You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/38092244

Child-reported hospital fears in 4 to 6-year-old children

Article  in  Pediatric Nursing · November 2008


Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

49 7,620

3 authors:

Marja Salmela Sanna Salanterä


Helsinki Metropolia University of Applied Sciences University of Turku
6 PUBLICATIONS   233 CITATIONS    317 PUBLICATIONS   6,527 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Eeva Aronen
University of Helsinki
132 PUBLICATIONS   3,821 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Empowering Surgical Orthopaedic Patients through Education View project

Disabled child's agency in play interaction with a nurse View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Eeva Aronen on 10 March 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Child-Reported Hospital Fears
In 4 to 6-Year-Old Children
Marja Salmela
Sanna Salanterä
Eeva Aronen
The aim of this study was to describe hospital-related fears of 4 to 6-year-old children. Data were collect-
ed by interviewing 4 to 6-year-old children (N = 90) in Finland, who were chosen through purposive sam-
pling in which volunteers were asked to take part in the study. Data were gathered by semi-structured inter-
view, supported by pictures, and reviewed by quantitative and qualitative methods. Results revealed that
more than 90% of children said they were afraid of at least one thing in a hospital. Most fears were cate-
gorized by nursing interventions, fears of being a patient, and fears caused by the developmental stage of
the child. Children also expressed fears caused by the unfamiliar environment or lack of information, child-
staff relations, and the physical, social, and symbolic environment. The main result was that hospital care
increased the amount of fears in 4 to 6-year-old children. The information is useful in preventing, identify-
ing, and alleviating the hospital fear of healthy or sick children.

eing admitted to the hospital immaturity, young children often have Only a few earlier studies were

B causes fear and anxiety in 4


to 6-year-old children. Ac-
cording to parents, 83% of
p reschool or kindergarten-aged chil-
d ren suffer from diff e rent kinds of anx-
been underestimated as re p o rters of
their own well being or considered as
u n reliable informants (Coyne, 1998,
2006). However, preschoolers want
and are able to express and discuss
found to describe the child reporting
fears related to hospital treatment. In a
Finnish study of 5 to 6-year-old chil-
dren (N = 9), participants re p o rt e d
that in a hospital, they were most
iety symptoms related to hospital fear their own experiences. The best way afraid of the unfamiliar environment,
even after a minor operation in a hos- to get information about childre n ’s the feeling of abandonment, pain,
pital (Rossen & McKeever, 1996). fears to is to ask them. It is difficult to bodily injuries, and the restriction of
Knowledge about preschoolers´ fears understand childre n ’s experiences their self-determination and fre e
in the hospital is mostly based on the without including their self-re p o rt choice (Ivanoff, Laijärvi, & Åstedt-
information given by parents or nurs- (Coyne, 1998; Lahikainen, Kraav, Kurki, 1999). In a study concern i n g
es and covers fear experienced in a Kirmanen, & Taimalu, 2006; Pelander children attending day surg e ry, 5 to 6-
certain surgical pro c e d u re or situation et al., 2006; Pelander & Leino-Kilpi, year-old children (N = 25) re p o rt e d
in nursing care (Brewer, Glenditsch, 2004). being afraid of injections and suturing,
Syblic, Tietjens, & Vacik, 2006; Although several studies describe but also of taking medicines and of the
Coyne, 1998; Mahajan et al., 1998; p reschool or kindergarten-aged chil- anesthesia mask. Lack of information,
Pelander, Nuutila, Salanterä, & Leino- d re n ’s reactions and anxiety toward unrealistic fears, as well as nausea,
Kilpi, 2006). Very little is known about nursing interventions (Gozal, Drenger, vomiting, and the need to undergo
h o s p i t a l - related fear as said by chil- Levin, Kadari, & Gozal, 2004; Kain, another operation also seemed to
d ren themselves. Because of their Mayes, O´Connor, & Cicchetti, 1996), cause fear (Flinkman & Salanterä,
no studies were found that describe 2004). Children with diabetes mellitus
what in the domains of nursing are the 1 to 19 years of age (N = 112) and
causes of their fears. Also, very little their parents reported being afraid of
Marja Salmela, MNSc, RN, is a Lecture r, information is available on the amount difficult symptoms that limit their
Helsinki Metropolia University of Applied and content of fears caused by being a activity and everyday life (Nordfeld &
Sciences, Health Care and Social Services, patient in a hospital, the hospital envi- Ludvigsson, 2005).
D e g ree Programme Nursing, Helsinki, ronment, interaction between nursing According to adult informants in
Finland.
staff and the child, and nursing inter- other studies, about 19% to 68% of
Sanna Salanterä, PhD, RN, is a Professor ventions reported by 4 to 6-year-old young children were afraid of injec-
of Clinical Nursing Science Department of c h i l d ren. Earlier studies have reported tions and needles (Kettwich et al.,
Nursing Science, University of Turku, that repeated admissions to the hospi- 2007; Majstorovic & Veerkamp,
Turku, Finland.
tal increase children’s anxiety (Brewer 2004). In earlier studies, parents have
Eeva T. Aronen, MD, PhD, is an Assistant et al., 2006). However, there is no reported that pain, separation fro m
P rofessor of Child Psychiatry and Senior information on whether the hospital parents, unfamiliar people, difficulties
Child Psychiatrist, Hospital for Children and fears of healthy preschoolers differ in breathing, blood samples, being
Adolescents, Child Psychiatry, Helsinki
University Central Hospital, University of
f rom the fears of children of the same held still, nursing pro c e d u res, and
Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland. age who are being treated in the hos- unfamiliarity with the norms of
pital. accepted behavior cause fear in chil-

PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2009/Vol. 35/No. 5 269


d ren (Gozal et al., 2004; Gullone, e x p ressed by the children themselves. by pictures. The interviewer discussed
2000; Jost, 1996; Koenig, Chesla, & The re s e a rch questions were: with the children certain re s e a rc h
Kennedy, 2003; LeRoy et al. 2003; • What are hospital-related fears of themes that had been determined in
N i c a s t ro & Whetsell, 1999; Snyder, 4 to 6-year-old children described advance. Interview themes were
2004). by themselves? f o rmed on the basis of Kim’s typology
In the hospital, a preschooler can • What are frequencies of fears in of domains or main contents for nurs-
also have typical fears of that part i c u- these children? ing (Kim, 2000; Kim & Kollak, 1999),
lar developmental stage, such as the • How do hospital-related fears of and were identified as a) fears caused
fear of darkness, loud noises, unfamil- healthy children interviewed in by being a patient; b) fears caused by
iar people, and separation from par- kindergarten differ from the fears the physical, social, and symbolic
ents. Preschoolers are also afraid of of children who are hospitalized? environment of the hospital; c) fears
f a i l u re, loss of control, being criticized caused by the patient-nurse relation-
or rejected, and punishments for real ship; and d) fears resulting from nurs-
or imagined misbehavior (Alsop- Methods of Data ing interventions (Burns & Grove,
Shields & Mohay, 2001; Brewer et al., Collection and Analysis 2001; Morse & Richards, 2002; Polit,
2006; Romino, Keatley, Secrest, & Beck, & Hungler, 2004). The interview
Good, 2005; Snyder, 2004). Because Data Collection method was flexible and child-cen-
of the rich imagination of preschool- Data were collected by interviewing tered; while interview themes directed
aged children, imaginary fears and 4 to 6-year-old children (N = 90) who the pro g ress of the interview, the child
fears caused by the lack of knowledge lived in the metropolitan area of was able to specify the content
are emphasized during this stage Finland (population about 1.5 mil- (Bricher, 1999; Faux, Walsh, &
(Deering & Cody, 2002; Flinkman & lion), of which 63 children were inter- Deatrick, 1988; Spradley, 1979). The
Salanterä, 2004). viewed in a kindergarten class and 27 subjects of the pictures corresponded
It is especially important to identify in two pediatric surgical wards in a to the interview themes drawn fro m
the possible fears of a child when car- university hospital during the period of the main domains of nursing (Kim,
ing for preschoolers. According to the study from 2004 to 2006. The 2000; Kim & Kollak, 1999). The pic-
several studies, preschoolers have c h i l d ren were chosen through purpo- t u re subjects were a) a sick child in
m o re hospital-related fears than older sive sampling, in which volunteers bed in the hospital with instruments
children because of their developmen- w e re asked to take part in the study. and parents nearby, b) a child in an
tal stage (Bevan et al., 1990; Gazal & The criteria for participation in the operating room, c) a child in bed sur-
Mackie, 2007; Gozal et al., 2004; study were age (4 to 6 years), the rounded by doctors and nurses, and
M a j s t o rovic & Veerkamp, 2004, 2005; c h i l d ’s ability to communicate in d) a child sitting in a wheelchair in a
Rennick, Johnston, Doughert y, Platt, Finnish, and the child’s typically corridor in the ward. The younger the
& Ritchie, 2002; Romino et al., 2005). developing (evaluated by kinder- child’s age, the more his or her discus-
The preschooler is not always able to garten and hospital personnel). sion on certain themes was facilitated
separate reality from the imaginary, A c c o rding to the phenomenological by having some concrete material,
and the child’s ability to express and research tradition, any person fulfilling such as pictures to look at and hold
cope with his or her fears is limited the inclusion criteria is a good inform- (Doverborg & Pramling, 2000).
( B rewer et al., 2006; Majstorovic & ant, and thus, purposive sampling was During the interv i e w, the interv i e w-
Veerkamp, 2004). It is also sometimes used (Spradley. 1979). er also observed the child and re c o rd-
difficult for a young child to know the Interviews were arranged with the ed the findings in a re s e a rch diary.
difference between pain and fear head nurses of the wards in the univer- Children’s answers were sometimes
(Young, 2005). A child whose fears sity hospital and with the heads of the so short that without the diary, reliable
a re taken into account and who is kindergarten classes. The first author analysis would have been impossible.
supported in coping with fears is usu- (M.S.) and interviewers informed the However, the interviewer did not inter-
ally more cooperative and less anx- personnel of the wards and kinder- pret the child’s behavior or answers,
ious than other children (Mahajan et garten classes of the study. The per- but only wrote down the objective
al., 1998). This child will often need sonnel then gave parents a data sheet observation, for example, if the child
less medication for pain and sedation, of the study with a written request for shook or nodded his or her head.
heal faster, and re t u rn home sooner permission to interview their child. All Ninety-four children originally
(Walworth, 2005). For children under- c h i l d ren whose parents gave a written a g reed to participate in the study. One
going surg e ry, the operation can be permission, who gave a verbal per- forbade tape-re c o rding, one fell asleep
perf o rmed more quickly, smoothly, mission themselves, and who were on before the interview began, and two
and with fewer personnel, making it the ward or in the kindergarten class did not want to discuss their fears at
easier for the child and parents (Kain during the period of the study were all. The interviews of these children
et al., 1996; Kristensson-Hallström, chosen to participate. were not used in analyzing data. Thus,
Elander, & Malmfors, 1997). Interviews were carried out by 20 final data consisted of 90 interv i e w s .
The first aim of this study was to trained interviewers who were gradu- The interviews were tape-re c o rd e d
describe hospital-related fears of 4 to ating nurses in their final semester. and transcribed to written form. The
6-year-old children using semi-stru c- Interviewers were trained to the spe- length of the transcribed interv i e w s
t u red interviews supported by pic- cial characteristics of interviewing ranged from 1 to 13 pages, and the
t u res. A second aim was to determ i n e c h i l d ren, and they were re q u i red 2 to 3 interviews lasted from 20 minutes to
if children who were hospitalized had pilot interviews by interviewing an hour.
m o re fears, or diff e rent fears, than healthy 4 to 6-year-old children. The
children who were interviewed at Data Analysis
re s e a rcher (M.S.) supervised the
kindergarten. The objective of the interviewing of the children. Data were analyzed by deductive
study was to describe the amount and Data were gathered by semi-stru c- content analysis and categorized into
quality of the hospital-related fears t u red interviews and were support e d pre-determined categories. Seven cat-

270 PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2009/Vol. 35/No. 5


Child-Reported Hospital Fears in 4 to 6-Year-Old Children

egories included a) fears related to Ta ble 1.


being a patient, b) the developmental Hospital-Related Fears Reported by 4 to 6-Year-Old Children (N = 90)
stage of the child, c) unknown experi-
ences and lack of information, d) the Single Expressions
physical environment, e) the social of Fear
and symbolic environment of the hos- Objects of Fear
pital, f) child-staff relations, and g) n %
nursing interventions. All expressions Fears of Being a Patient
of fear were categorized, including the
Pain 51 57
less than 6% of expressions that re p re-
sented unrealistic fears or figments of Staying in hospital 23 26
the child’s imagination. For example,
Being admitted to hospital 17 19
in these cases, the child said he was
afraid because “[the vaccine] is poi- Symptoms of the disease 15 17
son,” or “the needle goes through the
Bodily injuri e s 10 11
arm,” or “if [doctors] don’t have
masks, they smell bad.” Data were Becoming ill 6 7
reviewed by quantitative and qualita-
Fears Caused by the Developmental Stage of the Child
tive methods.
Being left alone 19 21
Ethical Considerations Imagination 13 14
The hospital’s ethics committee Loss of autonomy 10 11
and the City Social Services Unfamiliar people 9 10
Department both granted their
a p p roval for the study. All participat- Other fears caused by the developmental stage 9 10
ing parents received written inform a- Fears Caused by Unfamiliar Environment or Lack of Information
tion on the study before being asked
to consent. After receiving written Lack of information 18 20
consent from the parent or guardian, New and unknown experiences 15 17
the autonomy of the child was
respected by giving him or her oral Physical Environment
information on the study and asking Instruments and equipment 19 21
for oral consent to participate.
Interviews proceeded on the child’s Facilities of the hospital 11 12
terms, and all children received a Noises and smells of the hospital 9 10
small re w a rd (such as a sticker) after
the interview. Parents could take part Social and Symbolic Environment
in the interview if they wished. Each S e p a ration from the family 20 22
participant’s identity was protected so
that individuals could not be recog- Other fears caused by the social and symbolic environment 2 2
nized in the study. Children were Child-Staff Relations
assigned code numbers identifying
their gender, age, and place of inter- Exercise of power by adults 16 18
view (kindergarten or hospital), and Doctors 13 14
they were not requested to identify
themselves by names on the tape. The Nurses 10 11
information given by the child was Nursing Interventions
processed in confidentiality, which
was protected by coding all data and Shots 45 50
keeping all documentation in a safe Sample-taking and tests 29 32
location (Burns & Grove, 2001;
Munhall, 1999). Other nursing interventions 29 32
Operation 12 13
Results Medication 12 13
Results are presented here re g a rd-
ing the amount and types of fears that
4 to 6-year-old children have about
the hospital. Diff e rences that exist in Hospital-Related Fears of a were 8 children (9%) who expressed
e x p ressions of fears in children who 4 to 6-Year-Old Child. no hospital-related fears at all, 34
a re in the hospital at the time of the In this study, the children expressed (38%) who expressed 1 to 3 fears, 20
interview and those who are in kinder- 29 diff e rent types of fears related to (22%) who expressed 4 to 6 fears, and
garten are described, and girls’ and the hospital. Data included a total of 28 (31%) who expressed more than 7
boys’ expressions of fear are com- 441 expressions of fear. The amount hospital-related fears.
p a red. of child-reported fears ranged from no Most fears were categorized in the
fears to 22 fears (mean = 4.9). There category of nursing interventions (see
Table 1). Half of the children re p o rt e d

PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2009/Vol. 35/No. 5 271


Table 2. fears, and 30% mentioned 4 to 6 fears.
The Amount of Hospital-Related Fears Almost half (48%) of the children in
the hospital mentioned more than 7
Children in Children in hospital-related fears (see Table 2).
Amount Kindergarten Hospital Total The mean of hospital-related fears
of Fears expressed by children in kindergarten
n % n % n % was 4.2 and by children in the hospi-
tal was 6.48. When examining the per-
0 8 13 – – 8 9
centage distribution of fears, children
1 to 3 fears 28 44 6 22 34 38 in the hospital expressed substantially
more fears than children in kinder-
4 to 6 fears 12 19 8 30 20 22
garten (see Table 3). Fears related to
More than 7 fears 15 24 13 48 28 31 bodily injuries and hospital facilities
o c c u rred more frequently in those
Total 63 100 27 100 90 100
children in kindergarten than in chil-
dren who were hospitalized. Among
the children in the hospital, 70% said
they were afraid of shots, and 29 men- (62%) participated in the study. The they were afraid of pain, 52% of shots
tioned tests, sample-taking or exami- total number of individual, hospital- as well as other nursing interventions,
nations, and other nursing interv e n- related fears expressed by girls was 41% of being admitted to the hospital,
tions. The second largest category 151, while boys expressed 290 indi- and 37% of sample-taking and tests.
consisted of fears of being a patient. vidual fears. The mean of the number The fears that the children in kinder-
M o re than half of all the children said of fears expressed by girls was 4.44 garten expressed most often were the
they were afraid of pain, and more and by boys 5.18. When examining fear of pain (51%), shots (49%), and
than a quarter of them feared being the percentage distribution of fears, sample-taking and tests (31%). When
admitted to a hospital. The third boys re p o rted slightly more fears than the fears of children in kindergarten
l a rgest category consisted of fears girls. Boys expressed statistically sig- were compared with the fears of chil-
caused by the developmental stage of nificant more fears toward the re l a- dren in the hospital using the Pearson
the child. In this category, the most tions between the child and the staff Chi-Square test, statistically signifi-
common fears were the fear of being (Mann-Whitney U = 739.500, p = cant diff e rences were found in the
left alone (n = 19) and fears created 0.017). There was no significant dif- fears of being admitted to the hospital
by the child’s own imagination (n = ference in the number of fears ( [1] = 12.021, p = 0.001), doctors
13). This category also included chil- between diff e rent age groups of chil- ( [1] = 4.114, p = 0.043), other nurs-
d re n ’s expressions of being afraid of d ren. ing interventions ( [1] = 6.805, p =
the boogeyman, ghosts, grimace 0.009), and operations ( [1] = 5.293,
masks, or their own “bad thoughts.” Differences in Hospital-Related p = 0.021) (see Table 3).
Of all the interviewed children, 61 Fears Between Children When examining children’s fears
expressed they were afraid of the Interviewed in Kindergarten and according to the categories based on
physical, social, or symbolic enviro n- Children Being Treated at the the typology of nursing domains, there
ment of the hospital. Forty childre n Hospital were also statistically significant dif-
said they were afraid of factors related Of all the children participating in ferences between fears of the children
to the child-staff relations, and 33 the study, 63 were children who were in the hospital and those who were in
w e re afraid of the unfamiliar enviro n- interviewed in kindergarten and 27 kindergarten. Children in the hospital
ment or the lack of information. w e re hospitalized children who were mentioned significantly more often
An examination of all individual interviewed in two hospital wards. Of fears related to being a patient (Mann-
fears expressed by the childre n the children who were interviewed in Whitney U = 586.5, p = 0.007), the
revealed the largest categories were kindergarten, 25 (40%) were girls and developmental stage of the child
the fear of pain and shots (57% and 38 (60%) boys, and among them were (Mann-Whitney U = 652.5, p = 0.028),
50%, respectively). More than 30% of 26 (41%) 4-year olds, 20 (32%) 5- the relationship between the child and
the children said they were afraid of year olds, and 17 (27%) 6-year olds. the staff (Mann-Whitney U = 614, p =
sample-taking and tests as well as Of the hospitalized children, 9 (33 %) 0.008), and nursing practice (Mann-
other nursing interventions. More than w e re girls and 18 (67 %) were boys, Whitney U = 584.5, p = 0.007) (see
20% of the children said they were and among them were 8 (30%) 4-year Table 4).
afraid of staying in a hospital, separa- olds, 10 (37%) 5-year olds, and 9
tion from family, being alone, and (33%) 6-year olds.
instruments. About 20% of the chil- C h i l d ren in kindergarten listed 0 to Discussion
d ren expressed fears caused by the 22 diff e rent hospital-related fears (see The results of this study revealed
lack of information, being admitted to Table 2). Eight (13%) participants in that 91% of preschoolers said they are
a hospital, the exercise of power by kindergarten expressed no fears at all, afraid of at least one subject related to
adults, symptoms of their disease, or but most (44%) healthy childre n the hospital or medical procedures.
new and unknown experiences. More e x p ressed 1 to 3 fears related to a The percentage is larger than in earli-
than 10% of the respondents said they hospital. Under a quarter of the chil- er studies (Rossen & McKeever,
w e re afraid of doctors, things created d ren who were interviewed in kinder- 1996). Among children interviewed in
by their own imagination, medicines garten expressed more than 7 fears. kindergarten, 9% expressed no fear,
and taking medicines, operations, the The number of hospital-re l a t e d 63% expressed 1 to 6 fears, and 24%
hospital premises, losing autonomy, fears expressed by children in the hos- expressed more than 7 fears related to
and injuries to the body (see Table 1). pital varied from 1 to 14 fears. Among the hospital. Among the children inter-
Thirty-four girls (38%) and 56 boys these children, 22% mentioned 1 to 3 viewed in the hospital, little more than

272 PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2009/Vol. 35/No. 5


Child-Reported Hospital Fears in 4 to 6-Year-Old Children

Ta ble 3.
The Amount of Hospital-Related Fears Expressed by Children in Kinderg a rten and in the Hospital

Kindergarten Hospital Chi-Square Tests


(n = 63) (n = 27) (d f 1)
n % n % Value p
Fears of Being a Patient
Pain 32 51 19 70 2.95 0.086
Staying in hospital 16 25 7 26 0.003 0.958
Being admitted to hospital 6 10 11 41 12.02 0.001
Symptoms of the disease 9 14 6 22 0.857 0.355
Bodily injuries 9 14 1 4 2.14 0.143
Becoming ill 4 6 2 7 0.034 0.854
Fears Caused by the Developmental Stage of the Child
Being left alone 11 17 8 30 1.69 0.195
Imagination 9 14 4 15 0.00 0.948
Loss of autonomy 6 10 4 15 0.53 0.464
Unfamiliar people 6 10 3 11 0.053 0.818
Other fears caused by the developmental stage 4 6 5 19 0.121 0.087
Fears Caused by Unfamiliar Environment or Lack of Information
Lack of information 12 19 6 22 0.12 0.730
New and unknown experiences 10 16 5 19 0.095 0.758
Physical Environment
I n s t ruments and equipment 12 19 7 26 0.54 464
Facilities of the hospital 8 13 3 11 0.044 0.833
Noises and smells of the hospital 5 8 4 15 0.99 0.319
Social and Symbolic Environment
Separation from the family 10 16 9 33 2.24 0.135
Other fears caused by the social and symbolic environment 2 3 – – 0.43 0.510
Child-Staff Relations
Exercise of power by adults 8 13 8 30 3.70 0.054
Doctors 6 10 7 26 4.11 0.043
Nurses 5 8 5 19 2.14 0.143
Nursing Interventions
Shots 31 49 14 52 0.008 0.927
Sample-taking and tests 19 31 10 37 0.409 0.522
Other nursing interventions 15 24 14 52 6.80 0.009
Operation 5 8 7 26 5.29 0.021
Medication 6 10 6 22 2.64 0.104

a half (52%) expressed 1 to 6 hospital- fear of shots, sample-taking and tests, mental stage of the child (14%). In this
related fears, and 48% more than 7 other nursing interventions, operations, category, children most often men-
fears. All children who were inter- and medication. Almost as many fears tioned the fear of being left alone in the
viewed in the hospital expressed at w e re categorized as fears of being a hospital. More than 20% of preschool-
least one fear related to the hospital. patient (28%), such as the fear of pain, ers mentioned they were afraid of being
According to this study, fears being admitted to the hospital, staying left alone in the hospital. Less than 10%
expressed most often by 4 to 6-year- in the hospital, and symptoms of a dis- of preschoolers expressed that they
old children were caused by the prac- ease. The third largest category con- f e a red new and unfamiliar experiences,
tice of nursing (29%), for example, the sisted of fears related to the develop- the lack of information, the physical

PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2009/Vol. 35/No. 5 273


Ta ble 4.
The Expressions of Fears Expressed by Children in Kinderg a rten and in Hospital

Expressions of Fear U-Test (Exact Sig. 1-tailed)


Kindergarten Hospital
Categories of Fear n n U p
Fears of being a patient 76 46 586.500 0.007
Fears caused by the developmental stage of child 36 24 652.500 0.028
Fears caused by unknown environment or lack of information 22 11 810.00 0.359
Physical environment 25 14 749.00 0.136
Social and symbolic environment 12 9 778.00 0.254
Child-staff relation 19 20 614.00 0.008
Nursing interventions 76 51 584.500 0.007

Figure 1. fears, with nursing treatments and


Hospital-Related Fears Reported by 4 to 6-Year-Old Children anaesthesia being most important. In
this respect, results were similar to the
results of the present study, in which
the main individual fears reported by 4
to 6-year-old children were the fears of
pain and shots. The result concerning
the fear of injections supports the find-
ings of earlier studies. According to the
study by Kettwich et al. (2007), 68% of
children suffer from needle phobia.
Findings from this study pull
together earlier information on the
hospital fears of young children, but
they also elicit new information. The
study showed that a 4 to 6-year-old
child is capable and willing to discuss
his or her fears with the interviewer.
C h i l d ren expressed 29 different hospi-
t a l - related fears. In the data, there
were 17 fears that had also been men-
tioned in earlier studies and 12 fears
that became evident in this study as
reported by the children. These fears
are familiar from child care literature
but were not re f e rred to in the 200
studies reviewed for the literature
review of this study. These 12 fears
that children in this study expressed
were a) staying in the hospital (26% of
children), b) being left alone in the
hospital (21%), c) being admitted to
the hospital (19%), d) new and unfa-
miliar experiences (17%), e) doctors
e n v i ronment of the hospital, and child- body, pain, restriction of self-determ i- (14%), f) the facilities (10%), g) smells
staff relations (see Figure 1). nation, and submission. Similar fears and noises of the hospital (12 %), h)
It is difficult to compare results con- also occurred in this study; however, nurses (11%), i) getting ill (7%), j)
c e rning the number of child-reported except for pain, children expressed hospital rules (2%), k) falling asleep in
hospital fears with earlier studies m o re fears related to nursing interv e n- the hospital (2%), and l) other patients
because only very little similar informa- tions, such as the fear of shots, sample- (1%). The only fears that occurred in
tion on preschoolers is available. In taking, and tests, and only seldom earlier studies but were not mentioned
Ivanoff’s (1999) study, nine 5 to 6- (10% to 16% of children) expressed by the children in this study were the
year-old children were interviewed other main fears in Ivanoff’s study. In fears of failure and being rejected.
about their hospital fears. According to the qualitative study by Flinkman and A main finding of this study was
Ivanoff´s study, the main themes of 5 to Salanterä (2004), 25 children ages 5 to that more fears were present in hospi-
6-year-old childre n ’s hospital fears 6 years of age were interviewed during talized children than in their peers,
were the unfamiliar environment, the a day surg e ry procedure. In this study, which may suggest that being treated
feeling of abandonment, injuries of the the children mentioned 24 different in the hospital increases the number of

274 PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2009/Vol. 35/No. 5


Child-Reported Hospital Fears in 4 to 6-Year-Old Children

h o s p i t a l - related fears in 4 to 6-year- was suitable for 4 to 6-year-old chil- ual ability to verbalize their feelings
old children. Children who were in the d ren. During the interv i e w, it was pos- and thoughts. However, pictures made
hospital reported fear of being admit- sible to take into account the child’s it easy for children to identify with the
ted to the hospital more often than vocabulary and individual way of hospital experience and discuss their
children interviewed in kindergarten. communicating. The pre requisite for a own thoughts. The pictures had
C h i l d ren in the hospital also reported successful interview was interaction cheery motifs and colors, which might
the interaction between children and between the child and the interviewer, d e c rease the tru s t w o rthiness of the
staff (especially between children and and also the confidential and friendly results. The use of other, perh a p s
doctors), nursing interventions, and a t m o s p h e rein which the child had the more realistic and possibly alarming
operations more often than children in courage to express his or her thoughts pictures might have brought out more
kindergarten. (Deering & Cody, 2002). hospital fears.
In this study, results were classified The questions were well-defined The interrater reliability of the inter-
into categories drawn from the and simple, and the interviewer used viewers was not established, another
domains of nursing (Kim, 2000; Kim c o n c rete and action-oriented words limitation of the study. However, the
& Kollak, 1999). Kim proposed a sys- familiar to the child. When forming t ru s t w o rthiness of the data collection
tematic framework that can be used to questions, the child typically answer- was assured by training the interv i e w-
examine elements in the fields of nurs- ing “yes,” “no,” or “I don’t know” was ers to the special characteristics of
ing. The typology is composed of four c o n s i d e red. It is also often difficult for interviewing children, requiring pilot
domains: client, client-nurse, practice, a child to disagree with adults; thus, interviews, and instructing the process
and environment (Kim, 2000; Kim & they may be likely to answer in the of the interview in advance in a sys-
Kollak, 1999). When examining the way they think is acceptable. The tematic way (by first author M.S.). All
present results according to the interviewer emphasized to the child interviewers had studied the nursing
domains of nursing, it is obvious that that everything he or she said was care of children and re s e a rch meth-
about a half of the fears (49%) can be important and that there were no ods, a fact that increases the trustwor-
categorized to the client domain: fears w rong answers (Burns & Grove, 2001; thiness of the results. They had stud-
related to being a patient, the develop- Carney et al., 2003; Docherty & ied the developmental stage of a 4 to
mental stage of the child, unfamiliar Sandelowski, 1999; Eder & 6-year-old child, especially the child’s
experiences, and lack of information. Fingerson, 2002; Zoppi & Epstein, way and ability to express him/herself
Another main domain that causes fear 2002). In this study, the use of pictures and interact with adults. Further, the
in children is nursing interventions b rought out rich information on hospi- t ru s t w o rthiness of the interviews, data
(29% of expressions). Less than 14% tal-related fears. The pictures served analysis, and reporting was assured
of all expressions of fears were related as a stimulating basis for the interview, by listening to all re c o rdings of the
to the facilities of the hospital. The and helped the child become intere s t- interviews and by checking that the
main individual fear in this category ed in and concentrate on the conver- transcripts had been made reliably (by
was the fear of separation from family. sation, as well as connect his or her first author M.S.). The interv i e w e r s ’
Fears related to the symbolic enviro n- own experiences to the subject and observations of the children were
ment of the hospital (for example, the express his or her own feelings added to the data to be analyzed in the
fear of rules and regulations) were (Carney et al., 2003; Doverborg & cases when needed to clarify the
mentioned by one child only. Earlier Pramling, 2000; Styles & Arizpe, child’s answer (Burns & Grove, 2001;
studies on the hospital fears of chil- 2001). Cohen & Knafl, 1993). Tru s t-
d ren using the domains of nursing for worthiness of the data collection
categorization were not available. The Limitations of the Study might have decreased by the different
categorization used in this study In this study, some factors limited circumstances of each interv i e w. It is
p roved to be functional. It was possi- tru s t w o rthiness during data collection. also difficult to estimate or control the
ble to classify all expressions of fear First, it was not possible to control the significance of how parents and nurs-
by the children, and no category was selection of children. Second, several es contributed to the interview.
left empty. Because the categorization interviewers were used at the data col- One aim of this study was to give
is based on the main domains of nurs- lection stage. Finally, because nurses the preschoolers themselves the
ing, these results can be used when p rovided parents with information on opportunity to discuss their hospital-
p reventing, identifying, and relieving the study, it was not possible to control related fears. Neither the earlier hospi-
the fears of hospitalized preschoolers. the distributed information, especially tal experiences of the children nor the
Only a few methods of data collec- because kindergarten personnel and socio-economical background of their
tion are suitable for young children, the nurses’ commitment to the study families were controlled, which may
t h rough which it is possible for the may have fluctuated. Conversely, limit the exploitability of the results.
child to describe his or her own tru s t w o rthiness was increased by the
thoughts and experiences. Some stud- first author and interviewers giving Conclusions/Clinical
ies have used draw-and-tell conversa- oral and written information on the Implications
tions (Driessnack, 2006), children’s study to nurses (Burns & Gro v e , The results of this study revealed
drawings (Brewer et al., 2006), and 2001). Results of this study cannot be that more than 90% of children were
different Visual Analogue, Fear, generalized to all 4 to 6-year-old chil- afraid of at least one thing in a hospi-
Anxiety, or Aversion Scales (Kettwich d ren because of the data collection tal. These results suggest a gre a t
et al., 2007). In this study, researchers method. number of hospital-related fears
were interested in hospital-re l a t e d Differences in the children’s per- among 4 to 6-year-old children. In a
fears expressed by the children them- sonality and developmental stage hospital, the main fears of children are
selves, which is why data were collect- might decrease the trustworthiness of the fears of pain, different treatments
ed by semi-stru c t u red interviews sup- the results. The children’s ability to (especially injections), sample-taking,
ported by pictures. Because of its flex- concentrate and their interest in the and tests. Alleviating pain caused by a
ibility, the semi-stru c t u red interv i e w subject varied as well as their individ- disease or treatment decreases the

PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2009/Vol. 35/No. 5 275


child’s fear and increases his or her quences of these fears to small chil- & I. Holstein (Eds.), Handbook of inter -
feeling of safety. In addition to pain d ren. Research is also needed on the viewing research: Context & method. (p.
medication, the child needs support coping skills that children use when 181). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
f rom parents and staff, information, they try to cope with their fears, the Publications.
Faux, S.A., Walsh, M., & Deatrick, J. (1988).
and guidance. There are also methods content of these skills, and how nurs- Intensive interviewing with children and
suitable for the child’s developmental ing staff can support the coping of the a d o l e s c e n t s. We s t e rn Journal of
stage that the children can use to alle- child. Nursing Research, 10(2), 180-194.
viate pain and fear. These methods Flinkman, T., & Salanterä, S. (2004). The
(such as diff e rent types of relaxation) fears of a preschool child at day sur-
affect the child’s emotions and cogni- References gery. (Finnish, English abstra c t )
tion, allowing the child be an active Alsop-Shields, L., & Mohay, H. (2001). John Hoitotiede, 16(3), 121-131.
participant in his or her own care Bowlby and James Robertson: Theorists, Gazal, G., & Mackie, I. (2007). Distress relat-
scientists and crusaders for improve- ed to dental ex t raction for children under
(Salanterä, 1999). The presence of a
ments in the care of children in hospital. general anaesthesia and their parents.
p a rent and the fact that the child does Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35(1), 50- European Journal of Paediatric
not have to be alone in a hospital 58. D e n t i s t ry, 8(1), 7-12.
d e c reases his or her hospital fear. B evan, J., Johnston, C., Haig, M.J., Gozal, D., Drenger, B., Levin, P., Kadari, A., &
Hospital fear will also decrease if the Tousignant, G., Lucy, S., Kirnon, V., et Gozal, Y. (2004). A pediatric
physical environment of the hospital is al. (1990). Preoperative parental anxiety sedation/anesthesia program with dedi-
made entertaining and safe. Be- predicts behav i o u ral and emotional cated care by anesthesiologists and
coming familiar with the instruments responses to induction of anaesthesia nurses for procedures outside the oper-
and machines of the hospital also in children. Canadian Journal of ating room. Journal of Pediatri c s,
Anaesthesia, 37(2), 177-182. 145(1), 47-52.
d e c reases the child’s fear of the phys-
B r ewe r, S., Glenditsch, S., Syblik, C., Gullone, E. (2000). The development of nor-
ical environment of the hospital. Tietjens, M., & Vacik, H. (2006). mal fear: A century of research. Clinical
Knowledge evokes security and confi- Pe d i a t ric anxiety: Child life intervention Psychology Review, 20(4), 429.
dence in preschoolers and decreases in day surgery. Journal of Pediatric Ivanoff, P., Laijärvi, H., & Åstedt-Kurki, P.
unnecessary hospital fear. Nursing, 21(1), 13-22. (1999). Pre-school children’s ex p e ri-
Being admitted to a hospital is an Bricher, G. (1999). Children and qualitative ence of hospital fear (Finnish, English
event that may increase the fears of a research methods: A review of literature abstract). Hoitotiede, 11(5), 272-281.
4 to 6-year-old child. Children who related to interv i ew and interpretive Jost, K. (1996). Nursing standards for child
have personal experience of being processes. Nursing Research, 6(4), 65- development. The American Journal of
77. M a t e rnal/Child Nursing, 21(2), 67-71.
admitted to a hospital describe more
Burns, N., & Grove, S. (2001). The practice of Kain, Z., Mayes, L., O´Connor, T., & Cicchetti,
fears than healthy children in kinder- nursing research: Conduct, critique, and D. (1996). Preoperative anxiety in chil-
garten. There is no single method to utilization (4th ed.). Philadelphia: W.B. dren: Predictors and outcomes.
remove all hospital fears, but a pre re q- Saunders. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent
uisite for eliminating fear is the staff’s Carney, T., Murphy, S., McClure, J., Bishop, Medicine, 150(12), 1238-1245.
a w a reness of the issues that may E., Kerr, C., Parker, J., et al. (2003). Kettwich, S., Sibbitt, W., Brandt, J., Johnson,
cause children fear, and staff’s willing- Children’s views of hospitalization: An C., Wong, C., & Bankhurst, A. (2007).
ness to prevent and recognize possible exploratory study of data collection. Needle phobia and stress-reducing
fears help and support the child in cop- Journal of Child Health Care, 7(1), 27- medical devices in pediatric and adult
40. c h e m o t h e ra py patients. Journal of
ing. The fears of the child can also be
Cohen, M.Z., & Knafl, K.A. (1993). Evaluating Pe d i a t ric Oncology Nursing, 24(1), 20-
p revented and alleviated by giving the qualitative research. In P. Munhall & C. 28.
parents information on the child’s hos- Boyd (Eds.), Nursing research. A quali - Kim, H. (2000). The nature of theoretical
pital fears and on the meaning of sep- tative perspective (2nd ed.) (pp. 476- thinking in nu r s i n g. New Yo rk : Springer
aration from parents to the child. 92). N ew Yo rk : National League for Publishing Company.
Guiding the parents to support their Nursing. Kim, H., & Kollak, I. (1999). Nursing theories:
child is also important. Parental aware- C oy n e, I. (1998). Researching children: Conceptual and philosophical founda -
ness and participation in their child’s Some methodological and ethical con- tions. New York: S p ringer Publishing
hospitalization is essential for eff e c t i v e sidera t i o n s. Journal of Clinical Nursing, Company.
7(5), 409-416. Koenig, K., Chesla, C., & Kennedy, C. (2003).
coping. It would be desirable that a
Coyne, I. (2006). Consultation with children in Parents’ perspectives of asthma crisis
children’s hospital would be a place hospital: Children, parents´ and nurses´ hospital management in infants and tod-
that promotes the healing of the child p e r s p e c t i ve s. Journal of Clinical dlers: An interpretive view through the
and his or her feeling of safety, not a Nursing, 15(1), 61-71. lens of attachment theory. J o u rnal of
place that increases fear. Deering, C., & Cody, D. (2002). Commu- Pe d i a t ric Nursing, 18(4), 233-243.
This article describes the amount nicating effectively with children and Kristensson-Hallström, I., Elander, G., &
and content of hospital fears reported adolescents. American Journal of Malmfors, G. (1997). Increased parental
by 4 to 6-year-old children them- Nursing, 102(3), 34-41. participation in a paediatric surgical
selves. The information is useful when Docherty, S., & Sandelowski, M. (1999). day-care unit. Journal of Clinical
Focus on qualitative methods: I n t e r- Nursing, 6(4), 297-302.
trying to understand children’s experi-
viewing children. Research in Nursing & Lahikainen, A., Kra av, I., Kirmanen, T., &
ences of fear and anxiety in a hospital, Health, 22(2), 177-185. Taimalu, M. (2006). Child-parent agr e e-
during certain nursing processes, at D overborg, E., & Pramling, S. (2000). Att ment in the assessment of young chil-
the doctor’s or dentist’s office, and in förstå barns tankar. Metodik för barnin - dren’s fears: A comparative perspective.
child health clinics. The inform a t i o n tervjuer (3rd ed.). Stockholm, Sweden, J o u rnal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
can also be useful in preventing, iden- Liber AB. 37(1), 100-119.
tifying, and alleviating the hospital fear Driessnack, M. (2006). Draw-and-tell conve r- LeRoy, S., Elixson, M., O´Brien, P., Tong, E.,
of healthy or sick children. More sations with children about fear. Turpin, S., & Uzark, K. (2003).
re s e a rch is needed on the situations Qualitative Health Research, 16( 1 0 ) ,
1414-1436. continued on page 303
and contexts that give rise to chil-
Eder, D., & Fingerson, L. (2002). Interviewing
d re n ’s hospital fears and the conse- children and adolescents. In J. Gubrium

276 PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2009/Vol. 35/No. 5


Hospital Fears Rennick, J., Johnston, C., Dougherty, G., Platt,
R., & Ritchie, J. (2002). Children´s psy-
continued from page 276 chological response after critical illness
and exposure to invasive technology.
Recommendations for preparing children J o u rnal of Developmental & Behavioral
and adolescents for invasive cardiac pro- Pediatrics, 23(3), 133-144.
cedures. Circulation, 108(20), 2550- Romino, S., Keatley, V., Secrest, J., & Good, K.
2564. (2005). Home study program. Parental
Mahajan, L., Wyllie, R., Steffen, R., Kay, M., presence during anesthesia induction in
Kitaoka, G., Dettorre, J., et al. (1998). children. AORN Journal, 81(4), 780-792.
The effects of a psychological prepara- Rossen, B., & McKeever, P. (1996).The behav-
tion program on anxiety in children and ior of preschoolers during and after bri ef
adolescents undergoing gastrointestinal surgical hospitalizations. Issues in
endoscopy. Journal of Pediatric Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 19(2),
Gastroenterology & Nutrition, 27(2), 161- 121-133.
165. Salanterä, S. (1999). C a ring for children in
Majstorovic, M., & Veerkamp, J. (2004). pain – Nursing knowledge, activities and
Relationship between needle phobia and outcomes. Annales Universitatis
dental anxiety. J o u rnal of Dentistry for Turkuensis, Sarja D 345, Painosalama
Children, 71(3), 201-205. Oy, Turku.
Majstorovic, M., & Veerkamp, J. (2005). Snyder, B. (2004). Preventing treatment inter-
Developmental changes in dental anxi- ference: Nurses´ and parents´ interve n-
ety in a normative population of Dutch tion strategies. Pe d i a t ric Nursing, 30(1),
children. European Journal of Paediatric 31-40.
Dentistry, 6(1), 30-34. Spradley J. (1979). The ethnographic inter -
Morse, J., & Richards, L. (2002). README first view. New York: Harcourt Brace
for a user´s guide to qualitative methods. Jovanovich College Publisher.
New Delhi: Sage Publications. Styles, M., & Ari z p e, E. (2001). “A gorilla with
Munhall, P. (1999). Ethical considerations in grandpa´s eyes:” How children interpret
qualitative research. In P. Munhall, visual texts. A case study of Anthony
Nursing research. A qualitative perspec - Browne´s “Zoo.” Children’s Literature in
tive (pp. 395-408). New Yo rk : National Education, 32(4), 261-281.
League for Nursing. Walworth, D. (2005). Procedural-support
Nicastro, E., & Whetsell, M. (1999). Children´s music therapy in the healthcare setting: A
fears. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 14(6), cost-effectiveness analysis. Journal of
392-401. Pediatric Nursing, 20(4), 276-284.
Nordfeldt, S.,& Ludvigsson, J. (2005). Fear Young, K. (2005). Pediatric procedural pain.
and other disturbances of severe hy p o- Annals of Emergency Medicine, 45(2),
glycaemia in children and adolescents 160-171.
with type 1 diabetes mellitus. J o u rnal of Zoppi, K., & Epstein, R. (2002). Interviewing in
Pediatric Endocrinology,18(1), 83-91. medical settings. In J. G u b rium, & J.
Pelander, T., & Leino-Kilpi, H. (2004). Quality of Holstein (Eds.), Handbook of interview -
p e d i a t ric nursing care: Children´s ex p e c- ing research: Context & method ( p. 355).
tations. Issues in Comprehensive Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Pediatric Nursing, 27(3), 139-351.
Pelander, T., Nuutila, L., Salanterä, S., &
Leino-Kilpi, H. (2006). Children as info rm- Additional Reading
ants: A review of Finnish academic the- Sieben-Hein, D., & Steinmiller, E. (2005).
ses in nursing science (Finnish, English Wo rking with complex care patients.
abstract). Hoitotiede, 18(4), 162-174. J o u rnal of Pediatric Nursing, 20(5), 389-
Polit, D., Beck, C., & Hungler, B. (2004). 395.
Essentials of nursing research: Methods,
appraisal, and utilization (5th ed.).
Philadelphia: Lippincott.

PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2009/Vol. 35/No. 5 303


Copyright of Pediatric Nursing is the property of Jannetti Publications, Inc. and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

View publication stats

You might also like