You are on page 1of 34

Emotion Review

Emotion and Regulation are One!


Fo
Journal: Emotion Review

Manuscript ID: EMR-09-149.R1

Manuscript Type: Original Manuscript


r

Date Submitted by the


Pe

Author:

Complete List of Authors: Kappas, Arvid; Jacobs University Bremen, School of Humanities and
Social Sciences
er

Area/Discipline: social < psychology

Keywords: emotion regulation, facial expression, facial feedback

I argue that (1) emotions often are actively auto-regulating. The


Re

behavior implied by the emotional reaction bias to the eliciting


event or situation modifies or terminates the situation. (2) Certain
emotion components are likely to habituate dynamically, modifying
the emotional states. (3) Emotions are typically intra- and
interpersonal processes at the same time and modulating forces at
vi

Abstract: these different levels interact. (4) Emotions are not just regulated –
they regulate. Important conclusions of my arguments are that the
scientific analysis of emotion should not exclude regulatory
ew

processes and that effortful emotion regulation should be seen


relative to a backdrop of auto-regulation and habituation and not
the ideal notion of a neutral baseline. For all practical purposes
unregulated emotion is not a realistic concept.

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 1 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 1


1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13 Emotion and Regulation are One!
14
15
16 Arvid Kappas
17
18
19
20
Jacobs University Bremen
Fo

21
22
23
24
rP

25
26
27
28
29
ee

30
31
32
rR

33
34
35
36
37
ev

38
39
40
41
iew

42
43
44
45
46
47
48
Author Note
49
50
51
52 Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Arvid Kappas, School of
53
54 Humanities and Social Sciences, Jacobs University Bremen, Campus Ring 1, 28759 Bremen,
55
56
Germany; email: a.kappas@jacobs-university.de
57
58
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 2 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 2


1
2
3 Abstract
4
5
6
7 Emotions are foremost self-regulating processes that permit rapid responses and adaptations
8
9 to situations of personal concern. They have biological bases and are shaped ontogenetically
10
11 via learning and experience. Many situations and events of personal concern are social in
12
13
14 nature. Thus, social exchanges play an important role in learning about rules and norms that
15
16 shape regulation processes. I argue that (1) emotions often are actively auto-regulating. The
17
18 behavior implied by the emotional reaction bias to the eliciting event or situation modifies or
19
20
Fo

21 terminates the situation. (2) Certain emotion components are likely to habituate dynamically,
22
23 modifying the emotional states. (3) Emotions are typically intra- and interpersonal processes
24
rP

25
26
at the same time and modulating forces at these different levels interact. (4) Emotions are not
27
28 just regulated – they regulate. Important conclusions of my arguments are that the scientific
29
ee

30 analysis of emotion should not exclude regulatory processes and that effortful emotion
31
32
regulation should be seen relative to a backdrop of auto-regulation and habituation and not the
rR

33
34
35 ideal notion of a neutral baseline. For all practical purposes unregulated emotion is not a
36
37 realistic concept.
ev

38
39
40
41
Keywords: emotion regulation, facial expression, facial feedback
iew

42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 3 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 3


1
2
3
4
5
6 Emotion and Regulation are One!
7
8
9 I argue that it does not make sense to consider unregulated emotion and emotion
10
11 regulation as two separable processes. Instead the nature of emotion always implies
12
13
14
regulation at various physical and psychological levels. I discuss the relationship of emotion
15
16 theory and concepts of emotion regulation, and how these might affect how emotion and
17
18 emotion regulation are studied.
19
20
Fo

21
My first argument relates how current views on emotion regulation are often linked to
22
23
24 the outdated historical separation of emotion and reason. Specifically, the classical focus on
rP

25
26 the disruptive nature of emotion, implicitly or explicitly, suggests that emotions are regulated
27
28
29
because of a desire to return to an “unemotional baseline”. However, in my second argument I
ee

30
31 outline that responding to the situation that elicited the emotion in the first place leads to the
32
rR

33 regulation of emotion without such a motivation. I refer to this process as emotional auto-
34
35
36 regulation. In the case of negative stimuli the emotion tends to lead to its self-termination,
37
ev

38 whereas in the case of positive stimuli, the emotion tends to lead to its reinforcement.
39
40 Emotion eliciting stimuli and the organism interact in creating a temporary system that
41
iew

42
43 involves a cascade of events, the emotion process, which leads to the functional modification
44
45 of that system to address the affordances of current needs and goals. Rather than rationality
46
47
48
trying to reassert itself, as suggested by the classical view, it is a matter of a system
49
50 responding to a destabilization by creating a dynamic adaptation that creates again a steady
51
52 state. I also discuss the role of habituation in reaching stability of the system.
53
54
55
The social environment can take the role of the agent in a behavioral cascade that leads
56
57
58 to the auto-regulation of emotions. This is particularly relevant for young infants that have
59
60 limited means of acting on situations, but continues to be relevant in adulthood. Auto-

regulation is not only an intra- but also an inter-personal process.

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 4 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 4


1
2
3 I close my contribution with a discussion of the implications of a systems view on an
4
5
6 interpretation of experimental paradigms used to study emotion and emotion regulation.
7
8
9 The historical assumption of the non-emotional baseline of psychological functioning
10
11
12 Given how prominent and intense emotions can feel to us and how apparently
13
14
15
emotional our social environment can be, it is not surprising that philosophers, artists, poets,
16
17 and playwrights have tried to tackle this facet of human experience for thousands of years
18
19 (Gross, 1998; Lazarus & Lazarus, 1994; Power & Dalgleish, 2008; Solomon, 2000).
20
Fo

21
22 Particularly in western thought, passion has, for the longest time, been understood to clash
23
24 and tussle with reason, the latter being supposedly the better guide for decisions or behavior.
rP

25
26 It would appear that emotions can get us into trouble – and this is true for the negative ones,
27
28
29 for example, when we are provoked, or the positive ones, when we do something in a moment
ee

30
31 of passion. As a consequence, western folk-psychology, as well as many emotion scientists,
32
rR

33
assume that the natural baseline affective state of the adult human is non-emotional or neutral
34
35
36 and that once an emotion has been elicited, it needs to be actively wrangled back to that
37
ev

38 baseline – because emotions are perceived, as Averill (1994a, p. 265) put it, as being
39
40
41
“unruly”.
iew

42
43
44 Not all researchers assume that we strive for the neutral, unemotional stance. In a
45
46 slight variation, some authors, would argue that there is the need or want to reach and
47
48
49
maintain a desired state that typically is somewhat positive – even though there are likely
50
51 cultural (“ideal affect”, Tsai, 2007) and possibly idiosyncratic differences. In summary,
52
53 according to these views emotions are switched on by some elicitor and the “victim” is then
54
55
56 required to switch them back off or change them to a desirable state to avoid the negative
57
58 consequences of the emotional state.
59
60
Consider the following quote

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 5 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 5


1
2
3 [Emotions] can hurt us as well as help us (Parrott, 1993). They do so when they are of
4
5
6 the wrong type, when they come at the wrong time, or when they occur at the wrong
7
8 intensity level. At times such as these, we may be highly motivated to try to regulate
9
10
our emotions. (Gross, 2008, p. 498).
11
12
13
14 Who, i.e., which psychological entity decides when emotions are of the wrong type, at
15
16 the wrong time, or at the wrong intensity level? I do not intend to argue that there are
17
18 situations, particularly associated with pathology, where emotions are associated with
19
20
Fo

21 suffering and dysfunction. However, I suggest that in everyday life it is more the norm that
22
23 emotions regulate and are regulated according to the affordances they imply, rather than
24
rP

25
26
modulated via executive. I am referring to this process as the auto-regulation of emotion.
27
28
29 The auto-regulation of emotions
ee

30
31
32 It is rarely acknowledged that emotions are self-regulating in nature. In other words,
rR

33
34 emotions typically have properties that lead to their self-termination (i.e., regulation) via the
35
36
37 behavioral biases that the associated action tendencies introduce. They are not triggered and
ev

38
39 then wait until they are switched off. For example, if being afraid of a spider leads me to step
40
41
iew

on and kill it, then, the agent provocateur (spider) of the specific emotion episode is removed
42
43
44 and the fear cascade will come to an end by returning to some steady state. The same is true
45
46 for evasive behavior in the case of fear (see Power & Dalgleish, 2008). It is well known that
47
48
49
some fears are resistant to change, if they lead to the systematic evasion of the eliciting
50
51 situation. While this avoidance might be problematic if it interferes with the daily life of the
52
53 person – for example, professional problems might arise if someone refuses to take an
54
55
56 airplane – the other side of the coin is that, such evasive behaviors are indeed successful in
57
58 reducing confrontation with the anxiogenic stimulus orsituation. However, such fears are
59
60 usually seen as unwanted or even pathological and dysfunctional. As Franklin D. Roosevelt

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 6 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 6


1
2
3 stated in his first inaugural address in 1933: “So, first of all, let me assert my firm belief that
4
5
6 the only thing we have to fear is fear itself (Roosevelt, 1989, para 1).”
7
8
9 Of course, Roosevelt was likely referring to (a) a nation faced with economic turmoil,
10
11 rather than individuals grappling with spiders or snakes, and (b) a catastrophic paralysis1 that
12
13
14 is perhaps more typical of the possum than the typical fear episode in humans not suffering
15
16 from clinical fear/panic (see Power & Dalgleish, 2008). Nevertheless, his concern regarding
17
18 fear might resonate with researchers focusing primarily on the adaptive value that emotions
19
20
Fo

21 might provide to a species in the long run. In other words, many scientists appear to assume
22
23 that it is good that we have emotions, because otherwise our species, might not be here at all,
24
rP

25
26
but most of the present time emotions are useless at best, or harmful at worst and need to be
27
28 regulated (see also Averill, 1994b). But is it really true that fear is merely a great invention of
29
ee

30 evolution but is dysfunctional in the here-and-now? This depends on who or what the unit of
31
32
analysis is. Consider the formalized hypothetical scenario:
rR

33
34
35
36 (1) If stimulus situation X leads organism O to avoid X via a cascade of processes,
37
ev

38 which for the sake of convenience we may call E


39
40
41
(tuning attentional processes; facilitating recall of relevant situations and action
iew

42
43 scenarios, biasing the likelihood of certain behaviors, facilitating action by shifting
44
45 the balance of bodily activation to rapid energy transformation via locomotion,
46
47
48 deal with potential injury) and
49
50 (2) E leads consequentially to terminate X from attentional focus, then
51
52 (3) the E-cascade was functional in terminating itself.
53
54
55
56 Whether, to pick a concrete example, avoiding the dentist might lead to further tooth
57
58 decay and negative consequences for O is a different question, at a different time scale.2
59
60 However, seen from the point of view of a negative emotion as the subject or actor, then its

primary goal of self-termination is functional3. I have argued elsewhere that auto-regulation is

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 7 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 7


1
2
3 part of the defining properties of emotion and analyzing emotion without this intrinsic
4
5
6 property to regulate itself would appear short sighted (Kappas, 2008). It is important in this
7
8 context not to confuse auto-regulation (literally self-regulation) with automatic regulation,
9
10
i.e., automatic processes in emotion regulation that act outside of awareness and without
11
12
13 effort (e.g., Mauss, Bunge, & Gross, 2008).
14
15
16 Of course, one could argue that this type of process (auto-regulation) is part of what
17
18 current emotion scientists view as emotion regulation anyway and thus there would be little
19
20
Fo

21 new or different to this argument. However, that is not the case. Gross explicitly excludes the
22
23 emotion-as-regulator scenario from the definition and scope of his analysis of emotion
24
rP

25
26
regulation (e.g., Gross & Thompson, 2007; see Kappas, 2008). His views are typical for the
27
28 field and I do not want to imply that his views are unique in this respect, but he puts the
29
ee

30 argument very eloquently. Consider the following quote:


31
32
rR

33
… it isn't clear whether [emotion regulation] refers to how emotions regulate
34
35
36 something else, such as blood pressure, memory, or parent-child interactions
37
ev

38 (regulation by emotions) or to how emotions are themselves regulated (regulation of


39
40
41
emotions). Both usages have currency, but the problem with the first usage (regulation
iew

42
43 by emotions) is that one of the functions of emotion is the coordination of diverse
44
45 response systems (Levenson, 1999). Thus emotion regulation in this first sense is
46
47
48 redundant with emotion, in that all instances of emotion would constitute emotion
49
50 regulation. I therefore find the second usage more sensible (regulation of emotions), in
51
52 which emotion regulation refers to the heterogeneous set of processes by which
53
54
55 emotions are themselves regulated. (Gross, 2008, pp. 499-500)
56
57
58 There are reasons for this stance. Possibly, one could worry that including the
59
60 consequence of an emotion terminating itself is circular and difficult to clearly separate from

other processes. I believe indeed that there is circularity in this, as I view emotion as being

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 8 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 8


1
2
3 characterized by multiple nested feedback loops that involve intra- and interindividual causes
4
5
6 and effects. Circularity in this sense is design and not a flaw in dynamic self-organizing
7
8 systems (Kappas & Descôteaux, 2003; Mayne & Ramsey, 2001). Furthermore, I agree that
9
10
emotion and regulation are difficult to separate from each other conceptually. In fact, the goal
11
12
13 of the present contribution is to make the point that they should not be separated when trying
14
15 to analyze or to understand emotions (see also Campos, Frankel, & Camras, 2004). It is
16
17
18
important to emphasize that this distinction is not just of theoretical relevance. There are
19
20 manifest consequences of how one conceptualizes emotion regulation, for example, with
Fo

21
22 regard to how one might interpret the experimental paradigms that are typically used to study
23
24
this process, as I will outline further later.
rP

25
26
27
28 The fear example demonstrated that emotions might terminate themselves. However,
29
ee

30 could it be that the plausibility of self-termination is limited to fear? After all, “[f]ear is the
31
32
emotion theorist’s favourite emotion because it seems so vital and functional when it works
rR

33
34
35 and so dysfunctional and disabling when it goes awry” (Power & Dalgleish, 2008, p. 172).
36
37 By definition, emotions of negative valence are unpleasant and any action that diminishes or
ev

38
39
40 terminates them will be reinforced using simple learning mechanisms that are well studied
41
iew

42 and documented. Thus, as soon as an infant is able to direct its attention away from a
43
44 negative stimulus, a reinforcing feedback loop leads to shaping behavior with regard to the
45
46
47 specific stimulus. Diverting visual attention means for the infant that the threatening object
48
49 can be controlled. If object permanence has not yet been established, then “out of sight”
50
51
52
means indeed “out of mind”. More generally, regulatory skills are reinforced that are tuned
53
54 with every successful (and unsuccessful) episode (e.g., Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005).
55
56 Does this argument hold for other negative emotions? Surely, disgust is related to avoiding –
57
58
59 or even expelling stimuli – self-regulating in the process, but the same could be said of more
60
social emotions such as shame and embarrassment (Lewis, 2000a). Anger would appear to

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 9 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 9


1
2
3 involve unblocking an obstacle, social or otherwise, typically without aggression, but
4
5
6 involving some sort of confrontation (Averill, 1982; see also Cornelius, 1996).
7
8
9 Without implying that any analysis of the motivational thrust of emotion and the
10
11 implications for auto-regulation needs to follow a basic emotions view, I have chosen discrete
12
13
14 emotions to facilitate the discussion. However, I believe that any negative state will have the
15
16 tendency to self terminate. Even sadness that typically is characterized by withdrawal (Barr-
17
18 Zisowitz, 2000) can be interpreted as a powerful motivator for others for social support in
19
20
Fo

21 dealing with a situation of loss – what Campos et al. (2004) refer to as what emotions do to
22
23 others in interpersonal emotion regulation. More recently, Rimé (2009) emphasized that in
24
rP

25
26
discussions on emotion regulation there is a tendency to focus too much, if not exclusively, on
27
28 the individual and her means of dealing with a situation in the sense of a Lone Ranger
29
ee

30 hypothesis (Dunahoo, Hobfoll, Monnier, Julsizer, & Johnson, 1998) and ignoring the multi-
31
32
layered network structure of interpersonal emotion. The crying infant is calling for – and
rR

33
34
35 typically receiving – support (see Fridlund, 1994 for a discussion of the meaning of
36
37 intentionality in this context). The same mechanisms likely work in the adult. In this case the
ev

38
39
40 unit of analysis is a dyad or small group, rather than an individual. I will return to these social
41
iew

42 processes in the next section.


43
44
45 While negative emotions are prone to lead actively via negative feedback loops to their
46
47
48 own termination, positive emotions are desirable and, again by definition, pleasurable. The
49
50 absence of positive states leads to a quest for them (appetite/desire) and their presence leads to
51
52 attempts to prolong or reinforce them. While there appears to be less clarity concerning
53
54
55 specific action tendencies associated with many of the positive affective states (see
56
57 Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998), there is little ambiguity that there is a push, urge, or
58
59
motivation to search and amplify them (Oishi, Diener, & Lucas, 2007). At the risk of stating
60

the obvious: It feels good to feel good; anticipating feeling good is exciting. Why are we not

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 10 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 10


1
2
3 then constantly chasing that positive feedback loop to remain in positive emotional states? It
4
5
6 is long and well known that positive states have two phases: an appetitive activity and a
7
8 consummatory response (e.g., Tinbergen, 1951, in Arnold, 1960). The latter terminates the
9
10
former and is associated with refractory periods that terminate the appetitive behavior after
11
12
13 consumption, for example, after having eaten or, another example, in the context post-
14
15 orgasmic sexual satiety (see Georgiadis & Kortekaas, 2010). The neural mechanisms of these
16
17
18
two processes often referred to as wanting/reward and pleasure/liking are now well studied
19
20 (e.g., Berridge, 2009; see also Panksepp, 1998). The disjunction of these two processes is
Fo

21
22 crucial in understanding addictions and various compulsive behaviors (e.g., Kringelbach &
23
24
Berridge, 2009) and provides the necessary termination of the positive feedback loop to avoid
rP

25
26
27 the organism getting stuck. There is experimental evidence from humans and from rats that
28
29
ee

excessive brain self-stimulation via implanted electrodes can render individuals basically
30
31
32 dysfunctional (Smith, Mahler, Pecina, & Berridge, 2010). Thus, it is not surprising that a
rR

33
34 two-stage process design of positive emotion is in place that avoids individuals getting stuck
35
36
in an infinite positive feedback loop. Note that Berridge argues that most likely even more
37
ev

38
39 complex positive emotional states might use circuits derived from sensory pleasure – an
40
41
iew

important notion when considering whether satiety might also apply to states such as pride, or
42
43
44
aesthetic enjoyment (see Frijda, 2007, pp. 80-81). It should also be noted that when people
45
46 are asked how happy they feel in general, there appears to be an average somewhat above
47
48 neutral in the positive range (Oishi et al., 2007). Tsai (2007) argues that there might be
49
50
51 cultural differences in the ideal (positive) affect with regard to the level of arousal desired as
52
53 the ideal state.
54
55
56 It should also be noted that there are situations, that seem to contradict the simple rule
57
58
59 that positive emotions are desired and negative emotions are avoided. Why would people pay
60
money to see horror movies? Why would they ride a roller coaster if unpleasant emotions

occur at times? In media psychology the question of why people voluntarily search out

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 11 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 11


1
2
3 negative affect “for entertainment” is a frequently discussed topic (see Sparks & Sparks,
4
5
6 2000; Hoffner & Levine, 2007). In fact,Andrade and Cohen (2007) suggest that there might
7
8 be concurrent activation of negative and positive emotions. Similarly, people might engage in
9
10
tedious and unpleasant activities if they will at a later point be associated with delayed . Here
11
12
13 top-down regulation is involved to tolerate either a negative situation, or the absence of a
14
15 positive stimulus in the here-and-now, with view to later positive rewards (see Tamir, 2005;
16
17
18
Mischel & Ayduk, 2004; however, on the thwarting of delay of gratification for short term
19
20 hedonic gain see Tice, Baumeister, & Zhang, 2004).
Fo

21
22
23 In summary, there is a tendency of emotional states to self terminate – in the case of
24
rP

25
26
negative emotions this is a consequence of a negative feedback loop, where following the
27
28 natural action tendencies implied by the emotion leads to active self-termination. In the case
29
ee

30 of positive states, the two phase process associated with chasing and then relishing the
31
32
positive reward is associated with a tendency for a satiety that interrupts the positive feedback
rR

33
34
35 loop. Any theory dealing with emotion and regulation should take these processes into
36
37 account. Emotions are not something that switches on and stays on until some dedicated
ev

38
39
40 emotion regulation system leads to a return to a neutral steady state or homeostasis.
41
iew

42
43 Social auto-regulation
44
45
46 Young infants have few means to directly affect their environment apart from
47
48
49
regulating attention. I already mentioned the acquisition of self-control via effortful shifts of
50
51 visual attention in early childhood (Rueda et al., 2005). Campos et al. (2004) make a similar
52
53 point concerning the influence of locomotion and object manipulation on emotion and
54
55
56 emotion regulation. As the infant is able to confront or avoid the anxiogenic stimulus
57
58 different means of regulation are available, changing the means of coping dramatically.
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 12 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 12


1
2
3 However, social context provides a rich environment for interindividual exchange and
4
5
6 regulation even for very young infants (see also Campos et al., 2004; Holodynski &
7
8 Friedlmeier, 2006; von Salisch, 2008). Thus, expressive behavior is a route to (a) achieve a
9
10
modification of the eliciting stimulus and (b) the emotional state, via imitation and feedback.
11
12
13
14 Firstly, expressions of distress are likely to solicit external help. Caregivers are alerted
15
16 to the need for intervention and will act to remedy the cause of distress (e.g., Campos et al.,
17
18 2004). In this sense, the expression is part of a response cascade that transcends the
19
20
Fo

21 individual and recruits the social network (see also Fridlund, 1994). Again, one could
22
23 conceive of the emotion terminating itself as the regular case. In this sense schemata are
24
rP

25
26
likely to develop that are targeted at the link between caregiver and the solution of certain
27
28 problems (Tarabulsy, Tessier, & Kappas 1996). Positive expressions are also likely to solicit
29
ee

30 social engagement but in this case reinforcing or increasing the associated positive state.
31
32
rR

33
Secondly, there is likely a moderation of the infant’s emotional state via imitation and
34
35
36 feedback. Already Darwin (1872) clearly stated that he believed that an attenuation of
37
ev

38 expression would yield an attenuation of the underlying emotional state. In that sense, he was
39
40
41
one of the first to state a facial feedback hypothesis explicitly. However, it is obvious, that
iew

42
43 today there are contradictory results as regarding the effect of facial modulation on emotions
44
45 (see Kappas, 2008). Particularly, studies from James Gross and his colleagues repeatedly
46
47
48 found that facial suppression leads to augmented physiological arousal and thus potentially, a
49
50 negative health impact, as well as social costs (see Gross & Thompson, 2007; Butler & Gross,
51
52 2009). Other, earlier, studies had strongly suggested that an attenuation of expressive
53
54
55 behavior does indeed lead to a concordant down-regulation of the emotion system (see
56
57 McIntosh, 1996; Smith, McHugo, & Kappas, 1996). Research is needed to explain this
58
59
60
divergence of findings.

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 13 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 13


1
2
3 There is no question that expressive feedback systems in the sense of a monotonicity
4
5
6 effect (attenuation leads to down-regulation; amplification to up-regulation) would be highly
7
8 consistent with an interpersonal regulation process where empathy is associated with mimicry
9
10
and imitation (Kappas & Descôteaux, 2003). In light of recent findings with regard to a
11
12
13 mirror system in the human brain (e.g., Iacoboni, 2009) and the tendency to imitate facial
14
15 expressions (e.g., Dimberg, 1982), such an architecture would make much sense consistent
16
17
18
with Lipps’ (1905) mechanism of imitation driven empathy proposed a century ago. Lipps
19
20 had assumed that we understand how another person feels by imitating the expressions of an
Fo

21
22 interaction partner and then being influenced by our own expression.
23
24
rP

25
26
It is likely that in adulthood social processes continue to play a role in the auto-
27
28 regulation of emotion. Assume that someone overcome with grief regarding the death of a
29
ee

30 loved one displays strong signs of distress, the social environment is likely to intervene.
31
32
Thinking of such processes as an extension of early externalized auto-regulation processes
rR

33
34
35 suggests that their function might not only to receive comfort, but to also address specifically
36
37 the origin of the emotion, feelings of loss and loneliness, confusion, and dealing with concrete
ev

38
39
40 problems that can be addressed in social interaction.
41
iew

42
43 In summary, there is a social layer of emotion that permits the auto-regulation of
44
45 emotions using the immediate social network. Others can to achieve solutions that the
46
47
48 individual might not be able to reach on their own, particularly in early childhood. Given
49
50 these means it is again not necessary to assume that an emotion will remain active until
51
52 specific regulation processes kick in. This does not exclude that individuals will attempt to
53
54
55 modify their emotional state rather than the eliciting situation. However, again, a
56
57 consideration of emotion regulation requires all aspects of the dynamics of emotions,
58
59
60
including social and non-social auto-regulation.

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 14 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 14


1
2
3 Reconsidering the experimental situation in the light of theoretical issues regarding
4
5
6 emotion and regulation
7
8
9 Given the importance of motivational aspects in emotional processes, it is not
10
11 surprising that changes in action tendencies or action readiness have been consequently seen
12
13
14 by some researchers as central features of emotions. For example, the founder of modern
15
16 appraisal theory Magda B. Arnold (1960), or more recently Nico Frijda (1986, 2007) discuss
17
18 these action related aspects of emotions in detail. For these theorists there is no emotion
19
20
Fo

21 without an impact on motivational tendencies. Arnold stated:


22
23
24 As soon as we appraise something as worth having in an immediate and intuitive way,
rP

25
26 we feel an attraction toward it. As soon as we instinctively judge that something is
27
28
29 threatening, we feel repelled from it, we feel urged to avoid it. The intuitive appraisal
ee

30
31 of the situation initiates an action tendency that is felt as emotion, expressed in various
32
rR

33
bodily changes, and that eventually may lead to overt action (Arnold, 1960, p. 177).
34
35
36
37 Similarly, Frijda holds that “[i]f an event has no repercussion on an individual’s
ev

38
39 inclinations to act, one will hesitate to call it an emotion, except perhaps in the case of
40
41
iew

emotions evoked by art” (2007, p. 4). However, not all theorists focus primarily on the tight
42
43
44 coupling of emotions, actions, and action tendencies. In their often-cited comparison of over
45
46 100 theoretical statements defining emotions, Kleinginna and Kleinginna (1981) identified 11
47
48
49
topical clusters regarding the primary or secondary emphasis of the core element(s) of
50
51 emotions. Some definitions would focus for example, on the cognitive processes involved in
52
53 emotion, particularly the generation of emotion, or on expressive behavior. Of course, it is
54
55
56 these foci that define research agendas, including the experimental paradigms used to study
57
58 emotions. I believe that the strong focus on expressive behavior, or on cognitive processes
59
60 overshadowed the importance of behavior and action tendencies and thus has favored a

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 15 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 15


1
2
3 passive (solitary) participant in emotional research with consequences for bodily and
4
5
6 psychological processes.
7
8
9 For example, many emotion researchers, inspired by Darwin’s (1872) seminal work
10
11 The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, are primarily interested in the facial
12
13
14 expressions humans show when they are in a particular emotional state (see Russell,
15
16 Bachorowski, & Fernández-Dols, 2003; Parkinson, Fischer, & Manstead, 2005). Similarly,
17
18 the implications of William James’ arguments regarding the specificity of emotions have
19
20
Fo

21 triggered interest in emotional responses other than expressive behavior, such as peripheral
22
23 physiological activity (see Cornelius, 1996), or more recently, brain activity. Typical research
24
rP

25
26
goals in the field involve demonstrating that there is an intrapersonal coherence or correlation
27
28 between changes in emotional response systems (e.g., expressive behavior, peripheral
29
ee

30 autonomic nervous system manifestations, subjective experience; see Kappas, 2003, Russell
31
32
et al. 2003), and that there is similarity of such patterns between different individuals (see
rR

33
34
35 Mauss & Robinson, 2009).
36
37
ev

38 In a typical experimental paradigm in this context, participants are asked to take a seat
39
40
41
in front of some sort of display that will show still images (e.g., Cacioppo, Petty, Losch, &
iew

42
43 Kim, 1986; see Bradley & Lang, 2007) or films (e.g., McHugo, Smith, & Lanzetta, 1982;
44
45 Philippot, 1993; Gross & Levenson, 1995; see Rottenberg, Ray, & Gross, 2007) that have
46
47
48 been pre-tested to elicit specific emotional reactions. If the dependent variables include
49
50 physiological responses, the seat is often a reclining seat and subjects are connected with a
51
52 variety of wires and cables and often enough it is made clear to them that they are not to move
53
54
55 much in order not to create recording artefacts.4 What, however, does such a setting do to the
56
57 participant? Firstly, the setting counteracts spontaneous action tendencies. Despite all
58
59
reaffirmations in consent forms that subjects are free to terminate their participation at any
60

time, for any reason, and without any penalty, it is clear that the threshold to walking out on

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 16 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 16


1
2
3 an experimenter is higher than to change the channel on your TV at home if one finds a show
4
5
6 objectionable. Even looking away or covering one’s eyes with a hand (see Kappas, 2008)
7
8 runs counter to the experimental instructions or even physical possibility if both hands are
9
10
connected to devices. Thus, rather than an instance where the experimenters’ design results in
11
12
13 reducing error variance by simplifying and standardizing the experimental situation, as
14
15 intended, one could see this as a complication, where natural action tendencies are blocked.
16
17
18 A comment should be made also to the issue of social isolation. Of course the idea to
19
20
Fo

21 have participants isolated is an attempt to control for the effect of “display rules” by socially
22
23 isolating the participant (see also Keltner & Haidt, 2001). However, as there are well
24
rP

25
26
established effects of implicit sociality that are functional even in social isolation, the
27
28 paradigm opens the door to various uncontrolled implicit sociality effects and in effect
29
ee

30 transforming the participant into a free monadic radical ready to connect to any explicit or
31
32
implicit social context (Kappas, 2004) –
rR

33
34
35
36 Considering these issues, the stimulus condition that is appraised in a typical
37
ev

38 experiment becomes actually an interaction of three entities: Firstly, the selected stimulus
39
40
41
itself is a key component, say, an excerpt from a horror movie. Secondly, the experimental
iew

42
43 situation, which might be rather complex. Consider the example of being in a small room, tied
44
45 to equipment, with student assistants of the opposite sex potentially watching via hidden
46
47
48 cameras and no way to look away from the screen. Thirdly, the meaning and interpretation is
49
50 complicated by effects of the physical position in the reclining chair (Stepper & Strack, 2003;
51
52 see also Harmon-Jones & Peterson, 2009).
53
54
55
56 Arguably, this complication of what constitutes the actual stimulus, in this
57
58 hypothetical laboratory situation, does not necessarily invalidate existing findings. This
59
60 depends on the questions the researchers ask. If, for example, the subject would get angry at

the experimenters for putting her into such an unpleasant situation, but would correctly

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 17 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 17


1
2
3 indicate feeling angry, then the observed bodily changes could be related to the self-report and
4
5
6 contribute to answering the original research question. Of course, one could not state in one’s
7
8 research report that, as a rule, horror films are necessarily linked to getting angry, as in a
9
10
different situation the same film stimulus might have elicited quite different affective
11
12
13 responses (Richard Lazarus’ early research on physiological responses to stressful films is
14
15 certainly very informative regarding the complex relationship between a physical stimulus
16
17
18
and the interpretation; e.g., Lazarus & Alfert, 1964).
19
20
Fo

21 Much of the present laboratory research on emotion regulation is using the same
22
23 situational context as the passive reaction paradigm described above in typical “coherence”
24
rP

25
26
research with the added modification of asking participants to employ a particular strategy,
27
28 such as not showing anything on their face, performing a secondary task, or trying to
29
ee

30 cognitively focus on a particular aspect of the stimulus (Kappas, McHugo, & Lanzetta, 1989;
31
32
Gross & Levenson, 1993). There are two problems here. Firstly, does it make a difference to
rR

33
34
35 the participant whether s/he is in control of the choice of regulatory mechanism or whether an
36
37 experimenter dictates how an arousing situation needs to be dealt with (see Tamir & Mauss,
ev

38
39
40 in press)? Potentially, this lowers confidence and appraisals of emotion focused coping
41
iew

42 potential, or, phrased consistent with a self-regulation perspective, change the belief that the
43
44 emotion can be successfully regulated. However, this is not the critical issue I want to
45
46
47 address here, but the second issue which relates to the comparison of regulation and baseline,
48
49 respectively spontaneous condition. In fact, there is reason to doubt that comparison
50
51
52
conditions without any control instructions are indeed baseline conditions that would not
53
54 involve regulatory processes. Instead, such an experimental situation is a condition that is
55
56 characterized by taking habitual coping strategies away by counteracting spontaneous action
57
58
59 tendencies, whereas the experimental emotion regulation conditions then would consist,
60
according to this logic, of counteracting spontaneous action tendencies + taking habitual

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 18 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 18


1
2
3 emotion regulation strategies away + forcing a specific emotion regulation strategy. Needless
4
5
6 to say, this complicates the interpretation of the results.
7
8
9 In summary, researchers habitually create highly artificial situations that have little to
10
11 do with spontaneous regulation of emotional states in everyday life in social- and task
12
13
14 contexts. This is largely due to concepts of emotions in which action tendencies play a minor
15
16 role. The problem is confounded with the belief that social influences on emotions could be
17
18 controlled by having participants performing tasks in solitary conditions (see also Kappas,
19
20
Fo

21 2003)..
22
23
24 What happens to unregulated emotions? Habituation?
rP

25
26
27 There are different ways how emotions might decay or be terminated. We can consider
28
29
ee

30 auto-regulation, as described above as an active self-termination. It is active in that the


31
32 organism acts to do something that changes the situation and this the emotion. A passive
rR

33
34 process that modulates emotion is habituation. Habituation is pervasive in most bodily
35
36
37 systems, both at the physiological and at the psychological level (e.g., Breiter et al., 1996;
ev

38
39 Dawson, Schell, & Filion, 2007; Janssen, Prause, & Geer, 2007). It can be found at a cellular
40
41
iew

level and even in primitive organisms, such as the flatworm (Koopowitz, 1975). As Frijda
42
43
44 puts it in his Law of Habituation: “Continued pleasures wear off; continued hardships lose
45
46 their poignancy” (Frijda, 2007, p. 10). Thus, not only is there considerable spontaneous
47
48
49
recovery when emotional stimuli are terminated (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998), habituation
50
51 will, in many cases, lead to a reduction of physiological and psychological responses to
52
53 ongoing stimuli, as long as there is not continual, or at least frequent, change in either the
54
55
56 stimulation, or their appraisal, or both.
57
58
59 In typical research paradigms in the emotion regulation context, there are many
60
attempts to avoid habituation – for example, stimuli are typically brief and varied. However,

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 19 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 19


1
2
3 this creates again an artificial situation that likely has an impact on the engagement of the
4
5
6 participant. What are the conditions in the real world in which automatic or top-down
7
8 regulation occur (see Philippot, Baeyens, Douilliez, & Francart, 2004; Power & Dalgleish,
9
10
2008)? How frequent is it that in the real world one is being confronted with a rapid sequence
11
12
13 of positive and negative stimuli (while being immobilized; in a strange social situation; with a
14
15 fixed and externally determined regulation strategy prescribed)?
16
17
18 Habituation is a second order adaptation mechanism that leads to a decrease of
19
20
Fo

21 emotion in the natural world faced with ongoing stimulation. Its effects are likely additive to
22
23 the effects of auto-regulation, particularly to avoid lingering effects. An example of an
24
rP

25
26
exception to such processes are pathological states that are characterized by rumination –
27
28 specifically in depression (Gotlib & Joormann, 2010). In this case habituation seems to be
29
ee

30 compromised.
31
32
rR

33
There is so far little attempt to develop models of the various habituation processes
34
35
36 involved in the emotion cascade. Given that cognitive processes related to novelty, relevance,
37
ev

38 as well as mainly sensorial processes have very different dynamic properties, this is not a
39
40
41
trivial enterprise. It is known that certain dependent variables, such as electrodermal
iew

42
43 responses habituate much faster than event related potentials of the brain. Having this
44
45 knowledge would allow for proper baseline procedures against which regulation attempts,
46
47
48 spontaneous or prompted could be tested. More research is needed.
49
50
51 Two questions arise from the conclusion that emotions are intrinsically self-regulating
52
53 and subject of habituation processes. Firstly, what are the conditions that would require
54
55
56 additional regulatory processes? Secondly, if regulation is part and parcel of emotion, is there
57
58 any possibility to distinguish emotion from regulation?
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 20 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 20


1
2
3 As regards the question of additional regulatory processes – I have already mentioned
4
5
6 that pathological conditions might motivate volitional regulatory processes. However, even in
7
8 everyday situations individuals may wish to modulate their affective states as Gross and
9
10
colleagues have frequently argued. I do not deny the existence of such volitional regulation.
11
12
13 Yet, these processes are a) interacting with other regulating mechanisms and b) occur as a
14
15 foreground to concurrent processes such as auto-regulation and habituation. Thus, it would
16
17
18
appear strange to divorce these processes and exclude auto-regulation and habituation from an
19
20 analysis of emotion regulation.
Fo

21
22
23 Distinguishing emotion from regulation depends in part on the underlying emotion
24
rP

25
26
theory as I will outline in the conclusion. However, it can be pointed out that the structures
27
28 and processes involved in emotion regulation are also involved in “emotion-without-
29
ee

30 regulation”. Thus, I do not believe it useful to analyze one without considering the other.
31
32
rR

33
34
Conclusion: One!
35
36
37 Whether emotion and regulation appear better conceptualized separately or as part and
ev

38
39 parcel of the same system depends on the underlying emotion theory. For example, theories
40
41
iew

that assume the existence of affect programs appear to be consistent with the notion of
42
43
44 separating emotion and regulation. The notion of affect programs, developed by Silvan
45
46 Tomkins has influenced popular emotion theorists, particularly Paul Ekman (e.g., 2003; see
47
48
49
also Cornelius, 1996). Generally, the idea of affect programs involves coordinated response
50
51 cascades in different response modalities. Something triggers the affect and then a relatively
52
53 stereotypical package of responses ensues. Here one could distinguish between the “pure”
54
55
56 case where the affect program takes its undisturbed course and the case where the individual
57
58 attempts to modify the course or the outcome of the program.
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 21 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 21


1
2
3 A second class of theories can be traced back to William James (e.g., Damasio, 2000).
4
5
6 James focused on subjective experience, or in other words the feeling component, as the
7
8 essence of emotion. In contrast to the affect program view, it appears not necessary to make
9
10
strong assumptions regarding how strictly physiological and expressive responses are
11
12
13 coupled, because the analytical emphasis of the theories is on what causes feeling specifically.
14
15 To proponents of this view it might also appear logical to separate the “uncontrolled” from the
16
17
18
“controlled” case – in consequence divorcing emotion and regulation.
19
20
Fo

21 In contrast, systems approaches that focus on the existence of regulatory loops


22
23 between emotional components imply that there is no emotion without regulation of some sort
24
rP

25
26
(e.g., Kappas & Descôteaux, 2003; Lewis, 2000b; Campos, Frankel, & Camras, 2004). This
27
28 is not to say that there are different ways and levels of emotion regulation. Such a view is
29
ee

30 perfectly compatible with the notion that an individual might try to influence his or her
31
32
subjective state using techniques such as reappraisal, or modulation of expressive behavior.
rR

33
34
35 However, the reason these effortful regulation strategies work is, according to a systems view,
36
37 because of the highly interdependent architecture of the emotion system. As this architecture
ev

38
39
40 is at work whether conscious regulation efforts are or are not in place, it makes more sense to
41
iew

42 consider regulation a part of the emotion process.


43
44
45 I believe that specific research into the nature of how different emotional components
46
47
48 are coupled, such as studies on facial feedback (e.g., Laird, 1974; Lanzetta, Cartwright-Smith,
49
50 & Kleck, 1976; Zuckerman, Klorman, Larrance, & Spiegel, 1981; Ekman, Levenson, &
51
52 Friesen, 1983; Strack, Martin, & Stepper, 1988; see also McIntosh, 1996), should influence
53
54
55 our understanding of emotions. We have to come to terms with the consistent finding that
56
57 emotional components do not typically exhibit highly coherent and correlated responses (see
58
59
Mauss & Robinson, 2009). Rather than assume that there is an ideal emotional process, it
60

would be more productive to specify which factors influence inter-component coherence. It is

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 22 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 22


1
2
3 clear that the mutually regulatory influences between components should, or better must, be
4
5
6 part of a theory that tries to unify these findings.
7
8
9 The different regulation mechanisms that I have discussed in this contribution (and
10
11 others that I have not touched upon) whether they are “by evolutionary design”, by
12
13
14 implication, by top-down modulation, or otherwise, whether they are intra- or interindividual,
15
16 are, I believe, part of what most contemporary emotion scientists would call emotional (see
17
18 also Campos et al., 2004 for a related argument). They continuously modulate and trigger
19
20
Fo

21 moment-to-moment changes in all of the dependent variables that we take as criteria for
22
23 “emotion”. Thus, in this clearly specified sense, I consider the attempt of separating analyses
24
rP

25
26
of emotion and emotion regulation unproductive and hence emotion and regulation are one!
27
28
29
ee

30
31
32
rR

33
34
35
36
37
ev

38
39
40
41
iew

42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 23 of 33 Emotion Review

1
2
3 References
4
5
6 Andrade, E.B., & Cohen, J.B. (2007). On the consumption of negative feelings. Journal of
7
8 Consumer Research, 34, 283-300.
9
10
11 Arnold, M.B. (1960). Emotion and personality. Volume I: psychological aspects. New York,
12
13
14 NY: Columbia University Press.
15
16
17 Averill, J.R. (1994a). Emotions unbecoming and becoming. In P. Ekman & R.J. Davidson
18
19 (Eds.) The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 265-269). New York, NY:
20
Fo

21
22 Oxford University Press.
23
24 Averill, J.R. (1994b). Emotions are many splendored things. In P. Ekman & R.J. Davidson
rP

25
26 (Eds.) The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 99-102). New York, NY:
27
28
29 Oxford University Press.
ee

30
31 Averill, J.R., (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York, NY: Springer.
32
rR

33
Barr-Zisowitz, C. (2000). “Sadness” – Is there such a thing? In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-
34
35
36 Jones, and L.F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed) (pp. 607-622). New York,
37
ev

38 NY: Guilford.
39
40
41
Berggren, U., Hakeberg, M., & Carlsson, S.G. (2000). Relaxation vs. cognitively oriented
iew

42
43 therapies for dental fear. Journal of Dental Research, 79, 1645-1651. doi:
44
45 10.1177/00220345000790090201
46
47
48
49
Berridge, K.C. (2009). Wanting and liking: Observations from the neuroscience and
50
51 psychology laboratory. Inquiry, 52, 378-398. doi: 10.1080/00201740903087359
52
53 Bradley, M.M., & Lang, P.J. (2007). The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the
54
55
56 study of emotion and attention. In J.A. Coan & J.J.B. Allen (Eds.) Handbook of emotion
57
58 elicitation and assessment (pp. 29-46). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 24 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 24


1
2
3 Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Whalen, P. J., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L., et
4
5
6 al. (1996). Response and habituation of human amygdala during visual processing of
7
8 facial expression. Neuron, 17, 875-887. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80219-6
9
10
Butler, E.A., & Gross, J.J. (2009). Emotion and emotion regulation: Integrating individual and
11
12
13 social levels of analysis. Emotion Review, 1, 86-87. doi: 10.1177/1754073908099131
14
15 Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Losch, M. E., & Kim, H. S. (1986). Electromyographic activity
16
17
18
over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective
19
20 reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 260-268.
Fo

21
22 Campos, J., Frankel, C., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation. Child
23
24
Development, 75, 377-394.
rP

25
26
27 Cornelius, R.R. (1996). The science of emotion: Research and tradition in the psychology of
28
29
ee

emotion. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.


30
31
32 Damasio, A.R. (2000). The feeling of what happens: Body, emotion and the making of
rR

33
34 consciousness. London, UK: Vintage.
35
36
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London, UK: Murray.
37
ev

38
39
40 Dawson, M.E., Schell, A.M., & Filion, D.L. (2007). The electrodermal system. In J.T.
41
iew

42 Cacioppo, L.G. Tassinary, & G.G. Berntson (Eds.). Handbook of Psychophysiology (3rd
43
44
edition; pp. 159-181). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
45
46
47
48 Dimberg, U. (1982). Facial reactions to facial expressions. Psychophysiology, 19, 643-647.
49
50 doi; 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1982.tb02516.x
51
52
53 Dunahoo, C.L., Hobfoll, S.E., Monnier, J., Hulsizer, M.R., & Johnson, R. (1998). There’s
54
55
56 more than rugged individualism in coping. Part 1: Even the Lone Ranger had Tonto.
57
58 Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 11, 137-165.
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 25 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 25


1
2
3 Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed. Understanding faces and feelings. London: Weidenfeld
4
5
6 & Nicolson.
7
8 Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity
9
10
distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 1208-1210.
11
12
13 Fredrickson, B. L. & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the
14
15 cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191-220.
16
17
18
Doi: 10.1080/026999398379718
19
20
Fo

21 Fridlund, A,J. (1994). Human facial expression. An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA:
22
23 Academic Press.
24
rP

25
26
Frijda, N.H. (2007). The laws of emotion. Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum.
27
28
29 Frijda, N.H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
ee

30
31
32 Georgiadis, J.R., & Kortekaas, R. (2010). The sweetest taboo: functional neurobiology of
rR

33
34 human sexuality in relation to pleasure. In M.L. Kringelbach & K.C. Berridge (Eds.)
35
36
37 Pleasures of the Brain (pp. 178 – 201). New York, NY: Oxford University Press
ev

38
39
40 Gotlib, I.H., & Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: Current status and future
41
iew

42 directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 285-312


43
44
45 Gross, J.J. (2008). Emotion regulation. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, and L.F. Barrett
46
47 (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed) (pp. 497-512). New York, NY: Guilford.
48
49 Gross, J.J. (2001). Award for distinguished early career contributions to psychology.
50
51
52 American Psychologist, 56, 911-913.
53
54 Gross, J.J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of
55
56
General Psychology, 2, 271-299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
57
58
59 Gross, J.J., & Levenson, R.W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and
60
Emotion, 9, 87-108. doi: 10.1080/02699939508408966

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 26 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 26


1
2
3 Gross, J.J., & Levenson, R.W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and
4
5
6 expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970-986. doi:
7
8 10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.970
9
10
11 Gross, J.J., & Thompson, R.A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J.J.
12
13
14 Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3-24). New York, NY: Guilford
15
16 Publications.
17
18 Harmon-Jones, E., & Peterson, C.K. (2009). Supine body position reduces neural response to
19
20
Fo

21 anger evocation. Psychological Science, 20, 1209-1210. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-


22
23 9280.2009.02416.x
24
rP

25
26 Hoffner, C. A. & Levine, K. J. (2007). Enjoyment of mediated fright and violence: A Meta-
27
28
29 Analysis. Media Psychology, 7, 207-237.
ee

30
31
32 Holodynski, M. & Friedlmeier, W. (2006). Development of emotions and emotion regulation.
rR

33
34 Boston, New York: Springer
35
36
37
ev

38
Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annual Review of Psychology,
39
40 60, 653-670.
41
iew

42
43 Janssen, E., Prause, N., & Geer, J. (2007). The sexual response. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G.
44
45
46
Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (3rd ed; pp, 245-
47
48 2566). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
49
50
51 Kappas, A. (2008). Psssst! Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde are actually the same person! A tale of
52
53
regulation and emotion. In M. Vandekerckhove, C. von Scheve, S. Ismer, S. Jung, & S.
54
55
56 Kronast (Eds.) Regulating emotions: Social necessity and biological inheritance (pp. 15-
57
58 38). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 27 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 27


1
2
3 Kappas, A. (2004, October). Intra-and interpersonal determinants and consequences of facial
4
5
6 activation. Presented at the Forty-Fourth Annual Meeting of the Society for
7
8 Psychophysiological Research, Santa Fe, New Mexico, USA, October 21, 2004.
9
10
11 Kappas, A. (2003). What facial expressions can and cannot tell us about emotions. In M.
12
13
14 Katsikitis (Ed.), The human face: Measurement and meaning (pp. 215-234). Dordrecht:
15
16 Kluwer Academic Publishers.
17
18
19 Kappas, A. (2001). A metaphor is a metaphor is a metaphor: Exorcising the homunculus from
20
Fo

21
22 appraisal theory. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal
23
24 Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research (pp. 157-172). New York: Oxford
rP

25
26 University Press.
27
28
29
ee

30 Kappas, A. & Descôteaux, J. (2003). Of butterflies and roaring thunder: Nonverbal


31
32 communication in interaction and regulation of emotion. In P. Philippot, R.S. Feldman,
rR

33
34 & E.J. Coats (Eds.) Nonverbal behavior in clinical settings (pp. 45-74). New York, NY:
35
36
37 Oxford University Press.
ev

38
39 Kappas, A., McHugo, G, J., & Lanzetta, J. T (1989). Componential control and the modu-
40
41
iew

lation of emotional experience. Psychophysiology, 26, S37.


42
43
44
45 Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2001). Social functions of emotion. In T.J. Mayne & G.A. Bonanno
46
47 (Eds.) Emotions: Current issues and future directions (pp. 192-213). New York, NY:
48
49 Guilford Press.
50
51
52
53 Kleinginna, P., Jr., & Kleinginna, A. (1981). A categorized list of emotion definitions, with
54
55 suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 345-379.
56
57
58 Koopowitz, H. (1975). Activity and habituation in the brain of the polyclad flatworm
59
60
Freemania litoricola. Journal of Experimental Biology, 62, 455-467.

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 28 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 28


1
2
3 Kringelbach, M.L., & Berridge, K.C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure
4
5
6 and happiness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 479-487.
7
8 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.006
9
10
11 Laird, J. D. (1974). Self-attribution of emotion: The effects of expressive behavior on the
12
13
14 quality of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 475-
15
16 486.
17
18
19 Lanzetta, J. T., Cartwright-Smith J., & Kleck, R. E. (1976). Effects of nonverbal dissimulation
20
Fo

21
22 on emotional experience and autonomic arousal. Journal of Personality and Social
23
24 Psychology, 33, 354-370.
rP

25
26
27 Lazarus, R. S., & Alfert, E. (1964). The short-circuiting of threat by experimentally altering
28
29
ee

30 cognitive appraisal. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 195 205.
31
32
rR

33 Lazarus, R.S., & Lazarus, B.N. (1994). Passion & reason: Making sense of our emotions.
34
35 Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
36
37
ev

38
Lewis, M. (2000a). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame, guilt. In M.
39
40 Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, and L.F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed)
41
iew

42 (pp. 623-636). New York, NY: Guilford.


43
44
45 Lewis, M. D. (2000b). The promise of dynamic systems approaches for an integrated account
46
47 of human development. Child Development, 71, 36-43.
48
49 Lipps, T. (1905). Das Wissen von fremden Ichen. In T. Lipps (Ed.), Psychologische
50
51
52 Untersuchungen Vol. 1: Bewußtsein und Gegenstände (Vol. I, pp. 694-722). Leipzig:
53
54 Engelmann.
55
56
Mauss, I.B., Bunge, S.A., & Gross, J.J. (2008). Culture and automatic emotion regulation. In
57
58
59 M. Vandekerckhove, C. von Scheve, S. Ismer, S. Jung, & S. Kronast (Eds.) Regulating
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 29 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 29


1
2
3 emotions: Social necessity and biological inheritance (pp. 39-60). Malden, MA:
4
5
6 Blackwell Publishing.
7
8 Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and
9
10
Emotion, 23, 209-237. doi: 10.1080/02699930802204677
11
12
13
14 Mayne, T.J., & Ramsey, J. (2001). The structure of emotion: A nonlinear dynamic systems
15
16 approach. In T.J. Mayne & G.A. Bonanno (Eds.) Emotions: Current issues and future
17
18 directions (pp. 1-37). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
19
20
Fo

21
22 McHugo, G. J., Smith, C. A., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1982). The structure of self-reports of
23
24 emotional responses to film segments. Motivation and Emotion, 6, 365-385.
rP

25
26
27 McIntosh, D.N. (1996). Facial feedback hypotheses: Evidence, implications, and directions.
28
29
ee

30 Motivation and Emotion, 20, 121-147.


31
32
rR

33 Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The
34
35 dynamics of delay of gratification. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook
36
37
ev

38
of self-regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications (pp. 99-129). New York:
39
40 Guilford.
41
iew

42
43 Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (2007). The optimal level of well-being: Can we be too
44
45
46
happy? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 346-360. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-
47
48 6916.2007.00048.x
49
50
51 Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions.
52
53
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
54
55
56
57 Parkinson, B., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2005). Emotion in Social Relations:
58
59 Cultural, Group, and Interpersonal Processes. New York: Psychology Press.
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 30 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 30


1
2
3 Philippot, P. (1993). Inducing and assessing differentiated emotional feeling states in the
4
5
6 laboratory. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 171-193.
7
8
9 Philippoot, P., Baeyens, C., Douilliez, & Francart, B. (2004). Cognitive regulation of
10
11 emotion: Application to clinical disorders. In P. Philippot & R.S. Feldman (Eds.). The
12
13
14 regulation of emotion (pp. 71-97). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
15
16
17 Power, M., & Dalgleish, T. (2008). Cognition and emotion: From order to disorder (2nd ed.).
18
19 Hove: Psychology Press.
20
Fo

21
22
Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and empirical review.
23
24
rP

25 Emotion Review, 1, 60-85. doi: 10.1177/1754073908097189


26
27
28 Roosevelt, F. D. (1989). First inaugural address. In Joint Congressional Committee on
29
ee

30
Inaugural Ceremonies (Ed.) Inaugural Addresses of the Presidents of the United States.
31
32
rR

33 Washington, D.C.: U.S. G.P.O.: Supt. of Docs., U.S. G.P.O., 1989; Bartleby.com, 2001.
34
35 http://www.bartleby.com/124/pres49.html.
36
37
ev

38 Rottenberg, J., Ray, R.D., & Gross, J.J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using films. In J.A. Coan
39
40
41 & J.J.B. Allen (Eds.) Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 9-28).
iew

42
43 Oxford: Oxford University Press.
44
45
46 Rueda, M.R., Posner, M.I., & Rothbart, M.K. (2005). The development of executive attention:
47
48
49 Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28,
50
51 573-594. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2
52
53
54 Russell, J. A., Bachorowski, J.-A., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2003). Facial and vocal
55
56
57 expressions of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 329-349.
58
59 doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145102
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 31 of 33 Emotion Review

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 31


1
2
3 Smith, K.S., Mahler, S.V., Pecina, S., & Berridge, K.C. (2010). Hedonic hotspots: Generating
4
5
6 sensory pleasure in the brain. In M.L. Kringelbach, & K.C. Berridge (Eds.) Pleasures of
7
8 the brain (pp. 27-49). New York: Oxford University Press.
9
10
11 Smith, C.A., McHugo, G.J., & Kappas, A. (1996). Epilogue: Overarching themes and
12
13
14 enduring contributions of the Lanzetta research. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 237–253.
15
16 Solomon, R. C. (2000). The philosophy of emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones
17
18 (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 3-15). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
19
20
Fo

21 Sparks, G. G., & Sparks, C. W. (2000). Violence, mayhem, and horror. In D. Zillmann & P.
22
23 Vorderer (Eds.), Media entertainment: The psychology of its appeal (pp. 73-91).
24
rP

25
26
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
27
28
29 Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional
ee

30
31 feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 211-220. doi:10.1037/0022-
32
rR

33
3514.64.2.211
34
35
36
37 Strack, F., Martin, L. & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human
ev

38
39 smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and
40
41
iew

Social Psychology, 54, 768-777.


42
43
44
45 Tamir, M. (2005). Don't worry, be happy? Neuroticism, trait-consistent affect regulation, and
46
47 performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 449-461.
48
49
50 Tamir, M., & Mauss, I. B. (in press). Social-cognitive factors in emotion regulation:
51
52
53 Implications for well-being. In I. Nyklicek, A. Vingerhoets, M. Zeelenberg, & J.
54
55 Denollet (Eds.), Emotion regulation and well-being. Springer.
56
57
58 Tarabulsy, G. M., Tessier, R., & Kappas, A. (1996). Contingency detection and the contingent
59
60
organization of behavior in interactions: Implications for socioemotional development in

infancy. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 25-41.

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Emotion Review Page 32 of 33

Running head: EMOTION/REGULATION 32


1
2
3 Tice, D.M., Baumeister, R.F., & Zhang, L. (2004). The role of emotion in self-regulation:
4
5
6 Differing roles of positive and negative emotion. In P. Philippot & R.S. Feldman (Eds.).
7
8 The regulation of emotion (pp. 213-226). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
9
10
11 Tsai, J.L. (2007). Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences. Perspectives on
12
13
14 Psychological Science, 2, 242-259. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.x
15
16
17 von Salisch, M. (2008). Themes in the development of emotion regulation in childhood and
18
19 adolescence and a transactional model. In M. Vandekerckhove, C. von Scheve, S. Ismer,
20
Fo

21
22 S. Jung, & S. Kronast (Eds.) Regulating emotions: Social necessity and biological
23
24 inheritance (pp. 146-167). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
rP

25
26
27 Zuckerman, M.. Klorman. R., Larrance, D.T., & Spiegel, N.H. (1981). Facial, autonomic, and
28
29
ee

30 subjective components of emotion: The facial feedback hypothesis versus the


31
32 externalizer-internalizer distinction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41,
rR

33
34 929-944.
35
36
37
ev

38
39
40
41
iew

42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review
Page 33 of 33 Emotion Review

1
2
3 Footnotes
4
5
6
7 1
8 While the first part of the quote is often cited, the continuation is less known. “—nameless,
9
10 unreasoning, unjustified terror which paralyzes needed efforts to convert retreat into advance”
11
12
(Roosevelt, 1989, para 1)
13
14 2
15 An estimated 5 to 20% of the adult population suffer from high dental anxiety which may
16
17 lead to phobic avoidance behavior (Berggren, Hakeberg, & Carlsson, 2000).
18
19 3
20 It may appear unusual to refer to the self-interest of emotions, as if psychological states
Fo

21
22 themselves had intentions. However, many do not consider unusual to regard the interest and
23
24
fate of ideas in the context of discussions on “memes” (Dakwins, 1976). In both cases,
rP

25
26
27 intention, or interest, are but metaphors that relate to the evolutionary success or failure of a
28
29
ee

concept rather than the physical hardware upon which emotions or ideas rely.
30
31 4
32 My own research has often enough used such an experimental setting. Thus, my critical
rR

33
34 remarks also apply to some of my own research.
35
36
37
ev

38
39
40
41
iew

42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60

http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/emotion-review

View publication stats

You might also like