You are on page 1of 9

RES EARCH

◥ The reach of culture has similarly been dis-


REVIEW SUMMARY covered to span diversity in behavioral do-
mains, including foraging techniques, tool use,
ANIMAL CULTURE vocal communication, social customs and pref-

The burgeoning reach of animal culture


erences for particular prey, migratory path-
ways, nesting sites, and mates. The revelation
that cultural inheritance permeates many spe-
Andrew Whiten cies’ lives is increasingly recognized to have
profound implications for evolutionary biol-
ogy at large, because it provides a second form
BACKGROUND: Culture—the inheritance of an has witnessed an explosion in discoveries of inheritance that builds on the primary ge-
array of behavioral traditions through social about social learning and culture, not only in netic inheritance system, facilitating cultural
learning from others—was once thought spe- primates but also in a rapidly growing range evolution. The two inheritance systems may
cific to humans. Recent and accumulating evi- of animal species, from cetaceans to a diverse generate rich interactive effects, as they have
dence has shown that, to the contrary, culture array of birds, fish, and even invertebrates. in humans.
permeates the lives of a great diversity of ani- A plethora of innovative experiments has fur-
mals, with far-reaching implications for evolu- ADVANCES: Novel experimental designs have ther identified an array of cognitive processes
tionary biology, anthropology, and conservation. rigorously demonstrated the cultural trans- involved in learning from others, ranging from
Early evidence for animal culture emerged in mission and spread of behavioral innovations simple and ubiquitous forms to specialized ones
the mid–20th century in the discovery of re- introduced by researchers, both in the wild such as imitation and teaching. These forms
gional birdsong dialects and the spread of and in labs. New statistical methods have de- of social learning have been shown to be fur-
provisioned sweet potato washing in Japanese tected the signatures of behavioral innova- ther refined through a variety of selective biases,
monkeys. Stimulated by these discoveries, long- tions as they spread through social networks, such as conforming to majorities or copying
term studies of wild chimpanzees and orang- identifying culture in species (e.g., whales) for particularly skilled elders.
utans later in the century revealed complex which experiments are impractical. Through
cultures composed of multiple traditions span- these and other methodological advances, the OUTLOOK: United Nations bodies operating un-
ning diverse aspects of apes’ lives, from tool use reach of cultural learning is now known to der the aegis of international conventions have
to social and sexual behavior. encompass an unexpected range of species, recently recognized the importance of all that
In part through the accumulation of further with surprising new discoveries extending has been discovered about animal cultures, for
long-term field studies, the present century even to insects, from bees to fruit flies. conservation policies and practices. Among

Downloaded from https://www.science.org on September 06, 2021


sperm whales and chimpanzees, specific cultural

PHOTOS: (A) THOMAS MUELLER, (B) ETIENNE DANCHIN, (C) ISTOCK.COM/SKIBRECK, (D) ERICA VAN DE WAAL, (E) ALEX THORNTON, (F) JENNIFER ALLEN
entities, as opposed to genetically defined units,
A B C have been recognized as meriting conserva-
tion in their own right. This finding, in turn,
urges a greater focus on understanding cultural
phenomena in the wild. The task of rigorously
identifying social learning has relied heavily
on controlled experiments in captivity, but
field experiments are increasingly carried out.
These and other innovative methods to iden-
tify and trace animal cultures in the wild de-
serve to be developed and applied further to
D E F
wild populations.
The wealth of methodological advances and
empirical discoveries about animal cultures in
the present century provides an exciting foun-
dation from which to explore deeper ques-
tions. Do animal cultures evolve, cumulatively,
as human cultures have done so impressively
over past millennia? How profoundly does the
lifetime reach of culture in animals’ lives re-
Diversity in cultural species and behavioral domains. (A) After filial imprinting on the costumed human shape our understanding of behavioral ecology
pilot of a microlight aircraft, young cranes followed the flight path of this surrogate parent, adopting it and the fundamentals of evolution at large?
as a traditional migratory route. (B) Female fruit flies (left) that witness a male marked with one of two colors How close are human and animal cultures now
mating (top right) later prefer to mate with similarly colored males. This behavior is further copied by perceived to be, and where do the principal
others, initiating a tradition. (C) Bighorn sheep translocated to unfamiliar locations were initially sedentary,
but spring migration and skill in reaching higher-altitude grazing grounds expanded over decades, implicating
differences remain?

intergenerational cultural transmission. (D) Groups of vervet monkeys were trained to avoid bitter-tasting
corn of one color and to prefer the other. Later, when offered these options with no distasteful additive, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews,
both naïve infants and immigrating adult males adopted the experimentally created local group preference. St Andrews KY16 9JP, UK. Email: a.whiten@st-andrews.ac.uk
Cite this article as A. Whiten, Science 372, eabe6514
(E) Young meerkats learn scorpion predation because adults initially supply live prey with stingers removed (2021). DOI: 10.1126/science.abe6514
and later provide unmodified prey as the young meerkats mature. (F) A humpback whale innovation of
slapping the sea surface to refine predation, known as “lobtail feeding,” spread over two decades to create READ THE FULL ARTICLE AT
a new tradition in hundreds of other humpbacks. For reference citations, see the full article. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abe6514

Whiten, Science 372, 46 (2021) 2 April 2021 1 of 1


RES EARCH

◥ have followed the accumulation of long-term


REVIEW field records. For example, a humpback whale
foraging innovation called lobtail feeding was
ANIMAL CULTURE traced from its origins in just 1 or 2 individuals

The burgeoning reach of animal culture


to more than 600 others, through the collation
of 27 years of consecutive records (18). Exper-
imental designs that earlier typically focused
Andrew Whiten only on what one individual might socially
learn from another have been extended to
Culture can be defined as all that is learned from others and is repeatedly transmitted in this way, identify transmission chains in which a be-
forming traditions that may be inherited by successive generations. This cultural form of inheritance was havior spreads between multiple individuals, a
once thought specific to humans, but research over the past 70 years has instead revealed it to be core criterion for culture (19, 20) (Fig. 1). Such
widespread in nature, permeating the lives of a diversity of animals, including all major classes of “diffusion” experiments have naturally been
vertebrates. Recent studies suggest that culture’s reach may extend also to invertebrates—notably, easier to engineer with captive subjects than
insects. In the present century, the reach of animal culture has been found to extend across many in the wild, but their power in reliably iden-
different behavioral domains and to rest on a suite of social learning processes facilitated by a tifying a role for social learning has increas-
variety of selective biases that enhance the efficiency and adaptiveness of learning. Far-reaching ingly been applied in the field. For example,
implications, for disciplines from evolutionary biology to anthropology and conservation policies, are introduction into wild populations of pairs of
increasingly being explored. great tits trained to use either of two alter-
native foraging techniques demonstrated high

C
fidelity spread of whichever technique was
ulture shapes our human lives profound- mid-20th-century discoveries. First, the inno- seeded, continuing into a subsequent season
ly, so much so that what it means to be vation in titmice of tearing milk bottle tops to (21). An alternative to training initial models
human (our behavioral repertoires, for drink the cream beneath was tracked from to perform novel behaviors has been to cross-
example) differs enormously between, a small number of initial UK centers as it foster young, an approach particularly suited
and sometimes within, the many differ- diffused across the country over the next to birds, as eggs can simply be swapped. The
ent regions of the planet occupied by our spe- decade (11). Second, novel foraging behaviors adoptive species then provides natural models
cies, as well as across human history. In the past, in Japanese macaque monkeys were likewise who behave differently from what is normal for
it was thought that humans’ ability to have cul- tracked as they spread through kin networks the adopted chick species. Such cross-fostering
ture separates us from the rest of living systems (12). Finally, the discovery of regionally vary- remains rare but has demonstrated social

Downloaded from https://www.science.org on September 06, 2021


and the dynamic evolutionary forces that shape ing birdsong dialects was coupled with labo- learning of dietary preferences by blue tits
them. Yet, a broader perspective recognizes that ratory experiments to confirm the necessity fostered by great tits and vice versa (22). In
culture is intimately embedded in evolutionary of learning from existing models, thus pro- cases for which diffusion experiments of these
biology because it incorporates a second inher- viding early evidence of vocal cultures in different kinds are impractical (as, for exam-
itance system (social learning, or learning from animals (13). These beginnings were followed ple, with whales), a range of sophisticated sta-
others) built on the foundations of the primary, by a steady stream of other examples in the tistical techniques have been developed (14).
genetically based inheritance system (1, 2). More- 20th century, some based on naturalistic re- In the lobtail feeding example outlined above,
over, as recognized by Darwin himself (3) and cordings in nature and others on experiments cultural transmission was revealed because
addressed in more recent theoretical and em- with captive populations, demonstrating social the behavior could be shown specifically to
pirical works (4–7), this can in turn engender learning and the diffusion of new traditions, spread along social networks, as a social learn-
an elaborated manifestation of evolution: cumu- principally in a diversity of mammals, birds, ing hypothesis predicts (18).
lative cultural evolution, the results of which we and fishes (2, 7, 14).
see in our technologies, languages, customs, and These studies typically demonstrated a sin- The burgeoning reach of culture across animal
virtually all aspects of human life (8). gle tradition, as in the three examples outlined taxa and behavioral domains
Interactions between these two evolution- above. Research on chimpanzees, by contrast, Figure 2 offers illustrations of the reach of ani-
ary systems create the complexities of spiral- accumulated evidence during the latter part of mal culture across both animal taxa and be-
ing gene–culture coevolution (5, 6), the effects the century that communities differed in mul- havioral domains (23–38), adding to those
of which have become increasingly apparent tiple traditions, spanning a variety of behavior shown on the summary page (18, 39–43).
as the fields of human genomics and cultural patterns such as tool use, grooming styles, and Identifying cultural transmission depends on
evolution have progressed (9, 10). foraging techniques. By the turn of the cen- establishing a critical role for social learning,
tury, systematic collation of these records re- and controlled experiments arguably offer the
The discovery of animal cultures: An endeavor vealed as many as 39 such cultural variants, most compelling option in scientists’ toolbox
spanning seven decades with different communities identifiable by dis- to clarify this issue of causality. If we thus first
Though all of these manifestations of culture tinctive arrays of traditions (15), echoing the consider diffusion experiments of the kind
have historically been thought specific to our way human cultures are distinguished by ar- illustrated in Fig. 1, we find, in a survey of
species, research in recent decades has re- rays of technologies, social customs, and diets. the 34 such efforts published by 2008, that the
vealed that, in varied guises, they are shared This was soon followed by similar reports of focus on primates and birds evident in the
with a plethora of species and span a grow- multiple-tradition cultures in orangutans (16) field’s mid-20th-century origins persisted (19).
ing range of behavioral domains (2, 7). and cetaceans [whales and dolphins (17)]. These experiments provided evidence of cul-
Evidence of culture in nonhuman animals A rapidly expanding research literature in tural transmission of behavior in chimpanzees,
(henceforth “animals”) first emerged from three the present century has revealed cultural baboons, vervet monkeys, macaques, colobus
phenomena in a widening diversity of both monkeys, pigeons, blackbirds, magpie-jays,
School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, the species involved and the range of behav- cowbirds, keas, and chickens (19). There was
St Andrews KY16 9JP, UK. Email: a.whiten@st-andrews.ac.uk ioral domains implicated. Many such advances also positive evidence from two species of fish:

Whiten, Science 372, eabe6514 (2021) 2 April 2021 1 of 7


RES EARCH | R E V I E W

A B C
Releases: 12 12 12 12 12
GG CY ER

SK Generation 2
VV VV

Generation 3

BO KT RN ST
Generation 4

BB Generation 5
RI

60 releases
KE Solo
TA (control)

MA Pair
(control)
MS

Fig. 1. Cultural diffusion experiments. Three alternative designs are illustrated. failed to progress, owing to the behavior of a particular individual. (B) Open
(A) Transmission chain. One individual is allowed to observe an initial model diffusion (here, “open” refers to how any learning spreads in a population). In this
trained to perform either of two alternative behaviors. After testing, that observer example, populations of wild great tits were exposed to two models trained to
becomes the model for a third individual, and so on, simulating potential push a small door to either the blue or red side of an artificial feeder (21). Each
intergenerational transmission (each two-letter identifier represents an individual option spread with high fidelity to others. The diagrams represent social networks
chimpanzee). In this experiment, the first chimpanzee (GG or ER) learned to for three subpopulations, with original models marked by yellow circles, those
open an “artificial fruit” box by either lifting a hatch (left chain; blue arrows) or learning the local technique marked by red circles, and naïve individuals marked by
sliding it to one side (right chain; red arrows) (91). After four or five transitions, black circles. Red connecting lines represent diffusion across social networks;
fidelity remained at 100% for each alternative (although BB performed a hatch-lift black connecting lines represent other social networks. [Photo: Lucy Aplin]

Downloaded from https://www.science.org on September 06, 2021


action in 5% of trials). Such an approach has challenges—for instance, VV (C) Replacement design. One individual is replaced by a naïve individual after each
was intimidated by CY so was allowed to learn from ER instead. Solid black arrows transmission, so after multiple transmissions the population is entirely different
represent each individual’s progression from observer to model. Dashed black from the original. In this example, pigeons homed repeatedly in pairs before each
arrows indicate that MS observed both TA and MA (and the two-headed arrow replacement (77). Homing efficiency increased over time, demonstrating a
indicates that MA, in turn, observed MS). Gray arrows denote that the experiment cumulative aspect to the implicated cultural transmission.

guppies and platyfish. However, only 2 of these transmission. Similarly, since helping to lay the observer to watch a trained model, van de
34 studies (both on birds) pioneered such ex- foundation for animal culture study 70 years Waal et al. (42) first trained four whole groups
periments in the wild. To these 34 studies ago, birdsong research has confirmed regional of vervet monkeys to avoid either pink- or
from the first 60 years of the field, a further dialects in more than 80 species (31), so the blue-stained corn provisions (Fig. 2), by making
30 diffusion studies could be added over just phenomenon may well be common across many one unpalatable at a stage after the birth sea-
the next 8 years (20), highlighting accelerat- of the ~4000 species of songbirds. son when infants were not taking solid food.
ing discoveries. By this point, 13 of these ex- Surveying the field more broadly, we find When later tested with no unpalatable addi-
periments were extended to wild populations. that social learning has been identified in tive in either color, all 27 weaning infants, naïve
The first experiments to demonstrate cultural choices of diet, prey types, nest sites, migratory to the corn, matched their mother’s color choice,
diffusion in insects were also included, with pathways, and mates, as well as in shaping ignoring the (now equally palatable) alterna-
fruit flies being shown to adopt the prefer- foraging techniques, tool use, vocal repertoires, tive. This illustrates phase one, in which a
ences for alternative egg-laying substrates of courtship displays, predator recognition, circa- parent (typically the mother in primates and
conspecifics (36). dian rhythms, allogrooming patterns, social cus- many other mammals) provides the principal
Such controlled experiments are unlikely with toms, and even play patterns (2, 14, 37, 44, 45) model for what the infant learns. In birds,
species such as whales, but alternative routes (Fig. 2). both parents more often share this initial role,
to identifying social learning have emerged. and the prevalence of this phase will clearly
When new humpback songs appear frequently The reach of culture through the lifetime depend on the extent of parental care in the
and records show their rapid spread across A recent review distinguished three main species studied.
populations, neither genetic nor environmental phases of social learning that occur across The vervet experiment additionally found
explanations can account for this phenomenon, many primates (46): (i) an infant’s initial that dispersing adult males quickly adopted
and these occurrences have been aptly de- intimate relationship with its mother, (ii) the the local preference of the group they entered,
scribed as “cultural revolutions” (31). Ceta- later progressive expansion of a juvenile’s so- even when an opposite preference had been
ceans have contributed substantial evidence of cial network, and (iii) adult dispersal to new habitual in the group from which they came, a
cultural transmission across these and multiple groups and ranges. A field experiment with result attributed to a tendency to conform to
other behavioral domains, with a recent review wild vervet monkeys illustrates both the first the behavioral profiles of a majority of one’s
(25) able to cite as many as 70 publications re- and third phases (42). Whereas most social companions (42). This illustrates phase three,
porting evidence for social learning or cultural learning experiments begin by allowing an when dispersal creates a new role for social

Whiten, Science 372, eabe6514 (2021) 2 April 2021 2 of 7


RES EARCH | R E V I E W

A B C D

E F G H

Fig. 2. Illustrations of diversity in cultural phenomena across species and the conditions for incipient speciation (32). (E) Prey selection. Neighboring
behavioral domains. (A) Vocal and migratory traditions. In humpback whales, communities of bonobos display different preferences for prey animals, even in
new songs arise intermittently and spread rapidly across large populations (23), areas of large overlap between adjacent ranges, excluding ecological explanations
including eastward drift across the Pacific basin over several years (24). Juveniles for the differences (33) [Photo: Kokolopori Bonobo Research Project]. Ecotype
follow mothers on annual migrations between breeding and feeding grounds, clans of killer whales display quite different hunting traditions, such as specializing
assimilating these traditional routes (25). [Photo: Jennifer Allen] (B) Arbitrary and in seal versus fish prey (25, 34). (F) Predator recognition. Sticklebacks and
social customs. The arbitrary custom of placing a grass blade in one ear spread minnows developed fear responses to pike by learning from conspecifics and, in
from the innovator to other chimpanzees in just one among four sanctuary turn, passed this disposition on to others (35) [Photo: Mike Webster]. In birds,
enclosures (26) [Photo: Edwin van Leeuwen]. In capuchins, social customs—such experiments that seed fear of arbitrary objects have demonstrated cultural
as mutual placing of fingers in mouths, nostrils, and eyes—spread and may last transmission along chains of individuals (19). (G) Breeding sites. Preferences of

Downloaded from https://www.science.org on September 06, 2021


several years (27). (C) Tool use. Young capuchin monkeys selectively attend fruit flies for specific egg-laying substrates have been shown experimentally
to the best nut-crackers in regions where stone tools are so used (28) [Photo: to be adopted by others, from whom yet others learn (36) [Photo: Frederic Mery].
Tiago Falótico]. Similarly, young New Caledonian crows attend closely to complex There is evidence of similar effects in birds, leading to traditionally favored
parental tool use (29). Diffusion experiments with apes demonstrate that tool breeding sites (37). (H) Foraging techniques. A bumble bee trained to pull
use techniques spread through social learning (19, 20). (D) Vocal dialects. Local string to access an artificial food source was copied by other bees, who were
birdsong dialects persist for decades but also show evolutionary change, as subsequently copied by others, initiating a tradition (38) [Photo: Lars Chittka].
recognized in savanna sparrows (30) [Photo: Heather Williams]. Scores of studies Experiments with primates have shown that alternative techniques may
document such dialects in many avian species (31), in some cases creating spread differentially in the groups seeded (19, 20).

learning about an animal’s new habitat and “peering,” which involves close and sustained the serious study of the larger topic of culture.
the local social dynamics, drawing on the ac- visual attention to another’s actions, confirmed More recently, attention has additionally turned
cumulated knowledge of the local residents. nine different predictions consistent with the to what are variously termed “transmission
Phase two describes a common (but varia- implication of social learning, with juveniles biases” or “social learning strategies” that re-
ble) pattern in primates for the early relation- repeating acts they had just witnessed, from fine the targeting of social learning processes
ship with the mother to be supplemented by foraging to nesting. It was found that such according to cues such as those provided by
a widening social network, providing oppor- peering occurred in as many as 125 and 195 particularly successful or high-ranking indi-
tunities to learn from others with different different contexts in two communities studied. viduals (50). Finally, heavy reliance on cultural
knowledge and skill sets. Primate examples This led the authors to conclude that “when acquisitions requires appropriate storage ca-
include male juveniles apprenticing themselves looking closely at great ape skill acquisition, it pacity in the brain. Our human reliance on
to adult males to learn about sex-typical forag- seems that immatures learn virtually all of their culture delivers a vast volume of information
ing options (47) or tool use (28) (Fig. 2). skills socially” (48). Whether every instance of that taxes our memory banks, but arrays of
A recent study has argued, controversially, peering truly indexes social learning may be cultural knowledge may have important neu-
that the scope of all that is learned through debatable, but this study presents a welcome ral requirements in animals, particularly long-
such phases has been underestimated by re- challenge to critically test similar measures in lived species (51, 52).
search to date, which has typically relied on this and other species in the future. Social learning has been discovered to show
enumerating behavioral differences between a range of different forms among animals.
communities where genetic and environmen- The psychological reach of culture: An array Dissections of these forms over recent decades
tal explanations can reasonably be discounted of cognitive adaptations have themselves evolved along with empirical
(15, 16). As Schuppli and van Schaik (48) cor- A reliance on cultural transmission requires findings (53, 54). The most recent reviews
rectly note, this neglects cultural adaptation to associated cognitive capacities. Most funda- differ in details but converge in distinguish-
local ecologies, as well as cultural universals. mental are forms of social learning that have ing ~10 principal categories (55, 56). Those
In an earlier study in wild orangutans, these been dissected conceptually and experimen- illustrated in Fig. 3 differentiate what is so-
authors established that a behavior known as tally for more than a century (49), predating cially learned, with the associated cognitive

Whiten, Science 372, eabe6514 (2021) 2 April 2021 3 of 7


RES EARCH | R E V I E W

requirements varying from those generally kingdom, but with the added factor of learn- aboveground mounds and subterranean nests,
regarded as relatively simple to the relatively ing not from associations of events and re- the latter involving a tool set. A thick stick is
complex. There is not enough space here to inforcements in relation to one’s own behavior, first used to make a long tunnel underground,
offer a comprehensive analysis of these pro- but instead from those perceived in others’ with a finer stem—its end first fashioned into
cesses; instead I offer examples from either actions. This may be the most widespread form a more effective brush tip—then inserted to
end of the array. At the more elementary of social learning across numerous species fish out termites (60). The authors judged that
end, stimulus enhancement occurs when an and behavioral contexts. ecological factors could not explain the alter-
observer witnesses a model that focuses on At the other end of the scale is the copying native methods adopted locally, and because
particular objects and subsequently adopts of relatively complex behavioral routines, as these involved specific bodily postures, it was
the same focus. For example, bumble bees demonstrated in transmission chain experi- concluded that relatively high-fidelity bodily
that observed model bees on green or orange ments with chimpanzees (58, 59). A recent imitation was implicated. This finding is con-
flowers later visited those with the color they study discovered that each of 10 different sistent with experimental studies showing that
had seen preferred (57). Such effects would wild chimpanzee communities is character- chimpanzees may copy specific alternative
appear to require only the kind of associative ized by a distinctive combination of action sequences of component actions that they wit-
learning that is common across the animal elements used locally to fish for termites from ness (61) (Fig. 4). Nevertheless, the distribu-
tion of such forms of social learning among
animals has been, and continues to be, a focus
of debate in this field (56, 62, 63). Even bees
Social learning Imitative
Imitation May include matching:
have been shown to be sophisticated in their
learning from others Copying the form of an action shape, sequential
much copied social learning. In one study, instead of simply
structure, hierarchical
detail Object movement copying
structure, causal links, copying a demonstrator’s movement of an
Copying the form of a caused
Copying intentional links object to a target, the bees flexibly created an
tool or other object movement
copying what another Emulative improved version of this action (64). This topic
individual does End-state emulation Result emulation
few elements is ripe for imaginative and taxonomically di-
Copying only the end or Replicating the
copied verse studies to provide greater clarity in the
outcome of an action result(s) of an
action coming years.
The forms of social learning detailed in
Affordance learning About properties Goal emulation Fig. 3 have been found to be supplemented
Learning about operating characteristics About relationships Replicating the by a further range of modulating biases that
of objects or environment About functions (e.g., of tools) inferred goal(s) can refine their adaptive power. For example,
of an action

Downloaded from https://www.science.org on September 06, 2021


evidence of conforming to an option that
Observational conditioning most members of a population have already
Learning the positive or negative value of an object or event adopted (and thus likely tested already), itself
Stimulus enhancement
taking several different forms (65), comes from
Focused on an object birds (21), primates (42), and insects (40). A
Enhancement recent review of such modulating influences
Focusing attention on part of the environment
Local enhancement [i.e., transmission biases or social learning
Focused on a location strategies (50)] enumerates as many as 25 var-
iants, which include, as major categories, those
Fig. 3. A taxonomy of social learning processes. In each example, something new is learned from one that are state based (e.g., copy when uncertain),
or more individuals. The primary distinctions concern what is learned in this manner [for detailed dissections frequency dependent (e.g., copy majority, as
of this and related taxonomies, see (14, 53–56)]. The array portrayed here displays an approximate above), model based (e.g., copy high-rankers), or
gradation from processes with low cognitive requirements at the bottom to those with higher requirements content based (e.g., copy behavior seen to earn
above (see main text for further discussion). highest payoff). Evidence has also accumulated

M M
Outer bolt P P H
H L L
Inner bolt
Handle

P Ob
H L P H L
Ob

M M
Pin H
P L
P L
Inner bolt P
Pin H
Outer bolt Spin pin Ob Ob P H
P L L
Pull bolt Poke bolt H
H Handle L Open lid

Fig. 4. Experimental evidence for copying of behavioral sequences by four chimpanzees. A human model (M; upper graphic in each pair of sequences)
opened an “artificial fruit” box, removing bolts and a pin, which then allowed the model to disable a handle, after which a lid could be opened to gain a reward.
The model displayed different sequences composed of alternative forms of action, each of which were matched by chimpanzee observers (Ob; lower graphic in each
pair) beyond chance levels (61).

Whiten, Science 372, eabe6514 (2021) 2 April 2021 4 of 7


RES EARCH | R E V I E W

from both birds and mammals for active sup- tion: cultural evolution (1–7, 45, 62, 63, 72, 73) data, sampling of capuchin monkey nut-
port of parents and others to juveniles’ social (Fig. 5). cracking artifacts from an archaeological
learning, a phenomenon conceptualized as In organic evolution, selectively neutral ge- record of 3000 years indicates four differ-
“teaching” but defined in functional rather netic drift occurs as mutational changes in ent phases—distinguished by size, use-wear,
than the intentional terms familiar in human DNA are coupled with dispersion of popula- degree of percussive battering, and use of
contexts (66, 67). tions across time and space (74). In the cul- stone versus wooden anvils—that the authors
tural counterpart, “mutation” may occur through suggest must reflect adaptations to the food
Implications for anthropology: The evolution imperfect copying of others, coupled with sim- types exploited (76). Capuchin stone tool use
of human culture ilar kinds of dispersion patterns (72, 73). Long- has thus evolved over these millennia. In the
With regard to evolutionary anthropology, the term field studies may be required to detect case of the Savannah sparrow study noted
broadest implication of the discovery of wide- such changes, and these are progressively ac- above, 30 years was sufficient to detect changes
spread social learning and culture in animals cumulating. For example, a study of changes in song elements, some of which showed both
is that human culture did not spring up out in the song repertoires of Savannah sparrows an association with enhanced reproductive
of nowhere but instead has ancient evolu- over 30 years identified a subset of elements success and consistent directional change, in
tionary foundations. Further, the compara- that showed no consistent directionality in their line with Darwinian natural or sexual selec-
tive method can be applied to use studies of changes and were not associated with varia- tion (30).
humans’ living relatives, particularly primates, tions in reproductive success (whereas some All of the above examples appear to express
to reconstruct key ancestral cultural founda- other elements were linked to reproductive evolutionary change rather than the kind of
tions. Such work has by now generated a sub- success and thus fit other categories in Fig. 5, progressive advancement in features such as
stantial research literature reviewed elsewhere as discussed below) (30). Birdsong research complexity and efficiency that characterizes
(68–70), any serious treatment of which is provides many more such examples (31), and human cultural evolution, often known as
beyond the scope of this brief review. An il- similar processes are thought to exist in the cumulative culture and suggested to be exclu-
lustrative example is that identification of diffusion of whale songs (24). These sorts of sive to our species (62). However, experimental
cultural commonalities in great apes (includ- effects may be more common in such commu- evidence for such cumulative cultural buildup
ing humans), and hence attributed to their nication patterns than in behavior such as has begun to appear in diverse species and
common ancestry, includes the patterning of foraging, for which an ecological optimum contexts that range from pigeon homing (77)
multiple and diverse traditions in time and may exist. (Fig. 1) through pattern recognition in baboons
space (15, 16), a “portfolio” of social learning A similar methodological challenge is likely (78) to exploration and tool use in chimpanzees
capacities (48, 56) facilitating the spread and to exist when we turn to the other major cat- (79, 80). In all of these cases, it has been pos-
maintenance of traditions (58, 59), and cul- egory in Fig. 5: cultural evolution through sible to record cumulative change over short

Downloaded from https://www.science.org on September 06, 2021


tural contents such as tool use (68). Darwinian selection processes. Here the periods, given the experimental manipulations
Research on cultural phenomena in more Darwinian triumvirate of elements will be in involved.
distantly related species may also cast light on play—inheritance (through social learning), Long-term field studies have also revealed
principles that underlie convergent evolution. variation (in culturally transmitted behavioral evidence of forms of cumulative culture among
For example, teaching, as defined in func- elements), and selection according to the rela- animals. For example, bighorn sheep trans-
tional terms above, appears particularly com- tive fitness of these elements—hence shaping located to areas of the US in which they had
mon in long-lived and predatory species, in more optimal adaptation in the repertoires of previously been extirpated were initially se-
which the young have to vault from suckling descendant generations (1–7, 45, 72, 73). The dentary, but over many decades and multiple
to skillful hunting and killing (43, 66, 67). time frames for such changes may often be generations they became progressively migra-
Similarly, extended juvenility and a heavy re- longer than the lifetimes of human scientists: tory in the spring, developing an enhanced
liance on hunting were key innovations in Archaeological excavations have found evidence skill in arriving at higher-altitude pastures at
hominin evolution (71). of western chimpanzees’ use of stone ham- an optimal time (41). The authors conclude
mers to crack nuts extending over 4300 years, that this represents cycles of exploration and
Animal culture expands the scope with no indications of change (75). Yet this transmission of accumulated knowledge to
of evolutionary biology behavior must have evolved over some lon- subsequent generations. Similar effects may
Social learning provides a second inheri- ger time frame. Some such changes may await confirmation in other migratory spe-
tance system built on the more widespread occur only in response to very intermittent cies and other behavioral contexts (81).
foundations of genetic inheritance. In turn, this or long-term ecological perturbations. In Culture presents more than an echo of or-
creates the potential for a second form of evolu- contrast to the chimpanzee nut-cracking ganic evolution, however, precisely because it
operates through radically different processes.
Two examples are particularly notable. One is
Loss of characters
Cultural Selectively neutral, that, unlike genetic inheritance that occurs
cultural evolutionary between parents and offspring only (“verti-
evolution drift Change of characters cally”), social transmission may occur between
within a lineage,
through variation related and unrelated individuals in the same
Directional change temporal generation (“horizontally”) as well as
and differential Cultural evolution Accumulation of
through selection between generations (“obliquely”) (5). Individ-
transmission through Darwinian characters
selection (cumulative culture) uals may thus have multiple “cultural parents”
Stabilizing selection
from whom they learn. A second is that, un-
like the genetic inheritance received in a sin-
Adaptation to physical and biotic ecologies, the latter including social and psychological factors gle “package” at conception, social learning
can fine-tune adaptive responses to environ-
Fig. 5. A dissection of forms of cultural evolution. For elaboration and discussion, see the main text. mental changes throughout a lifetime. To-
[Based on figure 1 of (72)] gether, these characteristics generate more

Whiten, Science 372, eabe6514 (2021) 2 April 2021 5 of 7


RES EARCH | R E V I E W

elaborate dynamics in animals’ adaptive po- hitchhiking” (87). Over time, the more suc- 6. R. Boyd, P. Richerson, Culture and the Evolutionary Process
tentials, including the possibility of much cessful surviving cultures will thence contain (Univ. of Chicago Press, 1985).
7. A. Whiten, F. J. Ayala, M. W. Feldman, K. N. Laland, The
faster and more nimble adaptive change than unusually limited genetic variation. extension of biology through culture. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
would occur via gene-based evolution (37). Sixth and finally, cultural variation may drive 114, 7775–7781 (2017). doi: 10.1073/pnas.1707630114;
the incipient phases of speciation. Birdsong pmid: 28739924
Gene–culture coevolution provides a well-researched case. As songs dif-
8. P. J. Richerson, M. H. Christiansen, Eds., Cultural Evolution:
Society, Technology, Language and Religion (MIT Press, 2013).
A further major implication of animal culture ferentiate between populations, it becomes 9. P. J. Richerson, R. Boyd, J. Henrich, Gene-culture coevolution
for evolutionary biology arises from coevo- increasingly difficult for successful courtship in the age of genomics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107
(suppl. 2), 8985–8992 (2010). doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914631107;
lutionary dynamics between cultural systems and mating to occur between them. For ex- pmid: 20445092
and genetic inheritance (37), numerous ex- ample, a study of more than 4000 song record- 10. K. N. Laland, J. Odling-Smee, S. Myles, How culture shaped the
pressions of which are well known for humans ings from 581 species of two major avian clades human genome: Bringing genetics and the human sciences
(9, 10). Whitehead (82) pioneered the further (tanagers and ovenbirds) revealed associa- together. Nat. Rev. Genet. 11, 137–148 (2010). doi: 10.1038/
nrg2734; pmid: 20084086
exploration of such phenomena in cetaceans. tions between macroevolutionary bursts in 11. J. Fisher, R. A. Hinde, The opening of milk bottles by birds.
A more recent survey encompassing diverse vocal evolution and speciation, with faster Br. Birds 42, 347–357 (1949).
avian and mammalian taxa examined the evi- rates of vocal evolution predicted by the ex- 12. M. Kawai, Newly acquired pre-cultural behaviour of the natural
troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islet. Primates 6,
dence relating to six major types of coevolu- tent of reliance on song learning (32). 1–30 (1965). doi: 10.1007/BF01666109
tionary dynamics (83), as follows. 13. P. Marler, M. Tamura, Culturally transmitted patterns of vocal
First, cultural differences may exert selec- Outlook behavior in sparrows. Science 146, 1483–1486 (1964).
doi: 10.1126/science.146.3650.1483; pmid: 14208581
tion pressures on functional genes. For ex- The United Nations Environment Programme 14. W. J. E. Hoppitt, K. N. Laland, Social Learning: An Introduction to
ample, culturally based killer whale ecotypes has recently recognized the potential impor- Mechanisms, Methods and Models (Princeton Univ. Press, 2013).
differentiated by radically different prey choices tance of all that has been discovered about 15. A. Whiten et al., Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399,
682–685 (1999). doi: 10.1038/21415; pmid: 10385119
(e.g., seals versus salmon) have evolved associ- animal cultures for conservation policies and 16. C. P. van Schaik et al., Orangutan cultures and the evolution of
ated differences in anatomy and physiology, practices, for migratory and nonmigratory material culture. Science 299, 102–105 (2003). doi: 10.1126/
including the methionine cycle of their digestive species alike. An expert working group has science.1078004; pmid: 12511649
17. L. Rendell, H. Whitehead, Culture in whales and dolphins.
systems (34). made initial recommendations and a sum- Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 309–324 (2001). doi: 10.1017/
Second, selection may favor organic adapta- mary Science Policy Forum article ensued (88), S0140525X0100396X; pmid: 11530544
tions that support culture. In primates, a study later joined by other new approaches to the 18. J. Allen, M. Weinrich, W. Hoppitt, L. Rendell, Network-based
spanning 55 genera and 184 species has shown issues so raised (89, 90). diffusion analysis reveals cultural transmission of lobtail
feeding in humpback whales. Science 340, 485–488 (2013).
that cultural propensities are correlated with A core goal of conservation policies is to doi: 10.1126/science.1231976; pmid: 23620054
greater encephalization and longer juvenile maintain heritable variation and adaptive 19. A. Whiten, A. Mesoudi, Review. Establishing an experimental
science of culture: Animal social diffusion experiments.

Downloaded from https://www.science.org on September 06, 2021


and reproductive life spans (84). This and evolvability in natural populations, so the range
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 363, 3477–3488 (2008). doi: 10.1098/
other evidence have been adduced in support of discoveries outlined above begs an urgent rstb.2008.0134; pmid: 18799418
of a “cultural intelligence hypothesis” for the integration of the cultural components of 20. A. Whiten, C. A. Caldwell, A. Mesoudi, Cultural diffusion in
evolution of large brains and intelligence in heritable variation into conservation planning. humans and other animals. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 8, 15–21
(2016). doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.002; pmid: 29506791
primates (51, 52). This hypothesis could apply Notable examples include the increasing rec- 21. L. M. Aplin et al., Experimentally induced innovations lead to
to other species that are particularly reliant on ognition that different cultural variants of the persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature 518,
a richly cultural behavioral repertoire. same species may require differing manage- 538–541 (2015). doi: 10.1038/nature13998; pmid: 25470065
22. T. Slagsvold, K. L. Wiebe, Social learning in birds and its role in
Third, culture may affect coevolution be- ment approaches, that individuals such as shaping a foraging niche. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 366,
tween species. For example, in great tits, matriarchs merit special preservation as cul- 969–977 (2011). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0343; pmid: 21357219
controlled experiments that involve simulated tural repositories, and that animals reintro- 23. M. J. Noad, D. H. Cato, M. M. Bryden, M.-N. Jenner,
K. C. S. Jenner, Cultural revolution in whale songs. Nature 408,
prey have shown that social learning from the duced into the wild may lack critical cultural 537 (2000). doi: 10.1038/35046199; pmid: 11117730
disgust reactions of others to aposematic prey knowledge for which conservation efforts must 24. E. C. Garland et al., Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission
lead to a switch from this prey type to more compensate (88). of humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale. Curr. Biol.
21, 687–691 (2011). doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.019;
cryptic alternatives. This behavior imposes Recognition of such practical implications
pmid: 21497089
changing selection pressures on prey and their of the reach of animal culture, along with the 25. H. Whitehead, L. Rendell, The Cultural Lives of Whales and
respective defense mechanisms (85). implications for the broad range of scientific Dolphins (Univ. of Chicago Press, 2015).
26. E. J. C. van Leeuwen, K. A. Cronin, D. B. Haun, A group-specific
Fourth, culture may shape even neutral ge- disciplines discussed above, should help as-
arbitrary tradition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
netic diversity in space. For example, a subset sure a bright future for research in this field. Anim. Cogn. 17, 1421–1425 (2014). doi: 10.1007/s10071-014-
of dolphin families off the coast of Australia A new generation of scientists will now surely 0766-8; pmid: 24916739
27. S. Perry et al., Social conventions in wild white-faced capuchin
use sponges placed over their rostrums to pursue the wider reaches of culture in ani-
monkeys: Evidence for traditions in a Neotropical primate.
hunt fish in deep water. These dolphins have mals’ lives, aided by the substantial armory of Curr. Anthropol. 44, 241–268 (2003). doi: 10.1086/345825
developed substantially different maternally methodological advances developed over the 28. E. B. Ottoni, B. D. de Resende, P. Izar, Watching the best
nutcrackers: What capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know
transmitted mitochondrial haplotypes from past two decades.
about others’ tool-using skills. Anim. Cogn. 8, 215–219 (2005).
those of their nearby non–tool-using neigh- doi: 10.1007/s10071-004-0245-8; pmid: 15719240
RE FERENCES AND NOTES
bors (86). 29. C. Rutz, G. R. Hunt, J. J. H. St Clair, Corvid technologies:
1. A. Whiten, The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and How do New Caledonian crows get their tool designs?
Fifth, culture may reduce genetic diversity, humans. Nature 437, 52–55 (2005). doi: 10.1038/ Curr. Biol. 28, R1109–R1111 (2018). doi: 10.1016/
as is thought to be the case for the minimal nature04023; pmid: 16136127 j.cub.2018.08.031; pmid: 30253153
mitochondrial variation among sperm whales 2. A. Whiten, A second inheritance system: The extension of 30. H. Williams, I. I. Levin, D. R. Norris, A. E. M. Newman,
biology through culture. Interface Focus 7, 20160142 (2017). N. T. Wheelwright, Three decades of cultural evolution in
and other cetaceans that display matrilineal Savannah sparrow songs. Anim. Behav. 85, 213–223 (2013).
doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2016.0142; pmid: 28839918
social structures. In these structures, daugh- 3. C. Darwin, The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.028
ters remain with their mothers, and hence a (Murray, 1871). 31. C. K. Catchpole, P. J. B. Slater, Bird Song: Biological Themes
4. R. Dawkins, The Selfish Gene (Oxford Univ. Press, 1976). and Variations (Cambridge Univ. Press, ed. 2, 2008).
daughter is restricted to inheriting both her
5. L. L. Cavalli-Sforza, M. W. Feldman, Cultural Transmission and 32. N. A. Mason et al., Song evolution, speciation, and vocal
mother’s genes and the distinctive culture of Evolution: A Quantitative Approach (Princeton Univ. Press, learning in passerine birds. Evolution 71, 786–796 (2017).
her mother’s clan, a process dubbed “cultural (1981). doi: 10.1111/evo.13159; pmid: 28012174

Whiten, Science 372, eabe6514 (2021) 2 April 2021 6 of 7


RES EARCH | R E V I E W

33. L. Samuni, F. Wegdell, M. Surbeck, Behavioural diversity of Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 82, 58–75 (2017). doi: 10.1016/ 104, 3043–3048 (2007). doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607909104;
bonobo prey preference as a potential cultural trait. eLife 9, j.neubiorev.2016.12.018; pmid: 28034660 pmid: 17360606
e59191 (2020). doi: 10.7554/eLife.59191; pmid: 32869740 53. R. W. Byrne, Imitation of novel complex actions: What does the 76. T. Falótico, T. Proffitt, E. B. Ottoni, R. A. Staff, M. Haslam,
34. A. D. Foote et al., Genome-culture coevolution promotes rapid evidence from animals mean? Adv. Stud. Behav. 31, 77–105 Three thousand years of wild capuchin stone tool use.
divergence of killer whale ecotypes. Nat. Commun. 7, 11693 (2002). doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(02)80006-7 Nat. Ecol. Evol. 3, 1034–1038 (2019). doi: 10.1038/s41559-019-
(2016). doi: 10.1038/ncomms11693; pmid: 27243207 54. A. Whiten, V. Horner, C. A. Litchfield, S. Marshall-Pescini, 0904-4; pmid: 31235926
35. A. Mathis, D. P. Chivers, R. J. F. Smith, Cultural transmission of How do apes ape? Learn. Behav. 32, 36–52 (2004). 77. T. Sasaki, D. Biro, Cumulative culture can emerge from
predator recognition in fishes: Intraspecific and interspecific doi: 10.3758/BF03196005; pmid: 15161139 collective intelligence in animal groups. Nat. Commun. 8,
learning. Anim. Behav. 51, 185–201 (1996). doi: 10.1006/ 55. W. J. E. Hoppitt, K. N. Laland, Social processes influencing 15049 (2017). doi: 10.1038/ncomms15049; pmid: 28416804
anbe.1996.0016 learning in animals: A review of the evidence. Adv. Stud. Behav. 78. N. Claidière, K. Smith, S. Kirby, J. Fagot, Cultural evolution of
36. M. Battesti, C. Moreno, D. Joly, F. Mery, Spread of social 38, 105–165 (2008). doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)00003-X systematically structured behaviour in a non-human primate.
information and dynamics of social transmission within 56. A. Whiten, N. McGuigan, S. Marshall-Pescini, L. M. Hopper, Proc.R. Soc. B 281, 20141541 (2014). doi: 10.1098/
Drosophila groups. Curr. Biol. 22, 309–313 (2012). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of rspb.2014.1541; pmid: 25377450
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.050; pmid: 22264604 culture for child and chimpanzee. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 79. E. W. Menzel Jr., R. K. Davenport, C. M. Rogers, Protocultural
37. E. Danchin, L. A. Giraldeau, T. J. Valone, R. H. Wagner, 364, 2417–2428 (2009). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0069; aspects of chimpanzees’ responsiveness to novel objects.
Public information: From nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. pmid: 19620112 Folia Primatol. 17, 161–170 (1972). doi: 10.1159/000155425;
Science 305, 487–491 (2004). doi: 10.1126/science.1098254; 57. B. D. Worden, D. R. Papaj, Flower choice copying in pmid: 5031297
pmid: 15273386 bumblebees. Biol. Lett. 1, 504–507 (2005). doi: 10.1098/ 80. G. L. Vale, S. J. Davis, S. P. Lambeth, S. J. Schapiro, A. Whiten,
38. S. Alem et al., Associative mechanisms allow for social learning rsbl.2005.0368; pmid: 17148244 Acquisition of a socially learned tool use sequence in
and cultural transmission of string pulling in an insect. 58. A. Whiten, V. Horner, F. B. M. de Waal, Conformity to cultural chimpanzees: Implications for cumulative culture. Evol. Hum.
PLOS Biol. 14, e1002564 (2016). doi: 10.1371/journal. norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 437, 737–740 Behav. 38, 635–644 (2017). doi: 10.1016/j.
pbio.1002564; pmid: 27701411 (2005). doi: 10.1038/nature04047; pmid: 16113685 evolhumbehav.2017.04.007; pmid: 29333058
39. T. Mueller, R. B. O’Hara, S. J. Converse, R. P. Urbanek, 59. A. Whiten et al., Transmission of multiple traditions within and 81. A. M. Berdahl et al., Collective animal navigation and migratory
W. F. Fagan, Social learning of migratory performance. between chimpanzee groups. Curr. Biol. 17, 1038–1043 (2007). culture: From theoretical models to empirical evidence.
Science 341, 999–1002 (2013). doi: 10.1126/science.1237139; doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.031; pmid: 17555968 Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 373, 20170009 (2018). doi: 10.1098/
pmid: 23990559 60. C. Boesch et al., Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected rstb.2017.0009; pmid: 29581394
40. E. Danchin et al., Cultural flies: Conformist social learning in cultural diversity. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 910–916 (2020). 82. H. Whitehead, Cultural selection and genetic diversity in
fruitflies predicts long-lasting mate-choice traditions. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0890-1; pmid: 32451479 matrilineal whales. Science 282, 1708–1711 (1998).
Science 362, 1025–1030 (2018). doi: 10.1126/science.aat1590; 61. A. Whiten, Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by doi: 10.1126/science.282.5394.1708; pmid: 9831562
pmid: 30498121 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 112, 83. H. Whitehead, K. N. Laland, L. Rendell, R. Thorogood,
41. B. R. Jesmer et al., Is ungulate migration culturally 270–281 (1998). doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.112.3.270; A. Whiten, The reach of gene-culture coevolution in animals.
transmitted? Evidence of social learning from translocated pmid: 9770315 Nat. Commun. 10, 2405 (2019). doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-
animals. Science 361, 1023–1025 (2018). doi: 10.1126/science. 62. C. Tennie, J. Call, M. Tomasello, Ratcheting up the ratchet: On 10293-y; pmid: 31160560
aat0985; pmid: 30190405 the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 84. S. E. Street, A. F. Navarrete, S. M. Reader, K. N. Laland,
42. E. van de Waal, C. Borgeaud, A. Whiten, Potent social learning 364, 2405–2415 (2009). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0052; Coevolution of cultural intelligence, extended life history, sociality,
and conformity shape a wild primate’s foraging decisions. pmid: 19620111 and brain size in primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114,
Science 340, 483–485 (2013). doi: 10.1126/science.1232769; 63. J. Henrich, C. Tennie, “Cultural evolution in chimpanzees and 7908–7914 (2017). doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620734114;
pmid: 23620053 humans” in Chimpanzees and Human Evolution, M. N. Muller et al., pmid: 28739950
43. A. Thornton, K. McAuliffe, Teaching in wild meerkats. Eds. (Harvard Univ. Press, 2018), pp. 645–702. 85. R. Thorogood, H. Kokko, J. Mappes, Social transmission of
Science 313, 227–229 (2006). doi: 10.1126/science.1128727; 64. O. J. Loukola, C. J. Perry, L. Coscos, L. Chittka, Bumblebees avoidance among predators facilitates the spread of novel

Downloaded from https://www.science.org on September 06, 2021


pmid: 16840701 show cognitive flexibility by improving on an observed complex prey. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2, 254–261 (2018). doi: 10.1038/
44. J. A. Allen, Community through culture: From insects to behavior. Science 355, 833–836 (2017). doi: 10.1126/ s41559-017-0418-x; pmid: 29255302
whales. BioEssays 41, e1900060 (2019). doi: 10.1002/ science.aag2360; pmid: 28232576 86. A. M. Kopps et al., Cultural transmission of tool use combined
bies.201900060; pmid: 31631360 65. A. Whiten, Conformity and over-imitation: An integrative review of with habitat specializations leads to fine-scale genetic
45. L. M. Aplin, Culture and cultural evolution in birds: A review of variant forms of hyper-reliance on social learning. Adv. Stud. structure in bottlenose dolphins. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20133245
the evidence. Anim. Behav. 147, 179–187 (2019). doi: 10.1016/ Behav. 51, 31–75 (2019). doi: 10.1016/bs.asb.2018.12.003 (2014). doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.3245; pmid: 24648223
j.anbehav.2018.05.001 66. W. J. Hoppitt et al., Lessons from animal teaching. Trends Ecol. 87. H. Whitehead, F. Vachon, T. R. Frasier, Cultural hitchhiking in
46. A. Whiten, E. van de Waal, The pervasive role of social learning in Evol. 23, 486–493 (2008). doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.05.008; the matrilineal whales. Behav. Genet. 47, 324–334 (2017).
primate lifetime development. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72, 80 pmid: 18657877 doi: 10.1007/s10519-017-9840-8; pmid: 28275880
(2018). doi: 10.1007/s00265-018-2489-3; pmid: 29755181 67. A. Thornton, N. J. Raihani, The evolution of teaching. 88. P. Brakes et al., Animal cultures matter for conservation.
47. I. Agostini, E. Visalberghi, Social influences on the acquisition Anim. Behav. 75, 1823–1836 (2008). doi: 10.1016/ Science 363, 1032–1034 (2019). doi: 10.1126/
of sex-typical foraging patterns by juveniles in a group of j.anbehav.2007.12.014 science.aaw3557; pmid: 30808816
wild tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus nigritus). Am. J. 68. A. Whiten, The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and 89. H. S. Kühl et al., Human impact erodes chimpanzee behavioral
Primatol. 65, 335–351 (2005). doi: 10.1002/ajp.20120; ancestral apes. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 997–1007 (2011). diversity. Science 363, 1453–1455 (2019). doi: 10.1126/science.
pmid: 15834890 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0334; pmid: 21357222 aau4532; pmid: 30846610
48. C. Schuppli, C. P. van Schaik, Animal cultures: How we’ve only 69. C. P. van Schaik, The Primate Origins of Human Nature (Wiley 90. T. Gruber et al., Cultural change in animals: A flexible
seen the tip of the iceberg. Evol. Hum. Sci. 1, e2 (2019). Blackwell, 2016). behavioural adaptation to human disturbance. Palgrave
doi: 10.1017/ehs.2019.1 70. M. Tomasello, Becoming Human (Belknap Press, 2019). Commun. 5, 64 (2019). doi: 10.1057/s41599-019-0271-4
49. A. Whiten, R. Ham, On the nature and evolution of imitation in 71. M. D. Gurven, R. J. Davison, T. S. Kraft, The optimal timing of 91. V. Horner, A. Whiten, E. Flynn, F. B. M. de Waal, Faithful
the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. teaching and learning across the life course. Philos. Trans. R. replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission
Adv. Stud. Behav. 21, 239–283 (1992). doi: 10.1016/ Soc. B 375, 20190500 (2020). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0500; chains by chimpanzees and children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
S0065-3454(08)60146-1 pmid: 32475325 103, 13878–13883 (2006). doi: 10.1073/pnas.0606015103;
50. R. L. Kendal et al., Social learning strategies: Bridge-building 72. A. Whiten, Cultural evolution in animals. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. pmid: 16938863
between fields. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 651–665 (2018). 50, 27–48 (2019). doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110218-
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.003; pmid: 29759889 025040 AC KNOWLED GME NTS
51. C. P. van Schaik, J. M. Burkart, Social learning and evolution: 73. R. F. Lachlan, A. Whiten, “Cultural evolution in non-human For comments on draft manuscripts, I thank L. Aplin, L. Rendell,
The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B animals” in Oxford Bibliographies in Evolutionary Biology, R. Thorogood, C. Schuppli, C. van Schaik, and H. Whitehead.
366, 1008–1016 (2011). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0304; D. Futuyma, Ed. (Oxford Univ. Press, 2020). Competing interests: None declared.
pmid: 21357223 74. D. J. Futuyma, Evolution (Sinauer Associates, ed. 3, 2013).
52. A. Whiten, E. van de Waal, Social learning, culture and the 75. J. Mercader et al., 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the
‘socio-cultural brain’ of human and non-human primates. origins of percussive stone technology. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 10.1126/science.abe6514

Whiten, Science 372, eabe6514 (2021) 2 April 2021 7 of 7


The burgeoning reach of animal culture
Andrew Whiten

Science, 372 (6537), eabe6514.

We are not alone


Before the mid-20th century, it was generally assumed that culture, behavior learned from others, was specific to
humans. However, starting with identification in a few species, evidence that animals can learn and transmit behaviors
has accumulated at an ever-increasing pace. Today, there is no doubt that culture is widespread among animal
species, both vertebrates and invertebrates, marine and terrestrial. Whiten reviews evidence for animal culture and
elaborates on the wide array of forms that such culture takes. Recognizing that other species have complex and varied
culture has implications for conservation and welfare and for understanding the evolution of this essential component
of animal societies, including our own.
Science, this issue p. eabe6514

View the article online

Downloaded from https://www.science.org on September 06, 2021


https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.abe6514
Permissions
https://www.science.org/help/reprints-and-permissions

Use of think article is subject to the Terms of service

Science (ISSN 1095-9203) is published by the American Association for the Advancement of Science. 1200 New York Avenue NW,
Washington, DC 20005. The title Science is a registered trademark of AAAS.
Copyright © 2021 The Authors, some rights reserved; exclusive licensee American Association for the Advancement of Science. No claim
to original U.S. Government Works

You might also like