You are on page 1of 8

CYBERPSYCHOLOGY, BEHAVIOR, AND SOCIAL NETWORKING

Volume 14, Number 6, 2011


ª Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.
DOI: 10.1089/cyber.2010.0222

Watching Pornographic Pictures on the Internet: Role of


Sexual Arousal Ratings and Psychological–Psychiatric
Symptoms for Using Internet Sex Sites Excessively

Matthias Brand, Ph.D.,1,2 Christian Laier, M.Sc.,1,3 Mirko Pawlikowski, M.Sc.,1 Ulrich Schächtle, B.Sc.,1
Tobias Schöler, B.Sc.,1 and Christine Altstötter-Gleich, Ph.D. 3

Abstract

Excessive or addictive Internet use can be linked to different online activities, such as Internet gaming or
cybersex. The usage of Internet pornography sites is one important facet of online sexual activity. The aim of the
present work was to examine potential predictors of a tendency toward cybersex addiction in terms of subjective
complaints in everyday life due to online sexual activities. We focused on the subjective evaluation of Internet
pornographic material with respect to sexual arousal and emotional valence, as well as on psychological
symptoms as potential predictors. We examined 89 heterosexual, male participants with an experimental task
assessing subjective sexual arousal and emotional valence of Internet pornographic pictures. The Internet Ad-
diction Test (IAT) and a modified version of the IAT for online sexual activities (IATsex), as well as several
further questionnaires measuring psychological symptoms and facets of personality were also administered to
the participants. Results indicate that self-reported problems in daily life linked to online sexual activities were
predicted by subjective sexual arousal ratings of the pornographic material, global severity of psychological
symptoms, and the number of sex applications used when being on Internet sex sites in daily life, while the time
spent on Internet sex sites (minutes per day) did not significantly contribute to explanation of variance in IATsex
score. Personality facets were not significantly correlated with the IATsex score. The study demonstrates the
important role of subjective arousal and psychological symptoms as potential correlates of development or
maintenance of excessive online sexual activity.

Introduction pornography by employees in the workplace,12 the increased


risk of acquiring sexually transmitted diseases in individuals

N owadays, the Internet is used in daily life as a very


helpful tool. However, some individuals have an in-
ability to control their use of the Internet and may therefore
who excessively seek sexual partners through cybersex
sites,13 and—as a last example—the potential link between
pornography consumption and aggression.14 The importance
have problems in their everyday life with respect to social of considering correlates of excessive cybersex is also em-
relationships, work or academic processes, financial issues, phasized by Kafka.15
and psychological well-being.1–3 The phenomenon of exces- Although the topic of cybersex addiction has high clinical
sive Internet use is frequently called Internet addiction (e.g., relevance, it has been almost neglected in previous re-
Young,2 Block,4 Chou et al.,5 Widyanto and Griffiths,6 and search.16,17 In most of the studies on cognitive or personality
Praterelli and Browne7), although it has been termed differ- correlates of Internet activities in general, online/computer
entially in recent articles.8–11 gamers were primarily included in the samples18–20 or no
One form of Internet addiction is excessive cybersex, which distinction between different online activities has been
seems to be a rising problem, particularly in industrial na- made.21–24 Studies that particularly investigate experimen-
tions. Some authors claim that cybersex has the highest po- tally potential mechanisms of cybersex addiction are missing.
tential for developing an Internet addiction.8 Negative Cybersex comprises several active or passive forms, such
consequences of excessive cybersex comprise the use of online as seeking sexual partners in real life, sex chats, sex via web

1
General Psychology: Cognition, University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany.
2
Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Essen, Germany.
3
Differential and Personality Psychology, University of Koblenz-Landau, Germany.

371
372 BRAND ET AL.

camera, and so on. The consumption of cyberpornography is addiction,18,19,30–33 together with the literature on comorbid
also an important facet of cybersex. Although some infor- psychological symptoms in subjects with behavioral addic-
mation about sociodemographic characteristics of cyberpor- tions and disorders of impulse control,41–44 we particularly
nography users exist,16,17,25 no empirical study has assessed hypothesized that a tendency toward cybersex addiction—in
directly how the consumption of cyberpornography is per- terms of subjectively experienced negative consequences of
ceived by users.16 Young26 proposed that the expectation of cybersex in everyday life—is predicted by subjective sexual
finding sexual arousal and gratification is one key element of arousal when watching Internet pornographic material and
motivation for cybersex (see also Young3). This is principally global severity of psychological symptoms. We also hypoth-
in accordance with self-reports of subjects who excessively esized that the range of online sexual activities (number of
consume cyberpornography.27 However, as has been pointed online sex applications used) and the time spent on Internet
out by Griffith,28 no strong experimental empirical data exist sex sites additionally predict the degree of self-reported
that support the claims made by Young.3,26 On a theoretical problems in everyday life due to excessive cybersex. The
level, it makes sense to assume that subjects addicted to cy- hypotheses are also summarized in Figure 1.
bersex experience a positive emotional episode accompanied
by sexual arousal when consuming cybersex sites. It might Materials and Methods
also be that subjects who consume cybersex sites excessively
Participants
have a salience for the incentive in terms of sexual arousal (see
the discussion on ‘‘wanting’’ and ‘‘liking’’ as two components We examined 89 heterosexual male participants (mean age
of reward, e.g. Berridge et al.29). However, these speculations 23.98, SD ¼ 4.09 years). Mean education of the sample was
need to be tested empirically. 13.42 years (SD ¼ 1.71). Participants were recruited through a
On a hypothesized level, we see some parallels between local advertisement and were paid at an hourly rate for par-
cognitive and brain mechanisms potentially contributing to ticipation (10.00 e/hour). The advertisement indicated that
the maintenance of excessive cybersex and those described heterosexual men are invited to participate in a study on In-
for individuals with substance dependence or behavioral ternet pornography and that the subjects will be confronted
addiction (e.g., pathological gambling). For instance, it is with Internet pornographic material. Previous consumption
known that the brain of subjects with alcoholism or other of cybersex sites was not a necessary criterion for participa-
substance dependence reacts emotionally (activations of the tion. Inclusion criteria required that individuals have no
ventral striatum) when being confronted with alcohol- or history of neurological or psychiatric disease, as determined
drug-related pictures.30–32 Other studies also emphasize that by a screening. Any substance-related disorders were also
craving reactions (cue-reactivity) can be found in subjects exclusion criteria. All participants gave written informed
with behavioral addictions, such as pathological gambling33 consent prior to the investigation.
and—most recently—even in subjects who excessively play A total of 51 participants (57.3%) were in a heterosexual
World of Warcraft19 or other computer games.18 These partnership, 35 (39.3%) were single, and 3 (3.3%) did not re-
studies converge to the view that craving reactions on spond to this question. Mean age at first Internet use was
watching addiction-related stimuli are important correlates of 13.90 (SD ¼ 2.88) years meaning that the average duration of
the addictive behavior. Internet use was 10.08 (SD ¼ 2.88) years. Mean days per week
The limbic and para-limbic structures mentioned (e.g., of Internet use for personal reasons was 6.44 (SD ¼ 1.13) and
ventral striatum) are also involved in sexual behavior and are the subjects spent on average 223.87 (SD ¼ 107.88) minutes
connected with other brain structures that play important per day on the Internet (mean Internet use of 26.12 hours
roles in sexual arousal and sexual activity.34–40 Therefore, it per week). Regarding cybersex usage, all 89 subjects reported
seems plausible that those brain regions involved in proces-
sing sexual stimuli, and sexual arousal and activity, as well as
in craving reactions in individuals with behavioral addic-
tions, are also crucially relevant for development and main-
tenance of addictive behavior in the context of cybersex.

Aims and hypotheses of the present study


The main aim of the present study was to investigate po-
tential correlates and predictors of subjective complaints in
everyday life due to excessive online sexual activities (as a
measure of a tendency toward cybersex addiction) in a lab-
oratory experimental setting. As a main potential predictor of
these subjective complaints, we concentrated on the subjec-
tive evaluation of cyberpornographic pictures, that is, sub-
jective ratings of the pictures’ arousal. We also investigated
the degree of psychological symptoms such as social anxiety,
depression, and compulsivity. Furthermore, we assessed the
usage of specific cybersex applications, as well as several fa-
cets of personality (reward sensitivity, shyness). FIG. 1. Illustration of the hypothesized predictors of cy-
Given the literature on cue reactivity in substance-depen- bersex addiction in terms of subjectively experienced nega-
dent individuals and those with specific types of behavioral tive consequences of online sexual activities in everyday life.
CYBERSEX ADDICTION 373

that they had used cybersex sites at least once in their life. ‘‘online sexual activity’’ and ‘‘Internet sex sites’’ respectively
Mean age at first online sexual activity was 16.33 (SD ¼ 3.56) (we termed this modified version IATsex). This IATsex was
years. Mean days per week of cybersex use was 2.0 used to assess subjective complaints in everyday life due to
(SD ¼ 1.85, range ¼ 0–7) and the subjects spent on average online sexual activities and potential symptoms of cybersex
36.07 (SD ¼ 31.21, range ¼ 0–150) minutes per day on cyber- addiction. An example for an item of the original IAT and the
sex sites (72.14 minutes per week, SD ¼ 62.44, range ¼ 0–300). modified version (IATsex) is: ‘‘How often do you find that
The latter scores are in accordance with that reported previ- you stay online longer than you intended?’’ (original IAT)
ously.25,45,46 and ‘‘How often do you find that you stay on Internet sex sites
longer than you intended?’’ (IATsex). Both IAT versions used
Procedure consist of 20 items, and the scale used ranged from 1 to 5
(‘‘rarely’’ to ‘‘always’’), resulting in a potential score between
All questionnaires and the experimental paradigm were
20 and 100. Internal consistencies (Cronbach’s a) of these
administered to the participants in a laboratory setting. All
scales were IAT (a ¼ 0.878) and IATsex (a ¼ 0.842).
tasks and questionnaires were computer-based with the ex-
ception of the Symptom Checklist. The whole examination
Further information concerning online sexual activi-
including instructions and debriefing took approximately 75
ties. Participants were asked to indicate how often (on a
minutes.
scale of 0 to 4, where 0 ¼ ‘‘never’’ and 4 ¼ ’’always when on-
line’’) they use different types of cybersex (e.g., pornographic
Instruments
pictures, videos, literature, sex via web camera, sex chat,
Experimental paradigm. For the assessment of subjective searching for sexual partners). They were also asked how
emotional evaluations and arousal experienced while often (on a scale of 0 to 4, where 0 ¼ ‘‘never’’ and 4 ¼ ‘‘always
watching Internet pornographic stimuli, we used 40 stan- when online’’) they prefer different types of pornographic
dardized pictures that either showed a single masturbating material (e.g., one single naked or masturbating women, one
women or a male/female couple during sexual intercourse. women having sex with one man, two women and one man,
The sexual behavior was clearly shown on each picture. The two men and one women, group sex, sex between two wo-
women/men shown had an estimated age between 20 and 35 men or between two men). Finally, several sexual practices or
years. In order to make the pictures as comparable as possible fetishes (e.g., vaginal, oral, or anal penetration, striptease,
to the real world situation, we used a standard browser leather, fisting, mature, spanking, etc.) were listed, and sub-
window in which we pasted the different pictures. In the jects were asked whether or not they principally have a pref-
browser window, the Web site address was standardized erence toward these pornographic materials on the Internet
with a non-existing webpage (www.sexbild.de). All other (answer mode yes/no; all together 18 practices/fetishes were
information shown (time, programs opened, etc.) was also assessed).
standardized. On each picture, only one image was shown in
the center of the browser window. The images were selected Psychological–psychiatric symptoms, reward responsive-
from freely accessible Web sites containing legal content and ness, and shyness. Psychological–psychiatric symptoms
which were free of charge. The images did not contain any were assessed by the Symptom Check List (SCL-90-R),49
fetish relevant content. which consists of nine subscales: somatization, obsessive–
The subjects were asked to rate each image separately with compulsiveness, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxi-
respect to sexual arousal (scale ranging from 1 to 7, where ety, anger–hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation, and
1 ¼ ‘‘no sexual arousal’’ and 7 ¼ ‘‘high sexual arousal’’), psychoticism. In addition, a global severity index can be
emotional valence (scale ranging from 1 to 7, where calculated. Furthermore, we used the German short ques-
1 ¼ ‘‘negative emotional valence’’ and 7 ¼ ‘‘positive emotional tionnaire version50 of the BIS/BAS scale51 to assess reward
valence’’), and representativeness for cyberpornographic responsiveness and punishment sensitivity. We also assessed
material (scale ranging from 1 to 7, where 1 ¼ ‘‘the picture is shyness and sociability with the Shyness and Sociability
not representative’’ and 7 ¼ ‘‘the picture is highly represen- Scales by Asendorpf.52
tative’’). The rating of the representativeness of the pictures
was included to assure that we had selected material that was
Results
representative of the pictures that are consumed in everyday
life. The order of picture presentation was randomized. In- The mean rating scores on the three dimensions were near
ternal consistencies (Cronbach’s a) of the scales were: sexual the middle of the scales’ range: sexual arousal mean ¼ 3.65
arousal rating (a ¼ 0.951), emotional valence rating (SD ¼ 1.04), emotional valence mean ¼ 3.65 (SD ¼ 0.96), and
(a ¼ 0.962), and representativeness rating (a ¼ 0.977). representativeness mean ¼ 4.88 (SD ¼ 1.16). The IAT scores
and the IATsex scores were: IAT mean ¼ 30.67 (SD ¼ 9.2,
Two versions of the Internet Addiction Test. Subjective range 20–66), IATsex mean ¼ 23.66 (SD ¼ 5.56, range 20–56).
complaints in everyday life due to excessive usage of the The mean number of practices for which subjects had a
Internet and potential symptoms of Internet addiction were preference when using Internet pornographic websites was
assessed by a German version of the Internet Addiction Test 5.61 (SD ¼ 2.86). The IAT and IATsex were correlated highly
(IAT).47,48 The original English version was translated into (r ¼ 0.657, p < 0.001). The bivariate correlations between the
German by a bilingual English/German speaker and re- picture ratings, IATsex, and other variables are shown in
translated by a second bilingual speaker. In addition, we used Tables 1 and 2.
a modified version of the IAT in which the terms ‘‘online’’ or In order to evaluate further the relationships between
‘‘Internet’’ in the original IAT were replaced by the terms IATsex score (as the dependent variable) and the potential
374 BRAND ET AL.

Table 1. Correlations Between Mean Picture Ratings, IATsex, Time per Day Spent on Internet Sex Sites,
and Several Variables of Internet Sex Usage (Pearson Correlations)

Sexual arousal Emotional valence Representativeness Time


Domain/variable of pictures of pictures of pictures IATsex (min/day)

Sexual arousal of pictures 0.760** 0.268* 0.249* 0.123


Emotional valence of pictures 0.235* 0.162 0.094
Representativeness of pictures 0.010 0.046
IATsex 0.203
Number of practices/fetishes1 0.381** 0.270* 0.140 0.231* 0.346**
Frequency of using Internet sex applications:2 0.254* 0.178 0.230* 0.073 0.049
Sexual pictures
Sexual videos 0.459** 0.296** 0.097 0.322** 0.180
Sexual literature 0.038 0.040 0.091 0.078 0.163
Sex per web camera 0.096 0.094 0.150 0.174 0.113
Information about sex 0.035 0.037 0.104 0.024 0.066
Sex chat 0.177 0.236* 0.206 0.185 0.066
Searching for sexual partners 0.093 0.100 0.224* 0.502** 0.100
Overall3 0.169 0.081 0.021 0.365** 0.175

*p  0.05 (correlation is significantly different from zero with a ¼ 5%, two-tailed); **p  0.01 (correlation is significantly different from zero
with alpha ¼ 1%, two-tailed). Bold value indicates significant correlation.
1
Range 0–18; 2range 0–4 (0 ¼ ‘‘never,’’ 4 ¼ ‘‘always when online’’); 3mean frequency of using the above-mentioned Internet sex applications
(range 0–4).

predictors of sexual arousal rating, global severity of psy- an overall explanation of the IATsex score’s variance of
chological symptoms, mean overall use of Internet sex 12.7% (changes in R2 ¼ 0.06, changes in F ¼ 6.34, df1 ¼ 1,
applications, and time spent on Internet sex sites (see hy- df2 ¼ 86, p ¼ 0.014). By entering the mean usage of Internet
potheses), we calculated a hierarchical regression analysis sex applications as an additional predictor (third step), the
(all variables centralized).53 The order of variables included changes in R2 were also significant, resulting in an overall
in this regression analysis represents the order of hypothe- explanation of the IATsex score’s variance of 23.7% (changes
sized significance of the predictor variables (see hypothe- in R2 ¼ 0.11, changes in F ¼ 12.33, df1 ¼ 1, df2 ¼ 85, p ¼ 0.001).
ses). As a first step, sexual arousal rating was a significant Finally, entering the time in minutes/day spent on Internet
predictor of IATsex score (R2 ¼ 0.06, F ¼ 5.76, df1 ¼ 1, sex sites (fourth step) did not significantly contribute to ex-
df2 ¼ 87, p ¼ 0.018). When adding (second step) the global planation of variance of the IATsex score (changes in
severity index of psychological symptoms (SCL GSI score) R2 ¼ 0.004, changes in F ¼ 0.49, df1 ¼ 1, df2 ¼ 84, p ¼ 0.485;
as a predictor, the changes in R2 were significant, resulting in see Table 3 for further values).

Table 2. Correlations Between Mean Picture Ratings, IATsex, Time per Day Spent on Internet Sex Sites,
and Psychological–Psychiatric Symptoms, BIS/BAS, Shyness, and Sociability (Pearson Correlations)

Sexual arousal Emotional valence Representativeness Time


Domain/variable of pictures of pictures of pictures IATsex (min/day)

SCL-90-R
Somatization 0.103 0.068 0.022 0.095 0.093
Obsessive–compulsive 0.176 0.133 0.039 0.259* 0.136
Interpersonal sensitivity 0.230* 0.183 0.052 0.329** 0.188
Depression 0.212* 0.221* 0.005 0.299** 0.206
Anxiety 0.107 0.063 0.051 0.230* 0.073
Anger–hostility 0.105 0.169 0.091 0.187 0.152
Phobic anxiety 0.122 0.124 0.116 0.257* 0.186
Paranoid ideation 0.151 0.111 0.025 0.301** 0.080
Psychoticism 0.249* 0.226* 0.127 0.303** 0.259*
Global severity index 0.219* 0.219* 0.055 0.302** 0.240*
BIS/BAS
BIS 1: Anxiety/nervousness 0.023 0.027 0.210* 0.198 0.126
BIS 2: Frustration/tearfulness 0.071 0.037 0.270* 0.183 0.222*
BAS 1: Drive in view of a goal 0.048 0.008 0.180 0.178 0.051
BAS 2: Reward responsiveness 0.031 0.096 0.137 0.074 0.110
Shyness 0.146 0.099 0.253* 0.061 0.091
Sociability 0.164 0.013 0.026 0.036 0.087

*p  0.05 (correlation is significantly different from zero with a ¼ 5%, two-tailed); **p  0.01 (correlation is significantly different from zero
with a ¼ 1%, two-tailed). Bold value indicates significant correlation.
CYBERSEX ADDICTION 375

Table 3. Hierarchical Regression Analysis indeed emphasize that higher sexual arousal is linked to a
with the IATsex Score as the Dependent Variable tendency toward being addicted to cybersex and related
problems in everyday life.
b T p
Finally, we have to mention some important limitations of
Main effects ‘‘sexual arousal rating’’ 0.25 2.40 0.018 the current study. First, the sample was relatively small.
‘‘global severity index’’ 0.26 2.52 0.014 However, one has to keep in mind that subjects enrolled in
‘‘usage of Internet sex applications’’ 0.34 3.51 0.001 this investigation were assessed in a laboratory setting with
‘‘min/day on Internet sex sites’’ 0.07 0.70 0.485 an individual assessment, which makes the data revealed
more valid in comparison to studies using online question-
naires because we were able to control for environmental
Discussion
variables that may influence subjects’ responses on the tasks.
We found a positive relationship between subjective sexual In addition, we screened for previous psychiatric and neu-
arousal when watching Internet pornographic pictures and rologic diseases, which also contributes to the homogeneity
the self-reported problems in daily life due to the excessive- of the sample. Although we excluded subjects with any
ness of cybersex as measured by the IATsex. Subjective substance-related disorder, we did not document current
arousal ratings, the global severity of psychological symp- substance use in detail (e.g., alcohol, cannabis). Future studies
toms, and the number of sex applications used were signifi- may address potential correlations between a tendency to-
cant predictors of the IATsex score, while the time spent on ward cybersex addiction and consumption of different sub-
Internet sex sites did not significantly contribute to explana- stances. Second, we freely recruited our participants by
tion of variance in the IATsex score. advertisements, producing a sample that consisted of ‘‘nor-
The finding that subjective sexual arousal ratings while mal’’ healthy individuals. Accordingly, we had a non-clinical
watching Internet pornographic pictures is related to self-re- sample, although some of the subjects reported high IATsex
ported problems in daily life due to excessive use of cybersex scores, which potentially indicate symptom severity that
sites may be interpreted in the light of previous studies on fulfills diagnostic criteria for behavioral addictions.54 Our
cue reactivity in individuals with substance dependency or data need replication with a larger sample and with subjects
behavioral addictions. As outlined in the introduction, cue- suffering from sexual addiction. In future studies, potential
reactivity as a mechanism potentially contributing to the correlates of cybersex addiction in women and also in ho-
maintenance of addicted behavior has been demonstrated in mosexual men and women should be investigated. In our
several patient groups with either substance dependence or study, only heterosexual men were included, and the por-
behavioral addiction.18,19,30–33 These studies converge to the nographic stimuli used in the experimental design were se-
view that craving reactions on watching addiction-related lected with and for male eyes. Further studies may use
stimuli are important correlates of the addictive behavior. additional pornographic pictures representative for other
Although we did not examine brain correlates of watching samples with respect to gender and sexual orientation.
Internet pornographic pictures in our study, we found the Although the limitations mentioned have to be kept in
first experimental evidence for the potential link between mind, we conclude that our study revealed first reference for
subjective reactivity on Internet pornographic stimuli and a the important role of subjective arousal and psychological
tendency toward cybersex addiction. symptoms as potential correlates of development or mainte-
The relationship between self-reported problems in daily nance of excessive online sexual activity in heterosexual men.
life linked to cybersex (IATsex) and several psychological Given the lack of empirical studies on this topic,16,17,28 our
symptoms is consistent with a previous study by Yang et al.43 current study contributes to filling the gap and will hopefully
in which the SCL-90-R was also used to measure psycho- inspire future research on the very important topic of cyber-
logical symptoms in subjects with excessive Internet use in sex addiction.3
comparison to moderate and mild users. However, in the
study by Yang et al., no differentiation between specific types Disclosure Statement
of Internet usage (gaming, sex sites, etc.) was made. In our
sample, the global symptom severity (SCL GSI), as well as No competing financial interests exist.
interpersonal sensitivity, depression, paranoid thinking and
psychoticism, were correlated particularly with the IATsex References
score. In contrast, time spent on cybersex sites (minutes per
1. Young KS. Internet addiction: The emergence of a new
day) was widely unrelated to psychological symptoms. The
clinical disorder. CyberPsychology & Behavior 1998; 1:
actual time spent on cybersex sites was also not significantly
237–44.
correlated with the IATsex score. This means that for prob-
2. Young KS. Internet addiction: A new clinical phenomenon
lems in daily life (e.g., reduced control over online sexual and its consequences. American Behavioral Scientist 2004;
activities, problems with the own partner or in other inter- 48:402–15.
personal relationships, as well as problems in academic or 3. Young KS. Internet sex addiction: Risk factors, stages of
work life), the time spent on cybersex sites is not predictive. development, and treatment. American Behavioral Scientist
The results of our study—in particular the correlation be- 2008; 52:21–37.
tween subjective arousal ratings of pornographic material 4. Block J. Issues for DSM-V: Internet addiction. American
and the reported negative consequences of cybersex in daily Journal of Psychiatry 2008; 165:306–7.
life—are in line with Young.26 She proposed that the expec- 5. Chou C, Condron L, Belland JC. A review of the research on
tation of finding sexual arousal may be one of the key ele- Internet addiction. Educational Psychology Review 2005;
ments of motivation for online sexual activities.3 Our results 17:363–87.
376 BRAND ET AL.

6. Widyanto L, Griffiths M. ‘‘Internet addiction’’: A critical re- 27. Cavaglion G. Cyber-porn dependence: Voices of distress in
view. International Journal of Mental Health & Addiction an Italian internet self-help community. International Jour-
2006; 4:31–51. nal of Mental Health & Addiction 2009; 7:295–310.
7. Pratarelli ME, Browne BL. Confirmatory factor analysis of 28. Griffiths M. Sex on the Internet: Observations and implica-
Internet use and addiction. CyberPsychology & Behavior tions for Internet sex addiction. Journal of Sex Research 2001;
2002; 5:53–64. 38:333–42.
8. Meerkerk GJ, van den Eijnden RJJM, Garretsen HFL. Pre- 29. Berridge KC, Robinson TE, Aldridge JW. Dissecting com-
dicting compulsive Internet use: It’s all about sex! CyberP- ponents of reward: ‘‘Liking,’’ ‘‘wanting,’’ and learning. Cur-
sychology & Behavior 2006; 9:95–103. rent Opinion in Pharmacology 2009; 9:65–73.
9. Caplan SE. Problematic Internet use and psychosocial well- 30. Braus DF, Wrase J, Grüsser S, et al. Alcohol-associated
being: Development of a theory-based cognitive–behavioral stimuli activate the ventral striatum in abstinent alcoholics.
measurement instrument. Computers in Human Behavior Journal of Neural Transmission 2001; 108:887–94.
2002; 18:553–75. 31. Garavan H, Pankiewicz J, Bloom A, et al. Cue-induced co-
10. Davis RA. A cognitive–behavioral model of pathological caine craving: Neuroanatomical specificity for drug users
Internet use. Computers in Human Behavior 2001; 17:187–95. and drug stimuli. The American Journal of Psychiatry 2000;
11. LaRose R, Lin CA, Eastin MS. Unregulated internet usage: 157:1789–98.
Addiction, habit, or deficient self-regulation? Media Psy- 32. Grüsser S, Wrase J, Klein S, et al. Cue-induced activation of
chology 2003; 5:225–53. the striatum and medial prefrontal cortex is associated with
12. Cooper A, Golden GH, Kent-Ferraro J. Online sexual behav- subsequent relapse in abstinent alcoholics. Psychopharma-
iors in the workplace: How can human resource depart- cology 2004; 175:296–302.
ments and employee assistance programs respond effectively? 33. Potenza MN, Steinberg MA, Skudlarski P, et al. Gambling
Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity 2002; 9:149–65. urges in pathological gambling: A functional magnetic res-
13. McFarlane M, Sheana S, Rietmeijer C. The Internet as a onance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry 2003;
newly emerging risk environment for sexually transmitted 60:828–36.
diseases. Journal of the American Medical Association 2000; 34. Balfour ME, Yu L, Coolen LM. Sexual behavior and sex-
284:443–6. associated environmental cues activate the mesolimbic
14. Kingston DA, Fedoroff P, Firestone P, et al. Pornography use system in male rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:
and sexual aggression: The impact of frequency and type of 718–30.
pornography use on recidivism among sexual offenders. 35. Bancroft J. The endocrinology of sexual arousal. Journal of
Aggressive Behavior 2008; 34:341–51. Endocrinology 2005; 186:411–27.
15. Kafka MP. Hypersexual disorder: A proposed diagnosis for 36. Georgiadis JR, Holstege G. Human brain activation during
DSM-V. Archives of Sexual Behavior 2010; 39:377–400. sexual stimulation of the penis. The Journal of Comparative
16. Döring NM. The Internet’s impact on sexuality: A critical Neurology 2005; 493:33–8.
review of 15 years of research. Computers in Human Be- 37. Holstege G, Georgiadis JR, Paans AMJ, et al. Brain activation
havior 2009; 25:1089–101. during human male ejaculation. The Journal of Neuroscience
17. Griffiths M. Excessive Internet use: Implications for sexual 2003; 23:9185–93.
behavior. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2000; 3:537–52. 38. Hu SH, Wei N, Wang Q, et al. Patterns of brain activation
18. Thalemann R, Wölfling K, Grüsser SM. Specific cue reac- during visually evoked sexual arousal differ between ho-
tivity on computer game-related cues in excessive gamers. mosexual and heterosexual men. American Journal of Neu-
Behavioral Neuroscience 2007; 121:614–8. roradiology 2008; 29:1890–6.
19. Ko CH, Liu GC, Hsiao S, et al. Brain activities associated 39. Paul T, Schiffer B, Zwarg T, et al. Brain response to visual
with gaming urge of online gaming addiction. Journal of sexual stimuli in heterosexual and homosexual males. Hu-
Psychiatric Research 2009; 43:739–47. man Brain Mapping 2008; 29:726–35.
20. Teng C. Personality differences between online game players 40. Redouté J, Stoléru S, Grégoire M-C, et al. Brain processing of
and nonplayers in a student sample. CyberPsychology & visual sexual stimuli in human males. Human Brain Map-
Behavior 2008; 11:232–4. ping 2000; 11:162–77.
21. Chak K, Leung L. Shyness and locus of control as predictors 41. Brand M, Franke-Sievert C, Jacoby GE, et al. Neuropsycho-
of Internet addiction and Internet use. CyberPsychology & logical correlates of decision-making in bulimia nervosa.
Behavior 2004; 7:559–70. Neuropsychology 2007; 21:742–50.
22. Lu H. Sensation-seeking, Internet dependency, and online 42. Yang C. Sociopsychiatric characteristics of adolescents who
interpersonal deception. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2008; use computers to excess. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia 2001;
11:227–31. 104:217–22.
23. Morahan-Martin J, Schumacher P. Incidence and correlates 43. Yang C, Choe B, Baity M, et al. SCL-90-R and 16PF profiles
of pathological Internet use among college students. Com- of senior high school students with excessive Internet use.
puters in Human Behavior 2000; 16:13–29. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 2005; 50:407–14.
24. Niemz K, Griffiths M, Banyard P. Prevalence of pathological 44. Yen J, Ko C, Yen C, et al. The comorbid psychiatric
Internet use among university students and correlations symptoms of Internet addiction: Attention Deficit and
with self-esteem, the general health questionnaire (GHQ), and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), depression, social pho-
disinhibition. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2005; 8:562–70. bia, and hostility. Journal of Adolescent Health 2007; 41:
25. Cooper A, Scherer CR, Boies SC, et al. Sexuality on the in- 93–8.
ternet: from sexual exploration to pathological expression. 45. Cooper A, Delmonico DL, Griffin-Shelley E, et al. Online
Professional Psychology: Research & Practice 1999; 30:154–64. sexual activity: An examination of potentially problem-
26. Young KS. (2001) Tangled in the web: Understanding cybersex atic behaviors. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity 2004; 11:
from fantasy to addiction. Bloomington, IN: Authorhouse. 129–43.
CYBERSEX ADDICTION 377

46. Delmonico D, Miller J. The Internet sex screening test: A 52. Asendorpf J. (1997) Schüchternheits- und Geselligkeitsskalen für
comparison of sexual compulsives versus non-sexual com- Erwachsene [Shyness and sociability scales for adults]. Berlin:
pulsives. Sexual & Relationship Therapy 2003; 18:261–76. Humboldt-University, Institute of Psychology.
47. Young KS. (1998) Caught in the net: How to recognize the signs 53. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, et al. (2003) Applied multiple
of Internet addiction—And a winning strategy for recovery. New regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral science. 3rd ed.
York: John Wiley & Sons. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
48. Widyanto L, McMurran M. The psychometric properties of 54. Albrecht U, Kirschner NE, Grüsser SM. Diagnostic instru-
the Internet addiction test. CyberPsychology & Behavior ments for behavioural addiction: An overview. GMS Psy-
2004; 7:443–50. cho-Social-Medicine 2007; 4:1–11.
49. Franke GH. (2002) SCL-90-R—Die Symptom-Checkliste von
L.R. Derogatis. Göttingen, Germany: Beltz Test. Address correspondence to:
50. Hartig J, Moosbrugger H. Die ‘‘ARES-Skalen’’ zur Erfassung Matthias Brand, Ph.D.
der individuellen BIS- und BAS-Sensitivität: Entwicklung General Psychology: Cognition
einer Lang- und einer Kurzfassung. Zeitschrift für Differ- University of Duisburg-Essen
entielle und Diagnostische Psychologie 2003; 24:291–308. Forsthausweg 2
51. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral ac- 47057 Duisburg
tivation, and affective responses to impending reward and Germany
punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality &
Social Psychology 1994; 67:319–33. E-mail: matthias.brand@uni-due.de

You might also like