You are on page 1of 20

Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8): 639-658

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW 1170-229X/12/0008-0639/$49.95/0

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved.

A Systematic Review of Amnestic


and Non-Amnestic Mild Cognitive
Impairment Induced by Anticholinergic,
Antihistamine, GABAergic and
Opioid Drugs
Cara Tannenbaum,1,2 Amélie Paquette,2 Sarah Hilmer,3 Jayna Holroyd-Leduc4
and Ryan Carnahan5
1 Faculties of Pharmacy and Medicine, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
2 Centre de Recherche de l’Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
3 Sydney Medical School, Faculty of Medicine, University of Sydney, Royal North Shore Hospital, , Sydney,
NSW, Australia
4 Departments of Medicine and Community Health Sciences, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
5 Department of Epidemiology, The University of Iowa College of Public Health, Iowa City, IA, USA

Abstract Background: Mild cognitive deficits are experienced by 18% of community-


dwelling older adults, many of whom do not progress to dementia. The effect
of commonly used medication on subtle impairments in cognitive function
may be under-recognized.
Objective: The aim of the review was to examine the evidence attributing
amnestic or non-amnestic cognitive impairment to the use of medication with
anticholinergic, antihistamine, GABAergic or opioid effects.
Methods: MEDLINE and EMBASE were searched for randomized, double-
blind, placebo-controlled trials of adults without underlying central nervous
system disorders who underwent detailed neuropsychological testing prior to
and after oral administration of drugs affecting cholinergic, histaminergic,
GABAergic or opioid receptor pathways. Seventy-eight studies were identified,
reporting 162 trials testing medication from the four targeted drug classes. Two
investigators independently appraised study quality and extracted relevant data
on the occurrence of amnestic, non-amnestic or combined cognitive deficits
induced by each drug class. Only trials using validated neuropsychological tests
were included. Quality of the evidence for each drug class was assessed based on
consistency of results across trials and the presence of a dose-response gradient.
Results: In studies of short-, intermediate- and long-acting benzodiazepine
drugs (n = 68 trials), these drugs consistently induced both amnestic and non-
amnestic cognitive impairments, with evidence of a dose-response relation-
ship. H1-antihistamine agents (n = 12) and tricyclic antidepressants (n = 15)
640 Tannenbaum et al.

induced non-amnestic deficits in attention and information processing. Non-


benzodiazepine derivatives (n = 29) also produced combined deficits, but less
consistently than benzodiazepine drugs. The evidence was inconclusive for
the type of cognitive impairment induced by different bladder relaxant anti-
muscarinics (n = 9) as well as for narcotic agents (n = 5) and antipsychotics
(n = 5). Among healthy volunteers >60 years of age, low doses of commonly
used medications such as lorazepam 0.5 mg, oxybutynin immediate release
5 mg and oxycodone 10 mg produced combined deficits.
Conclusion: Non-amnestic mild cognitive deficits are consistently induced by
first-generation antihistamines and tricyclic antidepressants, while benzo-
diazepines provoke combined amnestic and non-amnestic impairments. Risk-
benefit considerations should be discussed with patients in order to enable an
informed choice about drug discontinuation or substitution to potentially
reverse cognitive adverse effects.

1. Introduction language or executive function. Single-domain MCI


is more frequent than multiple-domain MCI,
The impact of pharmaceutical substances on with non-amnestic and amnestic subtypes occur-
the cognitive function of older adults is of increas- ring with equal frequency.[9,10]
ing concern to both patients and their healthcare Population-based studies suggest that up to
providers. Ninety percent of persons aged 65 years 40% of persons who show initial deficits on neu-
and older take at least one prescription med- ropsychological testing revert to normal at 5-year
ication, and of these, almost half consume five follow-up, with only 15% per year going on to
drugs or more.[1,2] At the same time, up to 18% of develop dementia.[3,11-13] The effects of an under-
community-dwelling adults aged 65 years and lying disease, depression, resolving delirium, stress
older report memory impairment or have objec- or anxiety, and other conditions that can indepen-
tively demonstrable cognitive deficits that are dently elicit subjective and/or objective cognitive
greater than expected for their age but do not impairments may partially account for the fluctu-
significantly interfere with function.[3-5] Mild cogni- ating nature of neuropsychological test scores over
tive impairment (MCI) is a relatively new concept time.[14,15] Consumption of medication that can neg-
that has been introduced over the past 10 years to atively affect cognition is an alternate explanation
classify memory impairment that falls in the grey that may be under-recognized because of its infre-
zone between normality and dementia.[3] Alter- quent assessment in otherwise healthy individuals.
natively coined ‘benign senescent forgetfulness,’ Pharmacological interference has the potential
‘age-associated memory impairment’ or ‘cognitive to disrupt normal neurotransmission in areas of
impairment, no dementia,’[6-8] current guidelines the brain responsible for both amnestic and non-
divide these mild cognitive deficits into amnestic amnestic cognitive functioning.[16,17] The cholinergic
or non-amnestic subtypes, with each category sub- system projects to the cortex and hippocampus,
divided according to whether the objective cog- and is predominantly involved with memory stor-
nitive deficits are identified within a single or age and retrieval, as well as arousal, percep-
multiple cognitive domain.[3,9] Amnestic MCI tion and attention.[18,19] Blockade of histamine H1
predominantly affects short-term and/or long- receptors can produce sedation, diminished atten-
term memory, whereas non-amnestic MCI is char- tion and reduced psychomotor speed.[20] The in-
acterized by disturbances of attention/concentration, hibitory amino acid g-aminobutyric acid (GABA)
information processing and psychomotor speed, and certain opioid peptides may also play a role in

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
Drug-Induced Cognitive Impairment 641

modulating cognitive performance.[21-23] Although used were ‘hypnotics and sedatives’, ‘narcotics’,
other neurotransmitters exist, the cholinergic, ‘antipsychotic agents’, ‘cholinergic antagonists’,
histaminergic, GABAergic and opioid receptor ‘GABA agonists’, ‘histamine H1 antagonists’,
pathways are hypothesized to be most commonly ‘opiate alkaloids’, ‘anticonvulsants’, ‘antidepressive
implicated in modulation of learning, memory, agents’, ‘anti-anxiety agents’, ‘antiparkinson agents’
attention and executive function (figure 1).[16,17] and ‘bronchodilator agents’. The MeSH terms
The purpose of this review was to systematically for cognitive impairment that were used with the
examine the evidence attributing objectively mea- subheadings ‘chemically induced’, ‘drug effects’,
sured amnestic or non-amnestic cognitive deficits and ‘etiology’ when available included ‘amnesia’,
to the use of medication acting on cholinergic, ‘cognition disorders’, ‘dementia’, ‘memory dis-
histaminergic, GABAergic and opioid receptor orders’, ‘cognition’ or ‘memory’. A search in natural
pathways in otherwise healthy adults. language in all fields was added for the terms
‘cognitive impairment’, ‘cognitive dysfunction’,
‘memory loss’, ‘cognitive decline’, ‘memory decline’,
2. Methods
‘memory deficits’ and ‘cognitive effects’. Exclusions
2.1 Data Sources and Searches
were imposed for the exploded terms of ‘delirium’
and ‘encephalopathy’. In EMBASE, exploded
MEDLINE (OvidSP) was searched from 1948 subject heading drug terms were ‘hypnotic agent’,
to October 2011. EMBASE (OvidSP) was searched ‘sedative agent’, ‘narcotic agent’, ‘atypical anti-
from 1980 to October 2011. The search strategy psychotic agent’, ‘neuroleptic agent’, ‘cholinergic
employed an exploded search option that com- receptor blocking agent’, ‘4-aminobutyric acid re-
bined drug terms with selected terms for cognitive ceptor stimulating agent’, ‘benzodiazepine receptor
impairment. In MEDLINE, the MeSH drug terms affecting agent’, ‘histamine H1 receptor antagonist’,

Pathways
Cholinergic
Histaminergic
GABAergic
Opioid
Frontal cortex
Attention

Cingulum

Striatum
Memory Thalamus
Corpus callorum
Memory
Alertness
Pre-frontal cortex
Memory
Executive function
Visual cortex

Amygdala
Memory
Cerebellum
Memory
Hypothalamus
Alertness
Arousal
Alertness
Hippocampus
Memory
Attention

Fig. 1. Cholinergic, histaminergic, GABAergic and opioid receptor pathways mediating cognitive function. GABA = g-aminobutyric acid.

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
642 Tannenbaum et al.

‘opiate’, ‘anticonvulsive agent’, ‘antidepressant ports) and language (English only). Additional
agent’, ‘anxiolytic agent’, ‘antiparkinson agent’ articles were identified from manually searching
and ‘bronchodilating agent’. Exploded subject the bibliographies of retrieved articles. A flow-
headings terms for cognitive impairment included chart of the identification, screening, eligibility
‘memory disorder’, ‘cognitive defect’, ‘confusion’, and inclusion process is depicted in figure 2.
‘memory’, ‘cognition’ and ‘amnesia’. Studies were
limited to humans and adults 18 years and older, 2.2 Study Screening
and conservative limits were imposed for study
type (all phases of clinical trials, randomized Two reviewers independently screened the ti-
controlled trials, controlled clinical trials, multi- tles and abstracts of each of the articles identified
centre studies, evaluation studies and case re- in the search process according to the following

8062 records identified through


database searching:
3153 MEDLINE
4909 EMBASE

7118 records after duplicates removed

6746 records excluded


Population with dementia or other
underlying central nervous system
7118 records screened disorders, mixed nursing home population,
no evidence of neuropsychological testing,
co-administration of drugs, intravenous
use, non-experimental study design

311 full-text articles excluded


• Condition that affects central nervous system (103)
• Drug combination or duplicate study (20)
• Not orally administered (37)
372 full-text articles assessed
• No validated cognitive measures (18)
for eligibility
• Wrong drug class (28)
• Not a randomized, placebo-controlled study (73)
• Not healthy volunteers, excessive dose (7)
• Lack of documentation of concomitant drug use (18)

17 additional records identified


through hand or reference
searching
78 studies included in this review

GABA GABA Anticholinergic Anticholinergic


benzodiazepines non-benzodiazepines bladder relaxant drugs tricyclic antidepressants
63 single-dose trials 27 single-dose trials 3 single-dose trials 10 single-dose trials
5 repeated-dose trials 2 repeated-dose trials 6 repeated-dose trials 5 repeated-dose trials

H1 antihistamines H1 antihistamines Mixed


Opioids
first generation second generation mechanisms
5 single-dose trials
11 single-dose trials 14 single-dose trials 3 single-dose trials
0 repeated-dose trials
2 repeated-dose trials 4 repeated-dose trials 2 repeated-dose trials

Fig. 2. Flowchart of study identification, screening, eligibility and inclusion. GABA = g-aminobutyric acid.

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
Drug-Induced Cognitive Impairment 643

criteria: (i) study of human adults aged 18 years taking other central nervous system medications
and older; (ii) medication use affecting choliner- at the time of testing; and (vii) use of a random-
gic, histaminergic, GABAergic (benzodiazepine ized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial de-
or non-benzodiazepine) or opioid receptor path- sign. Medications were included if they could be
ways; (iii) measurement of cognitive outcomes; classified as having anticholinergic, antihistamine,
(iv) community-dwelling persons; and (v) absence GABAergic or opiate effects according to the
of schizophrenia, psychosis or other mental health CPS 2010 (Compendium of Pharmaceuticals and
disorders such as established dementia or delir- Specialities from the Canadian Pharmacists As-
ium. Only studies in volunteers without under- sociation)[24] or Lexi-Comp’s Drug Information
lying disorders affecting cognition were included, Handbook.[25] Neuropsychological tests measur-
as the intent was to study the impact of drug ad- ing amnestic or non-amnestic cognitive domains
ministration on repeat cognitive performance, that were described and referenced in a published
independent of potential effects of an underly- compendium of neuropsychological assessment
ing disease. Studies conducted in nursing home techniques were considered to be valid methods
residents were excluded, as it was impossible of cognitive outcome assessment.[26] Driving eval-
to distinguish cognitive test results from in- uations and physical function examinations (e.g.
dividuals with and without established cogni- body sway, balance tests) were not included.
tive dysfunction (e.g. dementia). Discrepancies Validated neuropsychological tests assessing am-
were resolved by a third reviewer or by consensus nestic domains, namely short-term memory and/
discussion. or delayed recall included Buschke’s selective re-
minding test, word list recall, story or paragraph
2.3 Study Selection
recall, digit span test, hidden objects and item
recognition tasks. Validated tests assessing non-
Full-text articles that passed the screening pro- amnestic domains included those assessing reaction
cess were assessed by two independent reviewers time/attention and concentration, specifically choice
for eligiblity for inclusion in the systematic re- reaction time, simple reaction time, discriminant
view. In order to exclude studies with biased re- reaction time, d2 task, critical fusion flicker test,
sults, methods for handling confounding, such as finger tapping, memory scanning speed, the digit-
randomization, allocation concealment, restric- symbol substitution test, digit-digit coding, digit
tion of participants to healthy volunteers, exclu- vigilance task, visual search tasks, tests of divided
sion of persons with underlying conditions that attention, and the continuous performance test.
could affect cognition, and documentation that Non-amnestic psychomotor/executive functions
no other central nervous system-active medica- were evaluated using validated tasks including
tions were being taken, were considered during the Stroop, complex figure tests, Groton maze
the study selection process. Studies that satisfied and the pegboard task.
the following criteria were included: (i) use of oral
medication; (ii) no drug combinations; (iii) medica- 2.4 Data Extraction
tions exerting pronounced anticholinergic, anti-
histamine, GABAergic or opiate effects; (iv) clear Data extraction was performed for each eligi-
documentation of cognitive outcomes, using de- ble study by two reviewers using a standardized
tailed validated neuropsychological tests (use data extraction sheet. The data extraction sheet
of the Mini-Mental State Examination alone was queried details of the population studied, in-
excluded); (v) healthy volunteer participants who cluding the sample size and age of the partici-
were without seizure disorders, stroke, Parkinson’s pants, the study design, the molecule tested, the
disease, major depression, anxiety, insomnia, dose and duration of medication use (single use
chronic pain or other conditions that could affect or repeated dosing), the timing of administration
performance on cognitive test batteries; (vi) clear of the neuropsychological assessments, the type
documentation that study participants were not of cognitive tests, and the results of the cognitive

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
644 Tannenbaum et al.

tests. Many studies reported more than one trial firm conclusions could be drawn. Whenever pos-
arm, often with different drugs or drug doses. sible, differences between young adults and those
Data for each trial arm were extracted separately. over age 60 years were compared. The results
For articles that reported the evaluation of from single-dose studies were compared with
multiple drugs from the same or different classes those from steady-state studies to try to ascertain
of medication, data were only extracted for whether tolerance to cognitive effects developed
those drugs with anticholinergic, antihistamine, over time. To calculate an effect size for each
GABAergic, or opioid receptor effects. Documen- drug class, we recorded the magnitude of dif-
tation of patients lost to follow-up was recorded. ference in test scores between each agent and
To evaluate whether a given drug caused cogni- placebo, and the standard deviation/standard
tive impairment in each individual trial, a change error within each group. In 31 of 78 publications
in the results of the neuropsychological tests from (39.7%), the means with or without standard de-
baseline to study endpoint was compared be- viations were only graphically represented in a
tween treatment and placebo arms. A drug was figure over multiple time points, or only p-values
considered to cause impairment if the mean group were indicated in the figures or embedded within
change in score on a cognitive task was reported the text to describe whether differences were sig-
as being significantly worse (p < 0.05) in the drug nificant or not. Of the remaining 47 publications
treatment arm compared with in the placebo arm. where actual test scores and standard deviations/
In many but not all studies, adjustments were standard errors of the test scores from the placebo
made for multiple comparisons. and drug groups were explicitly listed in a table or
within the text, there was wide variability in the
2.5 Data Analysis
different drugs, doses and time windows being test-
ed within the same drug class. For this reason, the
Descriptive statistics were used to characterize quality of the data was judged insufficient to quan-
the trials that were retained for qualitative synthe- tify effect sizes for the different drug classes as a
sis. Cognitive impairments were grouped under group. Short-acting benzodiazepines were defined
amnestic or non-amnestic subtypes. The quality as those having a median half-life of 1–12 hours
of evidence for each drug class was rated accord- according to Lexi-Comp’s Drug Information Hand-
ing to the recommended Grading of Recommen- book.[25] Intermediate-acting benzodiazepines were
dations Assessment, Development and Evaluation classified as having a median half-life of 12–40
(GRADE) approach,[27,28] where judgements re- hours, and long-acting benzodiazepines as having
flect the extent of confidence that specific drug median half-lives of 40–250 hours.
classes can provoke amnestic or non-amnestic
cognitive deficits. Judgements were assessed based 3. Results
on consistency of the results across trials, whether
a dose-response gradient was present, or if het- The primary search identified 8062 citations.
erogeneous results could be explained by plau- We considered 7118 citations after duplicates
sible confounding, such as an age effect.[27] A were removed. A total of 372 articles were retrieved
rating of ‘high quality’ was assigned if the data for full text review. A further 17 articles were
was consistent and a dose-response relationship added from searching the reference lists of re-
was present. Ratings of ‘moderate quality’ were trieved articles or through expert recommenda-
assigned when the findings were relatively con- tion. Seventy-eight publications met inclusion
stant across studies and discrepancies could be criteria for qualitative synthesis. A flowchart of the
explained by an age or drug effect. ‘Low quality’ identification, screening, eligibility and inclusion
evidence ratings were assigned when the results of process is depicted in figure 2. A total of 162 trials
different studies were mixed, if the studies were of different drugs and doses were reported in the
few or had small sample sizes, if the drug doses 78 publications retained for qualitative synthesis.
that were tested were excessively high, or if no Twenty percent of trials used a parallel group

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
Drug-Induced Cognitive Impairment 645

design. The remainder used a crossover trial de- deficits.[30,32,33,37,38,66,68-76] One 7-day study with
sign, generally with a 1-week washout period be- zolpidem showed no residual deficits.[75] Hetero-
tween drugs. Eighty percent tested single doses of geneity across studies could not be explained by
each medication and evaluated cognitive func- different drugs, doses, the type of neuropsycho-
tions immediately before and within 2–24 hours logical test, the analysis method or the age of the
post-administration of each pharmaceutical participants.
agent. Less than one-third of trials specifically
studied persons aged 60 years and older, or strat- 3.2 Anticholinergic Medication
ified the results by age group. Half of all studies
Two classes of anticholinergic agents were
had very small sample sizes (n £ 12), as is typical
studied: antimuscarinic agents used for the treat-
with medication challenge experiments.
ment of overactive bladder and the tricyclic anti-
depressants (table II). Three single-dose studies
3.1 GABAergic Medication and six steady-state trials of antimuscarinic agents
showed inconsistent effects, with impairments
GABAergic medication was analysed in two
predominantly attributable to the use of oxy-
groups: benzodiazepine derivatives (classical
butynin.[77-80] Amnestic and non-amnestic defi-
sedative-hypnotic agents such as lorazepam, flu-
cits varied widely across different drugs, doses
mazepam) and non-benzodiazepine derivatives
and durations of administration, with the most
(newer hypnotic medication such as zolpidem,
consistent effects seen during tests of divided at-
zopiclone and zaleplon) [table I]. Short-acting
tention and reaction time. All participants were
benzodiazepines consistently induced decrements
aged 60 years or older, so heterogeneity could not
on both amnestic and non-amnestic cognitive
be attributed to age. Two long-term studies re-
function tests, with evidence of a dose-response
ported significant loss to follow-up.[79,80] The
gradient.[29-42] Collie et al.[29] calculated a number
quality of evidence could only be judged as low-
needed to harm of 2 for midazolam 1.75 mg, and
to-moderate. The tricyclic antidepressants pro-
close to 1 for the higher 5.25 mg dose, even in
vided more robust evidence that anticholinergic
patients who did not report subjective impair-
activity deleteriously affects non-amnestic func-
ment. A similar profile of evidence implicated
tions, with deficits in attention and reaction time
intermediate-acting benzodiazepines.[39,41-61] Con-
induced in all single-dose studies.[81-87,93] Amnes-
sistent findings predominantly affecting memory
tic impairments occurred rarely and only at higher
storage, but also impacting attention, reaction
doses. Steady-state trials provided evidence of
time and specific psychomotor functions, char-
diminishing effects on non-amnestic function over
acterized single- and repeated-dose studies, without
time, with potential development of tolerance,
development of complete tolerance over 3 weeks
particularly in younger individuals.[88-92]
administration.[58] Adults aged 60 years and older
were more sensitive to low doses of benzodia- 3.3 H1 Antihistamines
zepines such as 0.5 mg of lorazepam than younger
adults.[43,44] The evidence implicating long-acting Consumption of first-generation H1 receptor
benzodiazepines was of slightly lower quality, blockers produced consistent decrements in non-
with inconsistencies that could only partially be amnestic tasks requiring attention and vigilance
explained by an age effect: younger adults show- after single-dose use in ten studies, with only one
ing fewer impairments than older adults on tests single-dose study of 25 mg showing no effect
of delayed recall, concentration, reaction time (table III).[44,77,94-100] Repeated use over 5 days
and tracking ability.[24,34,48,62-68] led to tolerance in one group of young adults,
Evidence implicating the non-benzodiazepine while only partial tolerance developed in another
GABAergic derivatives was only of moderate group of slightly older adults 51 years or younger,
quality, as only half of all studies reported sig- although doses of diphenhydramine were high
nificant amnestic and/or non-amnestic cognitive in the latter study (25 mg four times daily), and

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
Table I. Data of included studies for GABAergic benzodiazepine and non-benzodiazepine derivatives

646
Drugs and doses (mg) Sample size (n) Duration Neuropsychological tests Amnestic Non-amnestic impairments
[tested in the elderly]a impairments
GABAergic benzodiazepines
Midazolam 28[29] Single dose Delayed recall, Groton maze, Delayed recall, Groton maze at 5.25 mg
1.75, 5.25[29] recognition task recognition at both
doses

Triazolam 18,[30,37] 23,[32] Single dose Word lists, recognition, delayed recall, All tasks, all doses, DSST at 0.125 mg and 0.25 mg in 5/6 studies,
0.125,[30,31] 0.25,[30-37] 24,[33,35] 47,[31] verbal fluency, DS, DSST, CPT, CRT 7/7 studies 0.5 mg in 2/2 studies, CRT in 1/1 studies at
0.5[30,32] 52,[34] 61[36] 0.5 mg
[0.125,[30,31] 0.25[35,36]]

Temazepam 12,[38] 52[34] Single dose CFF, CRT, d2 test of attention, word No CRT, CFF
7.5[34,38] lists, DS, trail making test, story recall,
[7.5[38]] DSST

Oxazepam 9,[39] 30,[40] 10,[41] Single dose Word lists, TT, DSST, DS, word stem Delayed recall, word DSST impaired in 3/4 studies at both doses
15,[39] 30[39-42] 10[42] completion task stem completion, all

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved.


studies, both doses

Lorazepam 6,[49] 7,[47] 9,[39,55] Single dose CFF, SRT, TT, CPT, DS, DSST, Delayed recall in 1/2 TT, DSST, SRT, CRT, CFF, Groton maze,
0.5,[43,44] 1,[39,43,45,46] 10,[37,41,42] memory scanning, word stem studies at 0.5 mg, and Stroop impaired in all 15 studies where non-
1.5,[44,47] 2,[39,41,42,48-54] 12,[44-46,50,51] 18,[43] completion task, word lists, story and in all 20 other studies amnestic tasks were measured
2.5,[55,56] 3,[47] 5[48] 20,[56] 36,[52] 39,[54] paragraph recall, Groton maze, Stroop at all doses
[0.5,[43] 1[39]] 55[53]

Lorazepam 12[57] 3 days CRT, DS, word lists, picture recall Delayed recall CRT
2[57]

Lorazepam 16[58] 3 weeks Buschke, discriminant reaction Delayed recall at both CFF only at 1 mg bid
0.5 bid, 1.0 bid[58] time, CFF doses
[0.5 bid, 1.0 bid[58]]

Alprazolam 12,[59,60] 39[54] Single dose Story recall, SRT, DSST, and word lists Delayed recall in 4/4 DSST and SRT in all studies
0.25, 0.5, 0.75,[59] 1[54,60] studies, all doses

Alprazolam 16[58] 3 weeks Buschke, discriminant reaction Delayed recall CFF only at 1 mg bid
0.5 bid, 1.0 bid[58] time, CFF
[0.5 bid, 1.0 bid[58]]

Continued next page

Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)


Tannenbaum et al.
Table I. Contd
Drugs and doses (mg) Sample size (n) Duration Neuropsychological tests Amnestic Non-amnestic impairments
[tested in the elderly]a impairments
Clonazapam 10[61] Single dose CFF, CRT Not assessed CFF, CRT
2[61]

Diazepam 10,[48] 11,[63] Single dose Buschke, DSST, card sorting, word Delayed recall in 4/7 DSST, card sorting, PG, TT impaired in all
2,[62,63] 2.5,[64] 5,[48,63,64] 12[62,64,65] lists, CPT, TT, PG, complex figures, studies across all studies where tested, except in 1 study at 2 mg
10[48,63,65] picture recognition doses
[2.5,[64] 10[65]]

Flurazepam 52,[34] 18,[66] Single dose CFF, CRT, DSST, TT, memory recall, Delayed recall in 3/4 CFF, CRT, tracking test impaired in 3/3 studies,
15,[34] 30[66-68] 24,[68] 25[67] word lists, tracking test studies but not DSST in 2/4 studies
Drug-Induced Cognitive Impairment

[30[66,67]]

Clorazepate 10[48] Single dose Word list, CPT, TT No Not reported


7.5, 15[48]

GABAergic non-benzodiazepine derivatives

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved.


Zolpidem 12,[69] 36,[70] Single dose DSST, word lists, picture recognition, Delayed recall in 4/6 In a variety of non-amnestic tests (TT, PG, CPT,
7.5,[69] 10,[30,32,70-74] 18,[30,37] 10,[72,74] DS, SRT, CRT, TT, story, PG, CPT studies testing 10 mg DS, DSST, CRT), 1 study showed impairment at
20[30,32,33,37,73,74] 23,[32] 30,[73] 24,[33] and in 4/4 with 20 mg 7.5 mg, 5/8 studies showed impairments with
[10[70]] 70[71] 10 mg, and 3/4 with 20 mg

Zolpidem 24[75] 7 days CRT, CFF, memory scanning task, No No


5,[75] 10[24,75] word recognition task, tracking test
[5,[75] 10[24]]

Zolpidem MR 18,[66] 24,[68] 70[71] Single dose CFF, CRT, DSST, memory recall, No deficits at either Impairment in tracking time in 1/1 study and
12.5,[66,68,71] 25[68] tracking test dose in 3/3 studies DSST in 1/2 studies at 12.5 mg
[12.5,[68] 25[68]]

Zopiclone 30,[76] 36,[70] 12[38] Single dose DSST, CFF, CRT, SRT, word lists, DS, Delayed recall at SRT, tracking time and DSST impaired in 1/2
7.5,[70,76] 10[38] trail making test, tracking time, divided 7.5 mg in 1/3 studies studies, CRT, trail making, CFF impaired only at
[7.5,[70] 10[38]] attention, story recall and at 10 mg 10 mg

Zaleplon 10,[74] 24,[33] 30[73] Single dose Word list, DS, DSST, picture Delayed recall in 2/3 DSST in 1/3 studies at 10 and 20 mg
10,[33,73,74] 20[33,73,74] recognition, divided attention, TT studies of 10 mg, 2/3
at 20 mg
a Doses in square brackets were tested in groups of healthy volunteers aged 60 years and older in the referenced study.
bid = twice daily; CFF = critical flicker fusion; CPT = continuous performance task; CRT = choice reaction time; DS = digit span; DSST = digit-symbol substitution task; GABA = g-aminobutyric
acid; MR = modified release; PG = pegboard; SRT = simple reaction time; TT = tapping test.

Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)


647
648 Tannenbaum et al.

Table II. Data of included studies for anticholinergic drugs


Drugs and doses (mg) Sample Duration Neuropsychological tests Amnestic impairments Non-amnestic
[tested in the elderly]a size (n) impairments
Anticholinergic bladder relaxant drugs
Oxybutynin IR 12,[77] Single dose Buschke, DS, trail making, Delayed recall in 1 study at DSST, DS, trail making
5,[77] 10[77,78] 12[78] verbal fluency, DSST, TT, 5 mg and in 1/2 studies at and reaction times
[5,[77] 10[77]] reaction time, CPT, SRT, 10 mg impaired at both doses
CRT, DS
Oxybutynin ER 50[79] 3 weeks Name-face recognition, Delayed recall with 10, 15 mg Divided attention with
10, 15, 20[79] hidden objects, divided 15 mg only
[10, 15, 20[79]] attention, CRT,
discriminant reaction time
Darifenacin CR 129[80] 2 weeks SRT, CRT, word lists, item No Reaction time for 3.75 mg
3.75, 7.5, 15[80] recognition only
[15[80]]
Darifenacin IR 50,[79] 3 weeks Name-face recognition, No Divided attention only in
7.5,[79] 15[79,80] 129[80] hidden objects, divided 1/2 studies with 15 mg
[7.5,[79] 15[80]] attention, CRT,
discriminant reaction time
Solifenacin 12[78] Single dose SRT, CRT, DS, word and No No
10[78] picture recall
[10[78]]
Anticholinergic tricyclic antidepressants
Amitriptyline 6,[81] Single dose CPT, digit-digit coding, item No impairment in 1 study at CPT, reaction time and
25,[81-83] 37.5,[39] 15,[84,87] recognition and word lists, 25 mg, in 2/4 at 50 mg and in 1 DSST impaired at all
50,[84-87] 70[39] 9,[39,82] Buschke, DS, DSST, CRT, at 37.5 mg; delayed recall doses in all studies tested
[25,[81] 50[84,86]] 17,[85] CFF impaired in 1 study testing
12,[86] 70 mg
13[83]
Amitriptyline 10,[88] 7 days–2 CRT, CFF, memory Recall not reported in 1 study, CRT, CFF, tracking test
25,[88] 37.5,[89] 12,[89,91] weeks scanning task, word not assessed in 1 study, impaired in 3/5 studies;
70,[89] 75[90,91] 24[90] recognition task, tracking impaired at 37.5 and 70 mg effects diminish over
[25[88]] test, word lists, block test, time, becoming
DSST, CPT insignificant
Imipramine 24[92] 7 days 3 different CRT Not assessed Impairment in 1/3 CRT
50 bid[92] tests
[50 bid[92]]
a Doses in square brackets were tested in groups of healthy volunteers aged 60 years and older in the referenced study.
CFF = critical flicker fusion; CPT = continuous performance task; CR = controlled release; CRT = choice reaction time; DS = digit span;
DSST = digit-symbol substitution task; ER = extended release; IR = immediate release; SRT = simple reaction time; TT = tapping test.

memory function was not assessed.[101,102] Second- cognition in healthy volunteers were identified
generation H1-blocking agents do not appear to (table III).[22,46,51] One study of oxycodone 10 mg
affect any cognitive domain over the short or long induced significant impairments in both memory
term, with only one of 14 single-dose studies show- and attention in both older and middle-aged par-
ing changes in attention and memory with deslor- ticipants, as did 10 and 15 mg of morphine. Low-
atadine compared with placebo.[95-98,100-103] dose dextropropoxyphene failed to elicit deficits in
a small sample of middle-aged adults, although
3.4 Opioid Receptor-Binding Agents
the 200 mg dose affected attention and reaction
time. No studies were found that evaluated the
Five single-dose, randomized, crossover de- extent to which tolerance alleviated the adverse
sign trials testing the impact of opioid agents on cognitive effects.

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
Drug-Induced Cognitive Impairment 649

Table III. Data of included studies for antihistamine, opioid and antipsychotic drugs
Drugs and doses Sample Duration Neuropsychological tests Amnestic impairments Non-amnestic
(mg) [tested in the size (n) impairments
elderly]a
First-generation histamine H1 antagonists
Hydroxyzine 18[94] Single dose, Tracking test, divided Not assessed Tracking test, divided
50 mg[94] crossover attention, SRT attention, SRT
Promethazine 24,[95,98] Single dose CFF, CRT, TT, tracking, Not assessed in 3 studies, CFF, CRT, tracking
25,[93,95,96] 30[97] 9,[96] word recall word recall impaired in 1 impaired in all studies
20[97] study at 25 mg
Diphenhydramine 12,[44,77] Single dose Buschke, DS, trail making, No effect on recall in 4 DSST unimpaired at
25,[99] 50,[44,77,100] 37,[99] verbal fluency, DSST, TT, lower-dose studies, not 25 mg; DSST, DS,
75,[44] 100[44] 42[100] reaction time, CPT, CRT assessed in 1, impaired at reaction time, CPT
[25,[99] 50[77]] 100 mg impaired in all other
studies
Diphenhydramine 32[101] 5 days SRT, CPT, DS, DSST, Not assessed Only the number of errors
100[101] divided attention task, on the tracking test
discriminant reaction time increased
Triprolidine 10[102] 5 days CFF, CRT, memory Not assessed No
10[102] scanning
Second-generation histamine H1 antagonists
Cetirizine 9,[96] Single dose CFF, CRT, CPT, tracking Only assessed in 1 study No impairment in 13/14
2.5,[95] 5,[95,97] 10[95] 16,[103] time, DSST, recall where delayed recall was studies; CRT impaired
Levocetirizine 20,[97] found to be impaired with only for desloratadine
10[97] 24,[95,98] desloratadine 5 mg 5 mg
Loratadine 42[100]
10,[95,97] 20,[95] 40[95]
Desloratadine
5[96]
Fexofenadine
25,[103] 60,[98]
120,[98] 180[100]
Ebastine 10,[102] 5 days CFF, CRT, memory Not assessed No
10, 20, 30[102] 33[101] scanning, DS, DSST, SRT
Loratadine
7.5[101]
Opioids
Oxycodone 71[22] Single dose SRT, CRT, DSST, DS, Delayed recall SRT, sustained attention
10[22] word list recall, d2 task of
[10[22]] sustained attention
Morphine 12[46] Single dose SRT, CRT, CFF, memory Delayed recall and picture CFF
10,[46] 15[46] scanning, item recognition, recognition at both doses
recall
Dextropropoxyphene 12[51] Single dose SRT, CRT, number No Only CFF impaired at
100,[51] 200[51] vigilance, memory 200 mg
scanning, word recall, word
and picture recognition,
CFF
Mixed mechanism antipsychotics
Chlorpromazine 12[104] Single dose DS, DSST, face No No
50[104] recognition, story and
word recall

Continued next page

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
650 Tannenbaum et al.

Table III. Contd


Drugs and doses Sample Duration Neuropsychological tests Amnestic impairments Non-amnestic
(mg) [tested in the size (n) impairments
elderly]a
Risperidone 12[45] Single dose CFF, SRT, TT, CPT, DS, No TT, DSST
0.25,[45] 0.5[45] DSST memory scanning
[0.25,[45] 0.5[45]] test, word lists
Haloperidol 14[105] 4 days SRT, CRT, CFF, DS, word Only haloperidol impaired Only haloperidol
3[105] and picture recognition recall impaired reaction time
Olanzapine
3[105]
a Doses in square brackets were tested in groups of healthy volunteers aged 60 years and older in the referenced study.
CFF = critical flicker fusion; CPT = continuous performance task; CRT = choice reaction time; DS = digit span; DSST = digit-symbol substitution
task; SRT = simple reaction time; TT = tapping test.

3.5 Antipsychotics: Mixed Anticholinergic and non-amnestic impairments. Findings for the other
Antihistamine Effects drug classes were contradictory or of lesser quality
and did not permit sound conclusions to be drawn
Five small trials using typical and atypical about their specificity for affecting different cog-
antipsychotics were included in the qualitative nitive domains.
synthesis, three single-dose and two repeated- Large, population-based, epidemiological stud-
dose studies.[45,104,105] A 3 mg dose of haloperidol ies querying the association between medication
impaired recall and reaction time, while 0.25 and consumption and cognitive performance support
0.5 mg of risperidone only affected psychomotor the findings from this review. Data from 2765
speed. Chlopromazine and olanzapine did not persons participating in the Duke Established
elicit deficits. Unexplained heterogeneity resulted Population for Epidemiologic Studies of the Elderly
in a low-quality evidence rating, precluding con- showed that short-, intermediate- and long-acting
fident conclusions about the cognitive safety of benzodiazepine users made a significantly greater
these agents. number of errors on memory testing compared with
Table IV summarizes the findings and quality non-users, with sub-analyses examining increasing
of the evidence for each drug class according to dose and cumulative exposure over time suggest-
the GRADE criteria. ing a dose-response relationship.[106] An increased
occurrence of both amnestic and non-amnestic
4. Discussion cognitive deficits among benzodiazepine users was
also confirmed by 2105 respondents from the Lon-
Results of this systematic review of randomized gitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam in the Nether-
controlled experiments assessing the attribution of lands,[107] over 9000 participants from the Canadian
amnestic or non-amnestic cognitive deficits to the Study of Health and Aging[108] and 1389 people
use of GABAergic, histaminergic, anticholinergic aged 60–70 years recruited for the Epidemiology
and opioid medication in adults without under- of Vascular Aging study in Nantes.[109] Only the
lying central nervous system disorders reveal a smaller Eugeria Longitudinal Study of Cerebral
strong potential for several commonly used drugs Aging (n = 372) failed to detect an effect of ben-
to induce symptoms of forgetfulness and/or diffi- zodiazepines on cognition.[110] Regarding anti-
culty concentrating. Data on the short- and inter- cholinergic medication, multiple cross-sectional,
mediate-acting benzodiazepine GABAergic drugs population-based studies have shown consistent
suggest that a combined pattern of amnestic and associations between drug intake and non-amnestic
non-amnestic cognitive deficits consistently emerges cognitive impairments as well as other measures
with use of these agents. First-generation H1 anti- of global cognitive function, although most studies
histamine and tricyclic antidepressant drugs evoke simultaneously examined the cumulative effect of

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
Drug-Induced Cognitive Impairment 651

drugs with any anticholinergic properties rather retrospective analysis showed no effects of anti-
than by drug class alone.[13,111-118] One study of cholinergic agents on cognition.[119,120] For H1
tricyclic antidepressants and data from a small receptor antagonists, only one study has been

Table IV. Summary of findings/Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) quality rating
Mechanism Class effect No. of single-dose Summary of the evidence Level of
trials/no. at evidence quality
steady state
GABAergic Short-acting 20 single dose Fairly consistent impairments occur across a range of Moderate-high
benzodiazepines cognitive functions, including attention, psychomotor speed,
strategy use and problem solving, learning and delayed recall
up to 8 hours after use. Dose-response gradient present. No
data on repeated use.
Intermediate-acting 28 single dose/5 Impairment mainly in the ability to learn new information, i.e. High
benzodiazepines steady state the transfer of data from short-term memory to longer-term
storage up to 8 hours post-dose. Reduced
attention/concentration and slower reaction times may also
occur at higher doses. Consistent evidence of a dose-response
gradient across studies. Repeated-dose studies at steady state
show only partial tolerance to these effects with chronic use.
Long-acting 15 single dose Heterogeneity across studies can be explained by the age of Moderate
benzodiazepines the participants. Consistent and marked effects on cognitive
performance were evident in older adults across multiple
domains, but were absent at low doses in young adults.
Inconsistent dose-response effect.
Non- 27 single dose/2 Unexplained heterogeneity across studies unrelated to dose, Low
benzodiazepine steady state drug, type of test or age of the participants. Half the studies
derivatives show significant impairment in attention and memory recall,
while the other half shows no effect. One study at steady state
induced no cognitive deficits. Inconsistent evidence for a
dose-response gradient.
Anticholinergic Bladder 3 single dose/6 Inconsistent deficits on memory and attention tasks. Low-moderate
antimuscarinics steady state Heterogeneity across studies partially explained by different
batteries of neuropsychological tests, drug effects and dose,
with greater impairments consistently induced by single and
repeated high doses of oxybutynin.
Tricyclic 10 single dose/5 Significant effects on vigilance and attention, inconsistently on Moderate-high
antidepressants steady state memory. Evidence of a dose-response effect. Younger
individuals may develop tolerance with repeated use.
Histamine H1 First generation 11 single dose/2 Decreased alertness produces disturbances in attention and Moderate-high
antagonists steady state vigilance for up to 8 hours after single-dose use. Partial
tolerance developed in one study and complete tolerance
developed in another study of 5 days’ duration. Unclear dose-
response relationship.
Second generation 14 single dose/4 Only one small (n = 9) single-dose study with desloratadine High
steady state produced impairments in memory and reaction time. Other
drugs exhibited no effects on cognition during single and
repeated use.
Opioids Oxycodone, 5 single dose Single-use, high-dose consumption of oral opioid drugs elicits Low-moderate
morphine, impairments in concentration and memory storage. Slight age
dextropropoxyphene effect may be present, worse in older adults. Insufficient
evidence on low doses, repeated use or dose-response effect.
Mixed Antipsychotics 3 single dose/2 Unexplained heterogeneity across studies unrelated to age, Low
mechanisms steady state duration of use or typical vs atypical compounds.
Small studies.
GABA = g-aminobutyric acid.

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
652 Tannenbaum et al.

conducted, which found an association between correct for multiple comparisons in their analysis.
impaired cognition and diphenhydramine use, An alternate explanation relates to the age of the
but cognition was not specifically modelled as the participants. While age per se may not discrim-
outcome variable, so results can only be inter- inate between drug effects, age-related physio-
preted cautiously.[121] Opioid consumption was logical alteration may amplify the potential for
investigated in relation to cognitive performance cognitive impairments to occur via changes in
in a Canadian, cross-sectional, population-based receptor density and pharmacodynamics, redis-
study of 2035 older adults, of whom 1.5% used tribution of body composition, diminished he-
narcotic agents and performed significantly worse patic and renal clearance, or a more permissively
on a neuropsychological test of attention com- penetrable blood-brain barrier. Finally, the small
pared with non-users.[108] Of note, none of these sample sizes used in many of these studies may
observational studies were inception cohort stud- have rendered the achievement of statistical sig-
ies with participants who were treatment-naive nificance difficult, especially in the face of trivial
at baseline, so the results are less robust for de- impairments or a reduced sensitivity of the cho-
termining causality than data from randomized sen tests to detect impairments.
placebo-controlled trials, where baseline cognitive Because of the aforementioned considerations,
measures are collected prior to drug adminis- our systematic review was unable to include a
tration. Among the longitudinal trials that ex- valid meta-analysis for calculating the magnitude
ist,[111,122-124] data are conflicting and address the of effect or the frequency with which cognitive
question of whether continuous drug use can lead impairment could be expected to occur with each
to a progressive decline in cognitive status, which drug class. The studies included in this review
is separate from the topic targeted in this review. tended to report significant changes in group means
While it is clear that benzodiazepines and first- on various neuropsychological tests instead of
generation H1 antihistamines provoke cognitive the frequency with which individuals performed
deficits, inconclusive findings for the non-benzo- poorly on amnestic or non-amnestic tasks. The
diazepine derivatives and bladder antimuscarinic omission of actual test scores and/or standard
drugs warrant comment. Scrutiny of the results deviations of the test scores from the results in
for individual drugs within these classes points to almost 40% of the 78 studies retained for the re-
the possibility that some agents may preferentially view made it impossible to combine data from
induce deficits due to their specific formulations, different studies. The inability to conduct a meta-
physicochemical properties or receptor selectivity analysis encourages reflection on the type of in-
while others may not. If this is the case, then formation that is needed to better counsel patients
caution should be taken in lumping class effects on the prevention and reversal of drug-induced
with effects unique to individual pharmacolog- amnestic or non-amnestic cognitive deficits. Even
ical molecules, as they may have overlapping but if valid effect sizes could be calculated, without
differential properties. Within the anticholinergic data on the number of individuals in whom im-
drug class, for example, amitriptyline possesses pairments are expected to occur, patients will be
H1 antagonist properties while solifenacin does unable to gauge their personal risk of adverse
not. On the other hand, discrepant results could cognitive effects when choosing between phar-
also be explained by a lack of standardization in maceutical therapies. To ascertain causality with
the neuropsychological test batteries administered certainty, it would be ideal to conduct neuro-
by the different studies and the diverse statistical psychological tests in an n-of-1 trial before and
approaches employed to ascertain whether cog- after initiation of each agent; however, this is neither
nitive effects occurred among participants. Some practical nor feasible in a busy outpatient setting.
studies, particularly within the first-generation Furthermore, few studies evaluated whether tol-
H1 antihistamine class, exclusively evaluated non- erance to deleterious cognitive effects develops
amnestic functions, others used auto-administered over time with steady-state concentration of each
computerized test batteries, and many failed to drug. The results from the single-dose studies in-

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
Drug-Induced Cognitive Impairment 653

cluded in this review can therefore only be ap- signal early dementia. As a result, more patients
plied to medications such as benzodiazepines, are undergoing neuropsychological testing and
antihistamines and narcotic agents that are used are being diagnosed and monitored for amnestic
episodically on an as needed basis for the treat- or non-amnestic MCI. The impact of medications
ment of insomnia, anxiety, allergic symptoms, on cognitive function and their ability to affect
itchiness or pain, respectively. the results of neuropsychological test scores during
This review is limited by the search engines’ the clinical assessment of memory complaints is
strategy for identifying relevant articles; articles undervalued. This review provides a synthesis of
that did not list the selected keywords in their the evidence on the role medications can play
titles or abstracts would have been missed by the during the assessment of mild cognitive impair-
search strategy and excluded from this analysis. ment. There is a consistent body of evidence sug-
While great measures were taken to search the gesting that drug-induced MCI can occur with
references of pertinent articles, it is likely that episodic use of medication for insomnia, anxiety,
older papers may not have been captured,[125] as pruritus or allergic symptoms. Combined amnes-
well as studies that tested a multitude of phar- tic and non-amnestic deficits occur with the use of
macodynamic parameters and those that used benzodiazepine agents and may partially underlie
benzodiazepines as positive controls to test the older adults’ frequent complaints of forgetfulness
cognitive effects of other agents. As the effects of or difficulty concentrating. The interpretation of
benzodiazepines tend to be consistent, it is un- neuropsychological tests in patients being screened
likely that the inclusion of extra articles would for cognitive impairment needs to take into account
have altered the findings. An additional caveat is the effect of exogenous centrally active pharmaco-
that because the trials reviewed in this analysis logical substances.
were all conducted in adults without underlying The trade-off between the risks and benefits of
central nervous system disorders, they do not pharmacological therapy needs to be made on an
provide information regarding the full extent individual basis in accordance with each patient’s
of cognitive effects in individuals with chronic circumstances. Despite the known risks, some
diseases taking multiple medications over several patients may be better off taking certain drugs
years, which is becoming increasingly common when they need them than if the underlying dis-
for the aging population. Vulnerable older adults order were left untreated. Each patient has the
may be more susceptible to drug-induced cognitive right to an informed choice, with safer pharma-
deficits, especially in the presence of polyphar- cological or non-pharmacological alternatives of-
macy, which increases the risk of drug-drug inter- fered whenever possible and the risks and benefits
actions and elevated plasma levels of certain agents weighed in accordance with patient preferences.
over time. This review did not establish whether Future studies aimed at clarifying the extent and
cognitive disturbances elicited by GABAergic, an- type of impairment that can be expected with use
tihistamine, anticholinergic or opioid medication of various medications should employ standard-
have important repercussions on activities of daily ized and easily reproducible neuropsychological
living, especially as it appears that some degree of test batteries, with greater attention paid to pop-
tolerance may develop over time. Nor did the study ulation homogeneity and estimation of the num-
address whether interference with attention and ber-needed-to-harm statistic.
other executive functions increases the risk of
postural instability, falls, driving impairment or Acknowledgements
automobile accidents.
We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Audrey Attia,
5. Conclusions Marion Guillemont, Jean-Francois Cabana, Thomas Jubault
and Don Sheppard in the preparation of this manuscript.
This work was sponsored by The Michel Saucier Endowed
Patients are increasingly concerned with per- Chair in Geriatric Pharmacology, Health and Aging, from the
ceived memory and attention deficits that may Faculty of Pharmacy, Université de Montréal. The sponsors

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
654 Tannenbaum et al.

had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, 9. Luck T, Luppa M, Briel S, et al. Incidence of mild cognitive
management, analysis and interpretation of the data; and impairment: a systematic review. Dement Geriatr Cogn
preparation, review and approval of the manuscript. Disord 2010; 29 (2): 164-75
The financial disclosures for the authors are as follows: 10. Busse A, Hensel A, Guhne U, et al. Mild cognitive im-
Cara Tannenbaum was supported by The Michel Saucier pairment: long-term course of four clinical subtypes.
Endowed Chair in Geriatric Pharmacology, Health and Neurology 2006; 67 (12): 2176-85
Aging, from the Faculty of Pharmacy, Université de Montréal 11. Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Shen C, et al. Mild cognitive im-
and by a junior researcher award from the Fondation de Re- pairment, amnestic type: an epidemiologic study. Neu-
cherche en Santé du Québec. She declares having received rology 2004; 63 (1): 115-21
honoraria and/or consulting fees from Allergan Inc., Astellas, 12. Larrieu S, Letenneur L, Orgogozo JM, et al. Incidence and
Ferring and Pfizer during the past 3 years. Amélie Paquette outcome of mild cognitive impairment in a population-
was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health grant based prospective cohort. Neurology 2002; 59 (10): 1594-9
108262. Ryan Carnahan was supported by an Agency for 13. Artero S, Ancelin ML, Portet F, et al. Risk profiles for mild
Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) Centers for Edu- cognitive impairment and progression to dementia are
cation and Research on Therapeutics cooperative agreement gender specific. J Neurol, Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008; 79
#5 U18 HSO16094 (the Iowa Older Adults CERT). He has (9): 979-84
previously received research support from Eli Lilly and Co., 14. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, et al. Anxiety and
Forest Laboratories, Wyeth and Boehringer Ingelheim. All cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emo-
other authors declare that they have received no support from tion 2007; 7 (2): 336-53
any organization for the submitted work, have no financial 15. Lupien SJ, Fiocco A, Wan N, et al. Stress hormones and
relationships with any organizations that might have an in- human memory function across the lifespan. Psycho-
terest in the submitted work in the previous 3 years and have neuroendocrinology 2005; 30 (3): 225-42
no other relationships or activities that could appear to have
16. Waxman S. Clinical neuroanatomy. 26th ed. Columbus
influenced the submitted work. (OH): The McGraw-Hill Companies, 2009
Contributions: Cara Tannenbaum had full access to all of
the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity 17. Nestler EJ, Hyman SE, Malenka RC. Molecular neuro-
pharmacology: a foundation for clinical neuroscience. 2nd
of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Cara
ed. Columbus (OH): The McGraw-Hill Companies, 2009
Tannenbaum, Amélie Paquette and Ryan Carnahan con-
tributed to the design of the study. All authors contributed to 18. Hshieh TT, Fong TG, Marcantonio ER, et al. Cholinergic
the data collection, management, analysis and interpretation deficiency hypothesis in delirium: a synthesis of current
evidence. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2008; 63 (7): 764-72
of the data, and the preparation and review of the manuscript.
19. Lucas-Meunier E, Fossier P, Baux G, et al. Cholinergic
modulation of the cortical neuronal network. Pflugers
Arch 2003; 446 (1): 17-29
References 20. Passani MB, Giannoni P, Bucherelli C, et al. Histamine in
1. Kaufman DW, Kelly JP, Rosenberg L, et al. Recent patterns the brain: beyond sleep and memory. Biochem Pharmacol
of medication use in the ambulatory adult population of 2007; 73 (8): 1113-22
the United States: the Slone survey. JAMA 2002; 287 (3): 21. Barker MJ, Greenwood KM, Jackson M, et al. Cognitive
337-44 effects of long-term benzodiazepine use: a meta-analysis.
2. Gurwitz JH, Field TS, Harrold LR, et al. Incidence and CNS Drugs 2004; 18 (1): 37-48
preventability of adverse drug events among older persons 22. Cherrier MM, Amory JK, Ersek M, et al. Comparative
in the ambulatory setting. JAMA 2003; 289 (9): 1107-16 cognitive and subjective side effects of immediate-release
3. Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, et al. Mild cog- oxycodone in healthy middle-aged and older adults. J Pain
nitive impairment: ten years later. Arch Neurol 2009; 66 2009; 10 (10): 1038-50
(12): 1447-55 23. Bloom FE. Neurotransmission and the central nervous
4. Gauthier S, Reisberg B, Zaudig M, et al. Mild cognitive system. In: Brunton LL, Lazo JS, Parker KL, editors.
impairment. Lancet 2006; 367 (9518): 1262-70 Goodman & Gilman’s the pharmacological basis of
5. Graham JE, Rockwood K, Beattie BL, et al. Prevalence therapeutics. 11th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Compa-
and severity of cognitive impairment with and without nies, 2006: 317-39
dementia in an elderly population. Lancet 1997; 349 (9068): 24. Canadian Pharmacists Association. Compendium of
1793-6 pharmaceuticals and specialties (CPS 2010). Toronto
6. Heinik J. V. A. Kral and the origins of benign senescent (ON): Webcom Inc., 2010
forgetfulness and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psycho- 25. Lacy CF, Armstrong LL, Goldman MP, et al. Drug in-
geriatr 2010; 22 (3): 395-402 formation handbook (with international trade name in-
7. Goldman WP, Morris JC. Evidence that age-associated dex): 2009-2010. 18th ed. Hudson (OH): Lexi-Comp, 2009
memory impairment is not a normal variant of aging. Alz 26. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, editors. Neu-
Dis Ass Dis 2001; 15 (2): 72-9 ropsychological assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford
8. Chertkow H, Massoud F, Nasreddine Z, et al. Diagnosis University Press, 2004
and treatment of dementia: 3. Mild cognitive impairment 27. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, et al. What is ‘‘quality of
and cognitive impairment without dementia. CMAJ 2008; evidence’’ and why is it important to clinicians? BMJ
178 (10): 1273-85 2008; 336 (7651): 995-8

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
Drug-Induced Cognitive Impairment 655

28. Loke YK, Price D, Herxheimer A. Chapter 14: adverse ef- evidence for possible influences of time course. Psycho-
fects. In: Higgins JPT, Green S, editors. Cochrane hand- pharmacology (Berl) 1996; 128: 139-49
book for systematic reviews of interventions. Version 43. Pomara N, Willoughby LM, Hashim A, et al. Effects of
5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collabora- acute lorazepam administration on aminergic activity in
tion, 2011 [online]. Available from URL: http://www. normal elderly subjects: relationship to performance ef-
cochrane-handbook.org [Accessed 2012 Jun 27] fects and apolipoprotein genotype. Neurochem Res 2004;
29. Collie A, Maruff P, Snyder PJ, et al. Cognitive testing in 29 (7): 1391-8
early phase clinical trials: outcome according to adverse 44. Turner C, Handford ADF, Nicholson AN. Sedation and
event profile in a phase I study. Hum Psychopharmacol memory: studies with a histamine H-1 receptor antagonist.
2006; 21 (7): 481-8 J Psychopharmacol 2006; 20 (4): 506-17
30. Mintzer MZ, Griffiths RR. Triazolam and zolpidem: effects 45. Allain H, Tessier C, Bentue-Ferrer D, et al. Effects of risper-
on human memory and attentional processes. Psycho- idone on psychometric and cognitive functions in healthy
pharmacology (Berl) 1999; 144 (1): 8-19 elderly volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003; 165 (4):
31. Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, Shapiro L, et al. Sensitivity to 419-29
triazolam in the elderly. N Engl J Med 1991; 324 (24):
46. Hanks GW, O’Neill WM, Simpson P, et al. The cognitive
1691-8 and psychomotor effects of opioid analgesics: II. A ran-
32. Roehrs T, Merlotti L, Zorick F, et al. Sedative, memory, domized controlled trial of single doses of morphine,
and performance effects of hypnotics. Psychopharmacol- lorazepam and placebo in healthy subjects. Eur J Clin
ogy (Berl) 1994; 116 (2): 130-4 Pharmacol 1995; 48 (6): 455-60
33. Troy SM, Lucki I, Unruh MA, et al. Comparison of the 47. Shader RI, Dreyfuss D, Gerrein JR, et al. Sedative effects
effects of zaleplon, zolpidem, and triazolam on memory, and impaired learning and recall after single oral doses of
learning, and psychomotor performance. J Clin Psycho- lorazepam. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1986; 39 (5): 526-9
pharmacol 2000; 20 (3): 328-37
48. Healey M, Pickens R, Meisch R, et al. Effects of clor-
34. Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, Engelhardt N, et al. Pharma- azepate, diazepam, lorazepam, and placebo on human
cokinetic determinants of dynamic differences among memory. J Clin Psychiatry 1983; 44 (12): 436-9
three benzodiazepine hypnotics: flurazepam, temazepam,
and triazolam [published erratum appears in Arch Gen 49. Mac DS, Kumar R, Goodwin DW. Anterograde amnesia
Psychiatry 1989 Sep; 46 (9): 793]. Arch Gen Psych 1989; with oral lorazepam. J Clin Psychiatry 1985; 46 (4): 137-8
46 (4): 326-32 50. Micallef J, Soubrouillard C, Guet F, et al. A double blind
35. Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, Gouthro TA, et al. Distin- parallel group placebo controlled comparison of sedative
guishing a benzodiazepine agonist (triazolam) from a non- and amnesic effects of etifoxine and lorazepam in healthy
agonist anxiolytic (buspirone) by electroencephalography: subjects. Fundam Clin Pharmacol 2001; 15 (3): 209-16
kinetic-dynamic studies. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1994; 56 (1): 51. O’Neill WM, Hanks GW, White L, et al. The cognitive and
100-11 psychomotor effects of opioid analgesics: I. A randomized
36. Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, von Moltke LL, et al. Age and controlled trial of single doses of dextropropoxyphene,
gender effects on the pharmacokinetics and pharmaco- lorazepam and placebo in healthy subjects. Eur J Clin
dynamics of triazolam, a cytochrome P450 3A substrate. Pharmacol 1995; 48 (6): 447-53
Clin PharmacolTher 2004; 76 (5): 467-79 52. Stonnington CM, Snyder PJ, Hentz JG, et al. Double-blind
37. Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, Von Moltke LL, et al. Com- crossover study of the cognitive effects of lorazepam in
parative kinetics and response to the benzodiazepine healthy apolipoprotein E (APOE)-epsilon4 carriers. J Clin
agonists triazolam and zolpidem: evaluation of sex- Psychiatry 2009; 70 (10): 1379-84
dependent differences. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2000; 293 53. Greenblatt DJ, Scavone JM, Harmatz JS, et al. Cognitive
(2): 435-43 effects of beta-adrenergic antagonists after single doses:
38. Hemmeter U, Muller M, Bischof R, et al. Effect of zopi- pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of proprano-
clone and temazepam on sleep EEG parameters, psycho- lol, atenolol, lorazepam, and placebo. Clin Pharmacol
motor and memory functions in healthy elderly volun- Ther 1993; 53 (5): 577-84
teers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2000; 147 (4): 384-96 54. Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, Dorsey C, et al. Comparative
39. Curran VH, Schiwy W, Lader M. Differential amnestic single-dose kinetics and dynamics of lorazepam, alprazo-
properties of benzodiazepines: a dose-response compar- lam, prazepam, and placebo. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1988;
ison of two drugs with similar elimination half-lives. Psy- 44 (3): 326-34
chopharmacology (Berl) 1987; 92: 358-64 55. Schifano F, Curran HV. Pharmacological models of mem-
40. Buffett-Jerrott SE, Stewart SH, Bird S, et al. An examina- ory dysfunction? A comparison of the effects of scopola-
tion of differences in the time course of oxazepam’s effects mine and lorazepam on word valence ratings, priming and
on implicit vs explicit memory. J Psychopharmacol 1998; recall. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994; 115 (3): 430-4
12 (4): 338-47 56. Meador KJ, Gevins A, Leese PT, et al. Neurocognitive ef-
41. Buffett-Jerrott SE, Stewart SH, Teehan MD. A further fects of brivaracetam, levetiracetam, and lorazepam.
examination of the time-dependent effects of oxazepam Epilepsia 2011; 52 (2): 264-72
and lorazepam on implicit and explicit memory. Psycho- 57. Blin O, Simon N, Jouve E, et al. Pharmacokinetic and
pharmacology (Berl) 1998; 138: 344-53 pharmacodynamic analysis of sedative and amnesic effects
42. Stewart SH, Rioux GF, Connolly JF, et al. Effects of ox- of lorazepam in healthy volunteers. Clin Neuropharmacol
azepam and lorazepam on implicit and explicit memory: 2001; 24 (2): 71-81

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
656 Tannenbaum et al.

58. Pomara N, Tun H, DaSilva D, et al. The acute and chronic 73. Verster JC, Volkerts ER, Schreuder AHCML, et al. Residual
performance effects of alprazolam and lorazepam in the effects of middle-of-the-night administration of zaleplon
elderly: relationship to duration of treatment and self- and zolpidem on driving ability, memory functions, and
rated sedation. Psychopharmacol Bull 1998; 34 (2): 139-53 psychomotor performance. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2002;
59. Coldwell SE, Milgrom P, Getz T, et al. Amnestic and anxio- 22 (6): 576-83
lytic effects of alprazolam in oral surgery patients. J Oral 74. Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, von Moltke LL, et al. Com-
Maxillofac Surg 1997; 55 (10): 1061-70 parative kinetics and dynamics of zaleplon, zolpidem, and
60. Scavone JM, Greenblatt DJ, Goddard JE, et al. The placebo. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1998; 64 (5): 553-61
pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of sublingual 75. Fairweather DB, Kerr JS, Hindmarch I. The effects of
and oral alprazolam in the post-prandial state. Eur J Clin acute and repeated doses of zolpidem on subjective
Pharmacol 1992; 42 (4): 439-43 sleep, psychomotor performance and cognitive function
61. van der Meyden CH, Bartel PR, Sommers DK, et al. Ef- in elderly volunteers. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 1992; 43 (6):
fects of clobazam and clonazepam on saccadic eye 597-601
movements and other parameters of psychomotor per- 76. Mets MA, de Vries JM, de Senerpont Domis LM, et al.
formance. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 1989; 37: 365-9 Next-day effects of ramelteon (8 mg), zopiclone (7.5 mg),
62. Unrug-Neervoort A, Van Luijtelaar G, Coenen A. Cogni- and placebo on highway driving performance, memory
tion and vigilance: differential effects of diazepam and functioning, psychomotor performance, and mood in
buspirone on memory and psychomotor performance. healthy adult subjects. Sleep 2011; 34 (10): 1327-34
Neuropsychobiology 1992; 26 (3): 146-50 77. Katz IR, Sands LP, Bilker W, et al. Identification of med-
63. Friedman H, Greenblatt DJ, Peters GR, et al. Pharmaco- ications that cause cognitive impairment in older people:
kinetics and pharmacodynamics of oral diazepam: effect the case of oxybutynin chloride. J Am Geriatr Soc 1998;
of dose, plasma concentration, and time. Clin Pharmacol 46 (1): 8-13
Ther 1992; 52 (2): 139-50 78. Wesnes KA, Edgar C, Tretter RN, et al. Exploratory pilot
64. Block RI, DeVoe M, Stanley B, et al. Memory performance study assessing the risk of cognitive impairment or seda-
in individuals with primary degenerative dementia: its sim- tion in the elderly following single doses of solifenacin
ilarity to diazepam-induced impairments. Exp Aging Res 10 mg. Expert Opin Drug Saf 2009; 8 (6): 615-26
1985; 11 (3-4): 151-5 79. Kay G, Crook T, Rekeda L, et al. Differential effects of the
65. Cutson TM, Gray SL, Hughes MA, et al. Effect of a single antimuscarinic agents darifenacin and oxybutynin ER on
dose of diazepam on balance measures in older people. memory in older subjects. Eur Urol 2006; 50 (2): 317-26
J Am Geriatr Soc 1997; 45 (4): 435-40 80. Lipton RB, Kolodner K, Wesnes K. Assessment of cogni-
66. Blin O, Micallef J, Audebert C, et al. A double-blind, tive function of the elderly population: effects of dar-
placebo- and flurazepam-controlled investigation of the ifenacin. J Urol 2005; 173 (2): 493-8
residual psychomotor and cognitive effects of modified 81. Ghose K, Sedman E. A double-blind comparison of the
release zolpidem in young healthy volunteers. J Clin Psy- pharmcodynamic effects of single doses of lofepramine,
chopharmacol 2006; 26 (3): 284-9 amitryptyline and placebo in elderly subjects. Eur J Clin
67. Boyle J, Wolford D, Gargano C, et al. Next-day residual Pharmacol 1987; 33: 505-9
effects of gaboxadol and flurazepam administered at 82. Nathan PJ, Sitaram G, Stough C, et al. Serotonin, nor-
bedtime: a randomized double-blind study in healthy el- adrenaline and cognitive function: a preliminary in-
derly subjects. Hum Psychopharmacol 2009; 24 (1): 61-71 vestigation of the acute pharmacodynamic effects of a
68. Hindmarch I, Legangneux E, Stanley N, et al. A double- serotonin versus a serotonin and noradrenaline reuptake
blind, placebo-controlled investigation of the residual inhibitor. Behav Pharmacol 2000; 11 (7-8): 639-42
psychomotor and cognitive effects of zolpidem-MR in 83. Timoshanko A, Stough C, Vitetta L, et al. A preliminary
healthy elderly volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol 2006; 62 investigation on the acute pharmacodynamic effects of
(5): 538-45 hypericum on cognitive an psychomotor performance.
69. Cysneiros RM, Farkas D, Harmatz JS, et al. Pharmaco- BehavPharmacol 2001; 12: 635-40
kinetic and pharmacodynamic interactions between zolpi- 84. Branconnier RJ, Cole JO. Effects of acute administration
dem and caffeine. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2007; 82 (1): 54-62 of trazodone and amitiptyline on cognition, cardio-
70. Farber RH, Burke PJ. Post-bedtime dosing with indiplon vascular function, and salivation in the normal geriatric
in adults and the elderly: results from two placebo- subject. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1981; 1 (6 Suppl.): 82S-
controlled, active comparator crossover studies in healthy 88S
volunteers. Curr Med Res Opin 2008; 24 (3): 837-46 85. Iwamoto K, Takahashi M, Nakamura Y, et al. The effects
71. Greenblatt DJ, Legangneux E, Harmatz JS, et al. Dy- of acute treatment with paroxetine, amitriptyline, and
namics and kinetics of a modified-release formulation of placebo on driving performance and cognitive function in
zolpidem: comparison with immediate-release standard healthy Japanese subjects: a double-blind crossover trial.
zolpidem and placebo. J Clin Pharmacol 2006; 46 (12): Hum Psychopharmacol 2008; 23 (5): 399-407
1469-80 86. Rosenzweig P, Patat A, Zieleniuk I, et al. Cognitive per-
72. Otmani S, Demazieres A, Staner C, et al. Effects of pro- formance in elderly subjects after a single dose of be-
longed-release melatonin, zolpidem, and their combina- floxatone, a new reversible selective monoamine oxidase
tion on psychomotor functions, memory recall, and driv- A inhibitor. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1998; 64 (2): 211-22
ing skills in healthy middle aged and elderly volunteers. 87. Warot D, Berlin I, Patat A, et al. Effects pf befloxatone, a
Hum Psychopharmacol 2008; 23 (8): 693-705 reversible selective monoamine oxidase-A inhibitor, on

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
Drug-Induced Cognitive Impairment 657

psychomotor function and memory in healthy subjects. sedation, cognition, mood, and psychomotor perfor-
J Clin Pharmacol 1996; 36: 942-50 mance. Arch Intern Med 1997; 157 (20): 2350-6
88. Fairweather DB, Kerr JS, Hilton S, et al. A placebo con- 102. Hindmarch I, Shamsi Z. The effects of single and repeated
trolled double-blind evaluation of the pharmacodynamics administration of ebastine on cognition and psychomotor
of fengabine vs amitriptyline following single and multiple performance in comparison to triprolidine and placebo in
doses in elderly volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1993; 35 healthy volunteers. Curr Med Res Opin 2001; 17 (4): 273-81
(3): 278-83
103. Theunissen EL, van Kroonenburgh MJ, van Deursen JA,
89. Sakulsripong M, Curran HV, Lader M. Does tolerance et al. Stimulating effects of the antihistamine fexofenadine:
develop to the sedative and amnesic effects of antide- testing the dopamine transporter hypothesis. Psycho-
pressants? A comparison of amitriptyline, trazodone and pharmacology (Berl) 2006; 187 (1): 95-102
placebo. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 1991; 40: 40-8
104. Quigley N, Morgan D, Idzikowski C, et al. The effect of
90. Van Laar MW, Volkerts ER, Verbaten MN, et al. Differ- chlorpromazine and benzhexol on memory and psycho-
ential effects of amitriptyline, nefazodone and paroxetine motor function in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol
on performance and brain indices of visual selective 1996; 10 (2): 146-52
attention and working memory. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 2002; 162: 351-63 105. Beuzen JN, Taylor N, Wesnes K, et al. A comparison of the
effects of olanzapine, haloperidol and placebo on cogni-
91. Siepmann M, Krause S, Joraschky P, et al. The effects of
tive and psychomotor functions in healthy elderly volun-
St John’s wort extract on heart rate variability, cognitive
teers. J Psychopharmacol 1999; 13 (2): 152-8
function and quantitative EEG: a comparison with ami-
triptyline and placebo in healthy men. Br J Clin Pharma- 106. Hanlon JT, Horner RD, Schmader KE, et al. Benzodiazepine
col 2002; 54: 277-82 use and cognitive function among community-dwelling
92. van Laar MW, van Willigenburg AP, Volkerts ER. Acute elderly. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1998; 64 (6): 684-92
and subchronic effects of nefazodone and imipramine on 107. Bierman EJ, Comijs HC, Gundy CM, et al. The effect of
highway driving, cognitive functions, and daytime sleepi- chronic benzodiazepine use on cognitive functioning in
ness in healthy adult and elderly subjects. J Clin Psycho- older persons: good, bad or indifferent? Int J Geriatr
pharmacol 1995; 15 (1): 30-40 Psychiatry 2007; 22 (12): 1194-200
93. Curran HV, Sakulsriprong M, Lader M. Antidepressants 108. Ebly EM, Hogan DB, Fung TS. Potential adverse out-
and human memory: an investigation of four drugs with comes of psychotropic and narcotic drug use in Canadian
different sedative and anticholinergic profiles. Psycho- seniors. J Clin Epidemiol 1997; 50 (7): 857-63
pharmacology (Berl) 1988; 95: 520-7 109. Paterniti S, Dufouil C, Alperovitch A. Long-term ben-
94. Conen S, Theunissen EL, Vermeeren A, et al. Short-term zodiazepine use and cognitive decline in the elderly: the
effects of morning versus evening dose of hydroxyzine Epidemiology of Vascular Aging Study. J Clin Psycho-
50 mg on cognition in healthy volunteers. J Clin Psycho- pharmacol 2002; 22 (3): 285-93
pharmacol 2011; 31 (3): 294-301
110. Allard J, Artero S, Ritchie K. Consumption of psycho-
95. Shamsi Z, Kimber S, Hindmarch I. An investigation into tropic medication in the elderly: a re-evaluation of its ef-
the effects of cetirizine on cognitive function and psy- fect on cognitive performance. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
chomotor performance in healthy volunteers. Eur J Clin 2003; 18 (10): 874-8
Pharmacol 2001; 56 (12): 865-71
111. Ancelin ML, Artero S, Portet F, et al. Non-degenerative
96. Nicholson AN, Handford ADF, Turner C, et al. Studies on mild cognitive impairment in elderly people and use of
performance and sleepiness with the H1-antihistamine, anticholinergic drugs: longitudinal cohort study. BMJ
desloratadine. Aviat Space Environ Med 2003; 74 (8): 2006; 332 (7539): 455-8
809-15
112. Low LF, Anstey KJ, Sachdev P. Use of medications with
97. Hindmarch I, Johnson S, Meadows R, et al. The acute and
anticholinergic properties and cognitive function in a
sub-chronic effects of levocetirizine, cetirizine, loratadine,
young-old community sample. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
promethazine and placebo on cognitive function, psy-
2009; 24 (6): 578-84
chomotor performance, and weal and flare. Curr Med Res
Opin 2001; 17 (4): 241-55 113. Cancelli I, Gigli GL, Piani A, et al. Drugs with anti-
98. Ridout F, Hindmarch I. The effects of acute doses of fex- cholinergic properties as a risk factor for cognitive impair-
ofenadine, promethazine, and placebo on cognitive and ment in elderly people: a population-based study. J Clin
psychomotor function in healthy Japanese volunteers. Psychopharmacol 2008; 28 (6): 654-9
Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol 2003; 90 (4): 404-10 114. Cao YJ, Mager DE, Simonsick EM, et al. Physical and
99. Scavone JM, Greenblatt DJ, Harmatz JS, et al. Pharma- cognitive performance and burden of anticholinergics,
cokinetics and pharmacodynamics of diphenhydramine sedatives, and ACE inhibitors in older women. Clin
25 mg in young and elderly volunteers. J Clin Pharmacol Pharmacol Ther 2008; 83 (3): 422-9
1998; 38 (7): 603-9 115. Hilmer SN, Mager DE, Simonsick EM, et al. A drug bur-
100. Mansfield L, Mendoza C, Flores J, et al. Effects of fex- den index to define the functional burden of medications
ofenadine, diphenhydramine, and placebo on perfor- in older people. Arch Intern Med 2007; 167 (8): 781-7
mance of the test of variables of attention (TOVA). Ann 116. Lechevallier-Michel N, Molimard M, Dartigues JF, et al.
Allergy Asthma Immunol 2003; 90 (5): 554-9 Drugs with anticholinergic properties and cognitive per-
101. Kay GG, Berman B, Mockoviak SH, et al. Initial and formance in the elderly: results from the PAQUID Study.
steady-state effects of diphenhydramine and loratadine on Br J Clin Pharmacol 2004; 59 (2): 143-51

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)
658 Tannenbaum et al.

117. Merchant RA, Li B, Yap KB, et al. Use of drugs with dementia in an elderly general population: the 3-city study.
anticholinegic effects and cognitive impairment in com- Arch Intern Med 2009; 169 (14): 1317-24
munity-living older persons. Age Ageing 2008; 38 (1): 123. Campbell NL, Boustani MA, Lane KA, et al. Use of anti-
105-8 cholinergics and the risk of cognitive impairment in an
118. Uusvaara J, Pitkala KH, Tienari PJ, et al. Association be- African American population. Neurology 2010; 75 (2):
tween anticholinergic drugs and apolipoprotein e epsilon4 152-9
allele and poorer cognitive function in older cardio- 124. Fox C, Richardson K, Maidment ID, et al. Anticholinergic
vascular patients: a cross-sectional study. J Am Geriatr medication use and cognitive impairment in the older
Soc 2009; 57 (3): 427-31 population: the medical research council cognitive func-
119. Podewils LJ, Lyketsos CG. Tricyclic antidepressants and tion and ageing study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2011; 59 (8):
cognitive decline. Psychosomatics 2002; 43 (1): 31-5 1477-83
120. Bottiggi KA, Salazar JC, Yu L, et al. Lone-term cognitive 125. Curran HV. Tranquillising memories: a review of the ef-
impact of anticholinergic medications in older adults. Am fects of benzodiazepines on human memory. Biol Psychol
J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006; 14 (11): 980-4 1986; 23 (2): 179-213
121. Basu R, Dodge H, Stoehr GP, et al. Sedative-hypnotic use
of diphenhydramine in a rural, older adult, community-
based cohort: effects on cognition. Am J Geriatr Psychi- Correspondence: Dr Cara Tannenbaum, Faculties of Phar-
atry 2003; 11 (2): 205-13 macy and Medicine, Université de Montréal, 4565 Queen
122. Carriere I, Fourrier-Reglat A, Dartigues JF, et al. Drugs Mary Road, Montreal, Québec, Canada H3W 1W5.
with anticholinergic properties, cognitive decline, and E-mail: cara.tannenbaum@umontreal.ca

Adis ª 2012 Springer International Publishing AG. All rights reserved. Drugs Aging 2012; 29 (8)

You might also like