internal knowl- of preschooler: sharing, concentra- formance of daily life tasks was
edge of what a daddy does is formed. tion, impulse control, taking turns, iden tified as common to both ap-
The child then becomes more aware etc. In social play, the child gains proaches.
of adult roles and enjoys enacting valuable knowledge about the social Sensory integration and occupa-
these roles dra ma tica 11 y wi th 0 ther world and begins to participate as tional behavior concepts were syn-

The American Journal oj Occupational Therapy 373

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


thesized within a hierarchical model child. In Playas Exploratory Learning, J Occup Ther 31: 675-689, 1977
M Reilly, Editor. Beverly Hills: Sage 29. Von Bertalanffy L: General systems
of play development. Three hierar- Publications, 1974 theory-A critical review. In Modern
chicallevels of pia y-sensorimotor, 13. Takata N: Playas a prescription. In Systems Research for the Behavioral
constructive, and social-were dis- Playas Exploratory Learning, M Reilly, Scientist, W Buckley, Editor. Chicago:
Editor. Beverly Hills: Sage Publica- Aldine Publishing Co., 1968
cussed using open systems concepts tions, 1974 30.Boulding KE: General systems
of input, throughput, output, and 14. Knox SH: A play scale. In Playas theory-The skeleton of science. In
feedback. Each level of the hierarchy Exploratory Learning, M Reilly, Editor. Modern Systems Research for the
Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, 1974 Behavioral Scientist, W Buckley, Edi-
involves a separate arena of the 15. Hurff J: A play skills inventory. In Play tor. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Co.,
child's play experience. First, sen- as Exploratory Learning, M Reilly, Edi- 1968
sorimotor play emerges, forming tor. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, 31. Hall AD, Fagen RE: Definition of a sys-
1974 tem. In Modern Systems Research for
the basis upon which constructive, 16. Ayres AJ: Patterns of perceptual-motor the Behavioral Scientist, W Buckley,
then social play is to flourish. Later, dysfunction in children: A factor ana- Editor. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Co.,
as the child matures, these construc- lytic study. Percept Mot Skills 20: 335- 1968
368, 1965 32. Laszlo E: The Systems View of the
tive and social aspects of play 17. Ayres AJ: Interrelations among per- World, New York: George Braziller,
increase. With normal development, ceptual-motor abilities in a group of 1972
each level will produce competent normal children. Am J Occup Ther 20: 33. Feibleman JK: Theory of integrative
288-292, 1966 levels. In The Psychology of Effective
behavior; that is, with effective play 18.Ayres AJ: Interrelationships among Behavior, J Coleman, Editor. Glenview
a t each level of the model, the child perceptual-motor functions inch iId reno IL: Scott, Foresman, & Co., 1969
becomes competent in interacting Am J Occup Ther 20: 68-71, 1966 34. White RW: The urge toward compe-
19. Ayres AJ: Deficits in sensory integra- tence. Am J Occup Ther 25: 271-274,
with the environment. In a second tion in educationally handicapped chil- 1971
paper, interrelation between sensory dren. J Learn Disabi/2: 160-168, 1969 35. Reilly M: Competency as a battered
integration and play, implications 20.Ayres AJ: Types of sensory integrative concept. In 11 th Annual Distinguished
dysfunction among disabled learners. Lecture Series in Special Education
for dysfunction, and suggestions for Am J Occup Ther 26: 13-18,1972 and Rehabilitation, BC Ring, DR
assessment and treatment are dis- 21 Ayres AJ: The Effect of Sensory Inte- Shroeder, Editors. Los Angeles: Univer-
cussed. grative Therapy on Learning Disabled sity of Southern California School of
ChiJdren- The Final Report of a Re- Education, 1973
search Proiect, Pasadena, CA: Center 36. Piaget J: Play, Dreams, and Imitation in
for the Study of Sensory Integrative Childhood, New York: Norton, 1962
REFERENCES Dysfunction, 1976 37. Lowe M: Trends in the development of
1. Reilly M: Playas Exploratory Learning, 22. Ayres AJ: Interpreting the Southern representational play in infants from
Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, 1974 California Sensory Integration Tests, one to three years-An observational
2. Ayres AJ: Sensory Integration and Los Angeles: Western Psychological study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 16:
Learning Disorders, Los Angeles: Services, 1978 33-47,1975
Western Psychological Services, 1972 23. Ayres AJ: Southern California Sensory 38. Rosenblatt D: Developmental trends in
3. Vandenberg B: Play and development Integration Tests Manual, Los Angeles: infant play. In Biology of Play, B Tizard,
from an ethological perspective. Am Western Psychological Services, 1974 D Harvey, Editors. Philadelphia: JB
Psycho/33: 724-738, 1978 24. Robinson AL: Play: The arena for Lippincott, 1977
4. Ayres AJ: Sensory Integration and the acquisition of rules for competent 39. Knox SH: A play scale In Playas
Child, Los Angeles: Western Psycho- behavior. Am J Occup Ther 31: 248- Exploratory Learning, M Reilly, Editor.
logical Services, 1979 253, 1977 Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, 1974
5. Reilly M: A psychiatric occupational 25. Takata N: Playas a prescription. In 40. Siobin DI: Psycholinguistics, Glenview,
therapy program as a teaching model. Playas Exploratory Learning, M Reilly, IL: Scott, Foresman, 1971
Am J Occup Ther 20: 61-67, 1966 Editor. Beverly Hills: Sage Publica- 41. Eckerman CO, Whatley JL, Kutz SL:
6. Reilly M: The educational process. Am tions, 1974 Growth of social play with peers during
J Occup Ther 23: 299-307, 1969 26. Clark FC, Figone J, Parham LD: The the second year of life. Dev Psychol 11:
7. Moorhead L: The occupational history. differential effects of occupational 42-49, 1975
Am J Occup Ther 23: 329-334, 1969 therapy intervention on Head Start pre- 42. Bruner J: Peekaboo and the learning of
8. Black MM: The occupational career. schoolers with and without depressed rule structures. In Play-Its Role in
Am J Occup Ther 30: 225-228, 1976 postrotary nystagmus. Paper presented Development and EVolution, J Bruner,
9. Shannon PD: Occupational choice: at annual symposium of the Center for A Jolly, K Sylva, Editors. New York:
Decision-making play. In Playas Ex- the Study of Sensory Integrative Dys- Basic Books, 1976
ploratory Learning, M Reilly, Editor. function, Los Angeles, September 43. Garvey C: Play, Cambridge, MA: Har-
Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, 1974 1979 vard University Press, 1977
10. Florey L: An approach to play and play 27. Kielhofner G: General systems theory: 44. Rosen CE: The effects of sociodra-
development. Am J Occup Ther 25: Implications for theory and action in matic play on problem-solving behav-
275-280, 1971 occupational therapy. Am J Occup ior among culturally disadvantaged
11 Takata N: The play milieu-A prelimi- Ther 32: 637-645, 1978 preschool children. Child Dev 45: 920-
nary appraisal. Am J Occup Ther 25: 28. Kielhofner G, Burke JP: Occupational 927, 1974
281-284,1971 therapy after 60 years: An account of 45.Sutton-Smith B: Children at play. PSy-
12. Michelman SS: Play and the deficit changing identity and knowledge. Am chol Today, December 1971, pp 54-59

374 June 1982, Volume 36, Number 6


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930386/ on 09/28/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms

You might also like