You are on page 1of 8

Aggression and Violent Behavior 16 (2011) 420–427

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Aggression and Violent Behavior

Marriage markets and mating aggression help explain societal differences in


violent crime
Nigel Barber ⁎
4229 Silver Ct., Birmingham, AL 35213, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Violent crimes (murders, rapes, and assaults) are higher in countries with a relative scarcity of men according
Received 22 December 2010 to research using both United Nations and INTERPOL data and controlling for economic development, income
Accepted 18 January 2011 inequality, urbanization, population density, police presence, and drug trafficking (Barber, 2009a). This is an
Available online 1 February 2011
apparent contradiction given that males are more criminally violent and I argue that the most plausible
explanation is that there is more direct mating effort, and hence more violent crime, in countries having a
Keywords:
Homicide
relative scarcity of men (or a low sex ratio). Alternative explanations that are discussed and found wanting
Sex ratio include cultural determinism. Causal links connecting the marriage market and violent crime include possible
Marriage Markets sociological, physiological, and developmental mechanisms that offer exciting prospects for future
Mating Effort researchers.
© 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Adaptation in modern societies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 421


2. Defining adaptation in a way that suits socially-learned behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 421
2.1. Evolutionary Social Science: a causal evolutionary model for explaining societal differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 422
3. Marriage markets, sex ratios, and social behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 423
4. Violent crime and direct mating effort . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 423
5. The anthropology of mating aggression and violent crime . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 424
6. How to make sense of data linking marriage markets and violent crime . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 424
7. Alternative theoretical explanations for variation in violent crime . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 425
8. Filling in the causal mechanisms through which mating competition increases violence. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 425
9. Discussion and conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 426
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 427

Evolutionary theory rationalizes the great complexity of life for useful for analyzing variation in violent crime even in contemporary
all species and there is no reason why it cannot also illuminate the societies.
causes of variation among human societies (Barber, 2008; Richerson This claim rests upon two theoretical innovations. The first is an
& Boyd, 2004, p. 94–96). All of the complexity of human societies expanded definition of adaptation to include social learning, as well
must have emerged from the same process of natural selection that as genetically-determined variation, so as to render the concept
fashioned the social behavior of other species on planet Earth. applicable to modern societies. The second is the establishment of a
Admittedly, recent human societies, at least from the agricultural chain of causality that describes how natural selection may produce
revolution on, are qualitatively different from earlier societies due modern societal variation. In particular, varying marriage markets
to their complexity, level of technological development, modes of affect violent crime in systematic ways because those societies
information transmission, capacity to store food and concentrate having an excess of females (or a low sex ratio, defined as the
power and wealth, etc. The concept of adaptation is nevertheless number of men per 100 women) elicit more direct mating effort
thereby increasing male–male violence and violent crimes. This
paper discusses the role of adaptive mechanisms in accounting for
⁎ Tel.: + 1 205 956 0508. society-wide differences in violent crime rates with an emphasis on
E-mail address: Barnigel@gmail.com. proximate mechanisms.

1359-1789/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.avb.2011.01.001
N. Barber / Aggression and Violent Behavior 16 (2011) 420–427 421

1. Adaptation in modern societies Adaptations have design-like features in the sense that the phenotype
is explicitly suited for such adjustment, just as a key is suited to turn a
A gene-based evolutionary adaptation is a design-like match particular lock.
between features of the organism and the way that it makes a living In the past, evolutionary psychologists coped with the lack of a
(Reeve & Sherman, 1993; Williams, 1966). An adaptation is often fossil record of behavior in an ingenious, but unsatisfactory, way. They
thought of as providing the solution to an adaptive problem. The long assumed that humans are adapted to the environment of evolutionary
neck of a giraffe solves the problem of reaching into the higher adaptedness (or the ecological conditions that prevailed during the
branches of trees where these animals browse. Adaptations must two-million-year history of Homo) rather than current conditions.
increase biological fitness (or the probability of surviving and Further, they assumed that the brain comprised numerous special-
reproducing) so that individuals manifesting them are biologically purpose information processing devices, or modules, each of which
successful and transmit the alleles that underlie them into future was designed to solve some recurrent problem of the remote
generations at the expense of rival alleles. After many generations of evolutionary past, such as selecting a mate, detecting cheaters on
such genetic selection, favorable alleles get fixed in the population. social contracts, or acquiring language (Cosmides & Tooby, 1987). The
According to the gene-centered view of adaptation shared by structure of these modules was believed to be inherited via a genetic
many evolutionary psychologists an adaptation is found in virtually all program that was driven to fixation through persistent positive
members of a species, at least at the appropriate age or developmental selection so that all humans have essentially the same psychology
stage and if gender appropriate (Buss, Haselton, Shackelford, Bleske, & (with exceptions for gender differences and developmental changes).
Wakefield, 1998). This criterion of universality is far too restrictive if These assumptions are each disputable (Barber, 2008; Laland &
the concept of adaptation is to be applied to behavior, or to societal Brown, 2002). Evolutionary psychology has nevertheless stimulated
differences in behavior, for behavior is not only contingent upon the much empirical research predicated on the assumption that if we
immediate context but is also affected by the developmental understood what problems our ancestors faced, and imagined
environment. According to gene-centered adaptationism, the current possible components of the relevant solutions (or reverse engineer-
fitness impact of some characteristic is deemed irrelevant to its status ing), that we could conduct tests on modern populations to see
as an adaptation: what is required instead, is a demonstration that the whether their minds work as would be predicted using such logic.
characteristic was selected for over a long enough period in the It seems preferable that a natural-science approach to our species
evolutionary past to drive it to fixation in the species-typical genotype would focus on observable behavior rather than “the mind” that
(Buss et al., 1998; Symons, 1992). presupposes subjective self-reports, not to mention a complex system
These two criteria of adaptations having to be universal and of intervening variables, such as modular information processing
requiring deep roots in evolutionary history are too stringent to be mechanisms that are generally difficult to observe, or to reconcile
applied to behavior (as opposed to anatomy that may be studied in with known mechanism of neuroscience or gene expression (Barber,
the fossil record over millions of years). Indeed, insisting upon 2008). Moreover, this approach could not be applied to nonhuman
universality and evolutionary antiquity makes the concept of animals. By that criterion, it is inherently unsuitable as a natural-
adaptation peripheral to the social and behavioral sciences. The science approach and thus falls into a human-uniqueness trap that has
argument that selection removes genetic variability with respect to bedeviled evolutionary analyses of human behavior over the past
behavior — that is pivotal to the universality argument — flies in the century-and-a-half.
face of much personality research. In general, personality variation I argue that in addition to pan-human adaptations, we should also
partly reflects genetic polymorphisms that are maintained due to the consider that behavioral differences among local populations can be
trade-off between varied fitness costs and benefits associated with the adaptations to local conditions. This logic is quite general and draws
Big Five dimensions of personality, for example (see Nettle, 2006), or on the literature of animal behavior. Examples of socially-learned
individualism versus collectivism (Fincher, Thornhill, Murray, & adaptations include the song complexity of birds, rats specializing on
Schaller, 2008). pine cones in some local communities, or shellfish in others, and the
It is one thing to criticize the adaptationist program advanced by Mauritius kestrel developing a novel habit of nesting on cliffs to elude
evolutionary psychologists. How can adaptation be defined in a better predators (Avital & Jablonka, 2000; Barber, 2008; Colias & Colias,
way that allows us to comprehend societal differences in violent 1984). The prevailing definition for an adaptation among evolutionary
crime in adaptative terms? To begin with, we must include behavioral psychologists excludes such phenomena, leaving us with an
phenotypes as well as morphological ones and must therefore include impoverished account of animal behavior (much less that of humans)
a role of society-specific learning processes. in which learned solutions to adaptive problems that make the
difference between survival and extinction for individuals, or entire
2. Defining adaptation in a way that suits communities, are ignored as irrelevant to adaptation. Consequently,
socially-learned behavior we are left to conclude that most modern human behavior is
adaptively neutral or irrelevant. If we are to avoid that conclusion —
In summary, the definition of adaptation taken from evolutionary one that has been all too popular among opponents of evolutionary
biology is more suited to phenomena that can be observed over tens approaches to human behavior — then we must redefine adaptation in
of millions of years in the fossil record. It is less relevant to behavioral a manner that is more relevant and useful for behavioral scientists
evolution that occurs too rapidly and leaves little or no fossil record whose subject matter is behavior, including criminal behavior, and its
for comparative analysis. Defining behavioral adaptations as species underlying evolutionary biological mechanisms.
typical is far too restrictive to include socially learned adaptations that Here are the main criteria for a more useful definition of behavioral
vary among local populations. For, different local populations adaptations:
encounter varied challenges to survival and reproduction and
behavioral solutions to such problems that are partly dependent on 1. Behavioral adaptations crop up among local populations and do not
social learning traditions should be considered adaptations (Richer- have to be species typical.
son & Boyd, 2004) because they serve the same function as genetically 2. Behavioral adaptations need not be observable in the fossil record
evolved solutions to the same problem. In either case, an adaptation is over thousands of generations to be accepted as valid.
defined as a fitness-enhancing phenotype (whether genetically- 3. Current (or comparatively recent) fitness consequences are
based, learned, or both) that helps an organism to adjust to varied relevant for defining behavioral adaptations (contrary to the
habitats in ways that promote survival and/or reproductive success. arguments of Buss et al., 1998 and Symons, 1992 among others).
422 N. Barber / Aggression and Violent Behavior 16 (2011) 420–427

At a more general level, it is simply wrong to imagine that human Steinberg, & Draper 1991; Del Giudice, 2009), an approach that is
adaptations have been fixed over the past two million years, if only often referred to as “life history theory”.
because our ancestry extends back over many different species Modern human social behavior can be causally linked to ecological
during that time period who varied in size, habitat, diet, cranial pressures using four necessary key assumptions that form the core of
capacity, tool complexity, encephalization, and so on. Phenotypes Evolutionary Social Science (Barber, 2007, 2008). These assumptions
that enhanced fitness in the evolutionary past may continue to do are: (1) that modern societies owe their character to an interaction of
so, of course, although that is not always true. The human capacity hunter–gatherer adaptations with the modern environment (inter-
for storing food energy — an adaptation in past environments — actionism); (2) that some changes in societies are due to changes in
causes obesity and metabolic illnesses and is quite maladaptive individuals (methodological individualism); (3) that historical changes
today, for instance. By the same token, behaviors that enhance and cross-societal differences can be due to similar adaptational
fitness today, such as nonviolence, would not have had the same mechanisms (counter cultural relativism); (4) and that different social
consequence in the past when assaultive violence might have contexts modify individual development in adaptive ways (adaptive
brought immediate benefits rather than swift and long-lasting development). These four assumptions must be made if evolutionary
incarceration. Such changes in the fitness consequences of explanations are to be applied to the problem of explaining societal
behavior do not invalidate an adaptationist approach to modern differences in violent crime (or anything else). A corollary is that if any
behavior: they make it indispensable (when appropriately of these assumptions is wrong, evolutionary explanation for societal
modified). differences may be impossible.
4. Societal differences in behavior may be adaptive so that societal Each of the above mechanisms is relevant to variation in violent
variation in criminal violence is part of an adaptive response to crime. Interactionism means that human adaptations relevant to
varied marriage markets as explained in more detail below. In direct mating effort (which increases violent crime, Minkov, 2009) are
general, one finds that men emphasize direct mating effort in influenced by novel economic factors, such as the relative indepen-
societies, or communities, where women are sexually active dence of some working women from economic support by their
outside marriage, and this behavioral pattern bespeaks adaptive sexual partners, particularly in the context of generous state
variation in male sexual behavior. provisions for child care, such as those in modern social democracies.
If more individual women opt for sexual intercourse prior to marriage
A formal theoretical model that contemplates societal variation this increases the pool of women available for extramarital sex which,
from an adaptationist perspective is known as Evolutionary Social in turn affects the viability of direct mating effort by men as an
Science (Barber, 2007, 2008, 2009b). This specifies a chain of alternative to marriage (methodological individualism). Such phe-
causation whereby changes in local conditions, such as varied nomena can account both for societal differences and for changes in
marriage markets affect how evolved behavioral predispositions crime rates over time (counter cultural relativism, Barber, 2003a).
play out. Abusive parenting has been identified as a cause of antisocial
behavior and crime (see below). Evolutionary Social Science reinter-
prets such phenomena through the prism of adaptive development.
2.1. Evolutionary Social Science: a causal evolutionary model for Does parental behavior facilitate the kind of reproductive strategy
explaining societal differences that is likely to succeed in a particular social context? For instance,
children maturing in an urban slum might learn to be tough,
Evolutionary Social Science (not abbreviated to avoid confusion suspicious, and street smart thereby protecting themselves from
with evolutionarily stable strategies) shares features with both human being taken advantage of by others. Specific parenting practices, such
behavioral ecology and evolutionary psychology. The similarities as corporal punishment, may promote this phenotype (Nightingale,
and differences are outlined in Table 1, which is loosely based on 1993). This approach to adaptive development is within the
Winterhalder and Smith (2000). The table highlights many differ- mainstream of evolutionary psychology (Del Giudice, 2009).
ences in these related approaches. The key points are that behavioral Given this theoretical backdrop, variation in violent crime over
ecology focuses on human adaptations to life in subsistence societies generations, and among nation states, can be thought of as one
whereas Evolutionary Social Science focuses on modern societies. manifestation of ancient adaptations for mating competition as they
Evolutionary psychology focuses primarily on cross-societal univer- play out in varied modern environments. One aspect of differential
sals that are interpreted in terms of pan-human adaptations, although expression of mechanisms for mating competition is adaptive
most evolutionary psychologists recognize that psychological devel- development, the process through which the violent tendencies of
opment proceeds differently in response to varied environments, young people are affected by their familial and community surround-
such as varied levels of psychological stress during childhood (Belsky, ings. With this theoretical framework in place, it is time to review

Table 1
Evolutionary Social Science compared to closely related fields.

Evolutionary Social Science Behavioral ecology Evolutionary psychology

Adaptations Short-term, phenotypic Short-term, phenotypic Long-term, genetic


Expected current Intermediate High Low
adaptiveness
Explanatory focus Modern behavior Behavioral strategies of foragers Evolved psychological
mechanisms
Methods Compare societies, compare socialization Ethnographic observation Surveys, experiments
Hypothesis generation Behavior fits differences in developmental, adult social context Foraging optimality, evolutionarily Reverse engineering
stable strategies
Favored topics Individual differences, societal differences, reproductive strategies Subsistence, reproductive strategies Mate choice, sex
differences
Key constraints Economic, ecological, marriage market Ecological, material Cognitive, genetic
Key assumptions Interactionism, methodological individualism, adaptive Ecological determinism Universals, cognitive
development, counter cultural relativism modules
N. Barber / Aggression and Violent Behavior 16 (2011) 420–427 423

evidence that violent crime varies predictably with marriage market may be illustrated by prevalence rates of HIV/AIDS that are much
conditions such that high levels of direct mating effort are correlated higher in sub-Saharan Africa where adult sex ratios are comparatively
with high rates of violent crime. low compared to the Middle East where sex ratios are high, and HIV/
AIDS infection rates are low, indicating lower levels of extramarital
3. Marriage markets, sex ratios, and social behavior sexuality. Such regional differences in sexual behavior as a function of
the sex ratio are fairly easily explained.
The marriage market is most often measured using sex ratios, Where there is a substantial excess of females in the population,
although this approach can be criticized as being overly simplistic. In many of them have difficulty in marrying and are therefore more
general, though, the logic is that in societies having more women than likely to be sexually active outside marriage. The existence of a pool of
men, (i.e., a low sex ratio) it is more difficult for women to marry women who are sexually active outside marriage means that men
resulting in higher levels of non marriage for women, and higher may emphasize mating effort rather than competing over spouses. In
levels of non marital reproduction (Guttentag & Secord, 1983). Any of such societies, direct mating competition between men increases,
these variables might be substituted for the sex ratio as a measure of which has the consequence of increasing male–male violence (the
the marriage market. When using sex ratios as a measure of marriage possible mechanisms being sketched out below).
market conditions, it is worth pointing out that there are many Conversely, when there is a scarcity of females, women are in
relevant control variables. Income is one given that unemployment, or greater demand as marriage partners and are less likely to be sexually
having a minimum-wage job effectively disqualifies men from active outside marriage for various reasons. One reason is that
marriage in many societies (Staples, 1985). Age is critical because premarital sexuality damages a young woman's sexual reputation so
the sex ratio is relevant only for persons who are in an age range that she is less desirable as a marriage partner on the logic that she
where marriage is likely (e.g., 15–64 years). Some scholars prefer to provides less paternity confidence that men look for in a potential
restrict their analysis to the peak ages of first marriage for males and spouse (Barber, 2002). Another factor is that women in sexually
females and this makes sense in societies where most people marry restrictive societies are chaperoned, or protected by their male
once only. Marital status is important as well because married people relatives from the possibility of contact with sexual partners. Because
are theoretically removed from the marriage market. Nevertheless, few women are sexually active outside marriage, this invalidates a
there is some evidence that young married women play the field in masculine strategy of direct mating effort. Men are thus constrained
the sense that they are more likely to divorce and remarry if there is a to compete over marriage partners rather than sex partners (Gutten-
good supply of marriageable men (see Barber, 2003b). tag & Secord, 1983). Such indirect (nonviolent) reproductive effort
There are many different ways of measuring the marriage market usually takes the form of competition over economic resources that
and there are many different kinds of sex ratios that may be used for women prefer in a potential husband (Barber, 2002). Empirical
this purpose. Sex ratios based on narrow age ranges (e.g. b5 years) evidence shows that the distinction between both types of reproduc-
and that consider only single people are suitable for studying secular tive competition is psychologically, and behaviorally, salient.
changes in the marriage market within a particular society but the Research on American college students, for example, found that if
numbers are highly unstable given that they vary not only with women perceive their campus to lack men who would make
changing birth rates (given that women prefer to marry men who are committed husbands, they emphasize their own sexuality and engage
a few years older than themselves) but also with changing ages of in brief relationships with various men, for instance (Cashdan, 1993).
marriage both within and between the sexes. For this reason, many Cross-national research also found that both men and women in
researchers prefer to use a broad sex ratio covering most of men's and countries with a relative scarcity of men are more interested in casual
women's reproductive lives (or marriage careers in the case of sex (as measured by the Sociosexual Orientation Inventory, Schmitt,
women) because this is more stable over time and therefore 2005). Such direct mating effort is implicated in violent crimes.
preferable for comparative purposes. So what is the empirical
relationship between the marriage market and violent crime? 4. Violent crime and direct mating effort
Analysis of INTERPOL crime data (murders, rapes, and assaults)
found that violent crime is a predictable feature of countries with a A great deal of evidence bolsters the conclusion that violent crime
relative scarcity of men (ages 15–64 years, Barber, 2000a) in is produced by direct mating competition (Barber, 2007, 2009a,b).
regressions controlling for level of economic development. This is a Such evidence includes analysis of contextual factors in assault and
counter-intuitive result given that most of the violent crimes in any homicides, characteristics of offenders and victims, cross-national
country are committed by men. O'Brien (1999) also reported that research, and time series analyses. Characteristics of the criminal are
during years when there were more males, American rape rates were revealing: criminal violence in the form of assaults and homicides is
lower but Avakame (1999) concluded that a greater supply of males overwhelmingly a masculine activity and it is more likely to be
in the U.S. augments female homicide victimization. Walsh (2003) perpetrated on male victims (Campbell, Muncer, & Bibel, 2001; Daly &
reviews several studies conducted within the U.S. finding higher Wilson, 1988). Men are most violent during the ages when they are
violent crime in locations with relatively fewer males. Barber (2009a) actively dating, a phenomenon that could be partially mediated by
confirmed and extend Barber's (2000a) INTERPOL finding by using testosterone levels (Barber, 2002). Time series analyses of violent
better-quality data sources (UN murders and WHO homicides) and crimes in the U.S., England, and Scotland, also found that crime
considering additional control variables as possible alternative increases during years when women found it more difficult to marry
explanations. Lim, Bond, and Bond (2005) replicated the effect using (and are thus likely to be sexually active outside marriage, Barber,
the WHO data for a smaller sample. 2003a).
The effect of sex ratios on violent crimes is quite robust and fairly One of the more intriguing cross-national patterns in violent crime
substantial as assessed through different kinds of comparative study. is the fact that murders, assaults, and rapes are significantly higher in
Given that men have much higher violent crime rates compared to the Americas than they are in the rest of the world (Barber, 2006;
women, it is counterintuitive that societies having a greater Neapolitan, 1994). This difference is statistically explainable in terms
proportion of males should have lower crime rates. This apparent of higher proportions of single parenthood in these countries. (As an
contradiction can be resolved in terms of alternative mating strategies index of the number of women who are sexually active outside
of men in societies that vary in their sex ratios. marriage, single parenthood is a convenient measure of direct mating
When there is an excess of females in the population, women are effort.) Lim et al. (2005) also reported that the effects of the sex ratio
more likely to be sexually active outside marriage. This phenomenon (and economic development) on WHO homicides were mediated by
424 N. Barber / Aggression and Violent Behavior 16 (2011) 420–427

individual-level preference of love over status in a prospective mate, population. These data are counterintuitive given that crime rates are
indicating that violent crime increases where a higher premium is higher in males than females in all nations and thus call for
placed on love (or direct mating effort) than social status (suggesting explanation and discussion. The results are quite general showing
indirect competition via status enhancement). up in several different data sets for different populations, various time
Single parenthood is a convenient index of direct mating effort in a periods, and using different analytic approaches (cross-national,
country but it has some problems including co-residence of ostensibly ecological comparisons within countries, individual-level, cross-
single parents. Although “single” mothers may well live with the sectional, time-series, and so on.
“single” father of their offspring, cohabiting relationships are briefer It is reasonable to suspect that such results might be due to the
than marriages (Smock, 2000). Given that paternal investment in effects of various confounding variables. Yet, marriage-market effects
offspring is thereby reduced, men can engage in more direct mating on crime persist despite controlling most or all of relevant control
effort (Barber, 2002). The fact that more sexually active women are variables. This task is most easily accomplished in cross-national
without a permanent mate due to rapid dissolution of unions also comparisons because there are rather few reliable cross-national
creates more opportunities for brief sexual relationships than would predictors of violent crime (Barber, 2000a; Neapolitan, 1997). The
be true if they were formally married. For this reason direct mating effect could not be attributed to economic development, income
effort is likely to be correlated with reduced paternal (and parental) inequality, urbanization, level of policing, or the presence of a major
investment. This raises the question of whether mating effort, as such, international drug trafficking network (Barber, 2009a). (Warfare
is the important factor in violent crime, or whether crime is a product could also be ruled out because none of the countries in this study was
of reduced parental investment. actually at war and violent military deaths are not classified as
These factors can be teased apart in time-series analyses by murders or homicides in any case.)
looking for an immediate effect of single parenthood on crime We may thus conclude that violent crime rates are indeed higher
(tapping direct mating effort) versus one that is delayed by a in countries where there is a relative scarcity of males, despite the fact
generation (tapping parental investment during childhood of the that males are considerably more prone to criminal violence than
criminal). The immediate effect is far more important and when this is females. Although other possible third variables might explain this
statistically removed, the lagged effect has no predictive power effect, it is difficult to imagine what they might be, given that there is
according to Barber's (2004a) analysis using data for 39 countries. no other known factors with a sufficiently large effect size in cross-
Such ecological correlations are provocative but they do not fill in the national research on violent crime to qualify as alternative explana-
causal mechanisms through which mating effort might increase tions (Neapolitan, 1997). Admittedly, most researchers have paid
violent crime. Before attempting to fill in the causal links between insufficient attention to the role of single parenthood and the lack of
mating effort and violent behavior, it is helpful to think about these current data is a real problem here. When Barber (2009a) controlled
phenomena in a broad anthropological perspective. for teen parenthood as a proxy for the unavailable data on single
parenthood, he found that this had little effect on the conclusion that
5. The anthropology of mating aggression and violent crime countries with a greater proportion of adult men in the population
have lower crime rates. See Schmitt (2005) for analysis of other
In subsistence societies lacking frequent warfare, the primary possible third variables connecting sex ratios and sexual behavior,
motive for homicide seems to be sexual competition among men that might also be relevant to violent crime. Moreover, this
including the jealous aggression associated with lover's triangles counterintuitive effect of the sex ratio on violent crime is larger,
where men kill unfaithful spouses as well as sexual competitors (Daly more consistent, and better established perhaps than any other factor
& Wilson, 1988). This generalization prevails around the world from in comparative research (Walsh, 2003).
the !Kung of the Kalahari Desert in South Africa, to the Inuit of North Much data thus confirm Barber's (2000a) finding of crime
America, the Tiwi of North Australia, the Siriono of Bolivia, and the decreasing as masculinity of the population declined. These data are
Arapesh of New Guinea (Symons, 1979). Moreover, sexual jealousy is correlational, of course, and subject to various interpretations apart
the primary motive for violence against women in nation states from the causal conclusion that the level of violence is determined by
around the world (Barber, 2002; Daly & Wilson, 1988). direct mating competition in countries experiencing a scarcity of men.
There has been little systematic cross-societal investigation of the As to homicides, for instance, it is theoretically possible that the
association between mating competition and violent crime. Never- direction of causality is reversed and that a high murder rate simply
theless, several plausible indices of mating competition are positively serves to remove more males than females from the population. There
correlated with violent crimes in cross-national research. Violent are two good reasons for doubting this interpretation. First, other
crime correlates positively, if weakly, with divorce rates in developed crimes, such as rapes and assaults, that do not subtract males from the
countries, for instance (Neapolitan, 1999). population have similar correlations with the sex ratio (Barber, 2000a,
In societies with high rates of divorce, there are more sexually active 2004). Second, homicides are too rare to have much of an impact on
unmarried reproductive-age women, and hence a rationale for the sex ratio. Based on cross-national data, homicide rates generally
increased direct mating effort by men. A similar logic applies to societies average fewer than 8 per 100,000. Over the adult life of the individual,
with a scarcity of males having more violence given that such scarcity this amounts to less than 0.5% and would not be expected to have a
promotes premarital sexuality (Barber, 2000a,b,c, 2001, 2002, 2007) detectable impact on sex ratios expressed as whole percentages
because it is more difficult for women to marry creating a pool of (Central Intelligence Agency, CIA, 2006). Lim et al. (2005) also suggest
sexually active single women. Conversely, a scarcity of women that violence in low-sex-ratio societies is due to the greater resource
promotes premarital (and extramarital) chastity. These ideas suggest competition among women unable to find mates, which would be a
that violent crime should be linked to the marriage market in rival explanation to the mating aggression approach developed in this
predictable ways. If such patterns exist what is the best way of paper. Yet, when one considers that women's rates of violent crime
interpreting them, bearing in mind that such evidence is correlational? are an order of magnitude lower than those of men (Daly & Wilson,
1988), this is an almost absurdly inadequate alternative explanation.
6. How to make sense of data linking marriage markets and How, then, are we to explain why low-sex-ratio countries, and
violent crime societies in general, are so violent according to analysis of various
crime data (Barber, 2000a, 2009a,b)? A plausible interpretation is that
Considerable evidence thus shows that violent crime rates are when men are in greater demand in the marriage market than
higher in countries where males make up a lower proportion of the women, more females are sexually active outside marriage, either
N. Barber / Aggression and Violent Behavior 16 (2011) 420–427 425

because they are unable to marry, or because they use sexual intimacy night clubs and bars, are characterized by exceptionally high levels of
as a technique for competing over the romantic attentions of desirable male–male violence (Peterson, Krivo, & Harris, 2000). Apart from the
men (Guttentag & Secord, 1983). This sets the scene for more obvious fact that crimes of violence are bound to be more probable at
extensive direct mating competition by men — a process that is locations where violence-prone individuals meet, why are such
inherently violent due to a great variety of behavioral, physiological, venues particularly likely to evoke crimes of violence? This question
and societal mechanisms. Conversely, in societies having a scarcity of gets at the underlying behavioral mechanisms.
females, women are more sexually restrictive and are less likely to be At the behavioral level, violence frequently breaks out at night
sexually active outside marriage as this harms their otherwise clubs, bars, and other such dating venues over issues of “face,” or
excellent marriage prospects. Because so few women are sexually dyadic social status among possible mating competitors. Such
active outside marriage, direct mating competition is not a viable incidents may be provoked by one person accidentally jostling
strategy for men given that their sexual options are determined by the another in a crowded room, or getting into a dispute over who is
sexual proclivities of women. Although the marriage market is next to play on a pool table. When these disputes come to the
currently the most plausible explanation for societal variation in attention of police, due to property damage or injury, they are
violent crime based on the empirical research, it is certainly not, at classified as “trivial altercations,” because they appear to lack
this point, the preferred interpretation among criminologists and adequate motivation. Yet, there is a loss of status by men who back
social scientists. down in such confrontations. The view that such violence is really a
form of mating aggression rests on the claim that combatants in these
7. Alternative theoretical explanations for variation in altercations are often known to each other, implying a plausible
violent crime scenario of repeated encounters in which status-related conflict
recurs. Loss of face impairs their reputation and status among peers
The evidence implicating direct mating competition among men in and thereby indirectly reducing their appeal as dating partners
violent crime is thus rich, if mostly indirect, but it is not the only (Barber, 2002; Daly & Wilson, 1988).
current interpretation of the evidence, or even the one most widely Such hot-headedness is particularly characteristic of young
used. Many social scientists and other scholars prefer to discuss the adulthood and reflects physiological influences (the third category
association between violence and sexuality in terms of declining of causes), particularly levels of testosterone and other androgens
moral values that would permit both extramarital sexuality and (Archer, 2006). This is a complex relationship because testosterone
impulsive violence to occur in the same societies. This approach is levels are temporarily increased by competitive situations, and even
inherently circular and cannot be taken seriously as a scientific by sexual relationships (Archer, 2006).
explanation, however, and the same is true of other cultural It may be over simplistic to attribute criminal violence to any
determinist approaches that refer to a culture of violence (Barber, single hormone. However, in view of the comparative evidence for
2004, 2005, 2007, 2008). other species, this caveat is generally overstated. In other words,
Although expectations about sexual behavior, and about violent anyone who denies that androgens play an important role in violent
behavior, do clearly vary from one society to another, such phenomena behavior, including violent crime runs the risk of ignoring a pattern of
can be explained in terms of objective differences in the marriage evidence supportive of the contrary position. Thus, the time course of
market and its economic influences (Abrahamson, 1998; Barber, 2000b, testosterone production is linked in interesting ways to both mating
c, 2001, 2002; Guttentag & Secord, 1983). The same argument applies to competition and crime. Young men experience peaks of criminal
antisocial impulses more generally, including interpersonal violence incidence and testosterone production at around the same ages
(Barber, 2007). Many scholars who are not interested in evolutionary (Archer, 2006; von der Pahlen, Sarkola, Seppa, & Eriksson, 2002).
explanations of violent crime also prefer to discuss various psychological Criminal violence is associated with testosterone levels in other data.
factors (lack of social trust, frustration, etc.) that mediate the connection With marriage, men experience a simultaneous decline in criminal
between country differences in economics or sex ratios and violent offending and testosterone levels (Booth & Dabbs, 1993; Mazur &
crime (reviewed in Lim et al., 2005). Ideally, such approaches could be Michalek, 1998) compared to single men of the same age. The
reconciled with the evolutionary one developed here but doing so is relationship between testosterone and violence might be causal,
beyond the scope of this paper. although the true experiments necessary to draw this conclusion have
Societal variation in violent crime is partly a manifestation of male not been conducted and could not be conducted on humans for ethical
reproductive competition — based on evolved psychological predis- reasons. Divorce is something of a natural experiment in this respect,
positions — playing out in modern social contexts. Greater under- however, because when previously married men begin dating again,
standing of the means through which social contexts either reduce, or their testosterone rises against the normal tendency to decrease with
increase, direct sexual competition is thus of central importance for age and they are more likely to commit crimes of violence (Mazur &
understanding societal variation in criminal violence. Such work holds Michalek, 1998). Both phenomena may reflect a tendency to begin
out the promise of providing a natural-science explanation for staying out late at night in bars and other dating venues as well as
variation among modern societies that would replace moralistic, or increased alcohol consumption (Waite & Gallagher, 2000). In other
cultural-determinist alternatives (Barber, 2007). It is also important words, it may be that testosterone boosts violent crime but there are
to grasp the anthropological and sociological contexts of violence as many possible third variables in this relationship, several having to do
these affect the form of marriage — even whether it persists — with with resumption of dating behavior and frequenting high-crime
implications for mating effort and violence. How does increasing dating locations, such as bars. Increased alcohol consumption is yet
mating effort on the part of men boost violent crime rates? another possible third variable connecting masculine divorce with
increased risk of violent behavior.
8. Filling in the causal mechanisms through which mating Alcohol consumption disinhibits aggressive behavior. The disin-
competition increases violence hibiting effects of alcohol and other recreational drugs is widely
recognized as a prominent factor in the physiology of violent crime
How can direct male mating competition increase violent crime? (Giancola, 2002; Norris, Davis, George, Martell, & Heiman, 2002). By
At least three levels of mechanisms may be involved: sociological, reducing serotonin activity in the brain, alcohol contributes to
behavioral, and physiological. At a sociological level, patterns of impulsive aggression. (Alcohol actually reduces testosterone levels.)
association play a clear role. Thus, (mainly) young men assemble There is thus a rich network of factors associated with direct sexual
wherever sexually active women may be encountered. Venues such as competition that mediate the connection between scarcity of males in
426 N. Barber / Aggression and Violent Behavior 16 (2011) 420–427

the population and violent crime. Investigation of these probable links minimal practical consequences so far as reducing male–male mating
opens up many avenues for future research. In establishing such competition (and violent crime) is concerned. Moreover, the
linkages, it is important to emphasize that crimes of violence are quite association between single parenthood and child poverty found in
rare in our society: most dating men do not engage in violent Britain, the U.S., and other countries is not seen in Sweden where
behavior, although this might have been more common in forager households with children are more affluent, on average, than
communities (Symons, 1979). However rare crimes of violence are, households without children, reflecting state subsidies for raising
contemporary societies with more extramarital sexuality do have children (Home sweet home, 1995). Consequently, there is little
more violence on account of direct reproductive competition and the economic motive for formal marriage and marriage rates in Sweden
psychological adaptations underlying it. Such variation in sexual are indeed low. What is more, Swedish children born to single
behavior is a key factor in accounting for societal differences in violent mothers are as healthy as children of married parents and no more
crime. likely to be involved in serious crime, drug dependency, or unplanned
teenage pregnancy.
9. Discussion and conclusions One of the key practical differences between formal marriage and
cohabitation is that common-law unions are significantly shorter. The
The impact of direct mating competition on violent crime most common explanation for this difference is that cohabitation
committed by males is substantial in terms of the variance explained. arrangements are entered into without as much of an emotional
This key empirical role has been largely ignored by criminologists and commitment, so that the union is more likely to break up (Smock,
other social scientists to date (but see Walsh, 2003). The reasons for 2000). Another issue is that legal marriages have more inertia because
this relative neglect are probably complex but three reasons seem they are more difficult and expensive to dissolve, often with adverse
particularly obvious. First, the study of crime is compartmentalized economic consequences for either, or both, parties, such as payment
separately from the study of sexuality in an academic world that of alimony, or a reduced standard of living following the divorce due
encourages, and rewards, specialization and discourages synthesis. to maintaining two residences rather than one.
Second, criminology is a branch of sociology, a discipline that has been Ancestral marriage contracts came into being among foragers
particularly slow to adopt the sort of evolutionary analysis that is best because men who stayed with their spouse and helped her to raise
suited for integrating research on sexuality with research on violent children were more reproductively successful than men who
crime (Barber, 2008; Lopreato & Crippen, 1999). Third, sociologists attempted to father children with multiple mates without investing
have been more interested in accounting for violent crime (and crime substantially in any of them. Similarly, women who opted for
more generally) in terms of economic factors, such as poverty, or permanent marriages were more reproductively successful than
income inequality, rather than in terms of sexual behavior. Such those who mated with several men without securing investment in
factors are related to crime rates in comparative analyses (Pridemore their offspring.
& Trent, 2010) but that may be because poverty and inequality This basic bargaining arrangement between the sexes was
undermine marriage as a reproductive strategy (Barber, 2006, 2008). maintained following the Agricultural Revolution in which men
Although the empirical support for a connection between marriage continued to play a key role in food production, particularly given that
markets and violent crime is very good, skeptical readers might they frequently owned land, provided needed labor, or maintained
question the practical importance of such findings. To begin with, if control over technologies of food production, such as by fashioning
academics who study violent crime are unwilling to be receptive to plows, raising draught animals, or organizing crop rotations (Sakala,
the key explanatory role of mating aggression, then its clear relevance, 2000). It is, however disrupted by modern economic conditions. As
and predictive value may not matter greatly. Taking an optimistic women became more economically independent, thanks to increased
perspective, criminologists of the future may become more receptive participation in paid employment, and increasing wages relative to
to evolutionary ideas. men, there was an increase in the proportion of births to unmarried
There is an even more sobering reason that connecting marriage women, as reflected in Barber's (2003c) cross-national analysis.
markets with mating aggression and violent crime may prove to be of Women's economic independence is also bolstered by welfare states
limited value. That is that marriage itself is on the decline so that the having generous child support provisions and this is one reason that
orderly relationship between marriage markets and crime may no more births are to single women in social democracies as opposed to
longer be discernible. countries like the U.S., where social welfare programs relevant to
Marriage is declining in many developed countries by many women of reproductive age are targeted more narrowly at low-
different measures such as declining marriage rates and increasing income individuals.
divorce rates. A century ago, divorce rates in the U.S., and in Europe Although female economic independence might appear threaten-
were very low (typically well below 5% of marriages) whereas divorce ing to the continued existence of marriage (Popenoe, 1988), this is not
rates in these countries today are typically around 50% of marriage necessarily true. One reason is that marriage provides appreciable
rates (Martinson, 2006; OECD, 2010; Shorter, 1975). The proportion of household economies so that married couples may accumulate more
births outside wedlock has followed a similar trajectory. Another capital than single people having the same combined income (Waite
revealing statistic is the proportion of young women who cohabit. In & Gallagher, 2000). Marriage can thus be considered a social mobility
some European countries this number had exceeded 50% of women strategy as well as a reproductive strategy. That helps explain why
aged 20–25 years by the end of the 20th Century (Chesnais, 1996), single parenthood remains rare among middle class women in the U.S.
implying that sexual relations outside marriage is now the majority whereas it is much more common among low-income women
practice there. (Abrahamson, 1998) suggesting that middle class people are more
On the surface, these data suggest that marriage is on the verge of interested in social mobility. If marriage promotes social mobility, this
disappearing. Yet, marriage rates are much higher in some developed would also explain other puzzling phenomena, such as a tendency for
countries than others, e.g., the U.S. Another qualification is that formal women who begin their families as single mothers to marry
marriage can be mostly replaced by de facto marriage without much subsequently, a pattern that is fairly common among African
apparent change in living arrangements, or in the quality of life of Americans, for instance (Burton, 1990).
children, as illustrated by secular change in Swedish marriage There is a certain irony in seeking to establish the connection
(Popenoe, 1988). Although young Swedes are mostly uninterested between marriage markets and violent crime at a time when marriage
in formal marriage, children are very likely to spend their early years is becoming much less frequent and could actually disappear. Even
under the same roof as their fathers and formal marriage may have if marriage is about to disappear (and this is highly uncertain)
N. Barber / Aggression and Violent Behavior 16 (2011) 420–427 427

understanding the connection between marriage markets and violent Del Giudice (2009). Sex, attachment, and the development of reproductive strategies.
The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 1−67.
crime, as mediated by male–male mating aggression is just as Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence
important. By studying the simpler historical case in which most predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings
women married, we are in a better position to identify the key of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 275, 1279−1285.
Giancola, P. R. (2002). Alcohol-related aggression in men and women: The influence of
independent variables underlying societal differences in interpersonal dispositional aggressivity. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, 696−708.
aggression. We are then in a position to apply that knowledge to a Guttentag, M., & Secord, P. F. (1983). Too many women: The sex ratio question. Beverly
more complex post-marriage world (if that is what transpires). This Hills, CA: Sage.
Home sweet home (1995). De Economist, 25−27 September 9.
paper has sketched out the sociological, behavioral, and physiological Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2002). Sense and nonsense: Evolutionary perspectives on
mechanisms through which male–male direct mating competition human behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
increases crimes of violence. The focus on males is justified by their Lim, F., Bond, M. H., & Bond, M. K. (2005). Linking societal and psychological factors to
homicide rates across nations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 515−536.
huge overrepresentation in crimes of violence. A similar analysis
Lopreato, J., & Crippen, T. A. (1999). Crisis in sociology: The need for Darwin. Somerset, NJ:
could be applied to females. Transaction.
Martinson, V. K. (2006). Families in different contexts. In A. Loveless, & T. B. Holman (Eds.),
References The family in the new millennium, vol. 1. (pp. 124−152)Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.
Mazur, A., & Michalek, J. (1998). Marriage, divorce, and male testosterone. Social Forces,
Abrahamson, M. (1998). Out-of-wedlock births: The United States in comparative 77, 315−330.
perspective. Westport, CT: Praeger. Minkov, M. (2009). Risk-taking reproductive competition explains national murder
Archer, J. (2006). Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge rates better than socioeconomic inequality. Cross-Cultural Research, 43, 3−29.
hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 319−345. Neapolitan, J. L. (1994). Cross-national variation in homicides: The case of Latin
Avakame, E. F. (1999). Sex ratios, female labor force participation and lethal violence America. International Criminal Justice Review, 4, 4−22.
against women: Extending Guttentag and Secord's thesis. Violence Against Women, Neapolitan, J. L. (1997). Cross-national crime: A research review and sourcebook.
5, 1321−1341. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Avital, E., & Jablonka, E. (2000). Animal traditions: Behavioural inheritance in evolution. Neapolitan, J. L. (1999). A comparative analysis of nations with low and high levels of
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. violent crime. Journal of Criminal Justice, 27, 259−274.
Barber, N. (2000a). The sex ratio as a predictor of cross-national variation in violent Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals.
crime. Cross-Cultural Research, 34, 264−282. The American Psychologist, 61, 622−631.
Barber, N. (2000b). On the relationship between country sex ratios and teen pregnancy Nightingale, C. H. (1993). On the edge: A history of poor Black children and their American
rates: A replication. Cross-Cultural Research, 34, 26−37. dreams. New York: Basic.
Barber, N. (2000c). Why parents matter: Parental investment and child outcomes. Norris, J., Davis, K. C., George, W. H., Martell, J., & Heiman, J. R. (2002). Alcohols direct
Westport, CT: Bergin and Garvey. and indirect effects on men's self-reported sexual aggression likelihood. Journal of
Barber, N. (2001). On the relationship between marital opportunity and teen pregnancy: Studies on Alcohol, 63, 688−695.
The sex ratio question. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 259−267. O'Brien, R. M. (1999). Sex ratios and rape rates: A powercontrol theory. Criminology, 29,
Barber, N. (2002). The science of romance. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus. 99−114.
Barber, N. (2003a). The sex ratio and female marriage opportunity as historical OECD (2010). Family database Accessed at:. www.oecd.org/els/social/family/database
predictors of violent crime in England, Scotland, and the U.S.. Cross-Cultural on 8/8 2010
Research, 37, 373−392. Peterson, R. D., Krivo, L. J., & Harris, M. A. (2000). Disadvantage and neighborhood
Barber, N. (2003b). Divorce and reduced economic and emotional interdependence: A violent crime: Do local institutions matter. Journal of Research in Crime and
cross-national study. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 39, 113−142. Delinquency, 37, 31−63.
Barber, N. (2003c). Parental investment prospects and cross-national differences in Popenoe, D. (1988). Disturbing the nest: Family change and decline in modern societies.
single parenthood. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 163−177. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
Barber, N. (2004). Single parenthood as a predictor of cross-national variation in violent Pridemore, W. A., & Trent, C. L. S. (2010). Do the invariant findings of Land, McCall, and
crime. Cross-Cultural Research, 38, 343−358. Cohen generalize to cross-national studies of social structure and homicide.
Barber, N. (2005). Evolutionary explanations for societal differences in single Homicide Studies, 14, 296−335.
parenthood. Evolutionary Psychology, 3, 133−165. Reeve, H. K., & Sherman, P. W. (1993). Adaptation and the goals of evolutionary
Barber, N. (2006). Why is violent crime so common in the Americas? Aggressive research. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 68, 1−32.
Behavior, 32, 442−450. Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2004). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human
Barber, N. (2007). Evolutionary explanations for societal differences and historical change evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
in violent crime and single parenthood. Cross-Cultural Research, 41, 123−148. Sakala, E. A. (2000). Gender issues in animal draft power weeding technology in
Barber, N. (2008). The myth of culture: Why we need a genuine natural science of societies. Zambia. In P. Starkey, & T. Simalenga (Eds.), Animal power for weed control: A
Newcastle-upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Press. resource book of the Animal Traction Network for Eastern and Southern Africa
Barber, N. (2009a). Countries with fewer males have more violent crime: Marriage (ATNESA) (pp. 88−89). Wagenigen, The Netherlands: Technical Center for
markets and mating aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 49−56. Agricultural and Rural Cooperation (CTA).
Barber, N. (2009b). From steroids to nation states: An integrated evolutionary approach Schmitt, D. P. (2005). Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: A 48-nation study of
to violent crime. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14, 415−422. sex, culture, and strategies of human mating. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28,
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal 247−311.
development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Shorter, E. (1975). Illegitimacy, sexual revolution, and social change in modern Europe.
Child Development, 62, 647−670. In T. K. Rabb, & R. I. Rotberg (Eds.), The family in history: Interdisciplinary perspectives
Booth, A., & Dabbs, J. M. (1993). Testosterone and men's marriages. Social Forces, 72, (pp. 48−85). New York: Harper Torchbooks.
463−477. Smock, P. J. (2000). Cohabitation in the United States. Annual Review of Sociology, 2000,
Burton, L. A. (1990). Teenage childbearing as an alternative life-course strategy in 1−20.
multigenerational black families. Human Nature, 1, 123−143. Staples, R. (1985). Changes in black family structure: The conflict between family
Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G., Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., & Wakefield, J. C. (1998). ideology and structural conditions. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 391−404.
Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels. The American Psychologist, 53, 533−548. Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.
Campbell, A., Muncer, S., & Bibel, D. (2001). Women and crime: An evolutionary Symons, D. (1992). On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of human
approach. Aggressive Behavior, 6, 481−497. behavior. Adaptiveness and adaptation. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 427−444.
Cashdan, E. (1993). Attracting mates: Effects of paternal investment on mate attraction von der Pahlen, B., Sarkola, T., Seppa, K., & Eriksson, C. J. P. (2002). Testosterone, 5-
strategies. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 1−24. dihydrotestosterone and cortisol in men with and without alcohol-related
Central Intelligence Agency, CIA (2006). World factbook. Washington, D.C: Author. aggression. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, 518−526.
Chesnais, J. C. (1996). Fertility, family, and social policy in contemporary Western Waite, L. J., & Gallagher, M. (2000). The case for marriage. New York: Doubleday.
Europe. Population and Development Review, 22, 729−739. Walsh, A. (2003). The sex ratio: A biosocial explanation for racial variation in crime
Colias, N. E., & Colias, C. (1984). Nest building and bird behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton rates. In A. Walsh, & L. Ellis (Eds.), Biosocial Criminology: Challenging environmen-
University Press. talism's supremacy (pp. 61−82). Hampshire, NY: Nova Science.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1987). From evolution to behavior: Evolutionary psychology Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
as the missing link. In J. Dupre (Ed.), The latest on the best: Essays on evolution and University Press.
optimality (pp. 277−306). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Winterhalder, B., & Smith, E. A. (2000). Analyzing adaptive strategies: Human
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. behavioral ecology at twenty-five. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 51−72.

You might also like