You are on page 1of 11

Original Research

SAGE Open
April-June 2023: 1–11
Ó The Author(s) 2023
Exploring the Relationship Between DOI: 10.1177/21582440231173823
journals.sagepub.com/home/sgo
Cognitive Abilities and Adaptive
Components of Psychopathic Traits

Guillaume Durand1 , Bart P. F. Rutten2, and Jill Lobbestael2

Abstract
Although multiple studies have suggested a negative relationship between psychopathic traits and cognitive abilities, a few
identified positive associations between psychopathic traits and specific executive functions. This study examined the relation-
ship between a set of cognitive functions and the adaptive traits associated with psychopathic personality. Participants
(N = 107) completed measures of psychopathic traits and adaptive personality traits, as well as a series of cognitive-ability-
based tasks (measures of working memory, response inhibition, cognitive discrimination, and long-term memory). Results
showed that adaptive psychopathic traits were associated with faster response times on measures of working memory, cogni-
tive discrimination, and long-term recall. In contrast, psychopathic traits related to impulsivity were associated with slower
response times in incongruent trials of measures of cognitive discrimination and short-term recall. Higher levels of adaptive
and psychopathic traits related to fearlessness and dominance were associated with better performance in a working mem-
ory task without sacrificing accuracy for speed. These findings further support the multidimensionality of psychopathy;
whereas its maladaptive traits are related to cognitive impairments, its adaptive traits seem to be related to cognitive
superiority.

Keywords
psychopathic traits, executive functions, community sample, adaptive traits

Introduction Cognitive Abilities and Psychopathy


Psychopathy Although multiple studies suggest that highly psycho-
pathic individuals have a deficit in executive functions
Psychopathy is commonly defined by ruthlessness, cal-
(EF) and cognitive abilities, findings and conclusions are
lousness, lack of guilt and remorse, impulsivity, emo-
very inconsistent (Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000). Cognitive
tional detachment, social deviance, and poor behavioral
abilities include functions such as planning abilities,
control (Berg et al., 2013; Lynam & Miller, 2012).
behavioral inhibition, working memory, and cognitive
Although there is a consensus regarding the importance
flexibility (Zeier et al., 2012). Previous findings indicate
of the aforementioned traits, the inclusion of other traits,
that people classified as highly psychopathic often have
which could be considered adaptive, is highly debated
poor impulse control and difficulty adapting their beha-
(Lilienfeld et al., 2012; Lynam & Miller, 2012; Maples
vior. For instance, when asked to complete a flanker task
et al., 2014). Early conceptualizations of psychopathy by
(i.e., a task where a centrally presented target stimulus is
Cleckley (1941) included numerous non-maladaptive
flanked on both sides by distracter stimuli, which can be
traits in the set of characteristics observed in psycho-
paths, such as superficial charm, absence of delusion or
irrational thinking, absence of nervousness, and low sui- 1
Saint Paul University, Ottawa, ON, Canada
cide rate (Cleckley, 1988, pp. 338–339). Multiple addi- 2
Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
tional studies have confirmed that several psychopathic
traits are commonly associated with adaptive traits such Corresponding Author:
Guillaume Durand, School of Counselling, Psychotherapy and Spirituality,
as social charm, low neuroticism, and high resilience to Faculty of Human Sciences, Saint Paul University, 223 Main Street, Ottawa,
stress and anxiety (see Durand, 2019b) for a complete ON K1S 1C4, Canada.
review). Email: gdurand@ustpaul.ca

Creative Commons CC BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License
(https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of
the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages
(https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
2 SAGE Open

either the same as or different from the target stimulus), relationship between the PPI and a battery of test
incarcerated offenders diagnosed with psychopathy were assessing general executive functions (i.e., working
significantly less accurate in incongruent trials than non- memory, attention, planning, cognitive flexibility, sus-
psychopathic participants. This supports the definition of tained attention, and behavioral disinhibition) in
psychopathy as characterized by interference from undergraduate students (Sellbom & Verona, 2007).
poorer cognitive control, as well as by increased impul- There was a positive association between cognitive
sivity when making decisions (Zeier et al., 2012). These functioning and PPI-I, as well as a negative association
results are consistent with the findings of Roussy and between the former and PPI-II. The negative associa-
Toupin (2000). They concluded that institutionalized tion between PPI-II and reduced executive functions
juvenile delinquents with psychopathy doing a go/no-go was also identified by Micha1owski et al. (2015). Their
task (i.e., responding when a specific stimulus is on screen findings suggested a negative association between PPI-
and not responding when any other stimulus is pre- II and executive control (e.g., slower response time).
sented) and a stopping task (i.e., inhibiting the expected Contradictory results were also obtained in studies
response to a stimulus when hearing a signal tone) made examining psychopathic traits categorically rather than
more errors than juvenile delinquents not diagnosed with on a continuum in the general population using the
psychopathy. Similar trends were observed in undergrad- PPI. For instance, one study supported the positive
uate students who completed a self-report measure of association between high levels of PPI and cognitive
psychopathic traits (Wilkowski & Robinson, 2008). abilities. Salnaitis et al. (2011) divided their sample of
Specifically, individuals displaying high levels of psycho- undergraduate students into three groups based on
pathic traits related to impulsivity (but not to emotional their level of psychopathic traits (low, medium, or
deficits) had difficulties adjusting their behavior after an high), and had them solve the tower of Hanoi (a cogni-
incorrect response, and therefore made more errors in tive measure of planning, working memory, and prob-
cognitive tasks. The authors suggest that the impulsivity lem solving). Highly psychopathic individuals were
trait may prevent individuals from pausing after an error predominantly fast and accurate in their responses,
for long enough to evaluate their strategy and readjust. while individuals with low levels of psychopathic traits
were slow and accurate, and suggested that executive
functioning deficiencies may not be present in subclini-
Cognitive Abilities and Adaptive Psychopathic Traits cal psychopathic individuals from the general popula-
None of the studies discussed in the previous section tion. Similar results were observed in a study
used an instrument that included adaptive features. comparing undergraduate students based on PPI levels
However, multiple studies on cognitive abilities and psy- (low and high) on cognitive measures of risk-taking
chopathy were performed using a common measure of and decision-making, and response inhibition (Zimak
self-reported psychopathic traits that notably includes an et al., 2014). While individuals with high levels of psy-
adaptive component: the Psychopathic Personality chopathic traits displayed more inaccuracy and beha-
Inventory (PPI; Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996). The PPI is vioral inhibition, their response time was significantly
divided into two factors: PPI-I (Fearless Dominance, faster, and they exhibited better decision-making skills.
which includes several of the potentially more adaptive The highly variable results from studies comparing
features of psychopathy) and PPI-II (Impulsive PPI levels and cognitive abilities in the general popula-
Antisociality, which focuses on several maladaptive fea- tion could be due to the traits assessed by PPI-I. It
tures of psychopathy). includes only three traits thought to be related to the
A study by Snowden et al. (2013) on college students psychopathic personality—social potency, fearlessness,
identified a negative relationship between self-reported and stress immunity—and does not consider maladap-
levels of PPI and executive functions. Indeed, higher tive traits (coldheartedness, Machiavellian egocentri-
levels of psychopathic traits were associated with more city, impulsive nonconformity, blame externalization,
errors on the Porteus Maze test used to assess impulsiv- and carefree nonplanfulness). There is a newer self-
ity. This relationship was observed for both PPI-I and reported instrument, which covers a wider range of
PPI-II. Contradictory results came from another study adaptive features believed to be associated with high
using the PPI on a sample of undergraduates (Carlson levels of psychopathic traits (Durand, 2019b). The
et al., 2009), where self-reported psychopathic traits Durand Adaptive Psychopathic Traits Questionnaire
were linked to executive functioning measured with the (DAPTQ) is a 38-item questionnaire assessing eight
rotated heads task. A negative relationship was found adaptive traits known to correlate with psychopathic
between reaction time on the cognitive task and PPI-I, personality traits. The first step in its development was
but there was no association between reaction time and to identify the constructs considered adaptive that are
PPI-II. Similar results were obtained for the strongly associated with the diagnosis of psychopathy
Durand et al. 3

or psychopathic traits as measured through self-report. Instruments


Adaptive traits are defined as those that facilitate and Durand Adaptive Psychopathic Traits Questionnaire
improve the individual’s chances of survival in their (DAPTQ). The DAPTQ is a 38-item self-report instru-
environment (Durand, 2019b). Factor analysis sug- ment assessing adaptive traits known to correlate with
gested an 8-factor solution (Leadership, Logical psychopathic personality traits (Durand, 2019b). The
Thinking, Composure, Creativity, Fearlessness, Focus, DAPTQ uses a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from 1
Extroversion, and Management; Durand, 2019a). The (Strongly Disagree) to 6 (Strongly Agree). A validation
DAPTQ is not a measure of psychopathy, nor is it a study of the DAPTQ confirmed its incremental validity
measure of psychopathic traits. over the PPI-short form in measures related to successful
psychopathy, such as conscientiousness, stress and anxi-
ety immunity, and communication adaptability
Present Study (Durand, 2019a). Subsequent studies supported the relia-
The purpose of this study is threefold. First, due to the bility and validity of the DAPTQ as a measure of adap-
high variability of cognitive tasks across the studies in tive traits (Bronchain et al., 2020, 2021). In the present
the field of psychopathy, a pilot study examining the study, the internal consistency reliability of the DAPTQ
validity of different versions of cognitive tasks is neces- total was a = .92, and that of its subscales ranged from
sary. Second, while the DAPTQ was intended to expand a = .76 to .92.
on the PPI-I, it has not yet been compared to any beha-
vioral executive function measure. Therefore, a correla- Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Short Form (PPI-SF). The
tional analysis will be performed to identify the PPI-SF is a 56-item self-report instrument assessing psy-
relationship between the DAPTQ, PPI-I, and EF. Lastly, chopathic traits on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = false,
we will examine how efficiently the DAPTQ and PPI-I 4 = true; Lilienfeld & Widows, 2005). Factor analytic
predict EF. We hypothesize that the DAPTQ and PPI-I research showed that the subscales load on two different
will predict the variance of response time in cognitive factors. The Fearless Dominance (PPI-I) factor includes
tasks, whereas higher levels on the DAPTQ and PPI-I Stress Immunity, Social Potency, and Fearlessness sub-
will be associated with lower response time. scales, and the Impulsive Antisociality (PPI-II) factor
includes Blame Externalization, Machiavellian
Egocentricity, Carefree Nonplanfulness, and Impulsive
Methods Nonconformity subscales. The Coldheartedness subscale
does not load on either of the two factors (Benning
Participants et al., 2003). The DAPTQ total score was shown to cor-
A total of N = 107 participants (44 males, 63 females) relate positively with PPI-I (r = .66) and PPI-SF total
were recruited on social media and websites dedicated to (r = .46), but is not significantly associated with PPI-II
psychology research (e.g., callforparticipants.com, onli- (r = 2.04; Durand, 2019b). In the present study, the
nepsychresearch.co.uk, and facebook.com). The age of total PPI score had a Cronbach’s alpha of a = .76; PPI-
the participants ranged between 18 and 64 years old I, of a = .83; and PPI-II, of a = .76.
(M = 28.78, SD = 10.52). Over a third of the partici-
pants were university students (38%). Participants were The n-Back Task. The n-back task is a measure of atten-
located mostly in North America (51%) and Europe tion and working memory based on the design of
(39%). In terms of the latest diploma obtained, the larg- Schoofs et al. (2008). Participants were asked to monitor
est group of participants reported holding a high school a series of one-digit numbers from 0 to 9, presented in a
diploma (41%), followed by a bachelor’s degree (31%), random sequence. Participants had to choose between
a master’s degree (13%), and other (15%). The current two arrows on the keyboard (e.g., left arrow if seen n
study was approved and given the ‘‘exempt’’ status by steps before; right arrow if not seen n steps before).
the IntegReview Ethical Review Board (Austin, TX, Participants completed a practice trial of 15 stimuli, fol-
USA), under protocol number 11022016. No names or lowed by two blocks of 50 trials presented in random
other protected health information, as defined by the order, where the working memory load was varied by
Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act switching the difficulty between a 2-back and a 3-back
(HIPAA), were recorded. All participants filled out an condition. A fixation cross was presented for 500 ms, fol-
informed consent form with detailed information about lowed by a stimulus with auto-advance for 3,000 ms and
the nature, goal, and procedure of the study before the an inter-stimulus interval of 1200 ms. Target stimuli
experiment started. Participants remained anonymous (same as in the n-trials before) were presented randomly
throughout the study and were not compensated for with a probability of 33%. The first three trials of both
their time. conditions were removed for all participants.
4 SAGE Open

The Go/No-Go Task. The go/no-go task is designed to arrow key corresponding to the middle arrow (left arrow
assess cognitive attention and response inhibition. On- key for \ \ \ \ \; right arrow key for \ \ . \
screen instructions told the participant to press the space \). The arrows version did not include control trials.
bar of the keyboard every time the letter X appeared, The dependent measures were response time (RT) and
and to refrain from pressing any key when the screen dis- the number of errors for congruent trials, incongruent
played any other letter (A, D, Y, W). The task was com- trials, and control trials (for version 1). Although Zeier
posed of 48 consecutive trials, divided equally between et al.’s (2012) task version appears to be an improve-
presentations of the target cue and distracters. During ment over the regular flanker task, the version without
each trial, a blank screen was presented for 500 ms, fol- control trials has been used in more studies (Racer
lowed by a fixation cross for 1000 ms and a target letter et al., 2011; Sellbom & Verona, 2007).
for 2500 ms. The go/no-go paradigm has been used in
several other laboratory studies in the field of psychopa- The Penn Word Memory Test (PWMT). The PWMT is a
thy (Zimak et al., 2014) and is a valuable tool for asses- measure of short- and long-term episodic memory.
sing response discrimination. Scoring was performed by Participants were presented with 20 target words (1500
calculating the mean reaction time before responding to ms for target cue; 1500 ms for target word) and were
the go stimulus (the X) and by calculating the average asked to try to memorize them (Gur et al., 1993).
number of errors (i.e., pressing the space bar on Y or Immediately after, participants were instructed to identify
inhibit pressing). all the previously seen words from a list comprising 40
words (20 words from the first list and 20 distracters).
The Flanker Task. The flanker task is a common mea- Participants were asked to answer using a two-alternative
sure of cognitive control and perceptual discrimination forced choice response (left arrow if seen before; right
(Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). A row of five characters was arrow if not seen before). The target cue during the trial
presented to the participant. The target stimulus was in remained at 1,500 ms, and the trial auto-advanced to the
the middle of the row, flanked by two distracters on next trial after 3,000 ms. About 15 minutes later, at the
either side. The distracters were either very similar to very end of the study, participants were shown a second
(congruent) or very different (incongruent) from the tar- list (20 words from the first list, 20 new distracters), and
get stimulus. When distracter stimuli are incongruent, were asked to answer, once again using the two-
participants generally answer more slowly and less accu- alternative forced choice response. Distracters and target
rately than when answering congruent trials. words were equated for frequency, length, concreteness,
There are many versions of the flanker task. We used and imageability using Paivio’s norms (i.e., moral, fig-
one version with control trials and another without, with ment, event, prestige, hint, and origin; Paivio et al.,
participants completing either one or the other. The first 1968). The response time and number of errors were
version was based on the numbers/letters version used by selected as performance measures. For analyses, PWMT1
Zeier et al. (2012). Our participants completed 100 trials refers to the first series of trials, measuring short-term
divided into two blocks. For each trial, a fixation cross recognition, and PWMT2 refers to the second and last
appeared on screen for 500 ms, followed by the target and series of trials, measuring long-term recognition. Across
the flankers, which were set to auto-advance after 2500 all cognitive tasks, participants with an error rate of over
ms. The target appeared at fixation, and the distracters 75% were automatically excluded from the data set.
appeared to the left and right, equidistant from the target
(at approximately one degree of visual angle). As in Zeier
et al.’s (2012) study, the target stimulus was always a 5, 8, Experimental Procedure
G, or M, and the distracters were either another one of All participants first completed the PWMT1. Then, in
these characters or a pound sign (#). Participants were random order, they did the go/no-go, the n-back task,
asked whether the target was a letter or a number and and one of the two flanker tasks. The last task for all
responded using a two-alternative forced choice answer. participants was the PWMT2, which concerns long-term
This was followed by a 1000–1500 ms variable intertrial memory. Participants completed the cognitive tasks
interval. In congruent trials, the target and distracters using the Gorilla platform (www.gorilla.sc). Although
were from the same category (e.g., 8 5 8); in incongruent laboratory studies in the presence of an experimenter are
trials, they were different (e.g., G 5 G). In control trials, often thought to be more reliable, recent evidence sug-
the distracters were pound signs (e.g., # 5 #). gests that online behavioral experiments do not differ
The second flanker task version we used was the from laboratory studies and provide equally valid results
arrows version. The settings were the same, but the let- (Hilbig, 2016; Semmelmann & Weigelt, 2017). In order
ters and numbers were replaced with arrows (e.g., \ \ to exclude participants who stopped completing the tasks
\ \ \). Participants were asked to chose the keyboard without closing the study page, we added various
Durand et al. 5

Table 1. Descriptive Data From the Durand Adaptive Table 2. Descriptive Data From the n-Back, Go/No-Go, and
Psychopathic Traits Questionnaire and the Psychopathic Flanker Tasks and the Pennsylvania Word Memory Test.
Personality Inventory Short Form (N = 107).
N M SD
M SD
N-back
DAPTQ 2-back RT 96 809.11 151.80
Leadership 13.14 4.56 2-back errors 96 3.89 3.71
Logical Thinking 21.14 4.47 3-back RT 93 881.11 156.39
Composure 18.47 7.71 3-back errors 93 7.06 5.26
Creativity 16.36 4.79 Go/no-go
Fearlessness 16.88 6.33 RT 107 436.26 73.15
Focus 12.84 4.80 Errors 107 0.61 0.85
Extroversion 18.12 8.03 Flanker letters/numbers version
Management 11.78 3.49 Control RT 47 589.86 79.31
Total 128.76 27.61 Congruent RT 47 593.08 67.75
PPI-SF Incongruent RT 47 603.15 66.10
Machiavellian Egocentricity 14.12 3.57 Errors 47 3.02 2.01
Social Potency 17.14 4.16 Flanker arrows version
Fearlessness 13.47 5.03 Congruent RT 54 478.65 73.95
Coldheartedness 14.10 3.92 Incongruent RT 54 530.32 80.07
Impulsive nonconformity 15.94 3.93 Errors congruent 54 0.18 0.47
Blame externalization 13.28 4.98 Errors incongruent 54 2.18 2.52
Carefree nonplanfulness 14.74 2.90 PWMT
Stress immunity 17.64 3.72 PWMT1 RT 103 834.47 121.63
PPI-I 48.27 9.97 PWMT1 errors 103 6.51 3.01
PPI-II 58.09 9.14 PWMT2 RT 104 815.32 110.13
Total 120.46 14.26 PWMT2 errors 104 6.28 3.46

Note. Each participant was asked to complete only one of the two flanker
task versions. RT = reaction time.
checkpoints in the experimental setup of the Gorilla plat-
form. Participants missing more than 75% of the data
were automatically removed. There was no missing data the PPI-SF had similar means and standard deviations
in either of the two questionnaires. For the analyses, we as in other studies using those instruments (Bronchain
excluded response time trials answered in less than et al., 2020; Durand, 2018a, 2018b; Lee & Salekin,
150 ms or more than 1,500 ms (2,000 ms for PWMT1 2010). The DAPTQ total score again correlated posi-
and PWMT2). Errors rates for all cognitive tasks were tively with PPI-I (r = .81) and PPI-SF total (r = .59),
recoded as z-scores. but was not significantly associated with PPI-II
(r = 2.20). The n-back task yielded results similar to
Statistical Analyses those obtained by Schoofs et al. (2008). In their study,
the mean response time for 2-back was 715 ms for con-
We first computed the means and standard deviations trol and 879 ms for the stress group (compared to
for the questionnaires and cognitive tasks to ensure simi- 809 ms in the present study), and their mean response
larity with other studies using the same instruments. T- time for 3-back was 811 ms for control and 943 ms for
test analyses were used to compare cognitive tasks’ ver- the stress group (compared to 881 ms in the present
sion efficacy for the n-back task, the flanker task, and study). Our go/no-go response time was similar to the
the PWMT. A Pearson correlation was then used to results obtained by Harper et al. (2014), who reported
determine the strength of the association between the mean reaction times between 445 and 451 ms (com-
questionnaires and the cognitive tasks. Lastly, we per- pared to 436 ms in the present study). Our results for
formed a series of linear stepwise regression analyses on both versions of the flanker task were also similar to
all response time variables using the DAPTQ and PPI-I previous results. Zeier et al. (2012) reported mean
as predictors. response times between 579 and 610 ms for the letters/
numbers version (compared to 589–603 ms in the pres-
Results ent study), while Sellbom and Verona (2007) reported a
mean of 499 ms for the arrows version (compared to
Descriptive Data 478–530 ms in the current study).
Tables 1 and 2 show the descriptive data of the ques- On the other hand, our mean response times for the
tionnaires and all the cognitive tasks. The DAPTQ and PWMT were significantly faster than in other studies—
6 SAGE Open

around 800 ms, compared to previous results of around Table 3. Correlations Between the Durand Adaptive
1,400 ms (Gur et al., 2012; Moore et al., 2015). This dif- Psychopathic Traits Questionnaire, the Psychopathic Personality
ference may be due to the sample’s characteristics, Inventory Short Form, and Executive Functions.
whereas the aforementioned authors investigated the EF
Criterion DAPTQ PPI-SF PPI-I PPI-II
of a young sample with a mean age of 15 years.
Regarding the relationship between demographics Questionnaires
and levels of the DAPTQ and PPI, an ANOVA identi- DAPTQ — — — —
fied a sex-based difference for the PPI, where males PPI-SF .53*** — — —
PPI-I .77*** .73*** — —
scored higher than females (F(1, 105) = 6.122, p = .015, PPI-II 2.19* .58*** 2.07 —
d = 0.47). This was not the case on the DAPTQ (F(1, N-back
105) = 3.431, p = .067, d = 0.35). No other analyses of 2-back RT 2.35*** 2.18 2.26** .06
mean differences based on demographics could be car- 2-back errors 2.05 2.03 2.08 .08
ried out due to the small sample size. 3-back RT 2.32** 2.15 2.18 .00
3-back errors .17 .12 .08 .09
Go/no-go
RT 2.03 2.03 2.10 .01
Comparability of Alternative Cognitive Tasks Errors 2.07 2.04 2.05 2.01
Flanker letters/numbers version
To assess the reliability of cognitive tasks with multiple Control RT 2.41** 2.14 2.36* .24
paradigms, we first examined the mean differences Congruent RT 2.41** 2.17 2.30* .16
between the two versions of the n-back task, the flan- Incongruent RT 2.44** 2.10 2.35* .30*
ker tasks, and the PWMT. As expected, participants Errors .01 .05 .09 .01
Flanker arrows version
were significantly faster when answering the 2-back Congruent RT 2.33* 2.06 2.29* .23
task compared to the 3-back task (t(89) = 6.382, p Incongruent RT 2.27 .03 2.23 .28*
\ .001, d = 0.54). Within answered trials, participants Errors congruent .03 2.01 .18 2.14
also made fewer mistakes on the 2-back task than on Errors incongruent .14 2.01 2.01 2.07
the 3-back task (t(89) = 6.65, p \ .001, d = 0.72), most PWMT
PWMT-1 RT 2.10 .10 2.03 .24*
likely because the 2-back version was simpler than the PWMT-1 errors .16 .00 .04 2.06
3-back. PWMT-2 RT 2.20* 2.06 2.13 .09
As with the n-back task, a few differences were PWMT-2 errors .20* .12 .13 .04
observed between the two versions of the flanker task. In
the letters/numbers version, control trials yielded the Note. *p 2 .05. **p 2 .01. ***p 2 .001.
same reaction time as the congruent trials (t(46) = 0.67,
p = .504, d = 0.05). As expected, a small significant dif-
ference was nonetheless observed between response time Associations Between the DAPTQ, the PPI-SF, and
(RTs) of congruent and incongruent trials, where con- Executive Functions
gruent trials were answered faster (t(46) = 2.14, As shown in Table 3, Pearson correlations between the
p = .038, d = 0.15). Interestingly, differences between DAPTQ, the PPI-SF, and executive functions revealed
congruent and incongruent trials for the arrows flanker multiple significant results. As in Durand (2019a,
task were larger. In this version, congruent trials were 2019b), the DAPTQ was positively associated with PPI-
significantly faster than incongruent trials (t(53) = 13.39, SF and PPI-I, and significantly negatively associated
p \ .001, d = 0.67). Similarly, congruent trials elicited with PPI-II. Higher scores on the DAPTQ were associ-
fewer mistakes (t(53) = 5.70, p \ .001, d = 1.10). When ated with faster response times on the 2-back and 3-back
controlled for multiple testing (p = .05/3 = .016), three task; on the control, congruent, and incongruent trials of
ANOVAs detected no difference between the two flanker the letters/numbers flanker task; on the congruent trials
groups for the DAPTQ (F(1, 105) = 0.180, p = .672, of the arrows flanker task; and on the long-term recall
d = 0.08), the PPI-SF (F(1, 105) = 3.995, p = .048, PWMT2. Unexpectedly, the DAPTQ was also associated
d = 0.40), and age (F(1, 105) = 0.541, p = .464, with higher error rates on the long-term recall PWMT2,
d = 0.14). The flanker groups also did not differ by sex although the results were barely within the significance
(x2 (1, N = 107) = 0.718, p = .397). threshold (p = .040). The PPI-SF total score was not
A significant mean difference was observed between associated with any variables related to executive func-
response times for PWMT1 and PWMT2 (t(99) = 2.08, tions. Higher scores on the PPI-I were associated with
p = .040, d = 0.16). There were no significant differences faster RTs on the 2-back task; on the control, congruent,
in the number of errors between PWMT1 and PWMT2 and incongruent trials of the letters/numbers flanker
(t(98) = 1.22, p = .222, d = 0.11). task; and on congruent trials of the arrows flanker task.
Durand et al. 7

Table 4. Regression Analyses of DAPTQ and PPI-I on Response Time.

95% CI
Dependent variable Effect Estimate SE LL UL t p

RT 2-back
DAPTQ 21.897 0.532 22.953 20.841 23.568 .001
PPI-I 0.008 0.053 .958
RT 3-back
DAPTQ 21.840 0.577 22.986 20.693 23.188 .002
PPI-I 0.147 0.965 .337
RT flanker letters/numbers control
DAPTQ 21.206 0.404 22.019 20.392 22.985 .005
PPI-I 20.087 20.381 .705
RT flanker letters/numbers congruent
DAPTQ 21.030 0.345 21.725 20.335 22.985 .005
PPI-I 0.058 0.252 .802
RT flanker letters/numbers incongruent
DAPTQ 21.095 0.330 21.761 20.430 23.317 .002
PPI-I 0.027 0.118 .907
RT flanker arrows congruent
DAPTQ 21.075 0.429 21.936 20.215 22.507 .015
PPI-I 20.125 20.719 .475
RT PWMT-2
DAPTQ 20.813 0.386 21.579 20.046 22.104 .038
PPI-I 0.072 0.478 .634

Note. Standard error and confidence intervals are unavailable for non-significant predictors. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit.

Lastly, PPI-II was associated with slower response times Discussion


in incongruent trials for both the letters/numbers and
The purpose of the present study was (i) to compare the
arrows flanker task, as well as slower response times on
validity of different measures of cognitive abilities in the
short-term recall PWMT1.
field of psychopathic traits; (ii) to examine the relation-
ship between EF and adaptive personality traits related
to the psychopathic personality; and (iii) to assess the
Regression Analyses to Predict Executive Functions
predictive value of the DAPTQ and PPI-I for EF.
As shown in Table 4, a series of stepwise regression anal- Comparing versions of cognitive tasks yielded interest-
yses was performed on all response time variables using ing findings. The slower response time and higher num-
the DAPTQ total and PPI-I as predictors. The results are ber of errors in the 3-back task compared to the 2-back
as follows. A significant regression was observed for 2- task is similar to the results obtained by Schoofs et al.
back response time (adjusted R2 = .11, F(1,94) = 12.731, (2008). These results suggest that the 3-back task may
p \ .001); 3-back response time (adjusted R2 = .09, yield more variable results for group differences in work-
F(1,91) = 10.160, p = .002); flanker letters/numbers con- ing memory performance. This is likely because the 3-
trol response time (adjusted R2 = .15, F(1,45) = 8.913, back task is more challenging than the 2-back task, which
p = .005); flanker letters/numbers congruent response may be too easy for high performers.
time (adjusted R2 = .15, F(1,45) = 8.909, p = .005); flan- Similarly, a few differences emerged between the two
ker letters/numbers incongruent response time (adjusted versions of the flanker task. Participants were signifi-
R2 = .18, F(1,45) = 11.002, p = .002); flanker arrows cantly slower in the letters/numbers version and were
congruent (adjusted R2 = .09, F(1,52) = 6.284, p = .015); making more mistakes, especially in incongruent trials.
and PWMT2 response time (adjusted R2 = .03, These results mirror those obtained by Zeier et al. (2012),
F(1,102) = 4.426, p = .038). For all analyses, the who reported an effect size of d = 0.34 when comparing
DAPTQ was the sole predictor, and higher levels on the response time between congruent and incongruent trials.
DAPTQ predicted faster response times. No significant Another study using the arrows version obtained an
regression was found for the go/no-go response time, effect size between congruent and incongruent trials three
flanker arrows incongruent response time, or PWMT1 times larger (d = 0.99, Racer et al., 2011). These results
response time. suggest that using an arrow paradigm for the flanker
8 SAGE Open

task provides better results in discriminating between Higher levels of adaptive traits and PPI-I were associated
congruent trials and interfering stimuli. with faster RTs on all trials in the letters/numbers ver-
Lastly, differences were also identified in the PWMT. sion, and only with congruent trials in the arrows ver-
A previous study investigating the psychometric proper- sion. In contrast, PPI-II was associated with slower
ties of a cognitive battery that included the PWMT incongruent trials on both flanker task versions.
reported no difference in response time (d = 0.01) or Impairment in response inhibition is one of the most
accuracy (d = 0.14) between immediate and delayed recurrent reported problems among psychopathic indi-
word memory (Gur et al., 2010). Our participants, in viduals (Roussy & Toupin, 2000; Wilkowski &
contrast, were faster at the second time point, which may Robinson, 2008; Zeier et al., 2012). However, inhibitory
be due to a habituation effect. Repeating other tasks control has often been negatively associated with psycho-
besides the PWMT at different times could help deter- pathic traits when viewing them as a whole. Studies dif-
mine whether participants were only faster the second ferentiating the various aspects of psychopathic traits,
time because they already did the PWMT once, and for instance the behavioral aspects (e.g., social deviance
whether this is observed when repeating any other task, and violent behavior) from the psychological aspects
such as the n-back and flanker tasks. (e.g., callousness and low anxiety) find that inhibitory
Higher levels of adaptive and psychopathic traits were control may be impaired in individuals showing high lev-
associated with faster response time for working memory els of the former, but not in individuals displaying the
with high load (the 2-back task), but only adaptive traits latter. In fact, individuals showing callousness and low
were associated with the more complex working memory anxiety may even outperform typically developing indi-
task (the 3-back task). Interestingly, no significant results viduals in tasks requiring inhibitory control (Ross et al.,
were observed regarding accuracy on the n-back task. 2007).
These findings suggest that individuals with higher levels The results related to the PWMT1 and PWMT2
of adaptive traits did not lose accuracy while they gained were in line with our expectations for the PPI-II, but
speed. Similar results were obtained in previous studies. not for the DAPTQ. As with both flanker tasks, PPI-II
For instance, a study on incarcerated inmates suggested was associated with slower RTs on the PWMT1, but
that individuals with a higher deficit in interpersonal fea- not on the PWMT2. It is possible that higher levels of
tures (e.g., arrogance and a deceitful interpersonal style, PPI-II are related to higher levels of doubt in one’s
as identified on a psychopathy diagnosis measure) made answers and lower levels of motivation when complet-
fewer errors in tasks related to working memory (Hansen ing the first task of the study. Many studies have asso-
et al., 2007). These results are also in line with the work ciated high levels of psychopathic traits, particularly
of Carlsonet al. (2009). Overall, these results suggest that those related to the PPI-II, with lower self-esteem
higher levels of adaptive and PPI-I traits are associated (Durand, 2016; Falkenbach et al., 2013). Alternatively,
with better performance in working memory tasks, with- low self-esteem has been associated with high neuroti-
out sacrificing accuracy for speed. cism, which includes a host of factors, such as self-
Unexpectedly, no effects were found for the go/no-go doubt and worry (Scheier et al., 1994). In addition,
task. An early study in the field of cognitive abilities and adaptive traits were associated with faster RTs but a
psychopathic traits in undergraduate students has higher error rate on the PWMT2, though the DAPTQ
reported that individuals with higher levels of psycho- has not been associated with error rates in any other
pathic traits presented more difficulties inhibiting their task. It is possible that higher levels of adaptive traits
responses when facing a competing reward in a go/no-go are associated with a reduced attention span toward
paradigm (Lynam et al., 1999). These results mirror the end of the experiment, causing more errors on the
those previously reported, supporting an increase in last task. However, the lack of significant association
error on a go/no-go task in diagnosed juvenile psycho- between adaptive traits and error rates in all other
paths (Roussy & Toupin, 2000). Although only the sec- tasks indicates a potential type I error due to limited
ond study was categorical, it is possible that inhibition power, especially given the weak p-value (p = .040).
difficulties in a go/no-go paradigm are present mainly in In addition to the correlation analysis, we performed a
highly psychopathic individuals. An experiment focusing regression analysis to assess the predictive power of the
on groups of psychopathic individuals (low scorers and DAPTQ and PPI-I on the response time of cognitive abil-
high scorers on the PPI-SF) would provide more clarity ities. For all significant regression analyses, the DAPTQ
regarding the relationship between the go/no-go task was the sole predictor of the regression, with explained
and psychopathy. variance varying between 3% and 18%. It is important
The results obtained for both versions of the flanker to note that these results should be considered highly pre-
task further support previous findings regarding the liminary due to the small sample size, particularly on the
opposite associations observed with PPI-I and PPI-II. flanker task. Indeed, the sample size does not allow a
Durand et al. 9

thorough examination of the incremental validity of the Declaration of Conflicting Interests


DAPTQ over the PPI-I. Nevertheless, these preliminary The author(s) declared the following potential conflicts of inter-
findings are sufficiently encouraging to justify a larger est with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication
study focusing on the same variables to examine the of this article: Dr. Guillaume Durand receives royalties from
incremental validity of the DAPTQ over the PPI-I to pre- the sale of the Durand Adaptive Psychopathic Traits
dict EF. Questionnaire. Dr. Bart P.F. Rutten, and Dr. Jill Lobbestael
declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Limitations Funding
There are several limitations to this study. First, our sam- The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
ple size was modest, which further tampers the results authorship, and/or publication of this article.
obtained on the flanker tasks. Only half the participants
performed each of the two versions, and our results need Ethical Approval
to be verified in a larger study focusing on the arrows ver-
The current study was approved and given ‘exempt’ status by
sion of the flanker task. Second, the assessment of psy-
the IntegReview Ethical Review Board (Austin, TX, USA),
chopathic traits was performed using a brief self-reported under protocol number 11022016.
questionnaire—the PPI-SF. A future study could benefit
from the paradigm developed by Bronchain et al. (2020),
in which a large sample of individuals are divided into ORCID iD
four groups based on an extensive measure of psycho- Guillaume Durand https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5418-4429
pathic and adaptive traits. Third, our sample was not
only small but also very heterogeneous, which may fur- References
ther temper the results. The standard deviation for the
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Hicks, B. M., Blonigen, D. M., &
age of the sample was particularly large, which may influ- Krueger, R. F. (2003). Factor structure of the psychopathic
ence cognitive performance. A future study with a more personality inventory: Validity and implications for clinical
homogeneous group of participants (e.g., students from assessment. Psychological Assessment, 15(3), 340–350.
one university) may yield different results. Lastly, while https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.15.3.340
experiments using cognitive tasks often include hundreds Berg, J. M., Smith, S. F., Watts, A. L., Ammirati, R., Green, S.
of trials, each of our tasks used between 48 and 100 trials. E., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2013). Misconceptions regarding psy-
While the study gives a general idea of the relationship chopathic personality: Implications for clinical practice and
between the DAPTQ and EF, future studies will need to research. Neuropsychiatry, 3, 63–74. https://doi.org/10.2217/
increase the number of trials (and most likely decrease the npy.12.69
Bronchain, J., Chabrol, H., & Raynal, P. (2021). Adaptive psy-
number of EF tasks to remain time-efficient).
chopathic traits: Positive outcomes in a college student sam-
ple. Current Psychology, 40(10), 4997–5004. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s12144-019-00434-3
Conclusion Bronchain, J., Raynal, P., & Chabrol, H. (2020). Heterogeneity
This study extends the research on psychopathic traits of adaptive features among psychopathy variants. Personal-
ity Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 11(1),
and EF and tentatively supports the association between
63–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000366
the DAPTQ and EF. Specifically, adaptive traits and Carlson, S. R., Thái, S., & McLarnon, M. E. (2009). Visual P3
PPI-I are associated with better performance on cogni- amplitude and self-reported psychopathic personality traits:
tive tasks, as observed through a faster response times Frontal reduction is associated with self-centered impulsiv-
with similar accuracy. While encouraging, these results ity. Psychophysiology, 46(1), 100–113. https://doi.org/10.
remain highly preliminary due to the small number of 1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00756.x
participants, who were recruited on social media. Our Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity; an attempt to reinter-
findings confirm that psychopathic traits need to be stud- pret the so-called psychopathic personality. JAMA, 117(6),
ied as a heterogeneous construct by exploring its various 493. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.1941.02820320085028
individual facets, both adaptive and maladaptive, such as Cleckley, H. (1988). The mask of Sanity: An attempt to clarify
some issues about the so called psychopathic personality. (W.
the ones assessed by the DAPTQ.
A. Dolan, Ed., 5th ed.). C V Mosby Co. https://doi.org/10.
1097/00007611-195105000-00028
Durand, G. (2016). A replication of ‘‘using self-esteem to disag-
Acknowledgments gregate psychopathy, narcissism, and aggression (2013)’’.
This work is published thanks to an aid to publishing grant The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 12(2), r1–r5.
from Saint Paul University. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.12.2.r001
10 SAGE Open

Durand, G. (2018a). A French translation and validation of Lee, Z., & Salekin, R. T. (2010). Psychopathy in a noninstitu-
the Durand Adaptive Psychopathic Traits Questionnaire: tional sample: Differences in primary and secondary sub-
An investigation with community samples from France and types. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and
Canada. PLOS ONE, 13(9), e0204214. https://doi.org/10. Treatment, 1(3), 153–169. https://doi.org/10.1037/
1371/journal.pone.0204214 a0019269
Durand, G. (2018b). Demystification of the relationship Lilienfeld, S. O., & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and
between psychopathy and happiness. Journal of Happiness preliminary validation of a self-report measure of psycho-
Studies, 19(2), 381–395. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-016- pathic personality traits in noncriminal populations. Journal
9823-0 of Personality Assessment, 66(3), 488–524. https://doi.org/
Durand, G. (2019a). Incremental validity of the Durand adap- 10.1207/s15327752jpa6603_3
tive psychopathic traits questionnaire above self-report psy- Lilienfeld, S. O., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., Berg, J., Sell-
chopathy measures in community samples. Journal of bom, M., & Edens, J. F. (2012). The role of fearless domi-
Personality Assessment, 101(5), 493–502. https://doi.org/10. nance in psychopathy: Confusions, controversies, and
1080/00223891.2018.1464456 clarifications. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and
Durand, G. (2019b). The Durand adaptive psychopathic traits Treatment, 3(3), 327–340. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026987
questionnaire: Development and validation. Journal of Per- Lilienfeld, S. O., & Widows, M. (2005). Psychopathic personal-
sonality Assessment, 101(2), 140–149. https://doi.org/10. ity inventory–revised: Professional manual. Psychological
1080/00223891.2017.1372443 Assessment Resources.
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters Lynam, D. R., & Miller, J. D. (2012). Fearless dominance and
upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. psychopathy: A response to Lilienfeld et al. Personality Dis-
Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. https://doi.org/10. orders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 3(3), 341–353.
3758/BF03203267 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028296
Falkenbach, D. M., Howe, J. R., & Falki, M. (2013). Using Lynam, D. R., Whiteside, S., & Jones, S. (1999). Self-reported
self-esteem to disaggregate psychopathy, narcissism, and psychopathy: A validation study. Journal of Personality
aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(7), Assessment, 73(1), 110–132. https://doi.org/10.1207/
815–820. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.12.017 S15327752JPA730108
Gur, R. C., Jaggi, J. L., Ragland, J. D., Resnick, S. M., Shtasel, Maples, J. L., Miller, J. D., Fortune, E., MacKillop, J., Camp-
D., Muenz, L., & Gur, R. E. (1993). Effects of memory pro- bell, W. K., Lynam, D. R., Lance, C. E., & Goodie, A. S.
cessing on regional brain activation: Cerebral blood flow in (2014). An examination of the correlates of fearless domi-
normal subjects. The International Journal of Neuroscience, nance and self-centered impulsivity among high-frequency
72(1–2), 31–44. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/ gamblers. Journal of Personality Disorders, 28(3), 379–393.
8225798 https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2013_27_125
Gur, R. C., Richard, J., Calkins, M. E., Chiavacci, R., Hansen, Micha1owski, J. M., Droździel, D., & Harciarek, M. (2015).
J. A., Bilker, W. B., Loughead, J., Connolly, J. J., Qiu, H., Impulsive antisociality and executive control problems: Evi-
Mentch, F. D., Abou-Sleiman, P. M., Hakonarson, H., & dence from go/no-go and stop-signal tasks. Current Issues in
Gur, R. E. (2012). Age group and sex differences in perfor- Personality Psychology, 1(1), 36–41. https://doi.org/10.5114/
mance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children cipp.2015.49939
age 8221. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 251–265. https://doi.org/ Moore, T. M., Reise, S. P., Gur, R. E., Hakonarson, H., &
10.1037/a0026712 Gur, R. C. (2015). Psychometric properties of the Penn
Gur, R. C., Richard, J., Hughett, P., Calkins, M. E., Macy, L., Computerized Neurocognitive Battery. Neuropsychology,
Bilker, W. B., Brensinger, C, & Gur, R. E. (2010). A cogni- 29(2), 235–246. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000093
tive neuroscience based computerized battery for efficient Morgan, A. B., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic
measurement of individual differences: Standardization and review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neu-
initial construct validation. Journal of Neuroscience Meth- ropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical
ods, 187(2), 254–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth. Psychology Review, 20(1), 113–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/
2009.11.017 S0272-7358(98)00096-8
Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., Thornton, D., Waage, L., & Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness,
Thayer, J. F. (2007). Facets of psychopathy, heart rate varia- imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal
bility and cognitive function. Journal of Personality Disor- of Experimental Psychology, 76(1, Pt.2), 1–25. https://doi.
ders, 21(5), 568–582. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2007.21.5. org/10.1037/h0025327
568 Racer, K. H., Gilbert, T. T., Luu, P., Felver-Gant, J., Abdul-
Harper, J., Malone, S. M., & Bernat, E. M. (2014). Theta and laev, Y., & Dishion, T. J. (2011). Attention network per-
delta band activity explain N2 and P3 ERP component activ- formance and psychopathic symptoms in early
ity in a go/no-go task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 125(1), adolescence: An ERP study. Journal of Abnormal Child
124–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.06.025 Psychology, 39(7), 1001–1012. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Hilbig, B. E. (2016). Reaction time effects in lab- versus Web- s10802-011-9522-6
based research: Experimental evidence. Behavior Research Ross, S. R., Benning, S. D., & Adams, Z. (2007). Symptoms of
Methods, 48(4), 1718–1724. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428- executive dysfunction are endemic to secondary psychopa-
015-0678-9 thy: An examination in criminal offenders and
Durand et al. 11

noninstitutionalized young adults. Journal of Personality Journal of Research in Personality, 41(2), 276–294. https://
Disorders, 21(4), 384–399. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2007. doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.04.001
21.4.384 Semmelmann, K., & Weigelt, S. (2017). Online psychophysics:
Roussy, S., & Toupin, J. (2000). Behavioral inhibition deficits Reaction time effects in cognitive experiments. Behavior
in juvenile psychopaths. Aggressive Behavior, 26(6), Research Methods, 49(4), 1241–1260. https://doi.org/10.
413–424. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2337(200011)26: 3758/s13428-016-0783-4
6\413::AID-AB1.3.0.CO;2-Q Snowden, R. J., Gray, N. S., Pugh, S., & Atkinson, G. (2013).
Salnaitis, C. L., Baker, C. A., Holland, J., & Welsh, M. (2011). Executive function as a function of sub-clinical psychopa-
Differentiating tower of Hanoi performance: Interactive thy. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(7), 801–804.
effects of psychopathic tendencies, impulsive response styles, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.06.016
and modality. Applied Neuropsychology, 18(1), 37–46. Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2008). Putting the
https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2010.523381 brakes on antisocial behavior: Secondary psychopathy and
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distin- post-error adjustments in reaction time. Personality and
guishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self- Individual Differences, 44(8), 1807–1818. https://doi.org/10.
mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orien- 1016/j.paid.2008.02.007
tation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Zeier, J. D., Baskin-sommers, A. R., & Newman, J. P. (2012).
67(6), 1063–1078. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6. Cognitive control deficits associated with antisocial person-
1063 ality disorder and psychopathy. Personal Disord., 3(3),
Schoofs, D., Preuß, D., & Wolf, O. T. (2008). Psychosocial 283–293. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023137.Cognitive
stress induces working memory impairments in an n-back Zimak, E. H., Suhr, J., & Bolinger, E. M. (2014). Psychophysiolo-
paradigm. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(5), 643–653. gical and neuropsychological characteristics of non-incarcerated
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.02.004 adult males with higher levels of psychopathic personality
Sellbom, M., & Verona, E. (2007). Neuropsychological corre- traits. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,
lates of psychopathic traits in a non-incarcerated sample. 36, 542–554. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-014-9430-5

You might also like