You are on page 1of 11

Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

A longitudinal study of relationships between three burnout dimensions


among secondary school teachers

André Brouwers
The Open University, Heerlen, The Netherlands
Welko Tomic
The Open University, Heerlen, The Netherlands

Abstract negative self-evaluation in relation to his or her job


This study examined the direction and time-frame of the performances (Maslach & Leiter, 1997; Maslach,
relationships between three burnout dimensions among 1993; Schaufeli, Maslach, & Marek, 1993).
243 secondary school teachers. A review of the literature We observe that researchers in various domains
on this topic revealed six longitudinal studies (Van like anthropology, economics and business,
Dierendonck et al., 1997; Leiter, 1990; Lee & Ashforth,
1993; Taris et al., 2005; Te Brake et al., 2008; Parker &
education, medicine, psychology, and social work,
Salmela-Aro, 2011). Structural equation modeling continue to conduct research into the various
analyses indicated that emotional exhaustion had a aspects of burnout worldwide, aiming at finding
synchronous effect on depersonalization as well as on relationships, patterns, and applications for both the
personal accomplishment, while personal personal and professional issues that affect
accomplishment had a synchronous effect on personnel in a variety of professions and the
depersonalization. These findings offer partial sequence of three dimensions of burnout among
confirmation of the sequence of burnout dimensions different occupational groups (see for instance
proposed by Leiter and Maslach (1988), Taris et al. Alarcon, Edwards, & Menke, 2011; Steyn, 2010;
(2005), and Parker and Salmela-Aro (2011), but no
evidence was found in favor of the Golembiewski et al.
Lei, Youn Hee, & Dong, 2010; Chang, 2009;
(1986) model. It was concluded that school principals Vercambre, Brosselin, Gilbert, Nerrière, & Kovess-
and psychologists who want to prevent and treat teacher Masféty, 2009).
burnout at an early stage, before it becomes chronic, In the course of time, different process models
must be sensitive to the warning signs of emotional have been proposed to describe the relationships
exhaustion. between the three burnout dimensions.
Golembiewski, Munzenrider, and Stevenson (1986)
Keywords: burnout; longitudinal studies; secondary state that burnout progresses from
school teachers; structural equation modeling
depersonalization  reduced personal
accomplishment  emotional exhaustion.
Depersonalization arises when professional
Almost forty years ago Freudenberger (1974) detachment in dealing with others is no longer
coined the term “burnout” after he had noticed that functional and reducing the sufferer’s ability to
tired, discouraged people became sloppy and form necessary relationships with others and to
stopped making time for meaningful discussions perform well on the job. As a consequence, the
with their peers. They also experienced a loss of self-evaluation of job accomplishment becomes
self-confidence, and a sense of boredom. Maslach, negative, the ability to cope with job demands
Schaufeli, and Leiter (2001) took the view that declines, and feelings of being overextended are
burnout is an erosion of engagement: “What started likely to arise.
out as important, meaningful, and challenging work Another process model describing the sequence
becomes unpleasant, unfulfilling, and meaningless” of the burnout dimensions is the Leiter and
(p. 416). As a result, teachers may feel burned out
progresses from emotional exhaustion 
Maslach (1988) model. They state that burnout
when exhaustion replaces feeling energized,
cynicism replaces being involved, and depersonalization  reduced personal
ineffectiveness replaces feeling efficacious. accomplishment. In their view, feelings of being
Burnout is considered to be a psychological overextended arise initially in response to a work
syndrome consisting of the dimensions emotional environment that is demanding in a social-
exhaustion, depersonalization, and reduced psychological sense. With regard to teachers, this
personal accomplishment (Maslach & Jackson, could include factors such as heavy workloads,
1981). Emotional exhaustion refers to feelings of high role conflict and ambiguity, inadequate
being overextended, both emotionally and resources, large class size, extensive administrative
physically. Depersonalization is described as a bureaucracy, low autonomy, low collegiality, and
cynical, cold, and distant attitude to other people, disruptive and violent student behaviour (Chen &
who are usually the recipients of one’s services. Miller, 1997). The latter are particularly stressful
Reduced personal achievement refers to a person’s

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 23


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

problems (Brouwers & Tomic, 1999). To cope with of the two above-mentioned sequence concepts, the
exhaustion and stress, the overextended individual Leiter and Maslach (1988) model. (4) The findings
distances him or herself from others and becomes of the Taris, Le Blanc, Schaufeli, and Schreurs
cynical and cold in his or her responses toward study (2005) supported the prediction that higher
them. Once depersonalization occurs, the levels of exhaustion bring about higher levels of
individual may begin to feel ineffective on the job, depersonalization. In addition, indications were
and the self-evaluation of his or her found for an association between higher levels of
accomplishments is likely to become negative. depersonalization with lower levels of personal
Several studies have examined the sequence of accomplishment. Finally, higher levels of
three burnout dimensions. In their cross-sectional depersonalization are associated with higher levels
studies, Golembiewski and his colleagues found of exhaustion across time. (5) Te Brake, Smits,
evidence for their proposed sequence Wicherts, Gorter, and Hoogstraten (2008)
(Golembiewski & Munzenrider, 1993; examined the chronological sequence of the three
Golembiewski & Munzenrider, 1990; dimensions of burnout using a two-wave
Golembiewski & Roundtree, 1986). A few studies longitudinal design. In addition to the test of their a
performed by others also reported results consistent priori models, they constructed a model that best
with the so-called progressive phase model fitted their study data. This model indicated
constructed by Golembiewski and his colleagues emotional exhaustion to precede the development
(Gryskiewicz & Buttner, 1992; Scherer, Cox, Key, of depersonalization and personal accomplishment
& Stickney, 1992; Burke, 1989). However, these independently. The results showed that emotional
studies have been severely criticized on exhaustion should not be discarded as an early sign
methodological grounds. Leiter (1989) stated that of burnout. (6) Parker and Salmela-Aro (2011)
dichotomizing continuous scales, which is a compared and contrasted several major models in
common procedure when testing Golembiewski’s order to identify which model best represented the
model, leads to a loss of information, and development of school burnout in a four-wave
consequently to a less sensitive analysis. In longitudinal sample of high school students. The
addition, he showed by reanalyzing results suggest that Taris et al.'s (2005) model
Golembiewski’s data that the vast majority of provided a significantly better fit to the data than
significant differences between the phases of other tested models. This could indicate that similar
burnout could be explained by levels of emotional developmental processes underlie both school and
exhaustion, thereby subsuming the explanatory occupational burnout.
power of the other two dimensions of burnout, i.e., However, since all possible synchronous and
depersonalization and personal accomplishment. longitudinal effects in both directions were not
In other cross-sectional studies, evidence was systematically tested in all of the studies mentioned
found in favour of the sequence of burnout above, it remains unclear whether their final model
dimensions proposed by Leiter and Maslach (1988; was the best fit among other possible models (Zapf,
Cordes, Dougherty, & Blum, 1997; Greenglass, Dormann & Frese, 1996). More longitudinal
Burke, & Konarski, 1997; Byrne, 1994), i.e., research including tests of reciprocal effects is
typically elevation on each dimension proceeds in a evidently necessary. For instance, it is possible that
sequence. However, cross-sectional studies cannot emotional exhaustion might affect personal
solve the question once and for all. accomplishment and the latter might affect
Due to cross-sectional design limitations, exhaustion as well. Consequently, knowledge about
longitudinal studies have been conducted to test the causal relationships of hypotheses is needed.
process models to describe the relationships In studying burnout, discovering the correct
between the burnout dimensions. Of the six developmental model is important for two reasons.
significant longitudinal studies on this topic, (1) First, knowing the chronological sequence will
Van Dierendonck, Schaufeli, and Buunk (1997) facilitate early recognition of burnout, which is
found that emotional exhaustion affects personal essential to prevent and treat the syndrome at an
accomplishment, which in turn influences early stage. Second, it will help to develop a
depersonalization, while depersonalization in turn process model which is validated empirically and
influences emotional exhaustion. They stated that which will allow an understanding of the
these results did not provide conclusive evidence of antecedents and consequences of burnout (Van
either the ideas of Leiter and Maslach (1988) or Dierendonck et al., 1997).
those of Golembiewski et al. (1986). (2) Leiter The present study examined the direction and
(1990) showed that the only significant reciprocal time-frame (five months synchronous or
longitudinal effect across a six month time interval longitudinal) of the relationships between three
between the burnout dimensions was that of burnout dimensions among secondary school
personal accomplishment and emotional teachers longitudinally. Such a study is important
exhaustion. (3) The study conducted by Lee and because (1) previous research has not produced
Ashforth (1993) provided evidence in favour of one final conclusions about the sequence of the burnout

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 24


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

dimensions, and (2) it is reasonable to assume that Analysis revealed no significant differences
the sequence of the dimensions will vary among between Time 1 and Time 2 participants on the
different occupations. For instance, the emotional measures, including sex [χ2 (1) = .34, p > .05], age
and social-psychological demands involved in [t(242) = 1.27, p > .05], years of teaching
teaching students are in some respects completely experience [t(242) = 1.28, p > .05], emotional
different from the demands of caring for patients. exhaustion [t(242) = .18, p > .05],
Clearly, the professional detachment which can be depersonalization [t(242) = .00, p > .05], and
functional in caring for patients differs from the personal accomplishment [t(242) = .71, p > .05].
attitude required when dealing with students. It is
therefore reasonable to assume that the point at Measures
which functional detachment becomes Burnout was measured at Time 1 and Time 2
dysfunctional (Golembiewski et al., 1986) may using the Dutch version of the Maslach Burnout
differ from one occupation to the next. Inventory for teachers (MBI-NL-Ed; Schaufeli &
Contrary to growing interest in burnout in a Van Horn, 1995; Schaufeli, Daamen, & Van
wide range of occupations (Ahola, Honkonen, Mierlo, 1994; Maslach & Jackson, 1981). The
Virtane, Aromaa, & Lonnqvist, 2008) burnout has questionnaire includes 20 items divided into three
been studied above all principally in human service subscales: Emotional Exhaustion (EE; 8 items,
professions. In educational settings, interest is maximum score 48), Depersonalization (D; 5 items,
increasing because the teaching profession has maximum score 30), and Personal Accomplishment
noticeably evolved over recent years (Loonstra, (PA; 7 items, maximum score 42). The items were
Brouwers, & Tomic, 2009; Evers, Brouwers, & measured on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from
Tomic, 2002; Tomic & Tomic, 2008). never to every day. Scores on the scales are added
separately. High scores on the scales EE and D, and
Method low scores on the PA scale are indicative of
Participants burnout. Examples of items are: “I feel emotionally
Participants were teachers working in secondary drained from my work” (EE), “I feel burned out
schools in the province of Limburg in the from my work” (EE), “I’ve become more callous
Netherlands. Schools were randomly selected to toward people since I took this job” (D), “I feel
participate in the current study. Time 1 respondents students blame me for some of their problems” (D),
(N = 558) were those who participated during the “I feel exhilarated after working closely with my
first wave of data collection and who had recipients” (PA), and “I have accomplished many
completed the questionnaires. They represented worthwhile things in this job” (PA). In a study
53% of 1056 teachers asked to participate. Time 2 among secondary school teachers (N = 916),
respondents (N = 243) were teachers from the Time Schaufeli and Van Horn (1995) found Cronbach’s
1 sample who participated again at Time 2 and who Alphas of .87, .71 and .78 for the three subscales,
had also completed the questionnaires. The respectively. The three-factor structure of the
measurement points at Time 1 and Time 2 were Dutch version of the Maslach Burnout Inventory
October 2010 and March 2011, respectively, an for teachers is confirmed with confirmatory factor
interval of five months. The reason we opted for a analysis (Schaufeli, Daamen, & Van Mierlo, 1994).
five-month time interval was entirely practical. We
deliberately avoided measurement administration in Procedure
peak periods in which teachers are busier than The principals of 15 schools were asked to
usual, for instance examination periods. We cooperate in the study and were mailed and asked
expected that the two above-mentioned to hand out a questionnaire to every teacher in their
measurement points (October and March) would school along with a letter explaining the nature and
probably yield the highest response. Time 2 general aim of the study. All letters indicated that
respondents consisted of 179 male (74%) and 64 participation was voluntary, responses were
female (26%) teachers. The average age was 46.29 confidential, and data would be anonymized. At
years (SD = 8.20) with a range of 24 to 63 years. both measurement times, follow-up mail-outs were
The average teaching experience in years was used to increase the return rate. No incentive was
21.25 (SD = 8.92) with a range of 1 to 39 years. A provided for participation.
comparison with all teachers working in secondary This study has been performed among teachers
schools in the province of Limburg in 2009 working in general secondary education. The Dutch
(Central Finance Institute, 2009) showed that the system has three tracks of education, each aimed at
sample of the present study was representative in a different academic level. After primary school,
terms of sex [χ2 (1) = .97, p = .33], but not in terms students – approximately 12 years of age – can
of age [t(357) = 2.19, p = .01]. Compared with the pursue prevocational secondary education, which is
target group of all teachers working in secondary usually an introduction to middle vocational
schools in the province of Limburg, the teachers in education. This branch takes four years to complete
the sample were slightly older. (from 12 to 16 years). The second track known as

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 25


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

senior general secondary education lasts five years To examine the relationships between the three
and qualifies students to enter higher professional burnout dimensions, a three-step procedure was
education (ages 12 to 17). Pre-university education followed (Lee & Ashforth, 1993). In step 1, two
is the third track and lasts six years and prepares synchronous models named synchronous type 1
students for academic studies at university level models, were compared with the stability model to
(ages 12 to 18). reveal whether the synchronous paths were
Teachers in secondary education must be significant. In the stability model only the stability
certified. Grade one teacher qualification covers all paths were unconstrained, while the other paths
classes of university preparatory education and were fixed at zero (Maruyama, 1998). In
senior general secondary education. synchronous type 1 model, the stability paths
A master’s degree in the field of teaching and [arrows a & b] and the synchronous paths at Time 1
extra teacher training is also required. Second- and 2 which move in the same direction were

 Time 1 depersonalization [d], and Time 2


grade qualification is limited to the first three unconstrained (e.g., Time 1 emotional exhaustion

emotional exhaustion  Time 2 depersonalization


school years of secondary education. These
teachers need to have a degree of the School of
Education which involves four years of training at [g]) while the other synchronous paths as well as
higher professional education level. the lagged paths were fixed at zero [e, f, c, h].
In step 2, two synchronous models with
Analysis equality constraints - named synchronous type 2
A structural equation modeling (SEM) models - were compared with the best-fitting
procedure with maximum likelihood estimation synchronous type 1 model to reveal whether the
utilizing the AMOS computer program was synchronous paths met the stationarity assumption
employed to determine the most likely direction (James, Mulaik, & Brett, 1982). In synchronous
and time-frame (five months longitudinal or type 2 model, the stability paths were
synchronous)of the relationships between burnout unconstrained [a & b] and the synchronous paths at
dimensions. In testing longitudinal relationships, Time 1 and 2 which move in the same direction
SEM procedures have several advantages with were constrained to be equal (e.g., Time 1
respect to other analytical procedures such as the emotional exhaustion  Time 1 depersonalization
cross-lagged panel correlation technique and [d] was set to be equal to Time 2 emotional
hierarchical regression analyses. The structural exhaustion  Time 2 depersonalization [g]); the
equation modeling procedure provides tests that other synchronous paths as well as the lagged paths
allow for directional conclusions and can include were fixed at zero [e, f, c, h].
reciprocal relationships between variables (Martin,
2011; Zapf et al., 1996; Kessler & Greenberg,
1981).

a
Emotional Exhaustion Emotional Exhaustion
Time 1 Time 2

d c e f g h

Depersonalization b Depersonalization
Time 1 Time 2

Figure 1. Model for testing longitudinal relationships between emotional exhaustion and depersonalization

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 26


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

In step 3, two longitudinal models were other synchronous type 1 model, as reflected in the
compared with the best-fitting synchronous type 2 values on the fit indexes (e.g., TLI is 1.00 and .76,
model to reveal whether the lagged paths were respectively). The step 2 results indicated that the
significant. Then the best-fitting longitudinal model synchronous type 2 models were significantly
was compared with the best-fitting synchronous inferior to the best-fit synchronous type 1 model, as
model to reveal which model fitted the data best. In reflected in the chi-square difference tests [Δχ2 (1)
longitudinal models the stability paths [a & b] and = 15.44, p < .01 and Δχ2 (1) = 55.37, p < .01]. The

Time 1 emotional exhaustion  Time 2


one of the lagged paths were unconstrained (e.g., step 3 results indicated that only the longitudinal
model with the unconstrained lagged path of
depersonalization [e]), while the synchronous paths emotional exhaustion to depersonalization was
at Time 1 and 2 move into the same direction as the significantly superior to the best-fit synchronous
unconstrained lagged path were constrained to be type 2 model, as reflected in the chi-square
equal [d, g]. The other lagged path and the other difference test [Δχ2 (1) = 9.50, p < .01]. However,
synchronous paths were fixed at zero [f, c, h]. the synchronous type 1 model with the
Chi-square difference tests were used to unconstrained synchronous paths of emotional
examine the significance of unconstrained paths exhaustion to depersonalization fitted the data
(Bentler & Bonett, 1980). Adjusted Goodness of better than the best-fit longitudinal model, as
Fit Index (AGFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), and reflected in the values on the fit indexes (e.g., TLI
Comparative Fit Index (CFI) were used to examine is 1.00 and .92, respectively). So, the results
which of the models fit the data best. To estimate indicated that the synchronous type 1 model with
the CFI and TLI, the stability model served as the unconstrained synchronous paths of emotional
baseline. TLI and CFI are said to be relatively exhaustion to depersonalization fitted the data best.
robust to sample size bias and to have taken model For the relationship between depersonalization
simplicity and chi-square values into consideration and personal accomplishment, the step 1 results
(McDonald & March, 1990; Bentler, 1990). The indicated that the synchronous type 1 models were
model that performed significantly better than the significantly superior to the stability model, as
comparison model, as reflected in the chi- reflected in the chi-square difference tests [Δχ2 (2)
difference tests, and that showed the largest value = 120.20, p < .01 and Δχ2 (2) = 125.04, p < .01].
on TLI and CFI was considered to fit the data best. However, the synchronous type 1 model with the
unconstrained paths of personal accomplishment to
Results depersonalization fitted the data better than the
Table 1 presents means, standard deviations, other synchronous type 1 model, as reflected in the
internal consistency measures (i.e., Cronbach’s values on the fit indexes (e.g., TLI is .81 and .74,
Alpha), and intercorrelations of variables. Table 2 respectively). The step 2 results indicated that the
presents the fitness of the models to examine the chi-squares of the synchronous type 2 models were
synchronous and longitudinal relationships between higher than chi-square of the best-fit synchronous
the three burnout dimensions. type 1 model. The difference was significant for the
For the relationship between emotional synchronous type 2 model with equally constrained
exhaustion and depersonalization, the step 1 results paths of depersonalization to personal
indicated that the synchronous type 1 models were accomplishment [Δχ2 (1) = 10.46, p < .01] and not
significantly superior to the stability model, as significant for the other synchronous type 2 model
reflected in the chi-square difference tests [Δχ2 (2) [Δχ2 (1) = 2.32, p = .13]. This means that the best-
= 154.81, p < .01 and Δχ2 (2)= 136.89, p < .01]. fit synchronous type 2 model is not significantly
However, the synchronous type 1 model with the inferior to the best-fit synchronous type 1 model.
unconstrained paths of emotional exhaustion to The step 3 results indicated that only the chi-square
depersonalization fitted the data better than the of the longitudinal model with the unconstrained
Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, Internal Consistency Measures, and Intercorrelations Between Variables
Scale Intercorrelations
Variable M SD α 1 2 3 4 5
Time 1
1. Emotional Exhaustion 16.30 9.43 .91 -
2. Depersonalization 5.71 4.18 .72 .61 -
3. Personal Accomplishment 27.01 6.64 .86 -.45 -.50 -

Time 2
4. Emotional Exhaustion 16.83 9.51 .92 .84 .55 -.41 -
5. Depersonalization 6.50 4.13 .71 .58 .69 -.52 .61 -
6. Personal Accomplishment 27.29 6.48 .86 -.53 -.51 .75 -.53 -.63
Note. N = 243

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 27


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

lagged path of personal accomplishment to For the relationship between emotional


depersonalization was lower than chi-square of the exhaustion and personal accomplishment, the step
best-fit synchronous type 2 model, but the 1 results indicated that the synchronous type 1
difference was not significant [Δχ2 (1) = 1.60, p = models were significantly superior to the stability
.20]. The longitudinal model with the model, as reflected in the chi-square difference
unconstrained lagged path of personal tests [Δχ2 (2) = 177.16, p < .01 and Δχ2 (2) =
accomplishment to depersonalization fitted the data 166.21, p < .01]. However, the synchronous type 1
better than the other longitudinal model, as model with the unconstrained paths of emotional
reflected in the values on the fit indexes (e.g., TLI exhaustion to personal accomplishment fitted the
is .80 and .68, respectively). However, the best-fit data better than the other synchronous type 1
model is the most parsimonious model which is not model, as reflected in the values on the fit indexes
significantly inferior to another model. The (e.g., TLI is 1.00 and .41, respectively). The step 2
synchronous type 2 models were the most results indicated that the synchronous type 2
parsimonious models after the stability model. So, models were significantly inferior to the best-fit
the results indicated that the synchronous model synchronous type 1 model, as reflected in the chi-
with equality constrained paths of personal square difference tests [Δχ2 (1) = 7.84, p < .01 and
accomplishment to depersonalization fitted the data Δχ2 (1) = 46.97, p < .01]. The step 3 results
best. indicated that only the chi-square of the

Table 2
Chi-squares, Chi-square Differences, and Fit Indexes of the Structural Equation Models
χ2 df Δχ2 p AGFI TLI CFI
Emotional Exhaustion (EE) ↔ Depersonalization (DP)
Null Model 603.83 6 .07
Stability Model 156.86 4 446.97 .001 .50 .62 .74
Synchronous Type 1 Models
EE → DP 2.05 2 154.81 .001 .98 1.00 1.00
DP → EE 19.97 2 136.89 .001 .81 .76 .88
Synchronous Type 2 Models
EE → DP 17.49 3 139.37 .001 .90 .87 .91
DP → EE 57.42 3 99.44 .001 .70 .53 .64
Longitudinal Models
EE → DP 7.99 2 148.87 .001 .92 .92 .96
DP → EE 45.13 2 111.73 .001 .60 .44 .72

Depersonalization (DP) ↔ Personal Accomplishment (PA)


Null Model 497.00 6 .12
Stability Model 139.67 4 357.33 .001 .54 .59 .72
Synchronous Type 1 Models
DP → PA 19.47 2 120.20 .001 .81 .74 .87
PA → DP 14.63 2 125.04 .001 .86 .81 .90
Synchronous Type 2 Models
DP → PA 25.09 3 114.58 .001 .85 .78 .84
PA → DP 16.95 3 122.72 .001 .89 .86 .90
Longitudinal Models
DP → PA 23.67 2 116.00 .001 .78 .68 .84
PA → DP 15.35 2 124.32 .001 .85 .80 .90

Emotional Exhaustion (EE) ↔ Personal Accomplishment (PA)


Null Model 584.21 6 .13
Stability Model 90.67 4 409.75 .001 .66 .77 .85
Synchronous Type 1 Models
EE → PA 1.91 2 177.16 .001 .98 1.00 1.00
PA → EE 27.64 2 166.21 .001 .74 .41 .70
Synchronous Type 2 Models
EE → PA 9.75 3 167.31 .001 .94 .90 .92
PA → EE 48.88 3 155.82 .001 .72 .29 .47
Longitudinal Models
EE → PA 8.71 2 174.19 .001 .91 .85 .92
PA → EE 38.64 2 166.90 .001 .66 .15 .59

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 28


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

longitudinal model with the unconstrained lagged exhaustion and depersonalization differs from one
path of emotional exhaustion to personal occupational group to the next.
accomplishment was lower than chi-square of the The direction of the relationship between
best-fit synchronous type 2 model, but the depersonalization and personal accomplishment
difference was not significant [Δχ2 (1) = 1.04, p = showed the latter affecting the former. The same
.31]. So, the results indicated that the synchronous result was found by Van Dierendonck et al. (1997).
type 1 model with the unconstrained synchronous These results suggest that neither the model
paths of emotional exhaustion to personal constructed by Leiter and Maslach (1988) nor the
accomplishment fitted the data best. model devised by Golembiewski et al. (1986)
suffice on this point. A possible explanation for the
Discussion influence of personal accomplishment on
The present study examined the direction and depersonalization could be that the subjects
time-frame (five months longitudinal or attributed their own negative self-evaluation of
synchronous) of the relationships between the three their job performances to their clients, patients, or
burnout dimensions among secondary school students. Individuals can develop a cold and distant
teachers. Although teacher burnout has been attitude toward their clients and students, a reaction
extensively studied, only a small number of studies to blaming them for their own poor job
used a longitudinal design. In addition, few studies performance.
have taken a close look at how the burnout The direction of the relationship between
dimensions develop. The results partly confirm the emotional exhaustion and personal accomplishment
sequence of burnout dimensions proposed by Leiter showed the former affecting the latter. This result is
and Maslach (1988), and the empirical findings of partly in accordance with the Leiter and Maslach
Taris et al. (2005), Te Brake et al. (2008), and (1988) model, in which emotional exhaustion
Parker and Salmela-Aro (2011), but no evidence influences personal accomplishment mediated
was found in favour of the Golembiewski et al. through depersonalization. Leiter (1989) modified
(1986) model. his model by adding a direct effect of emotional
In accordance with studies of Taris et al (2005), exhaustion on personal accomplishment. Evidence
Te Brake et al. (2008), and Parker and Salmela-Aro in favour of this effect was found in cross-sectional
(2011), the direction of the relationship between studies involving human resource employees
emotional exhaustion and depersonalization (Cordes et al. 1997) and elementary school teachers
showed the former affecting the latter. This result (Byrne, 1994). Van Dierendonck et al. (1997)
confirms the Leiter and Maslach (1988) proposition found in their longitudinal study that emotional
that emotional exhaustion precedes exhaustion influenced personal accomplishment as
depersonalization. Leiter and Maslach (1988) stated well. However, Leiter’s (1990) longitudinal study
that depersonalization is a coping response by involving mental health workers produced evidence
which individuals may distance themselves in favour of the reverse relationship. By
psychologically to stem the demands on their reanalyzing Leiter’s data, Van Dierendonck et al.
emotional reserves. The callous, cynical, or (1997) showed also that the model in which
detached way in which they deal with the situation personal accomplishment influences emotional
and the individuals with whom they interact exhaustion fitted the data best. It is conceivable that
provides a type of emotional buffer (Cordes et al., the differences in the participants’ work
1997). Emotional exhaustion was found to affect environment, which stemmed directly from the
depersonalization in studies involving human type of occupations they practiced, were
resource employees (Cordes et al., 1997), responsible for the different outcomes of the
supervisors and managers (Lee & Ashforth, 1993), studies.
and teachers (Greenglass et al., 1997; Byrne, 1994). In summary, the direction of the relationships
These studies were cross-sectional, except for the between the dimensions of burnout as found in the
longitudinal study conducted by Lee and Ashforth present study supported the following sequence
(1993). One of the shortcomings of the latter study, proposition for the teaching profession: emotional
however, is that it did not systematically test the exhaustion arises first (for instance, as a
relationships between the dimensions of burnout in consequence of working in a demanding
both directions. By reanalyzing Lee and Ashforth’s environment), followed by a parallel development
(1993) data, Van Dierendonck et al. (1997) showed of personal accomplishment and depersonalization,
that the reverse model, in which depersonalization wherein depersonalization is influenced by
had an influence on emotional exhaustion, fitted the personal accomplishment as well. This proposed
data best. Their own longitudinal study involving sequence is, broadly speaking, in accordance with
nurses, orderlies, physicians, therapists, and the ideas of Schaufeli and Van Dierendonck
physiotherapists showed the same result (Van (1993), who hypothesize in general that (1) burnout
Dierendonck et al. 1996). It is conceivable that the can be represented as a two-dimensional construct
direction of the relationship between emotional consisting of emotional exhaustion and negative

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 29


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

attitudes, and (2) emotional exhaustion precedes in addition, the sample of respondents was
negative attitudes (Van Dierendonck, Schaufeli, & representative with respect to sex. Third, there were
Sixma, 1994). The construct of negative attitudes no significant differences between Time 1 and
consists of the depersonalization and the personal Time 2 participants on the measures, including sex,
accomplishment dimensions of burnout. In a cross- years of teaching experience, and burnout. Fourth,
sectional study among general practitioners Van the current study examined the chronological
Dierendonck et al. (1994) found evidence in favour sequence of the three burnout dimensions using a
of the hypothesized direction of the relationships two-wave longitudinal design. Fifth, a strong
between the constructs. method of analysis (SEM) was applied which is
The time-frame of the relationships between the particularly suited to the analysis of longitudinal
burnout dimensions are all synchronous. This data (Tomarken & Waller 2005). Sixth, a
means that the burnout dimensions influenced each theoretical framework was adapted that may help to
other within a brief time-frame. These findings are organize research findings across investigations.
in accordance with those produced by Van In addition, the present study makes a number
Dierendonck et al. (1997), who interpreted them as of major practical contributions. First, there have
an indication of the interconnectedness of the been few longitudinal studies examining the
burnout dimensions. relationships between burnout dimensions and
Several limitations may have influenced the systematically testing all possible synchronous and
results of the current study. First, the variables were longitudinal effects in both directions. Furthermore,
measured at only two points in time. Given the the relationships between dimensions of burnout
synchronous effects found in the present study, have never been studied longitudinally in the
proposing a more definitive sequence between the teaching profession. Because the present study’s
burnout measures will require future studies to findings regarding the direction of the relationship
measure burnout: (1) with a shorter time-lag between emotional exhaustion and
between the measurement points than five months, depersonalization differ from those produced by
and (2) at more than two points in time. others (e.g., Van Dierendonck et al., 1997; Leiter,
Second, burnout was measured by a self-report 1990), the present study shows the importance of
questionnaire. This implies that common method examining these relationships among diverse
variance and consistency effects may bias the occupational groups. The findings of the current
relationships between the dimensions (Doty & study extend our knowledge about the
Glick, 1998). Unfortunately there is, as yet, no chronological sequence of the three dimensions of
burnout measure which is less sensitive in that burnout.
regard (Schaufeli, Enzmann, & Girault, 1993). It is Second, the present study showed that
recommended that future research should emotional exhaustion is the first indicator in the
supplement self-report measures with data from development of burnout among secondary school
alternate sources, such as colleagues, family teachers. The results of the present study can
members, students, and supervisors (Wanous & facilitate early recognition of the gradual erosion
Colella, 1989). caused by burnout, making it possible to prevent
A third impediment is the attrition rate, a quite and treat burnout at an early stage before it
common phenomenon in longitudinal research becomes chronic (Van Dierendonck et al., 1997).
(Plewis, 1985; Reynolds, Frank, & Kathy, 2005; It was concluded that teachers who develop
Taris, 2000). A relatively large number of burnout first become exhausted emotionally. They
participants dropped out between time one then develop a negative attitude towards their
measurement (N = 558) and time two (N = 243). students as well as towards themselves in relation
Similar drop-out rates were reported in Baer’s to their own work performance. School principals
(2003) meta-analysis, Lei & Wu (2007), and Te and psychologists who aim to detect teacher
Brake et al. (2008). However, the attrition analyses burnout at an early stage must know the warning
revealed that the samples did not significantly signs of emotional exhaustion. From the current
differ in terms of demographic data or in any study we can conclude that early signs of emotional
burnout dimension, so the internal validity of the exhaustion should be taken as an early warning for
findings was not compromised. teacher burnout risk.
Despite its limitations, the current study has The present study extends our knowledge about
some important strengths. First, the study employed the sequence of the burnout dimensions among
an established and validated instrument with known teachers. The teaching profession is full of potential
psychometric properties, i.e., the three dimensions threats with regard to physical and mental health
of the Maslach Burnout Inventory, allowing for the (see, for example, Chen & Miller, 1997).
comparison of findings across studies. Reliability Consequently, early recognition of this gradually
analysis shows that the measurements satisfy developing process is of great importance. An
psychometric standards (Nunnally & Bernstein, essential component of teacher-directed preventive
1994). Second, the sample size was substantial and, health care is to gain a clear understanding of the

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 30


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

mechanisms that form part of the burnout process Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18(6),
among this occupational group. However, 685-701.
speculations about the practical relevance of the Doty, D. H., & Glick, W. H. (1998). Common
study are premature. More research is needed to methods bias: Does common methods variance
understand the sequence of three dimensions of really bias results? Organizational Research
burnout among different occupational groups. The Methods, 1, 374-406.
work reported in the current study is only a step, Evers, W., Brouwers, A., Tomic, W. (2002).
and future longitudinal and experimental designs in Burnout and self-efficacy: A study on teachers'
particular, are needed in this area to develop beliefs when implementing an innovative
prevention and intervention programs. educational system in the Netherlands. British
Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(2), 227-
References 244.
Freudenberger, H. J. (1974). The staff burn-out
Ahola, K., Honkonen, T., Virtane, M., Aromaa, A., syndrome in alternative institutions.
& Lonnqvist, J. (2008). Burnout in relation to Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice,
age in the adult working population. Journal of 12(1), 73-82.
Occupational Health, 50, 362-365. Golembiewski, R. T., & Munzenrider, R. F., &
Alarcon, G. A., Edwards, J. M., & Menke, L. E. Stevenson, J. G. (1986). Stress in
(2011). Student Burnout and Engagement: A organizations: Toward a phase model of
Test of the Conservation of Resources Theory. burnout. New York: Preager.
The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary Golembiewski, R. T., & Munzenrider, R. F. (1990).
and Applied, 145(3), 211-227. Phases of burnout, modes and social support:
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a Contributions to explaining differences in
clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical physical symptoms. Journal of Managerial
review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Issues, 2(2), 176-183.
Practice, 10(2), 125–143. Golembiewski, R. T., & Munzenrider, R. F. (1993).
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative Fit Indexes in Health-related covariants of phases of burnout:
structural models. Psychological Bulletin, A replication. Organization Development
107(2), 238-246. Journal, 11(1), 1-12.
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Golembiewski, R. T., & Roundtree, B. H. (1986).
Significance tests and goodness of fit in the Phases of burn-out and properties of work
analysis of covariance structures. Psychological environments: Replicating and extending a
Bulletin, 88, 588-606. pattern of covariants. Organization
Brouwers, A., & Tomic, W. (1999). Teacher Development Journal, 4(2), 25-30.
burnout, perceived self-efficacy in classroom Greenglass, E., Burke, R. J., & Konarski, R.
management, and student disruptive behavior in (1997). The impact of social support on the
secondary education. Curriculum and Teaching, development of burnout in teachers:
14(2), 7-26. Examination of a model. Work and Stress,
Burke, R. J. (1989). Toward a phase model of 11(3), 267-278.
burnout. Group & Organization Studies, 14(1), Gryskiewicz, N., & Buttner, E. H. (1992). Testing
23-32. the robustness of the progressive phase model
Byrne, B. M. (1994). Burnout: Testing for the for a sample of entrepreneurs. Educational and
Validity, Replication, and Invariance of Causal Psychological Measurement, 52(3), 747-751.
Structure across Elementary, Intermediate, and James, L. R., Mulaik, S. A., & Brett, J. M. (1982).
Secondary Teachers. American Educational Causal analysis: Assumptions, models, and
Research Journal, 31(3), 645-673. data. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Central Finance Institute (2009). Information for Kessler, R. C., & Greenberg, D. F. (1981). Linear
schools and school administration. The Hague: panel analysis: Models of quantitative change.
Ministry of Education, Culture and Science. New York: Academic Press.
Chang, M. L. (2009). An appraisal perspective of Lee, R. L., & Ashforth, B. E. (1993). A
teacher burnout: examining the emotional work longitudinal study of burnout among
of teachers. Educational Psychology Review, supervisors and managers: Comparisons
21(3), 193-218. between the Leiter and Maslach (1988) and
Chen, M., & Miller, G. (1997). Teacher stress: a Golembiewski et al. (1986) models.
review of the international literature. ERIC, Organizational behavior and human decision
Information Analysis ED 410187. processes, 54, 369-398.
Cordes, C. L., Dougherty, T. W., & Blum, M. Lei W., Youn Hee K., & Dong, W. (2010). A
(1997). Patterns of burnout among managers review of research and strategies for burnout
and professionals: A comparison of models. among Chinese nurses. British Journal of
Nursing, 19(13), 844-50.

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 31


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

Lei, P-W., & Wu, Q. (2007). Introduction to Reynolds, J., Frank, K., & Kathy, H. (2005). The
structural equation modeling: issues and problem of attrition in survey research on
practical considerations. Educational health: Evidence from ten longitudinal surveys.
Measurement: Issues and Practice, 26(3), 33- Conference Papers: American Sociological
43. Association, Annual Meeting, Philadelphia, pp.
Leiter, M. P. (1989). Conceptual implications of 1-28.
two models of burnout: A response to Schaufeli, W. B., & Van Dierendonck, D. (1993).
Golembiewski. Group & Organization Studies, The construct validity of two burnout measures.
14(1), 15-22. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 14, 631-
Leiter, M. P. (1990). The impact of family 647.
resources, control coping, and skill utilization Schaufeli, W. B., & Van Horn, J. E. (1995).
on the development of burnout: A longitudinal Maslach Burnout Inventory voor leraren (MBI-
study. Human Relations, 43(11), 1067-1083. NL-Le): Voorlopige handleiding. [Maslach
Leiter, M. P., & Maslach, C. (1988). The impact of Burnout Inventory for Teachers: Preliminary
interpersonal environment on burnout and guide]. University of Utrecht: PAGO.
organizational commitment. Journal of Schaufeli, W. B., Daamen, J., & Van Mierlo, H.
Organizational Behavior, 9, 197-308. (1994). Burnout among Dutch teachers: An
Loonstra, B., Brouwers, A., & Tomic, W. (2009). MBI-validity study. Educational and
Feelings of existential fulfillment and burnout Psychological Measurement, 54(3), 803-812.
among secondary school teachers. Teaching Schaufeli, W. B., Enzmann, D., & Girault, N.
and Teacher Education, 25, 752-757. (1993). Measurement of burnout: A review. In
Martin, A. J. (2011). Prescriptive statements and W.B. Schaufeli, C. Maslach, T. Marek (Eds.),
educational practice: what can Structural Professional burnout: Recent developments in
Equation Modeling (SEM) offer? Educational theory and research (pp. 199-215).
Psychology Review, 23(2), 235-244. doi: Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.
10.1007/s10648-011-9160-0. Schaufeli, W. B., Maslach, C., & Marek, T. (1993).
Maruyama, G. M. (1998). Basics of structural Professional burnout: Recent developments in
equation modeling. Thousand Oaks: Sage. theory and research. Washington, DC: Taylor
Maslach, C. (1993). Burnout: A multidimensional & Francis.
perspective. In W.B. Schaufeli, C. Maslach, T. Scherer, R. F., Cox, M. K., Key, C. C., & Stickney,
Marek (Eds.), Professional burnout: Recent F. A. (1992). Assessing the similarity of
developments in theory and research. (pp. 19- burnout dimensions in two business samples.
32). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis. Psychological Reports, 71(1), 28-30.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1981). The Steyn, C. (2010). The relationship between burnout
measurement of experienced burnout. Journal and role identity among client service
of Occupational Behavior, 2, 99-113. employees. Pretoria: The University of Pretoria.
Maslach, C., & Leiter, M. P. (1997). The truth PhD Dissertation.
about burnout: How organizations cause Taris, T. W. (2000). A primer in longitudinal data
personal stress and what to do about it. San analysis. London: Sage.
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Taris, T. W., Le Blanc, P. M., Schaufeli, W. B., &
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. Schreurs, P. J. G. (2005). Are there causal
(2001). Job burnout. In S. T. Fiske, D. L. relationships between the dimensions of the
Schacter, & C. Zahn-Waxler (Eds.), Annual Maslach Burnout Inventory? A review and two
Review of Psychology, 52, pp. 397-422. longitudinal tests. Work & Stress, 19(3), 238-
McDonald, R. P., & Marsh, H. W. (1990). 255.
Choosing a multivariate model: Noncentrality Te Brake, H., Smits, N., Wicherts, J. M., Gorter, R.
and Goodness of Fit. Psychological Bulletin, C., & Hoogstraten, J. (2008). Burnout
107(2), 247-255. development among dentists: a longitudinal
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). study. European Journal of Oral Sciences, 116,
Psychometric theory (3rd Ed.). New York: 545–551.
McGraw-Hill. Tomic, W., & Tomic, E. N. (2008). Existential
Parker, P. D., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2011). fulfillment and burnout among principals and
Developmental processes in school burnout: a teachers. Journal of Beliefs & Values, 29, 11-
comparison of major developmental models. 27.
Learning and Individual Differences, 21(2), Tomarken, J.A., & Waller, G.N. (2005). Structural
244-248. equation modeling: Strengths, limitations, and
Plewis, I. (1985). Analysing change: measurement misconceptions. Annual Review of Clinical
and explanation using longitudinal data. New Psychology, 1, 31–65.
York, NY: Wiley. Van Dierendonck, D., Schaufeli, W. B., & Buunk,
B. P. (1997). Burnout and equity: A

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 32


Brouwers & Tomic: Teacher burnout dimensions

longitudinal study. In D. Van Dierendonck


(Ed.), Balancing give and take (pp. 79-102).
Enschede: Print Partners Ipskamp.
Van Dierendonck, D., Schaufeli, W. B., & Sixma,
H. (1994). Burnout among general practitioners:
a perspective from equity theory. Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 86-100.
Vercambre, M-N., Brosselin, P., Gilbert, F.,
Nerrière, E., & Kovess-Masféty, V. (2009).
Individual and contextual covariates of burnout:
a cross-sectional nationwide study of French
teachers. BMC Public Health, 9:333 doi:
10.1186/1471-2458-9-333.
Wanous, J. P., & Colella, A. 1989. Organizational
entry research: Current status and future
directions. In G. R. Ferris & K. M. Rowland
(Eds.), Research in personnel and human
resources management, 7 (pp. 59-120).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Zapf, D., Dormann, C., & Frese, M. (1996).
Longitudinal studies in organizational stress
research: A review of the literature with
reference to methodological issues. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 1(2), 145-
169.

Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain & Culture 33

You might also like