You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/225592934

Children’s Depression Inventory in Estonia

Article  in  European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry · April 2007


DOI: 10.1007/s00787-007-0650-z

CITATIONS READS

23 3,533

6 authors, including:

Algi Samm Airi Värnik


University of Tartu Tallinn University
9 PUBLICATIONS   182 CITATIONS    146 PUBLICATIONS   11,438 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Merike Sisask Kairi Kõlves


Tallinn University Griffith University
93 PUBLICATIONS   5,303 CITATIONS    196 PUBLICATIONS   4,560 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

SUPRME-Suicide prevention among school pupil through internet and media View project

PhD thesis: Translating evidence into practice: wellbeing and suicide prevention in rural communities View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Anne-Liis von Knorring on 29 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2008)
17:162–170 DOI 10.1007/s00787-007-0650-z ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION

Algi Samm Children’s Depression Inventory in Estonia


Airi Värnik
Liina-Mai Tooding Single items and factor structure by age and gender
Merike Sisask
Kairi Kõlves
Anne-Liis von Knorring

j Abstract The aim of study was study. Factor analysis obtained


Accepted: 8 August 2007
Published online: 14 September 2007 to estimate the score of symptoms five factors: anhedonia, ineffec-
of depression with the Children’s tiveness, negative self-esteem,
Depression Inventory (CDI) negative mood and interpersonal
among Estonian schoolchildren problems. Significant gender and
aged 7–13-year-old, according to age differences were found: girls
A. Samm (&) Æ A. Värnik Æ M. Sisask age and gender differences, and to reported more symptoms of
K. Kõlves identify the components in factor anhedonia and negative self-es-
Estonian-Swedish Mental Health and analysis characterising self-re- teem, and boys reported more
Suicidology Institute, ported childhood symptoms of symptoms of ineffectiveness.
Estonian Centre of Behaviour &
Health Science
depression. The applicability of Younger children reported more
Õie 39 the CDI in 7-year-old children was symptoms of anhedonia and inef-
Tallinn 11615, Estonia also estimated. The number of fectiveness, and older children
E-Mail: algi.samm@mail.ee subjects in the study was 725 (342 negative self-esteem. The study
L.-M. Tooding Æ A. Samm Æ A. Värnik girls and 383 boys), and the mean serves as baseline data before
Faculty of Social Sciences age was 10.2 (SD 1.7). The mean intervention of the EC project
Tartu University total score of the CDI for the ‘‘European Alliance Against
Tartu, Estonia whole sample was 9.96 (SD = 6.3, Depression’’.
A. Värnik Æ M. Sisask range 0–39, median 9.0). The
Faculty of Social Sciences mean scores of symptoms of
Tallinn University
Tallinn, Estonia
depression among children did
not differ by gender or age. There j Key words Children’s Depres-
A.-L. von Knorring were no significant differences in
Dept. of Neuroscience, Child and sion Inventory (CDI) –
Adolescent Psychiatry
the CDI mean scores between 7- symptoms – childhood depres-
University Hospital year-old compared to older sion – multifactorial structure
Uppsala, Sweden schoolchildren in the present

need to have direct assessment concerning children’s


Introduction mental health [15, 17].
Depression in children and adolescents is a major The CDI, devised by Maria Kovacs [15, 17] is one
public health problem. Self-rated symptoms of of the most widely used instruments in epidemio-
depression inventories have a significant role in logical studies to assess children and adolescents’ self-
clinical and population-based research with the pur- report levels of depression. Several items concern the
pose of studying the distribution of specific symp- consequences of depression in contexts that are spe-
toms, in order to define clinical or alarm thresholds or cifically relevant to children e.g. school. The CDI has
in screening for symptoms of depression [27]. The typically been used to explore the prevalence of
ECAP 650

Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI), a self-rated symptoms of childhood depression in the epidemio-
symptoms scale, was developed in response to the logical studies, both in clinical and community sam-
A. Samm et al. 163
CDI in Estonian schoolchildren

ples [1, 2, 15, 17, 18, 20, 26, 27, 29, 31]. To our in 1996 and 2004 as a regular screening. A stratified
knowledge, there are no studies concerning the sampling method was used [4]. The study approached
screening of childhood depression in populations of grades 1–6 in all 10 schools of the county. Three
post-soviet countries. classes from each grade were randomly selected for
Studies have shown that children are able to the study. All children in the selected 18 classes were
experience depressive affects and cognitions from an supposed to participate.
early age. From 8-year-old and onwards, children are The children were 7–13-year-old and were divided
considered to be able to report their own depressive during the analysis into two age groups: a younger
emotions and cognitions separate from their envi- group (7–10-year-old) from grades 1 to 4, and an
ronmental context [3, 14, 16, 17, 27, 29, 31]. About 7- older group (11–13-year-old), who attended grades 5
year-old have been included only in some studies [12, and 6. The reasons for grouping were, first, that the
23, 25] and the applicability of the CDI to 7-year-old age of 7–10-year-old is considered to be pre-puberty
and younger has been matter of debates. and 11–13-year-old to be peri-puberty. Second, pri-
Studies using the CDI as a diagnostic tool have mary school in the Estonian educational system is
confirmed the reliability and validity of the instrument divided into two stages, grades 1–4 where the class
[13, 15, 20, 30–32]. Although research has shown that teacher teaches all subjects, and grades 5–6 where
this inventory is moderately correlated with other subject teachers teach specific subjects.
measures of depressive disorder, the instrument is The final number of subjects was 725 (383 boys
insufficient for diagnosis in a clinical population [5, 7, and 342 girls). The mean age of the entire sample was
15, 20, 32]. A primary diagnostic measure should be a 10.2 (SD 1.7): 10.3 (SD 1.7) for boys and 10.2 (SD 1.7)
structured interview to determine diagnostic status [7]. for girls, which were statistically non-significant.
Kovacs emphasises the CDI as an assessment tool in Distribution of children by gender and age for both
treatment-outcome studies [15]. study years is shown in Table 1. The distribution of
The aim of this study was (a) to estimate the score children by grades was similar to the age cohorts.
of symptoms of depression with the CDI in a popu-
lation of schoolchildren by gender and age, (b) to j Instrument
identify the components characterising self-reported
childhood symptoms of depression and (c) to evaluate The present study used Maria Kovacs’ 27-item self-
if the CDI is also applicable to 7-year-old children. report questionnaire, which contains items regarding
cognitive, emotional and behavioural aspects of
depression in children. The CDI quantifies a range of
Methods symptoms of depression including disturbed mood,
hedonic capacity, vegetative functions, self-evaluation
j Subjects and interpersonal behaviour [15, 17].
In the present study a 26-item questionnaire was
The study was conducted among elementary school used to represent the original 27-item CDI. The
children in a district of Estonia (Haapsalu/Läänemaa) question concerning suicidal tendencies was excluded

Table 1 Gender distribution by age


groups, 1996 and 2004 1996 2004 Total

Gender n % Mean age (SD) n % Mean age (SD) n % Mean age (SD)

Boys
7–10 years 77 8.9 (1.0) 108 8.7 (1.1) 185 8.8 (1.1)
11–13 years 122 11.7 (0.7) 68 11.8 (0.7) 190 11.7 (0.7)
Total 199 52.0 10.6 (1.6) 176 53.8 9.9 (1.8) 375 52.8 10.3 (1.7)
Missinga 7 1 8
Girls
7–10 years 71 8.9 (1.2) 92 8.6 (1.2) 163 8.7 (1.2)
11–13 years 112 11.5 (0.6) 59 11.8 (0.8) 171 11.6 (0.7)
Total 183 48.0 10.5 (1.6) 151 46.2 9.9 (1.8) 334 47.2 10.2 (1.7)
Missinga 7 1 8
Both genders
7–10 years 148 8.9 (1.1) 200 8.7 (1.1) 348 8.8 (1.1)
11–13 years 234 11.6 (0.6) 127 11.8 (0.7) 361 11.7 (0.7)
Total 382 100 10.6 (1.6) 327 100 9.9 (1.8) 709 100 10.2 (1.7)
Missinga 14 2 16
a
Age was missing
164 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2008) Vol. 17, No. 3
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2007

in line with Moilanen’s recommendations (1990) to alpha, which was 0.82 for the entire sample. Item to-
avoid the possibility that consciousness about a pre- tal-score correlation coefficients (Pearson), r = 0.28–
viously unconscious suicidal idea could emerge in the 0.53, were all significant at the P < 0.0001 level for the
child’s mind [22]. The CDI was translated from Eng- whole sample.
lish to Estonian and back with high coincidence. The mean total score for the whole sample was 9.96
(SD = 6.3, range 0–39, median 9.0). There were no
statistically significant differences in the CDI mean
j Procedure scores of the study years (1996 and 2004), and there
were no gender or age effects. Thus, both study years
The children filled in the questionnaire during a were handled as one entire sample.
regular school lesson and were asked to endorse one Although no differences between genders were
of the three statements, that best described the way he found in the total CDI scores, there were significant
or she had felt and thought during the preceding differences in the scores of the single items (Table 2).
2 weeks, on each item of the questionnaire. The re- Boys scored higher in items such as ‘‘I have to push
search assistant slowly read out the separate items to myself all the time to do my schoolwork’’, and ‘‘I can
the whole class in the presence of the teacher, while never be as good as kinds’’. Girls scored higher in
each subject read along silently on his/her own copy items such as ‘‘I feel like crying every day’’, and ‘‘I
and marked the answers. The purpose of this proce- cannot make up my mind about things’’, and ‘‘Most
dure was to assist those children with reading or days I do not feel like eating’’.
attention problems [15]. The pupils had the possi- Significant differences were found in the CDI mean
bility to ask questions if they didn’t understand an scores of the single items between the two age groups
item, which were then clarified by the teacher or re- 7–10 years and 11–13 years (Table 3). The younger
search assistant. In this way the anonymity was age group scored higher in the items: ‘‘Nothing is fun
guaranteed. at all’’, ‘‘I am sure that terrible things will happen to
Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS 13.0 me’’, ‘‘I do not want to be with people at all’’, ‘‘I have
for Windows. Differences between the mean values of trouble sleeping almost every night’’, ‘‘I am tired all
continuous data for different groups were analysed the time’’, and ‘‘I worry about aches and pain all the
with an independent sample t-test and an analysis of time’’ (Table 3). The older age group scored higher in
variance (ANOVA) and controlled with non-para- the items ‘‘I do everything wrong’’, and ‘‘I hate my-
metric tests (Mann–Whitney U-test and Kruskal– self’’.
Wallis). To extract factors of the CDI principal com-
ponents procedure by a Varimax rotation was used. j Factor analysis
The items which had loadings of more than 0.30 on
any of the factors were accepted. Reported factors In order to discover the CDI items’ distribution,
were analysed by gender and age and gender/age sample factor extraction was conducted with the
interaction using ANOVA. Cronbach alpha was used eigenvalue of 1, and seven factors were reported
to calculate the internal consistency of the whole describing 47.9% of the total variance. A 5-factor
questionnaire as well as single factors. model was chosen because it was clearly interpretable
Since of the high number of tests carried out, the as five distinct dimensions of depression and was best
significance level of 0.01 was used to compare single suited to describe our data. This explained 39.9% of
items. In other comparisons the significance level of the variance (Table 4).
0.05 was applied. Factor one, anhedonia, (Cronbach a = 0.65) ex-
Missing values by single items had a mean value of plained 9.6% of the variance, and psychosomatic
seven and the median was six (range 3–17). Two complaints and absence of pleasure was brought out.
individuals had three, 11 had two and 42 had one Analysis indicated significant effects regarding gender
missing item. If more than three of the items were (F = 9.0, df = 1, P = 0.003) and age (F = 47.8, df = 1,
missing, the form was excluded from the analysis P < 0.001). However, no differences between gender
(n = 9). and age interaction effects (F = 2.2, df = 1, P = 0.139)
were reported. Anhedonia was reported more in girls
than boys, and was expressed more in the younger age
Results group than the older.
Factor two, ineffectiveness, (Cronbach a = 0.66)
j The reliability and scores of the CDI explained 8.9% of the variance and included a pessi-
mistic view of one’s coping ability at school. Analysis
The reliability was estimated by internal consistency indicated significant effects regarding gender
and analysed by means of the coefficient of Cronbach (F = 18.4, df = 1, P < 0.001) and age (F = 28.4,
A. Samm et al. 165
CDI in Estonian schoolchildren

Table 2 Single items’ scores of the


CDI by gender No. Items of CDI Boys Girls t P

Mean SD Mean SD

1. I am sad all the time 0.07 0.29 0.11 0.33 )1.38 0.168
2. Nothing will ever work out for me 0.77 0.73 0.70 0.71 1.30 0.193
3. I do everything wrong 0.74 0.47 0.70 0.48 1.28 0.200
4. Nothing is fun at all 0.36 0.49 0.30 0.47 1.84 0.070
5. I am bad all the time 0.13 0.39 0.07 0.28 2.23 0.026
6. I am sure that terrible things will happen to me 0.41 0.65 0.39 0.60 0.36 0.717
all the time
7. I hate myself 0.20 0.47 0.22 0.48 )0.66 0.510
8. All bad things are my fault 0.22 0.47 0.21 0.47 0.25 0.805
9. I feel like crying every day 0.08 0.30 0.21 0.47 )4.51 0.000
10. Things bother me all the time 0.49 0.73 0.52 0.73 )0.70 0.484
11. I do not want to be with people at all 0.19 0.54 0.27 0.65 )1.78 0.075
12. I can not make up my mind about things 0.49 0.62 0.63 0.62 )3.15 0.002
13. I look ugly 0.56 0.52 0.56 0.55 )0.08 0.936
14. I have to push myself all the time to do my schoolwork 0.76 0.78 0.55 0.71 3.81 0.000
15. I have trouble sleeping almost every night 0.23 0.51 0.28 0.57 )1.33 0.186
16. I am tired all the time 0.31 0.61 0.36 0.64 )1.15 0.252
17. Most days I do not feel like eating 0.30 0.56 0.42 0.61 )2.84 0.005
18. I worry about my aches and pains all the time 0.43 0.66 0.56 0.72 )2.46 0.014
19. I feel alone all the time 0.35 0.59 0.43 0.57 )1.97 0.500
20. I never have fun at school 0.48 0.61 0.41 0.56 1.56 0.119
21. I do not have any friends 0.30 0.49 0.37 0.51 )1.82 0.069
22. I do very badly in subjects I used to be good at 0.67 0.72 0.59 0.72 1.59 0.113
23. I can never be as good as kinds 0.62 0.71 0.44 0.66 3.38 0.001
24. Nobody really loves me 0.29 0.50 0.35 0.56 )1.43 0.152
25. I never do what I am told 0.26 0.48 0.26 0.47 )0.02 0.988
26. I get into fights all the time 0.20 0.45 0.18 0.39 0.75 0.456

Table 3 Single items’ scores of the


CDI by age groups (7–10 and 11–13) No. Items of CDI 7–10 year 11–13 year t P

Mean SD Mean SD

1. I am sad all the time 0.10 0.34 0.08 0.28 1.09 0.277
2. Nothing will ever work out for me 0.72 0.74 0.73 0.70 )0.22 0.823
3. I do everything wrong 0.65 0.50 0.79 0.44 )4.23 0.000
4. Nothing is fun at all 0.39 0.51 0.27 0.44 3.24 0.001
5. I am bad all the time 0.10 0.36 0.10 0.32 0.00 1.000
6. I am sure that terrible things will happen 0.52 0.70 0.28 0.53 5.20 0.000
to me all the time
7. I hate myself 0.15 0.41 0.28 0.53 )3.71 0.000
8. All bad things are my fault 0.18 0.46 0.24 0.47 )1.47 0.143
9. I feel like crying every day 0.16 0.42 0.11 0.36 1.67 0.095
10. Things bother me all the time 0.49 0.72 0.53 0.75 )0.75 0.455
11. I do not want to be with people at all 0.29 0.66 0.17 0.51 2.80 0.005
12. I cannot make up my mind about things 0.56 0.68 0.55 0.56 0.16 0.877
13. I look ugly 0.52 0.53 0.61 0.53 )2.32 0.020
14. I have to push myself all the time to do my schoolwork 0.61 0.80 0.71 0.72 )1.68 0.094
15. I have trouble sleeping almost every night 0.31 0.58 0.20 0.49 2.77 0.006
16. I am tired all the time 0.44 0.71 0.24 0.51 4.24 0.000
17. Most days I do not feel like eating 0.39 0.64 0.32 0.53 1.62 0.105
18. I worry about aches and pain all the time 0.60 0.74 0.39 0.62 4.11 0.000
19. I feel alone all the time 0.43 0.63 0.35 0.53 1.93 0.055
20. I never have fun at school 0.40 0.60 0.50 0.56 )2.27 0.023
21. I do not have any friends 0.37 0.51 0.30 0.49 2.06 0.040
22. I do very badly in subjects I used to be good at 0.59 0.75 0.66 0.69 )1.41 0.160
23. I can never be as good as kinds 0.50 0.69 0.56 0.69 )1.21 0.229
24. Nobody really loves me 0.32 0.55 0.32 0.51 )0.04 0.971
25. I never do what I am told 0.24 0.48 0.28 0.47 )0.97 0.334
26. I get into fights all the time 0.22 0.45 0.16 0.39 2.16 0.031
166 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2008) Vol. 17, No. 3
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2007

Table 4 Principal component


analysis with Varimax rotation of CDI CDI item no. Loadings
items
Factor 1 2 3 4 5

I. Anhedonia
18. I worry about aches and pain all the time 0.614 0.067 )0.061 0.021 )0.096
16. I am tired all the time 0.564 0.028 )0.021 0.183 0.211
15. I have trouble sleeping almost every night 0.505 0.078 0.210 )0.058 0.156
19. I feel alone all the time 0.472 )0.030 0.190 0.195 0.292
6. I am sure that terrible things will happen to me all the time 0.421 0.012 )0.046 0.235 0.105
10. Things bother me all the time 0.416 0.222 0.049 0.383 )0.306
17. Most days I do not feel like eating 0.409 0.178 0.156 )0.165 0.144
4. Nothing is fun at all 0.350 0.225 0.298 0.052 0.165
12. I cannot make up my mind about things 0.324 0.289 0.159 0.012 0.077
II. Ineffectiveness
14. I have to push myself all the time to do my schoolwork 0.139 0.7490 )0.068 0.077 0.004
22. I do very badly in subjects I used to be good at 0.138 0.5770 0.264 )0.125 0.099
23. I can never be as good as kinds 0.009 0.5470 0.201 0.245 0.141
2. Nothing will ever work out for me 0.278 0.5290 0.342 )0.009 )0.108
3. I do everything wrong )0.140 0.3800 0.291 0.211 0.169
III. Negative self-esteem
13. I look ugly )0.057 0.244 0.664 )0.047 0.077
24. Nobody really loves me 0.168 0.141 0.606 0.223 0.035
21. I do not have any friends 0.235 )0.026 0.462 )0.139 0.423
7. I hate myself 0.068 0.239 0.460 0.269 0.078
IV. Negative mood
9. I feel like crying every day 0.324 )0.160 0.359 0.346 )0.144
5. I am bad all the time 0.050 0.101 )0.042 0.624 0.077
1. I am sad all the time 0.228 )0.114 0.329 0.539 0.163
8. All bad things are my fault 0.027 0.234 0.219 0.473 0.211
V. Interpersonal problems
26. I get into fights all the time 0.299 0.110 0.023 0.154 0.655
25. I never do what I am told )0.052 0.396 )0.125 0.258 0.476
20. I never have fun at school 0.042 0.263 0.272 0.036 0.428
11. I do not want to be with people at all 0.291 )0.044 0.115 0.106 0.413

CDI, Children’s Depression Inventory numeration of the items reflects the list of M. Kovacs’ questionnaire, which was used
in the study
Extraction method, Principal component analysis; Rotation method, Varimax with Kaiser normalization, a rotation
converged in 24 iterations
To extract factors of the CDI principal components procedure by a Varimax rotation was used. The items which has
loadings of more than 0.30 or any of the factors were accepted as it described in the paragraph of procedure (page 3 in
this article) and marked in bold in table 4

df = 1, P < 0.001), but no differences between gender df = 1, P = 0.763), nor age effects (F = 0.24, df = 1,
and age interaction effects (F = 0.02, df = 1, P = 0.625), and no gender or age interaction
P = 0.878) were reported. The boys had higher scores (F = 0.12, df = 1, P = 0.734) effects.
than the girls. The older children had remarkably Factor five, interpersonal problems, (Cronbach
higher scores of ineffectiveness than the younger a = 0.48) explained 6.5% of the variance. This factor
children. brought out lack of communication skills and loneli-
Factor three, negative self-esteem, (Cronbach ness. There were no gender (F = 1.0, df = 1,
a = 0.58) showed 8.3% of the variance, and reflected P = 0.318), age (F = 2.6, df = 1, P = 0.107) or gender
low self-esteem and feelings of being unloved. There and age interaction (F = 1.2, df = 1, P = 0.281) effects.
were significant gender (F = 7.2, df = 1, P = 0.008)
and age effects (F = 5.2, df = 1, P = 0.023), but no j Applicability in 7-year-old
gender and age interaction effects (F = 0.4, df = 1,
P = 0.504). The girls reported more symptoms than About 63, seven-year-old schoolchildren participated
the boys. The older age group had lower self-esteem in the study: 28 boys and 35 girls. The mean total
than the younger age group. score in this age group was 9.3 (SD = 5.1, range 0–
Factor four, negative mood, (Cronbach a = 0.46) 23), and the median was 9.0. No significant differ-
explained 6.6% of the variance, and described feelings ences were found between the mean total score of
of sadness and guilt. There were no gender (F = 0.09, 7- and 8-year-old [10.5 (SD = 6.0) (t = )1.22,
A. Samm et al. 167
CDI in Estonian schoolchildren

% Negative self-esteem (Factor 3) also showed a sig-


30
7 8-10 11-13 nificant age effect (F = 6.1, df = 1, P = 0.014), but no
25 differences to gender or to gender and age interaction
20
effects (Factor 3); 8–10-year-old had lower self-esteem
than 7-year-old.
15 Negative mood (Factor 4) showed a significant age
10
effect (F = 4.1, df = 1, P = 0.043) but no differences
depending on gender or to gender and age interaction
5 effects. A 7-year-old scored higher on negative mood
than 8–10-year-old.
0
0-3 4-7 8-11 12-15 16-19 20-23 24-27 28-31 32-35 36-39 There were no differences concerning age effects in
Total CDI score interpersonal problem factor (Factor 5).
Fig. 1 Distribution of CDI scores by age; 7-, 8–10 and 11–13-year-old
Discussion

P = 0.225)], or 8–10-year-old [10.4 (SD = 6.4), The aim of this study was to estimate the scores of
(t = )1.29, P = 0.198)], or 8–13-year-old [10.1 symptoms of depression with the use of the CDI in a
(SD = 6.4) (t = )0.84, P = 0.404)]. sample of Estonian schoolchildren, to identify the
The distribution of CDI scores between 7, 8–10 and components characterising self-reported childhood
11–13-year-old, was similar (H = 2.40, P = 0.301), symptoms of depression, and to evaluate if the CDI is
and is shown in Fig. 1. also applicable in 7-year-old children.
When the mean scores of the single items between
7- and 8-year-old were compared, only one significant j CDI scores by gender and age
difference was found: the item ‘‘I hate myself’’ was
scored higher by 8-year-old (t = )2.763, P = 0.007). The mean score in our sample was similar to previ-
Between seven and 8–10-year-old there were six ously reported findings in a non-clinical sample of
items, which differed significantly: 7-year-old scored Italian [27], Canadian [15] and American student
higher in ‘‘I am sad all the time’’ (t = 2.78, P = 0.006) samples [15, 26, 31]. In our study the mean scores for
and lower in ‘‘I do everything wrong’’ (t = )3.32, both genders were higher than those reported in a
P = 0.001), ‘‘I look ugly’’ (t = )3.32, P = 0.001), comparative Swedish study [20].
‘‘Nothing will ever work out for me’’ (t = )3.33, There are inconsistencies in previous findings of
P = 0.001), ‘‘I hate myself’’ (t = )2.49, P = 0.013), the gender and age effect. In some studies, including
and ‘‘I can never be as good as kinds’’ (t = )2.67, the present study, no significant differences in the
P = 0.008). total CDI scores were found between boys and girls [2,
The factor analysis showed a significant age dif- 15]. Other studies have indicated that prepubertal
ference between 7- and 8-year-old in the items rep- girls’ scores were slightly lower than boys’, but girls
resented by ineffectiveness (Factor 2). There were scored higher from age 13 [24, 35]. Young adolescents
significant effects for age (F = 8.3, df = 1, P < 0.005), scored lower than pre-adolescence children [20, 27].
but no differences to gender or to gender and age This study found that both boys and girls scored
interaction effects were reported; 7-year-old had highest in the item titled ‘‘Nothing will ever work out
lower scores of ineffectiveness than 8-year-old. for me’’, but statistically significant gender differences
Between 7- and 8–10-year-old there were significant were found in other items. Boys scored higher than
differences in four of the five factors. A significant effect girls in the items: ‘‘I have to push myself all the time
for age (F = 6.2, df = 1, P < 0.014) in the anhedonia to do my schoolwork’’, and ‘‘I can never be as good as
factor (Factor 1) was found. A 7-year-old compared to kinds’’, and girls scored higher in the items ‘‘I feel like
8–10-year-old had higher symptom loadings. However, crying every day’’, and ‘‘I cannot make my mind
no differences depending on gender or gender and age about things’’, and ‘‘Most days I do not feel like eat-
interaction effects were reported. ing’’. These findings suggest the centrality of gender
Significant effects for age (F = 14.1, df = 1, specific symptoms of depression.
P < 0.001) and gender (F = 5.1, df = 1, P < 0.025), The statement with the highest score in younger
but no differences to gender and age interaction ef- children, ‘‘Nothing will ever work out for me’’, and
fects were found for the ineffectiveness factor (Factor that for older children, ‘‘I do everything wrong’’, re-
2). The 8–10-year-old had remarkably higher scores flect the children’s vulnerability towards external de-
of ineffectiveness than the 7-year-old, and the boys mands. Younger children also complained more often
reported higher scores than the girls. about tiredness.
168 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2008) Vol. 17, No. 3
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2007

j Factorial structure of the CDI able to experience depressive affects and cognitions,
and that children aged eight and older are considered
Depression measured by the CDI is accepted as a able to report their own depressive moods and cog-
multidimensional construct in which individual dif- nitions as something separate from their environ-
ferences by age and gender usually appear [2, 27]. The mental contexts [14, 16, 17, 27, 29].
number and essence of the extracted factors from the As one purpose of the present study was to eval-
CDI have varied in different studies. The 5-factor uate the applicability of the CDI in 7-year-old chil-
model of the present study was clearly interpretable as dren, we compared the scores of 7-year-old with the
five distinct dimensions of depression, a model, which scores of older ages. There were no significant dif-
has also been supported by other studies [2, 5, 6, 33]. ferences between 7- and 8-year-old’s mean total
In the present study significant gender and age scores. Comparing 8–10-year-old children with 7-
differences were found in three of five factors. Girls year-old by single items, the differences between the
reported more symptoms of anhedonia and negative two groups might be explained by their development
self-esteem, and boys reported more symptoms of of abstract thinking and vocabulary. The younger
ineffectiveness. Similar results were reported by Aluja children rate themselves in more concrete terms,
and Blanch [2]. Boys showed anhedonia more often which is expected due to the immaturity of their
than girls in Finland, and also tended to score higher cognitive development. The older children are able to
on low self-confidence [22]. judge their own normative actions with respect to
The factor analysis of the present study found their own normative standards and they use more
symptoms of anhedonia to be expressed more in generalised trait-like descriptions when describing
younger children than older children. Young children themselves and seem to be increasingly vulnerable to
scored remarkably higher scores of ineffectiveness the feelings of helplessness and hopelessness [34].
and lower self-esteem. These findings are in line with Overall physical, psychological and intellectual
the study of Chan where the level of symptom of development in children has accelerated in many
depression tended to be highest among older students countries, and now also symptoms of depression
[5]. In a community-based sample of children, the might be possible to express in a more valid way by
highest scored factor was anhedonia. Individual self- the younger children. For example, mean norms of
efficacy might have a different relationship to IQ-tests of children have been inflated over time
depression among children dependent on differences suggested to be caused by environmental factors such
in socio-cultural environments [33]. In Estonian as better nutrition, mental stimulation and schooling
society, boys have greater demands from their social [21, 28].
context, which expresses gender differences such as According to our study, it could be concluded that
complaints about more ineffectiveness than girls. the CDI is applicable to 7-year-old concerning scores.
In 1997, Chan presented a causal model of per- However, there are some differences. We must keep in
ceived academic and social competence; perceived mind that the 7-year-old children in our study are
incompetence may lead to depression, and depression sampled from a normal population. Whereas in
may lead to perceived incompetence, an idea, which is clinical samples the children probably have more
similar to the ineffectiveness factor in our study. Chan cognitive and developmental problems. Therefore,
suggests that a third unknown factor may cause both our statement is not valid for clinical populations.
perceived incompetence and depression [5].
Our result supports the interpretation revealing
that girls tend to ‘‘internalise’’ depression at an earlier j Ethical and methodological considerations
age than boys, and that they tend to manifest inter-
nally focused characteristics, e.g. sadness, negative Estonia had the highest suicide rate in Europe during
body-image, somatic preoccupation, more often [14, the mid 1990’s, when this study was planned [36]. To
20, 31]. Boys tend to manifest more behavioural avoid possible harm to vulnerable children, we didn’t
components of depressive moods, and that estimation include the question about suicidal ideation, which
of incompetence is an important predictor of might have been be too stressful to answer, particu-
depression among boys [29]. larly for children of the younger age group [22].
Children from 5 years of age have a concept of death,
but suicide attempts and especially completed sui-
j Applicability of the CDI in the 7-year-old cides are rare before puberty [8, 19]. However, suicide
attempts have been reported in children younger than
Little is known about depression in early childhood; five and a few cases of completed suicide in this age-
more research is available about adolescent depres- range have been registered in the Nordic countries
sion. Previous studies have shown that children are [9].
A. Samm et al. 169
CDI in Estonian schoolchildren

An informed consent was obtained from the school This suggests that the high CDI score in this population
administrations. The CDI questionnaire was well ac- could be even higher than our findings suggest.
cepted by Estonian schoolchildren, parents and
teachers, and no refusals to complete the question- j Challenges for prevention
naire were met. Children filled in the questionnaire in
the presence of their schoolteacher and a research Earlier studies have reported that the majority of
assistant. Parents were informed about the study’s depressed children were left without psychiatric
design. They also had questionnaires to fill in and assessment and help. One possible solution could be
none of them forbade their child from participating. to provide self-reported screening, including items
Accordingly, this study had the indirect approval about symptoms of depression as a part of any
from all of the parents of the children who partici- regular check-up (or screening instrument) for
pated in it. As the study was anonymous, the children schoolchildren, which could be used as a basis for
could not be later identified. opening discussion with the children’s caregivers
The data was gathered twice (1996 and 2004) with [11, 29].
an interval of 8 years, in the same district and with The present study serves as a baseline for evalua-
the same method. One of the authors (AV) partici- tion by the EC project ‘‘European Alliance Against
pated in obtaining material for both study years and Depression’’: a multilevel system of intervention,
led the study. which involves a comprehensive network of different
The fact that all of the children responded may be countries, and is currently expanding into the area of
explained by the fact that Estonians lived under a childhood mental health involving symptoms of
totalitarian system for more than 50 years, which depression [10].
made people, in general, compliant. Moreover, Esto-
nian schoolchildren still have a high respect for j Acknowledgements The current study was funded by the Esto-
teachers’ authority. nian Health Insurance Fund (Grant no. 144/88) and an Estonian
Scientific Foundation project number 7132, ‘Suicide trend in Es-
The 7% of the children not attending school on the tonia during independence: What are the associations with socio-
day, when the self-reported inventory was conducted, political, economic and public health indicators?’. Ingrid Danilov,
probably had a worse health status and higher CDI Liivia Anion and Tiina Kütt from Läänemaa county are thanked for
ratings than the children who participated in the study. their assistance in data collection.

References
1. Almqvist F, Ikaheimo K, Kumpulainen 6. Craighead WE, Curry JF, Ilard SS Alliance Against Depression (EAAD): a
K, Tuompo-Johansson E, Linna SL, (1995) Relationship of Children’s multifaceted, community-based action
Puura K, Moilanen I, Rasanen E, Depression Inventory factors to major programme against depression and
Tamminen T, Piha J (1999) Design and depression among adolescents. Psychol suicidality. World J Biol Psychiatry
subjects of a Finnish epidemiological Assess 7:171–176 http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/
study on psychiatric disorders in 7. Fristad MA, Emery BL, Beck SJ (1997) content~content=a776598473~db=
childhood. Eur Child Adolesc Psychia- Use and abuse of the Children’s all~order=pubdate
try 8(Suppl 4):3–6 Depression Inventory. J Consult Clin 11. Hodges KK, Siegel LJ, Mullins L, Griffin
2. Aluja A, Blanch A (2002) The children Psychol 65:699–702 N (1983) Factor analysis of the Chil-
depression inventory as predictor of 8. Groholt B (1999) Suicide and suicide dren’s Depression Inventory. Psychol
social and scholastic competence. Eur J attempt among children and adoles- Rep 53:759–763
Psychol Assess 18:259–274 cents. A clinical and epidemiological 12. Ialongo NS, Edelsohn G, Kellam SG
3. Angold A, Weissman MM, John K, study from Norway 1990–1994. Uni- (2001) A further look at the prognostic
Merikangas KR, Prusoff BA, Wick- versity of Oslo, Oslo power of young children’s reports of
ramaratne P, Gammon GD, Warner V 9. Groholt B, Ekeberg O, Wichstrom L, depressed mood and feelings. Child
(1987) Parent and child reports of Haldorsen T (1998) Suicide among Dev 72:736–747
depressive symptoms in children at low children and younger and older ado- 13. Kazdin AE (1990) Childhood depres-
and high risk of depression. J Child lescents in Norway: a comparative sion. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
Psychol Psychiatry 28:901–915 study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psy- 31:121–160
4. Burns RB (2000) Introduction to re- chiatry 37:473–481 14. Keenan K, Hipwell A, Duax J, Stouth-
search methods. Sage Publications, 10. Hegerl U, Wittman M, Arensman E, amer-Loeber M, Loeber R (2004) Phe-
Thousand Oaks van Audenhove C, Bouleau JH, van der nomenology of depression in young
5. Chan DW (1997) Depressive symptoms Feltz-Cornelis C, Gusmao R, Kopp M, girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psy-
and perceived competence among Löhr C, Maxwell M, Meise U, Mirjanic chiatry 43:1098–1106
Chinese secondary school students in M, Oskarsson H, Sola VP, Pull C, Pycha 15. Kovacs M (1985) The Children’s
Hong Kong. J Youth Adolesc 26:303– R, Ricka R, Tuulari J, Värnik A, Pfeif- Depression Inventory (CDI). Psycho-
319 fer-Gerschel T (2007) The European pharmacol Bull 21:995–998
170 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2008) Vol. 17, No. 3
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2007

16. Kovacs M (1986) A developmental 24. Olsson GI, von Knorring AL (1999) 31. Smucker MR, Craighead WE, Craig-
perspective on methods and measures Adolescent depression: prevalence in head LW, Green BJ (1986) Normative
in the assessment of depressive disor- Swedish high-school students. Acta and reliability data for the Children’s
ders: the clinical interview. In: Rutter Psychiatr Scand 99:324–331 Depression Inventory. J Abnorm Child
M, Izard C, Read P (eds) Depression in 25. Orvaschel H, Beeferman D, Kabacoff R Psychol 14:25–39
young people: developmental and (1997) Depression, self-esteem, sex, 32. Sorensen MJ, Frydenberg M, Thastum
clinical perspectives. Guilford Press, and age in a child and adolescent M, Thomsen PH (2005) The Children’s
New York clinical sample. J Clin Child Psychol Depression Inventory and classification
17. Kovacs M (1992) Children’s Depression 26:285–289 of major depressive disorder: validity
Inventory manual. Multi-Health Sys- 26. Pace TM, Mullins LL, Beesley D, Hill JS, and reliability of the Danish version.
tems, North Tonawanda, NY Carson K (1999) The relationship be- Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 14:328–
18. Kovacs M, Obrosky DS, Sherrill J tween children’s emotional and behav- 334
(2001) Developmental changes in the ioral problems and the social responses 33. Steele RG, Little TD, Ilardi SS, Fore-
phenomenology of depression in girls of elementary school teachers. Con- hand R, Brody GH, Hunter HL (2006) A
compared to boys from childhood on- temp Educ Psychol 24:140–155 confirmatory comparison of the factor
ward. J Affect Disord 74:33–48 27. Poli P, Sbrana B, Marcheschi M, Masi G structure of the Children’s Depression
19. Lansdown R, Benjamin G (1985) The (2003) Self-reported depressive symp- Inventory between European American
development of the concept of death in toms in a school sample of Italian and African American youth. J Child
children aged 5–9 years. Child Care children and adolescents. Child Psy- Fam Stud 15:773–788
Health Dev 11:13–20 chiatry Hum Dev 33:209–226 34. Terwogt MM, Stegge H (1995) Emo-
20. Larsson B, Melin L (1992) Prevalence 28. Prifitera A, Weiss LG, Saklofske DH tional behaviour and emotional
and short-term stability of depressive (1998) The WISC-III in context. In: understanding: A developmental fugue.
symptoms in schoolchildren. Acta Prifitera A, Saklofske DH (eds) WISC- In: Goodyer IM (ed) The depressed
Psychiatr Scand 85:17–22 III. Clinical use and interpretation. child and adolescent. Developmental
21. Madge N, Tizard J (1980) Intelligence. Scientist-practitioner perspectives. and clinical perspectives. Cambridge
In: Rutter M (ed) Scientific foundations Academic Press, London, p 2 University Press, Cambridge, pp 27–52
of developmental psychiatry. Heine- 29. Puura K, Almqvist F, Tamminen T, 35. Twenge JM, Nolen-Hoeksema S (2002)
mann Medical Books, London, pp 245– Piha J, Kumpulainen K, Rasanen E, Age, gender, race, socioeconomic sta-
266 Moilanen I, Koivisto AM (1998) Chil- tus, and birth cohort differences on the
22. Moilanen I (1990) Components of dren with symptoms of depres- Children’s Depression Inventory: a
childhood depression. Reports of Psy- sion—what do the adults see? J Child meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol
chiatria Fennica Report No 91, pp 85– Psychol Psychiatry 39:577–585 111:578–588
91 30. Reinhard HG, Bowi U, Rulcovius G 36. http://www.stat.ee ES
23. Murphy DA, Marelich WD, Hoffman D (1990) Stability, reliability and factor
(2000) Assessment of anxiety and structure of a German version of the
depression in young children: support Children’s Depression Inventory (Kov-
for two separate constructs. J Clin acs 1983). Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr
Child Psychol 29:383–391 18:185–191

View publication stats

You might also like