You are on page 1of 15

Do you ever daydream about winning the lottery?

It only costs a small amount, a slight risk, with the possibility of a substantial
reward.

But will it make you happy? Will it give you long-lasting happiness?

Undoubtedly, there will be a temporary peak in happiness, but will all your troubles
finally fade away?

That is what we will investigate today. We explore the economics of happiness and
whether money can buy happiness. In this post, we will start by broadly exploring
the topic and then look at theories and substantive research findings. We’ll even
have a look at previous lottery winners.

For interested readers, we will list interesting books and podcasts for further
enjoyment and share a few of our own happiness resources.

Ka-ching: Let’s get rolling!

Before you continue, we thought you might like to download our three Meaning
and Valued Living Exercises for free. These creative, science-based exercises will
help you learn more about your values, motivations, and goals and give you the
tools to inspire a sense of meaning in the lives of your clients, students, or
employees.
This Article Contains
• What Is Happiness Economics?
• Theory of the Economics of Happiness
• Can Money Buy Happiness? 5 Research Findings
• 6 Fascinating Books and Podcasts on the Topic
• Resources From PositivePsychology.com
• A Take-Home Message
• References

What Is Happiness Economics?


Happiness economics is a field of economics that recognizes happiness and
wellbeing as important outcome measures, alongside measures typically used, such
as employment, education, and health care.

Economics emphasizes how specific economic/financial characteristics affect our


wellbeing (Easterlin, 2004).

For example, does employment result in better health and longer lifespan, among
other metrics? Do people in wealthier countries have access to better education
and longer life spans?
In the last few decades, there has been a shift in economics, where researchers
have recognized the importance of the subjective rating of happiness as a valuable
and desirable outcome that is significantly correlated with other important
outcomes, such as health (Steptoe, 2019) and productivity (DiMaria et al., 2020).

Broadly, happiness is a psychological state of being, typically researched and


defined using psychological methods. We often measure it using self-report
measures rather than objective measures that are less vulnerable to
misinterpretation and error.

Including happiness in economics has opened up an entirely new avenue of


research to explore the relationship between happiness and money.

Andrew Clark (2018) illustrates the variability in the term happiness economics with
the following examples:

1. Happiness can be a predictor variable, influencing our decisions and


behaviors.
2. Happiness might be the desired outcome, so understanding how and why
some people are happier than others is essential.

However, the connection between our behavior and happiness must be better
understood. Even though “being happy” is a desired outcome, people still make
decisions that prevent them from becoming happier. For example, why do we
choose to work more if our work does not make us happier? Why are we unhappy
even if our basic needs are met?

An example of how happiness can influence decision-making

Sometimes, we might choose not to maximize a monetary or financial gain but


place importance on other, more subjective outcomes.

To illustrate: If faced with two jobs — one that pays well but will bring no joy and
another that pays less but will bring much joy — some people would prefer to
maximize their happiness over financial gain.

If this decision were evaluated using a utility framework where the only valued
outcomes were practical, then the decision would seem irrational. However, this
scenario suggests that psychological outcomes, such as the experience of
happiness, are as crucial as other socio-economic outcomes.

Economists recognize that subjective wellbeing, or happiness, is an essential


characteristic and sometimes a desirable outcome that can motivate our decision-
making.

In the last few decades, economics has shifted to include happiness as a


measurable and vital part of general wellbeing (Graham, 2005).
The consequence is that typical economic questions now also look at the impact of
employment, finances, and other economic metrics on the subjective rating and
experience of happiness at individual and country levels.

Theory of the Economics of Happiness

The theory of the economics of happiness


can be summarized as follows (Bruni, 2007):

Happiness is such a vital outcome in society and economic activity that it must be
involved in policy making. The subjective measure of happiness is as important as
other typical measures used in economics.

Many factors can contribute to happiness. In this post, we consider the role of
money. The relationship between happiness, or subjective wellbeing, and money is
assumed to be positive: More money means greater happiness.

However, the relationship between money and happiness is paradoxical: More


money does not guarantee happiness (for an excellent review, see Graham, 2005).

Specifically, low levels of income are correlated with unhappiness. However, as our
individual wealth increases and our basic needs are met, our needs change and
differ in their importance.

Initially, our happiness is affected by absolute levels of income, but at a certain


threshold, we place importance on relative levels of income. Knowing how we rank
and compare to other people, in terms of wealth and material possession,
influences our happiness.

The relationship between wealth and happiness continues to increase, but only to a
certain point; at this stage, more wealth does not guarantee more happiness
(Easterlin, 1974; Diener et al., 1993).

This may be at odds with our everyday lived experience. Most of us choose to work
longer hours or multiple jobs so that we make more money. However, what is the
point of doing this if money does not increase our happiness? Why do we seem to
think that more money will make us happier?

History of the economics of happiness


The relationship between economics and happiness originated in the early 1970s.
Brickman and Campbell (1971, as cited in Brickman et al., 1978) first argued that the
typical outcomes of a successful life, such as wealth or income, had no impact on
individual wellbeing.

Easterlin (1974) expanded these results and showed that although wealthier people
tend to be happier than poor people in the same country, the average happiness
levels within a country remained unchanged even as the country’s overall wealth
increased.

The inconsistent relationship between happiness and income and its sensitivity to
critical income thresholds make this topic so interesting.

There is some evidence that wealthier countries are happier than others, but only
when comparing the wealthy with the poor (Easterlin, 1974; Graham, 2005).

As countries become wealthier, citizens report higher happiness, but this


relationship is strongest when the starting point is poverty. Above a certain income
threshold, happiness no longer increases (Diener et al., 1993).

Interestingly, people tend to agree on the amount of money needed to make them
happy; but beyond a certain value, there is little increase in happiness (Haesevoets
et al., 2022).

Measurement challenges

Measuring happiness accurately and reliably is challenging. Researchers disagree


on what happiness means.

It is not the norm in economics to measure happiness by directly asking a


participant how happy they are; instead, happiness is inferred through:

• Subjective wellbeing (Clark, 2018; Easterlin, 2004)


• A combination of happiness and life satisfaction (Bruni, 2007)

Furthermore, happiness can refer to an acute psychological state, such as feeling


happy after a nice meal, or a lasting state similar to contentment (Nettle, 2005).

Researchers might use different definitions of happiness and ways to measure it,
thus leading to contradictory results. For example, happiness might be used
synonymously with subjective wellbeing and can refer to several things,
including life satisfaction and financial satisfaction (Diener & Oishi, 2000).

It seems contradictory that wealthier nations are not happier overall than poorer
nations and that increasing the wealth of poorer nations does not guarantee that
their happiness will increase too. What could then be done to increase happiness?

Can Money Buy Happiness? 5 Research Findings


What is the relationship between income/wealth and happiness? To answer that
question, we looked at studies to see where and how money improves happiness,
but we’ll also consider the limitations to the positive effect of income.

Money buys access; jobs boost happiness

Overwhelming evidence shows that wealth is correlated with measures of


wellbeing.

Wealthier people have access to better healthcare, education, and employment,


which in turn results in higher life satisfaction (Helliwell et al., 2012). A certain
amount of wealth is needed to meet basic needs, and satisfying these needs
improves happiness (Veenhoven & Ehrhardt, 1995).

Increasing happiness through improved quality of life is highest for poor


households, but this is explained by the starting point. Access to essential services
improves the quality of life, and in turn, this improves measures of wellbeing.

Most people gain wealth through employment; however, it is not just wealth that
improves happiness; instead, employment itself has an important association with
happiness. Happiness and employment are also significantly correlated with each
other (Helliwell et al., 2021).

Lockdown on happiness

The World Happiness Report (Helliwell et al., 2021) reports that unemployment
increased during the COVID-19 pandemic, and this was accompanied by a marked
decline in happiness and optimism.

The pandemic also changed how we evaluated certain aspects of our lives; for
example, the relationship between income and happiness declined. After all, what is
the use of money if you can’t spend it? In contrast, the association between
happiness and having a partner increased (Helliwell et al., 2021).

Wealthier states smile more, but is it real?


If we took a snapshot of happiness and a country’s wealth, we would find that
richer countries tend to have happier populations than poorer countries.

For example, based on the 2021 World Happiness Report, the top five happiest
countries — which are also wealthy countries — are Finland, Iceland, Denmark,
Switzerland, and the Netherlands (Helliwell et al., 2021).

In contrast, the unhappiest countries are those that tend to be emerging markets
or have a lower gross domestic product (GDP), e.g., Zimbabwe, Tanzania, and India
(Graham, 2005; Helliwell et al., 2021).

At face value, this makes sense: Poorer countries most likely have other factors
associated with them, e.g., higher unemployment, more crime, and less political
stability. So, based on this cross-sectional data, a country’s wealth and happiness
levels appear to be correlated. However, over a more extended period, the
relationship between happiness and GDP is nil (Easterlin, 2004).
That is, the subjective wellbeing of a population does not increase as a country
becomes richer. Even though the wealth of various countries worldwide has
increased over time, the overall happiness levels have not increased similarly or
have remained static (Kahneman et al., 2006). This is known as a happiness–income
paradox.

Easterlin (2004) posits four explanations for this finding:

1. Societal and individual gains associated with increased wealth are


concentrated among the extremely wealthy.
2. Our degree of happiness is informed by how we compare to other people,
and this relative comparison does not change as country-wide wealth
increases.
3. Happiness is not limited to only wealth and financial status, but is affected by
other societal and political factors, such as crime, education, and trust in the
government.
4. Long-term satisfaction and contentment differ from short-term, acute
happiness.

Kahneman et al. (2006) provide an alternative explanation centered on the method


typically used by researchers. Specifically, they argue that the order of the
questions asked to measure happiness and how these questions are worded have a
focusing effect. Through the question, the participant’s attention to their happiness
is sharpened — like a lens in a camera — and their happiness needs to be over- or
underestimated.

Kahneman et al. (2006) also point out that job advancements like a raise or a
promotion are often accompanied by an increase in salary and work hours.
Consequently, high-paying jobs often result in less leisure time available to spend
with family or on hobbies and can cause more unhappiness.

Not all that glitters is gold

Extensive research explored whether a sudden financial windfall was associated


with a spike in happiness (e.g., Sherman et al., 2020). The findings were mixed.
Sometimes, having more money is associated with increased life satisfaction and
improved physical and mental health.

This boost in happiness, however, is not guaranteed, nor is it long. Sometimes,


individuals even wish it had never happened (Brickman et al., 1978; Sherman et al.,
2020).

Consider lottery winners. These people win sizable sums of money — typically more
extensive than a salary increase — large enough to impact their lives significantly.
Despite this, research has consistently shown that although lottery winners report
higher immediate, short-term happiness, they do not experience higher long-
term happiness (Sherman et al., 2020).
Here are some reasons for this:

• Previous everyday activities and experiences become less enjoyable when


compared to a unique, unusual experience like winning the lottery.
• People habituate to their new lifestyle.
• A sudden increase in wealth can disrupt social relationships among friends
and family members.
• Work and hobbies typically give us small nuggets of joy over a more extended
period (Csikszentmihalyi et al., 2005). These activities can lose their meaning
over a longer period, resulting in more unhappiness (Sherman et al., 2020;
Brickman et al., 1978).

Sherman et al. (2020) further argue that lottery winners who decide to quit their job
after winning, but do not fill this newly available time with some type of meaningful
hobby or interest, are also more likely to become unhappy.

Passive activities do not provide the same happiness as work or hobbies. Instead, if
lottery winners continue to take part in activities that give them meaning and
require active engagement, then they can avoid further unhappiness.

Happiness: Is it temperature or climate?

Like most psychological research, part of the challenge is clearly defining the topic
of investigation — a task made more daunting when the topic falls within two very
different fields.

Nettle (2005) describes happiness as a three-tiered concept, ranging from short-


lived but intense on one end of the spectrum to more abstract and deep on the
other.

The first tier refers to transitory feelings of joy, like when one opens up a birthday
present.

The second tier describes judgments about feelings, such as feeling satisfied with
your job. The third tier is more complex and refers to life satisfaction.

Across research, different definitions are used: Participants are asked about
feelings of (immediate) joy, overall life satisfaction, moments of happiness or
satisfaction, and mental wellbeing. The concepts are similar but not identical, thus
influencing the results.

6 Fascinating Books and Podcasts on the Topic


Most books on happiness economics are textbooks. Although no doubt very
interesting, they’re not the easy-reading books we prefer to recommend.

Instead, below you will find a range of books written by economists that explore
happiness. These should provide a good springboard on the overall topic of
happiness and what influences it, in case any of our readers want to pick up a more
in-depth textbook afterward.

If you have a happiness book you would recommend, please let us know in the
comments section.

1. Happiness: Lessons from a New Science – Richard Layard

Richard Layard, a lead economist based in London, explores in his book if and how
money can affect happiness.

Layard does an excellent job of introducing topics from various fields and framing
them appropriately for the reader.

The book is aimed at readers from varying academic and professional backgrounds,
so no experience is needed to enjoy it.

Find the book on Amazon.

2. Happiness by Design: Change What You Do, Not How You Think – Paul Dolan
This book has a more practical spin. The author explains how we can use existing
research and theories to make small changes to increase our happiness.

Paul Dolan’s primary thesis is that practical things will have a bigger effect than
abstract methods, and we should change our behavior rather than our thinking.

The book is a quick read (airport-perfect!), and Daniel Kahneman penned the
foreword.

Find the book on Amazon.

3. The Psychology of Money: Timeless Lessons on Wealth, Greed and Happiness –


Morgan Housel

This book is not necessarily about happiness economics, but it is close enough to
the overall theme that it is worth mentioning.
Since most people are concerned with making more money, this book helps teach
the reader why we make the decisions we do and how we make better decisions
about our money.

This book is a worthwhile addition to any bookcase if you are interested in the
relationship between finances and psychology in general.

Find the book on Amazon.

4. Happiness: The Science Behind Your Smile – Daniel Nettle

If you are interested in happiness overall, then we recommend Happiness: The


Science Behind Your Smile by Daniel Nettle, a professor of behavioral science at
Newcastle University.

In this book, he takes a scientific approach to explaining happiness, starting with an


in-depth exploration of the definition of happiness and some of its challenges.

The research that he presents comes from various fields, including social sciences,
medicine, neurobiology, and economics.

Because of its small size, this book is perfect for a weekend away or to read on a
plane.

Find the book on Amazon.

5 & 6. Prefer to listen rather than read?

One of our favorite podcasts is Intelligence2, where leading experts in a particular


field gather to debate a particular topic.
One episode is dedicated to the debate on whether money can
buy happiness. The debaters do not limit themselves to only the topic of happiness
— what it is, how to measure it — but extend into other practical topics such as
policy making and how this can be adjusted to change the happiness of a
population.

Another interesting spin on the happiness economics topic is


how altruism can increase happiness, specifically by donating money to others.

This show’s host, Dr. Laurie Santos, argues that we can increase our happiness by
not hoarding our money for ourselves but by giving it to others instead. If you are
interested in this episode, or any of the other episodes in the Happiness Lab
podcast series, then head on over to their page.

Resources From PositivePsychology.com


There are several resources available at PositivePsychology.com for our readers to
use in their professional and personal development.

In this section, you’ll find a few that should supplement any work on happiness and
economics. Since the undercurrent of the topic is whether happiness can be
improved through wealth, a few resources look at happiness overall.

Valued Living Masterclass

Although knowledge is power, knowing that money does not guarantee happiness
does not mean that clients will suddenly feel fulfilled and satisfied with their lives.

For this reason, we recommend the Valued Living Masterclass, for professionals to
help their clients find meaning in their lives. Rather than keeping up with the
Joneses or chasing a high-paying job, professionals can help their clients connect
with their inner meaning (i.e., their why) as a way to find meaning and gain
happiness.

Three free exercises


If you want to try it out before committing, look at the Meaning & Valued Living
exercise pack, which includes three exercises for free.

Recommended reading

Read our post on Success Versus Happiness for further information on balancing
happiness with success, in any domain. This topic is poignant for readers who
conflate happiness and success, and will guide readers to better understand their
relationship and how the two terms influence each other.

For readers who wonder about altruism, you would find it interesting that rather
than hoarding, you can increase your happiness through volunteering and
donating. In this post, the author, Dr. Jeremy Sutton, does a fabulous job of
approaching altruism from various fields and provides excellent resources for
further reading and real-life application.

Our last recommendation is for readers who want to know more about measuring
subjective wellbeing and happiness. The post lists various tests and apps that can
measure happiness and the overall history of how happiness was measured and
defined. This is a good starting point for researchers or clinicians who want to
explore happiness economics professionally.

17 Meaning & Valued Living Exercises

Lastly if you’re looking for more science-based ways to help others discover
meaning, check out this collection of 17 validated meaning tools for practitioners.
Use them to help others choose directions for their lives in alignment with what is
truly important to them and can make them happy.

A Take-Home Message
As you’ve seen in our article, the evidence overwhelmingly clarifies that money
does not guarantee more happiness … well, long-term happiness.

Our happiness is relative since we compare ourselves to other people, and over
time, as we become accustomed to our wealth, we lose all the happiness gains we
made.

Money can ease financial and social difficulties; consequently, it can drastically
improve people’s living conditions, life expectancy, and education.

Improvements in these outcomes have a knock-on effect on the overall experience


of one’s life and the opportunities for one’s family and children. Nevertheless,
better opportunities do not guarantee happiness.

Our intention with this post was to illustrate some complexities surrounding the
relationship between money and happiness.
Knowing that money does not guarantee happiness, we recommend less expensive
methods to improve one’s happiness:

• Spend time with friends.


• Cultivate hobbies and interests.
• Stay active and eat healthy.
• Try to live a meaningful life.
• Give some love (go smooch your partner or tickle your dog’s belly).

Diamonds might be a girl’s best friend, but money is a fair weather one, at best.

We hope you enjoyed reading this article. Don’t forget to download our three
Meaning and Valued Living Exercises for free.
REFERENCES

▪ Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and
accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 36(8), 917.
▪ Bruni, L. (2007). Handbook on the economics of happiness. Edward Elgar.
▪ Clark, A. E. (2018). Four decades of the economics of happiness: Where
next? Review of Income and Wealth, 64(2), 245–269.
▪ Csikszentmihalyi, M., Abuhamdeh, S., & Nakamura, J. (2005). Flow. In A. J.
Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 598–
608). Guilford Publications.
▪ Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Seidlitz, L., & Diener, M. (1993). The relationship
between income and subjective well-being: Relative or absolute? Social
Indicators Research, 28, 195–223.
▪ Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Money and happiness: Income and subjective
well-being across nations. Culture and Subjective Well-Being, 185, 218.
▪ DiMaria, C. H., Peroni, C., & Sarracino, F. (2020). Happiness matters:
Productivity gains from subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness
Studies, 21(1), 139–160.
▪ Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some
empirical evidence. In P. A. David & M. W. Reder (Eds.), Nations and
households in economic growth: Essays in honor of Moses Abramovitz (pp. 89–
125). Academic Press.
▪ Easterlin, R. A. (2004). The economics of happiness. Daedalus, 133(2), 26–33.
▪ Graham, C. (2005). The economics of happiness. World Economics, 6(3), 41–
55.
▪ Haesevoets, T., Dierckx, K., & Van Hiel, A. (2022). Do people believe that you
can have too much money? The relationship between hypothetical lottery
wins and expected happiness. Judgment and Decision Making, 17(6), 1229–
1254.
▪ Helliwell, J., Layard, R., & Sachs, J. (Eds.) (2012). World happiness report. The
Earth Institute, Columbia University.
▪ Helliwell, J. F., Layard, R., Sachs, J. D., & Neve, J. E. D. (2021). World happiness
report 2021.
▪ Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2006).
Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing
illusion. Science, 312(5782), 1908–1910.
▪ Nettle, D. (2005). Happiness: The science behind your smile. Oxford University
Press.
▪ Sherman, A., Shavit, T., & Barokas, G. (2020). A dynamic model on happiness
and exogenous wealth shock: The case of lottery winners. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 21, 117–137.
▪ Steptoe, A. (2019). Happiness and health. Annual Review of Public Health, 40,
339–359.
▪ Veenhoven, R., & Ehrhardt, J. (1995). The cross-national pattern of happiness:
Test of predictions implied in three theories of happiness. Social Indicators
Research, 34, 33–68.

You might also like