You are on page 1of 16

Received: 28 August 2018 

|   Revised: 18 December 2018 


|
  Accepted: 20 January 2019

DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12459

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Religiousness and the HEXACO personality factors and facets in


a large online sample

Michael C. Ashton1   | Kibeom Lee2

1
Department of Psychology, Brock
University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada
Abstract
2
Department of Psychology, University of Objective: To examine the associations of religiousness with personality
Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada characteristics.
Method: We obtained self‐ratings of religiousness along with self‐reports on the
Correspondence
Michael C. Ashton, Department of HEXACO Personality Inventory—Revised in a sample of nearly 200,000 online re-
Psychology, Brock University, St. spondents. Respondents also indicated their religious affiliation, religiousness of up-
Catharines, ON L2S 3A1, Canada.
bringing, and political orientation; a subset of the respondents also indicated attitudes
Email: mashton@brocku.ca
about immigration and foreign aid.
Results: Religiousness showed weak associations (|r|s < 0.15) with several
HEXACO factors but somewhat stronger associations with the Fairness and Altruism
facets (both rs > 0.20). On those facets, participants with the highest religiousness
self‐rating (7 on a 1‐to‐7 scale) averaged about 1 SD higher than did participants with
the lowest religiousness self‐rating. In addition, religiousness was negatively related
to the Unconventionality facet among persons whose upbringing was very religious,
but not among persons whose upbringing was very nonreligious. Religiousness/per-
sonality associations were generally quite similar within different religious affilia-
tions (Hinduism, Islam, Judaism, Buddhism, and several branches of Christianity)
but were stronger within relatively religious countries than within nonreligious coun-
tries. Despite the positive association of religiousness with the Altruism facet, reli-
giousness was uncorrelated with pro‐out‐group attitudes (i.e., favoring multicultural
immigration and foreign aid).
Conclusion: The findings advance our understanding of religiousness/personality
associations.

1  |   IN T RO D U C T ION 1.1  |  Links of religiousness with


personality factors
The purpose of the present report is to examine the associa-
tions of self‐rated religiousness with self‐report personality Religiousness can be viewed as a psychological individual
characteristics. Although this topic has been investigated in difference characteristic that differs conceptually from per-
many previous studies, the present research has the advan- sonality characteristics in several ways. One useful deline-
tages of combining (a) a very large sample containing many ation of the features of religion—features not shared with
participants from each of several major religions with (b) a personality traits—is Saroglou’s (2011) list of four dimen-
differentiated assessment of personality, examining many sions. According to Saroglou’s model, religiousness involves
narrow facet‐level traits organized within six broad HEXACO believing in some external transcendence or supernatural
dimensions. agents; bonding with some transcendent reality, usually

Journal of Personality. 2019;1–16. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jopy © 2019 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.     1 |


|
2       ASHTON and LEE

through a public or private ritual; behaving according to of 56 countries, Schmitt and Fuller (2015) obtained a simi-
some specified set of norms or values; and belonging to a lar pattern of personality/religiousness correlations, with
community of believers. But the conceptual differences be- Agreeableness and Conscientiousness correlating 0.16 and
tween religiousness and personality traits do not rule out the 0.13, respectively, with religiousness in their overall sample.
possibility of empirical associations, and such associations Note that the results of Gebauer et al. and of Schmitt and
have been examined in many previous investigations. Fuller were reported separately by country or by world region
Meta‐analyses of previous studies on religiousness and but not by religion, and we are not aware of any study that
personality (e.g., Saroglou, 2010; see also Saroglou, 2017) has undertaken a large sample comparison of personality/re-
have shown that self‐rated religiousness is modestly related ligiousness associations across several major religions.
to the Big Five personality dimensions of Conscientiousness
and Agreeableness. Other work based on the HEXACO
1.3  |  Links of religiousness with
model of personality has also found religiousness to be
personality facets
modestly related to Conscientiousness as well as to three
HEXACO dimensions (Agreeableness, Honesty‐Humility, Most research on personality/religiousness relations has fo-
and Emotionality) that are independently related to Big Five cused on broad dimensions of personality, particularly the
Agreeableness (see review in Ashton & Lee, 2014). Other Big Five or more recently the HEXACO factors. However,
personality dimensions are not consistently related to reli- there are certain narrower personality traits—commonly
giousness, but the Openness to Experience dimension (both called facet‐level traits—that might be expected to be par-
Big Five and HEXACO) does relate to what could be called ticularly relevant to religiousness. For example, religions
the style of religious expression: high Openness persons tend (and religious communities) generally seem to encourage
to adopt a mystical, spiritual form of religiousness, whereas their members to have ethical integrity, to be forgiving of
low Openness persons tend to adopt a traditional, even fun- others, and to act altruistically, at least in their dealings with
damentalist form of religiousness (Saroglou, 2010; Saucier & other in‐group members. These tendencies might lead one
Skrzypińska, 2006). to expect relatively strong associations of religiousness with
certain HEXACO facets, such as Fairness (a facet of Honesty‐
Humility assessing ethical integrity), Forgivingness (a facet
1.2  |  Personality/religiousness associations
of Agreeableness), and Altruism (an “interstitial” facet that
across religions and countries
divides its loadings across Honesty‐Humility, Agreeableness,
The meta‐analytic findings reported by Saroglou (2010) and Emotionality).1 As another example, the tendency to re-
were mainly based on samples from Western countries and ject religious beliefs might be more likely among persons
in which most participants were of Christian religious back- who are nonconforming, unconventional “free thinkers”
ground. However, the few non‐Western, non‐Christian sam- or who are highly intellectually curious, and therefore one
ples of Saroglou’s meta‐analysis did not differ much from might expect religiousness to correlate negatively with the
the other samples in their patterns of personality/religious- HEXACO facets of Unconventionality (a facet of Openness)
ness associations. More recently, several investigations have and Inquisitiveness (also a facet of Openness, and assessing
shown that the links between personality and religiousness intellectual curiosity), particularly among persons whose up-
are rather similar across countries, including non‐Western, bringing was religious.
majority non‐Christian countries. For example, in a series of
studies, Aghababaei and colleagues (Aghababaei et al., 2016;
1.4  |  Interpreting personality/religiousness
Aghababaei, Wasserman, & Nannini, 2014) have found that
associations
HEXACO personality/religiousness associations in mainly
Muslim countries (Iran and Malaysia) were similar to those The data to be examined in the present research are based
in mainly Christian countries (Poland and USA). Also, in a entirely on self‐reports, and this situation raises the possibil-
very large sample study of personality and religiousness in 66 ity that any personality/religiousness associations could be
countries representing every continent, Gebauer et al. (2014) due to biases shared between personality and religiousness
found that Big Five Agreeableness and Conscientiousness self‐reports. For example, if persons who consider them-
were positively related to religiousness (with beta‐weights selves very religious are also inclined to overestimate their
of 0.15 and 0.10, respectively, across the full international own prosociality—whether through hypocrisy or through
sample). Consistent with the predictions of their “sociocul- genuine misperception—then this could produce an artifac-
tural motives perspective,” Gebauer et al. also found some tual association between measures of religiousness and of
tendency for the correlations to be stronger within the na- those prosocial personality traits. Although we think that this
tional samples having higher mean levels of religiousness. scenario is plausibly responsible for at least some part of any
In another worldwide study based on a similarly diverse set correlations based on self‐reports, we think it is not the chief
ASHTON and LEE   
|
   3

source of such correlations. Two lines of evidence instead political orientation (left‐vs.‐right) and, in a subset of our
suggest that the associations are in large part due to a real sample, participants’ endorsement of two pro‐out‐group at-
substantive association between religiousness and prosoci- titudes, specifically, favorability toward giving of foreign
ality. First, several previous studies have shown (self‐rated) aid by one’s country and toward worldwide immigration into
religiousness to be associated not only with self‐reports of one’s country. We have noted previously that religiousness is
prosocial traits but also with observer reports of those same associated positively both with prosocial personality traits—
traits as provided by (for example) family members, teachers, particularly Altruism—and also with right‐wing political ori-
or colleagues (see review by Saroglou, 2010). Second, reli- entation, despite the negative association between right‐wing
giousness has been found to be associated with actual charita- political orientation and Altruism (Lee, Ashton, Griep, &
ble giving (even, to some extent, to secular causes) and with Edmonds, 2018). We expected that the endorsement of pro‐
actual prosocial behaviours as observed in laboratory studies out‐group attitudes would be roughly uncorrelated with reli-
(see reviews by Saroglou, 2010, 2012). giousness, given the positive links of religiousness with both
Altruism and right‐wing political orientation, which would
be expected to show opposing associations with the pro‐out‐
1.5  | Personality/religiousness
group attitudes.
associations and religious upbringing
Considerable research has examined personality/religious-
1.7  | Summary
ness relations, but less attention has been focused on the pos-
sibility that those relations may differ as a function of the To summarize, our plan for the present research was as fol-
religiousness of one’s upbringing: that is, the personality lows: (1) to examine the associations of self‐rated religious-
traits associated with religiousness among people raised in ness with self‐reports on the factor‐ and facet‐level scales of
nonreligious households may differ from those among people the HEXACO‐PI‐R, both in the entire participant sample and
raised in religious households. One prominent exception is in subsamples representing different religious affiliations or
the work of McCullough, Tsang, and Brion (2003), who in different countries; (2) to examine the associations of reli-
a longitudinal study of gifted children found that religious- giousness with personality as a function of (self‐rated) reli-
ness in adulthood was related to religiousness of upbringing giousness of upbringing; and (3) to examine the associations
but that the association was stronger for persons lower in of religiousness with some pro‐out‐group attitudes, to find
emotional stability. Another exception is that of Altemeyer out whether any links between religiousness and prosocial
and Hunsberger (1997), who conducted a series of interviews personality characteristics would carry over to this intergroup
among university students who had (a) abandoned their family expression of prosociality.
religion or (b) converted to a religion after having been raised
without a religion. They reported that the former students
(whom they called “amazing apostates”) had abandoned their
2  |  M ETHOD
religions mainly because of intellectual skepticism, whereas
2.1  | Participants
the latter students (whom they called “amazing believers”)
had joined religions mainly because of personal crisis (e.g., Participants completed the questionnaires of the present
death of a loved one, substance abuse problems). research at an online survey site (http://hexaco.org) be-
tween the dates of October 19, 2014 and October 18, 2017.
Respondents were not directly recruited but instead visited
1.6  |  Religiousness and pro‐out‐
the website and completed the questionnaires for self‐explo-
group attitudes
ration purposes. We screened participants’ responses to the
To the extent that religious persons really do tend to be more items of the personality questionnaire (HEXACO‐100; see
prosocial in their personality characteristics, this prosociality below) on the basis of the following criteria: (1) completion
is not necessarily expected to be universal in its application. of all items; (2) correct responses to three “attention‐check”
Instead, religiousness might promote prosociality toward items; and (3) two other response quality checks (i.e., coher-
in‐group members in good standing while simultaneously ence of responses and use of varied response options; for de-
limiting prosociality toward out‐group members and toward tails see Lee & Ashton, 2018). The screening thresholds in
lapsed in‐group members. As Saroglou (2013, p. 446) has put criterion (3) were set conservatively so that only obviously
it, religion’s “coalitional dimension (community and shared nonpurposeful responses were removed; fewer than 1% of
normative beliefs and practices) emphasizes the in‐group ver- the respondents who passed the first two screening criteria
sus out‐group barriers, thereby limiting the extent of proso- were eliminated by the third. For the purpose of the present
ciality apparently inherent in the very nature of religion.” research, we examined only those participants who passed all
To investigate this possibility, we examined participants’ of the screening criteria listed above and who also responded
|
4       ASHTON and LEE

to each of the sex, age, and religiousness self‐rating items


2.2.3  |  Pro‐out‐group attitudes
(described below) with a response other than “prefer not to
answer.” The resulting sample size was 195,279; of these par- After completing the HEXACO‐100 self‐report and before
ticipants, 46% were women and the mean age was 32.2 years completing the demographic questions, most participants
(SD = 13.8). completed a set of several additional personality or atti-
tude questions. (The particular set of questions differed
across participants according to the date when they com-
2.2  | Measures pleted the questionnaires because we replaced a given set
of questions every few months with some other set.) For
2.2.1  | Personality the purpose of the present report, we focus on one such set
The English language version of the HEXACO‐100 (Lee & of questions administered to a subsample of 7,119 partici-
Ashton, 2018) was used to assess the HEXACO personality pants, specifically, a set of four questions assessing out‐
factors. The order of the items in the online survey is the group‐related attitudes, two of which are of interest for the
same as that of the paper‐and‐pencil version of the question- present report:
naire, and each item was presented one at a time. As men-
tioned above, three attentiveness check items were embedded My country’s government should be generous in
evenly throughout the questionnaire (e.g., “This is an atten- helping poor people in other countries
tiveness check; please indicate ‘neutral’.”). Responses were My country should admit immigrants from
made on a 5‐point scale with anchors ranging from strongly countries all around the world
disagree (1) to strongly agree (5).
Each of these items was administered using a 7‐point re-
sponse scale (1 = very strongly disagree to 7 = very strongly
2.2.2  |  Religiousness and other agree). The two items correlated 0.56 with each other, and we
relevant variables averaged them to form an overall pro‐out‐group attitude score.
After completing the HEXACO‐100 and a few attitude
questions (see below), participants responded to a series
of demographic questions, including questions asking
3  |  RESULTS
about age, sex, nationality, ethnic origin, and educational
3.1  |  Religious affiliations
level. The question about religious affiliation was phrased
as “What is your religious background?” and included Table 1 shows the number of respondents who indicated each
the response options Sikh, Hindu, Christian, Muslim, religious affiliation. As seen in the table, about two‐thirds of
Jewish, Buddhist, Other East Asian religious traditions, the respondents indicated Christianity as their religion, and
Aboriginal/Native religious traditions, Other, No reli- as noted in the Methods section, we obtained the denomina-
gious background, and Prefer not to answer. Beginning in tional category of nearly 40% of those Christian participants.
August 2015, we also added a follow‐up question to find Among those Christians for whom denominational informa-
the denominational category of participants who indicated tion was available, nearly 40% were Catholics, nearly 30%
a Christian religious background, asking “If you indicated were Protestants, over 20% were “Other” Christians, and
Christian to the above question, which of the following about 5% were Eastern Orthodox Christians. (We suspect
describes your background?” and providing the response that many of the “Other” Christians are unaware that their
options Catholic, Protestant, Eastern Orthodox, Other, and denomination actually belongs to the Protestant category,
Prefer not to answer. We thereby obtained denominational but we cannot differentiate those persons from those whose
information for almost 40% of our participants who indi- denomination is really non‐Protestant.) Although the large
cated a Christian religious background. majority of the sample was Christian, there were also sub-
We also included among the demographic questions a sin- stantial numbers of Hindus, Jews, Muslims, and Buddhists,
gle 7‐point item each for religiousness (“How religious do each representing between 1.5% and 3% of the entire sample;
you consider yourself to be?” 1 = not at all to 7 = extremely), samples for other religions were smaller and are not reported
for political orientation (“How would you describe your po- in the table. About 20% of the respondents indicated no reli-
litical orientation?” 1 = very left‐wing to 7 = very right‐ gious affiliation.
wing), and (in the second and third years) for religiousness of We also considered that our Buddhist sample could con-
upbringing (“How religious was your upbringing?” 1 = not sist partly of Asian‐origin participants, most of whom would
at all to 7 = extremely). Each item also included the response have been raised as Buddhists, but partly of non‐Asian‐or-
option of “prefer not to answer,” coded as missing data for igin participants, most of whom would have converted to
our analyses. Buddhism. We therefore report results for the Buddhist
T A B L E 1   Means, standard deviations, and correlations of demographic and personality variables with religiousness
Total (N = 195,279) Religion Total (N = 139,640) Hindu (N = 4,450) Muslim (N = 6,956) Jewish (N = 3,224) Buddhist (N = 3,658)

M SD r α M SD r M SD r M SD r M SD r M SD r
ASHTON and LEE

Religiousness 2.82 1.83 – 3.19 1.84 – 3.26 1.74 – 3.80 1.72 – 2.64 1.64 – 3.20 1.57 –
Sex 0.46 0.50 0.07 0.45 0.50 0.09 0.34 0.47 0.10 0.39 0.49 0.06 0.45 0.50 0.11 0.47 0.50 0.03
Age 32.24 13.80 0.05 33.26 14.24 0.00 27.33 9.72 0.16 25.69 8.77 −0.03 35.43 16.21 −0.02 30.34 12.22 0.18
Honesty‐Humility (H) 3.29 0.73 0.13 0.88 3.31 0.73 0.15 3.15 0.75 0.10 3.19 0.74 0.21 3.28 0.72 0.06 3.35 0.70 0.08
Emotionality (E) 3.11 0.64 0.11 0.84 3.12 0.63 0.12 3.12 0.60 0.22 3.10 0.61 0.11 3.23 0.64 0.11 3.17 0.58 0.06
Extraversion (X) 3.22 0.67 0.11 0.87 3.25 0.67 0.10 3.24 0.67 0.11 3.24 0.66 0.05 3.37 0.68 0.06 3.20 0.64 0.09
Agreeableness (A) 2.89 0.65 0.11 0.87 2.89 0.64 0.12 2.84 0.64 0.05 2.90 0.64 0.11 2.82 0.65 0.04 2.97 0.65 0.03
Conscientiousness (C) 3.52 0.58 0.09 0.83 3.53 0.57 0.09 3.46 0.58 0.08 3.47 0.59 0.08 3.56 0.58 0.07 3.48 0.57 0.04
Openness (O) 3.70 0.58 −0.03 0.82 3.70 0.57 −0.07 3.68 0.57 −0.11 3.66 0.58 −0.13 3.78 0.57 −0.05 3.76 0.57 0.06
H: Sincerity 3.26 0.90 0.08 0.76 3.27 0.89 0.08 3.31 0.94 0.09 3.29 0.94 0.11 3.16 0.89 0.02 3.36 0.89 0.06
H: Fairness 3.51 1.06 0.24 0.82 3.58 1.05 0.24 3.55 1.06 0.19 3.61 1.08 0.31 3.62 1.05 0.17 3.59 1.01 0.11
H: Greed Avoidance 2.96 1.03 0.02 0.84 2.95 1.02 0.03 2.70 1.04 −0.02 2.69 1.03 0.08 2.99 1.03 −0.04 3.06 1.02 0.03
H: Modesty 3.44 0.88 0.06 0.77 3.43 0.88 0.08 3.03 0.90 0.02 3.18 0.91 0.12 3.34 0.89 0.02 3.40 0.85 0.04
E: Fearfulness 2.80 0.85 0.07 0.69 2.81 0.85 0.07 2.86 0.82 0.11 2.83 0.84 0.06 2.90 0.87 0.04 2.91 0.83 0.01
E: Anxiety 3.51 0.89 0.01 0.73 3.50 0.88 0.02 3.50 0.81 0.06 3.49 0.84 0.02 3.60 0.88 0.06 3.51 0.84 −0.01
E: Dependency 2.85 0.90 0.09 0.76 2.86 0.90 0.10 2.85 0.93 0.18 2.80 0.91 0.08 3.04 0.90 0.07 2.93 0.88 0.05
E: Sentimentality 3.28 0.87 0.15 0.73 3.30 0.86 0.15 3.27 0.86 0.26 3.27 0.86 0.15 3.37 0.85 0.15 3.35 0.81 0.11
X: Social Self‐Esteem 3.54 0.79 0.06 0.70 3.57 0.78 0.05 3.48 0.81 0.06 3.46 0.81 0.04 3.67 0.79 0.02 3.50 0.77 0.06
X: Social Boldness 3.02 0.90 0.04 0.73 3.04 0.89 0.03 2.98 0.88 0.05 3.03 0.86 −0.02 3.27 0.89 0.01 2.93 0.84 0.06
X: Sociability 3.05 0.92 0.11 0.78 3.08 0.91 0.11 3.21 0.91 0.14 3.22 0.89 0.07 3.20 0.91 0.06 3.09 0.88 0.06
X: Liveliness 3.27 0.87 0.12 0.80 3.30 0.86 0.11 3.30 0.87 0.10 3.25 0.87 0.07 3.35 0.89 0.08 3.28 0.83 0.10
A: Forgivingness 2.53 0.86 0.15 0.78 2.55 0.86 0.16 2.48 0.87 0.09 2.57 0.88 0.14 2.47 0.83 0.03 2.63 0.86 0.07
A: Gentleness 3.06 0.82 0.09 0.72 3.06 0.81 0.10 3.03 0.80 0.08 3.11 0.80 0.11 2.93 0.85 0.07 3.14 0.81 0.05
A: Flexibility 2.80 0.77 0.07 0.65 2.80 0.77 0.07 2.82 0.75 0.07 2.83 0.78 0.07 2.75 0.79 0.04 2.95 0.75 0.02
A: Patience 3.16 0.93 0.03 0.81 3.16 0.92 0.03 3.02 0.93 −0.07 3.10 0.95 0.02 3.15 0.92 0.00 3.17 0.93 −0.03
C: Organization 3.33 0.91 0.07 0.74 3.35 0.90 0.07 3.29 0.90 0.12 3.28 0.91 0.09 3.31 0.95 0.07 3.31 0.87 0.03
C: Diligence 3.78 0.76 0.10 0.72 3.81 0.74 0.09 3.72 0.75 0.06 3.73 0.77 0.06 3.92 0.74 0.07 3.75 0.74 0.03
C: Perfectionism 3.57 0.76 0.05 0.69 3.57 0.76 0.06 3.55 0.76 0.06 3.56 0.76 0.02 3.58 0.78 0.02 3.55 0.77 0.03
C: Prudence 3.38 0.77 0.04 0.71 3.40 0.76 0.04 3.26 0.80 −0.02 3.30 0.80 0.05 3.45 0.76 0.03 3.32 0.76 0.02
O: Esthetic Appreciation 3.54 0.84 0.06 0.66 3.55 0.83 0.03 3.42 0.83 −0.01 3.43 0.85 −0.07 3.54 0.85 0.04 3.67 0.78 0.09
O: Inquisitiveness 3.75 0.85 −0.08 0.71 3.76 0.84 −0.12 3.78 0.82 −0.10 3.73 0.85 −0.08 3.85 0.83 −0.05 3.72 0.81 0.02
O: Creativity 3.76 0.85 0.01 0.73 3.75 0.84 −0.01 3.75 0.82 −0.03 3.75 0.83 −0.07 3.86 0.83 −0.03 3.78 0.84 0.06
O: Unconventionality 3.75 0.67 −0.10 0.58 3.73 0.66 −0.13 3.79 0.66 −0.19 3.73 0.66 −0.18 3.86 0.64 −0.13 3.85 0.66 0.00
  

Interstitial: Altruism 3.77 0.74 0.23 0.65 3.81 0.72 0.23 3.77 0.74 0.22 3.84 0.73 0.23 3.90 0.72 0.15 3.84 0.69 0.16
|   5

(Continued)
T A B L E 1   Continued
|
Buddhist Asian Buddhist Non‐Asian All Christian Christian: Roman Christian: Protestant Christian: Eastern Other Christian No Religion
(N = 2,310) (N = 1,333) (N = 121,352) Catholic (N = 19,615) (N = 13,281) Orthodox (N = 2,426) (N = 9,615) (N = 37,332)
6      

M SD r M SD r M SD r M SD r M SD r M SD r M SD r M SD

Religiousness 3.04 1.53 – 3.46 1.60 – 3.17 1.85 – 2.95 1.71 – 3.45 2.00 – 3.04 1.73 – 3.95 1.88 – 1.34 0.80
Sex 0.49 0.50 0.06 0.45 0.50 −0.01 0.46 0.50 0.09 0.40 0.49 0.07 0.44 0.50 0.08 0.42 0.49 0.11 0.46 0.50 0.06 0.44 0.50
Age 27.66 9.64 0.12 34.92 14.60 0.18 33.94 14.44 0.01 27.63 11.51 −0.01 31.37 13.89 −0.03 26.86 9.35 0.09 27.04 1.46 −0.03 28.76 11.64
Honesty‐Humility (H) 3.27 0.68 0.05 3.48 0.72 0.09 3.32 0.72 0.15 3.27 0.72 0.08 3.41 0.73 0.16 3.18 0.72 0.08 3.37 0.69 0.15 3.23 0.75
Emotionality (E) 3.21 0.56 0.10 3.11 0.60 0.03 3.11 0.63 0.12 3.09 0.64 0.12 3.11 0.67 0.10 3.10 0.63 0.15 3.13 0.65 0.09 3.06 0.67
Extraversion (X) 3.17 0.62 0.11 3.26 0.66 0.05 3.24 0.67 0.10 3.25 0.70 0.17 3.21 0.70 0.11 3.22 0.68 0.13 3.26 0.71 0.12 3.12 0.70
Agreeableness (A) 2.93 0.64 0.01 3.05 0.67 0.05 2.89 0.64 0.12 2.98 0.65 0.10 2.98 0.65 0.13 2.89 0.64 0.06 3.09 0.64 0.13 2.85 0.67
Conscientiousness (C) 3.45 0.56 0.06 3.53 0.57 −0.02 3.54 0.57 0.10 3.54 0.59 0.12 3.53 0.60 0.09 3.53 0.60 0.07 3.57 0.59 0.07 3.46 0.60
Openness (O) 3.60 0.55 −0.04 4.03 0.50 0.11 3.70 0.57 −0.07 3.71 0.58 −0.06 3.76 0.58 −0.09 3.78 0.54 −0.04 3.65 0.60 −0.10 3.67 0.60
H: Sincerity 3.32 0.87 0.04 3.43 0.90 0.07 3.26 0.89 0.08 3.26 0.88 0.04 3.28 0.89 0.07 3.36 0.91 0.01 3.35 0.87 0.07 3.22 0.92
H: Fairness 3.56 0.99 0.13 3.63 1.05 0.07 3.57 1.05 0.25 3.50 1.04 0.20 3.69 1.04 0.27 3.37 1.06 0.21 3.65 1.01 0.25 3.28 1.07
H: Greed Avoidance 2.93 0.98 −0.03 3.28 1.03 0.07 2.97 1.02 0.03 2.88 1.04 −0.04 3.11 1.03 0.04 2.81 1.03 −0.03 2.88 1.04 0.04 2.98 1.04
H: Modesty 3.28 0.83 −0.01 3.59 0.86 0.06 3.46 0.87 0.09 3.43 0.88 0.03 3.54 0.87 0.08 3.16 0.90 0.06 3.58 0.84 0.07 3.44 0.91
E: Fearfulness 3.05 0.79 0.05 2.67 0.85 0.01 2.80 0.85 0.08 2.79 0.85 0.07 2.77 0.87 0.05 2.80 0.81 0.09 2.78 0.87 0.06 2.77 0.86
E: Anxiety 3.54 0.80 0.01 3.45 0.91 −0.02 3.50 0.89 0.02 3.47 0.90 0.03 3.45 0.93 −0.01 3.43 0.86 0.08 3.50 0.92 −0.02 3.51 0.93
E: Dependency 2.97 0.87 0.08 2.87 0.88 0.03 2.86 0.89 0.10 2.85 0.91 0.11 2.90 0.93 0.12 2.91 0.92 0.09 2.89 0.91 0.09 2.78 0.93
E: Sentimentality 3.29 0.79 0.14 3.43 0.83 0.06 3.30 0.86 0.15 3.26 0.89 0.15 3.30 0.90 0.13 3.26 0.87 0.18 3.35 0.87 0.14 3.16 0.92
X: Social Self‐Esteem 3.46 0.76 0.05 3.57 0.78 0.06 3.57 0.78 0.06 3.55 0.81 0.10 3.56 0.80 0.08 3.51 0.78 0.09 3.55 0.80 0.07 3.46 0.83
X: Social Boldness 2.83 0.81 0.04 3.12 0.87 0.04 3.04 0.89 0.03 3.00 0.91 0.08 3.01 0.92 0.05 2.99 0.83 0.03 2.98 0.94 0.05 2.94 0.92
X: Sociability 3.15 0.86 0.11 2.99 0.90 0.01 3.06 0.91 0.11 3.12 0.94 0.19 3.00 0.96 0.11 3.11 0.92 0.16 3.12 0.94 0.13 2.94 0.93
X: Liveliness 3.23 0.81 0.12 3.36 0.86 0.04 3.31 0.86 0.12 3.33 0.89 0.16 3.26 0.89 0.11 3.27 0.91 0.12 3.39 0.89 0.14 3.15 0.90
A: Forgivingness 2.56 0.84 0.06 2.74 0.88 0.06 2.56 0.85 0.17 2.64 0.86 0.13 2.68 0.87 0.20 2.62 0.87 0.09 2.80 0.87 0.18 2.45 0.86
A: Gentleness 3.11 0.81 0.03 3.19 0.82 0.08 3.05 0.81 0.10 3.14 0.81 0.10 3.12 0.83 0.08 3.05 0.78 0.08 3.27 0.80 0.11 3.03 0.85
A: Flexibility 2.96 0.73 0.03 2.95 0.80 0.02 2.80 0.77 0.07 2.87 0.79 0.08 2.80 0.79 0.05 2.74 0.78 0.03 2.94 0.78 0.06 2.75 0.79
A: Patience 3.09 0.91 −0.08 3.31 0.95 0.00 3.17 0.91 0.04 3.25 0.93 0.00 3.33 0.92 0.07 3.15 0.92 −0.02 3.36 0.92 0.05 3.17 0.96
C: Organization 3.32 0.85 0.06 3.30 0.90 −0.01 3.35 0.90 0.08 3.34 0.93 0.11 3.27 0.93 0.08 3.33 0.91 0.08 3.39 0.93 0.06 3.28 0.93
C: Diligence 3.70 0.73 0.04 3.85 0.75 −0.02 3.82 0.74 0.10 3.82 0.77 0.14 3.81 0.77 0.10 3.79 0.77 0.07 3.85 0.76 0.09 3.69 0.79
C: Perfectionism 3.51 0.76 0.04 3.62 0.77 −0.02 3.57 0.76 0.06 3.61 0.76 0.08 3.58 0.78 0.04 3.64 0.78 0.02 3.64 0.74 0.03 3.55 0.78
C: Prudence 3.29 0.76 0.03 3.37 0.77 −0.02 3.41 0.76 0.05 3.39 0.78 0.02 3.44 0.78 0.05 3.38 0.81 0.03 3.39 0.78 0.04 3.33 0.79
O: Esthetic 3.51 0.77 0.04 3.95 0.72 0.09 3.56 0.82 0.04 3.57 0.83 0.03 3.60 0.83 0.02 3.61 0.82 0.04 3.57 0.84 0.00 3.44 0.87
Appreciation
O: Inquisitiveness 3.59 0.80 −0.05 3.94 0.77 0.08 3.76 0.84 −0.12 3.71 0.86 −0.09 3.82 0.84 −0.15 3.82 0.81 −0.06 3.47 0.91 −0.15 3.72 0.86
O: Creativity 3.58 0.84 0.00 4.12 0.72 0.06 3.75 0.84 −0.01 3.77 0.85 −0.02 3.79 0.85 −0.01 3.89 0.81 −0.01 3.80 0.86 −0.01 3.73 0.87
O: Unconventionality 3.70 0.65 −0.11 4.10 0.60 0.07 3.72 0.66 −0.12 3.78 0.65 −0.13 3.82 0.66 −0.15 3.82 0.62 −0.11 3.76 0.66 −0.15 3.80 0.67
Interstitial Altruism 3.75 0.68 0.16 3.99 0.69 0.12 3.81 0.72 0.23 3.77 0.73 0.21 3.86 0.74 0.24 3.69 0.74 0.23 3.93 0.69 0.24 3.62 0.79
ASHTON and LEE

Notes. Sex coded as male = 0, female = 1. Denominational category obtained for under 40% of Christian respondents (see text). Religiousness item on a 1‐to‐7 scale; personality variables on a 1‐to‐5 scale.
ASHTON and LEE   
   7
|
sample as a whole as well as Buddhist subsamples consisting We also examined (see Table 1) the associations of reli-
of persons reporting an Asian ethnic background and of per- giousness with the personality variables within the various
sons reporting a non‐Asian ethnic background. religions of our sample (Hindu, Muslim, Jewish, Buddhist,
and Christian) and within the denominational categories of
Christianity (Roman Catholic, Protestant, Eastern Orthodox,
3.2  |  Descriptive statistics
and Other). (Recall that we assessed the denominations within
Also given in Table 1 is the mean level of religiousness for Christianity only after August 2015, so the Christian denom-
the sample as a whole and for each of the religion categories. inational category is available for under 40% of the Christian
For the sample as a whole, the mean level of religiousness participants.) As seen in Table 1, there was some modest ten-
was 2.82 (SD = 1.83). Overall, nearly 40% of the respondents dency for associations to be weaker among the Jewish and
indicated the lowest level of religiousness (i.e., a 1 on the Buddhist (particularly non‐Asian Buddhist) subsamples than
1‐to‐7 scale, indicating “not religious at all”). As would be among the Hindu, Muslim, and Christian subsamples; how-
expected, this lowest level was indicated by the large major- ever, the general trends are similar across all of the religions
ity of persons indicating “none” as their religious affiliation, and all of the Christian denominational categories.
and among all persons who indicated “none,” the mean level One small but interesting difference between the person-
of religiousness was very low (M = 1.33, SD = 0.79). In the ality/religiousness associations for Buddhists of non‐Asian
entire sample, only about 5% of the respondents indicated the ethnic background and for all other religious groupings in-
highest level of religiousness (i.e., a 7 on the 1‐to‐7 scale, volved the Openness to Experience scale: whereas Openness
indicating “extremely religious”). For respondents who indi- generally showed very small negative correlations with re-
cated one of the religious affiliations considered here, mean ligiousness in the various religious groups (rs ranging from
religiousness ranged from 2.64 (SD = 1.64) for Jewish re- −0.04 to −0.14), it actually showed a positive correlation
spondents to 3.80 (SD = 1.72) for Muslim respondents. with religiousness (r = 0.11) among Buddhists of non‐Asian
Table 1 also shows, for the full sample of respondents ethnic background. This result is consistent with the possi-
who answered the religiousness item, the means and stan- bility that Buddhism tends to attract high-Openness converts
dard deviations for sex (coded as male = 0, female = 1), age, from persons in whose cultures Buddhism is not a widely
and the 6 factor‐level scales and 25 facet‐level scales of the observed religion. In the present sample, the mean level of
HEXACO‐100 (see http://hexaco.org/scaledescriptions for Openness for Buddhists of non‐Asian background (M = 4.03,
scale descriptions). Recall that religiousness was assessed SD = 0.50) was considerably higher than that of the full sam-
with a single item using a 1‐to‐7 response scale, whereas the ple (M = 3.70, SD = 0.58) and of any other religious group.
HEXACO scale scores are computed as means across items Other mean‐level personality differences between religious
using a 1‐to‐5 response scale. Alpha reliabilities are also re- groups were generally small and can be attributed in part to
ported for the HEXACO scales; because religiousness was differences between those groups in their age and sex dis-
assessed by a single item, an alpha reliability cannot be cal- tributions within our sample (for age and sex differences in
culated for that variable.2 HEXACO‐PI‐R scale scores, see Ashton & Lee, 2016; Lee
& Ashton, 2018, respectively). We caution readers against
interpreting any small differences that might remain, as our
3.3  |  Religiousness/personality associations
religious group samples may not be equally representative of
for the full sample and by religious affiliation
their respective populations.
In the full sample, correlations of religiousness with person- We now return to the results based on the entire sample.
ality characteristics (see Table 1) were mainly very weak: Even the larger facet‐scale correlations with religiousness
among the six factor‐level scales, Openness was roughly correspond to effect sizes between the conventional thresh-
uncorrelated with religiousness, and the other five factors olds for “small” and “medium” effects. However, even these
showed small positive correlations ranging from 0.09 to 0.13. modest associations reflect rather large differences in these
Most of the 25 facet‐level scales also showed near‐zero or personality variables between the least and most religious
weak associations with religiousness, but religiousness did respondents. Table 2 shows the mean scores for Fairness,
correlate 0.24 with Fairness (a facet of Honesty‐Humility) Altruism, Sentimentality, and Forgivingness for respondents
and 0.23 with Altruism (an “interstitial” facet intended at each of the seven levels of religiousness. As seen in the
to load jointly on Honesty‐Humility, Emotionality, and table, each of these scales showed monotonically increasing
Agreeableness). The only other facets whose correlations means with higher religiousness. In the case of Fairness, the
with religiousness exceeded those yielded by every factor‐ difference in mean scores between persons at the highest and
level scale were Sentimentality (a facet of Emotionality as- lowest religiousness levels (M = 4.27 [SD = 0.85] versus
sessing interpersonal sensitivity and empathy; r = 0.15) and M = 3.30 [SD = 1.09]) was about a full standard deviation
Forgivingness (a facet of Agreeableness; r = 0.15).3,4 unit. The corresponding difference for Altruism was nearly as
|
8       ASHTON and LEE

large (M = 4.22 [SD = 0.63]) versus M = 3.61 [SD = 0.80]). 0.08 for the least religious countries in the present data set
For Forgivingness, the corresponding difference was slightly (Norway, Sweden, Finland, Belgium, Spain, and Denmark,
more than half of a standard deviation unit (M = 2.97 [SD = respectively), whereas it showed correlations of 0.25, 0.24,
0.90]) versus M = 2.42 [SD = 0.86]), and for Sentimentality, 0.28, 0.24, 0.10, and 0.14 for the most religious countries
it was slightly less than half of a standard deviation unit (Pakistan, Philippines, Indonesia, Malaysia, India, and USA,
(M = 3.54 [SD = 0.86] versus M = 3.11 [SD = 0.92]). respectively). As another example, the corresponding cor-
relations for Altruism were 0.17, 0.14, 0.13, 0.14, 0.14, and
0.18 for the least religious countries, and 0.25, 0.39, 0.33,
3.4  |  Religiousness/personality associations
0.30, 0.24, and 0.22 for the most religious countries. It thus
by country
appears that prosociality differs more strongly as a function
We also investigated the religiousness/personality cor- of religiousness within relatively religious countries than
relations within each country to find out whether there are within relatively nonreligious countries.
meaningful cross‐national variations in such relationships, Although Openness to Experience showed a near‐zero
and whether any such cross‐national variations are related correlation with religiousness in the full sample (r = −0.03),
to national‐level religiousness. We operationalized national‐ it tended to show somewhat negative correlations in religious
level religiousness as the mean religiousness self‐rating by countries. For example, Openness/religiousness correlations
participants within each country of our sample. This kind of were 0.02, 0.06, 0.01, 0.03, −0.01, and 0.00 for the least re-
mean score has been found to be highly correlated with a na- ligious countries, whereas those correlations were −0.08,
tional‐level religiousness index externally derived from rep- −0.11, −0.12, −0.06, −0.17, and −0.09 for the most religious
resentative independent samples (Gebauer et al., 2014; Lee countries. This pattern of results was even more pronounced
et al., 2018). for one of the Openness facets, namely Unconventionality
We report the correlations of religiousness with person- (which showed religiousness correlations of −0.04, 0.00,
ality variables for each country of our sample in Table 3, in- −0.04, −0.03, −0.08, and −0.01 for the least religious coun-
cluding only those countries that had at least 300 participants tries vs. −0.14, −0.26, −0.15, −0.15, −0.24, and −0.17 for
in our sample. Religiousness correlations are reported for the the more religious countries). It thus appears that people high
six factor‐level scales as well as a set of five selected facet in Unconventionality (and in Openness, to a lesser degree)
scales. Specifically, Table 3 includes the four facet scales dis- tend to be less religious only in countries that are relatively
cussed earlier in this section and also the Unconventionality religious.5
scale, which showed some interesting cross‐national variation
in its association with religiousness (as well as an interesting
3.5  |  Religious upbringing,
interaction with religious upbringing, to be discussed in the
religiousness, and personality
next subsection). In addition, the correlations of national‐
level religiousness means with the personality/religiousness We also examined the associations of religious upbringing
correlations are shown in the bottom row of Table 3. with the personality variables. Religious upbringing was
In general, the personality/religiousness relationships largely unrelated to personality, showing absolute correla-
tended to be stronger in the more religious countries than in tions of 0.10 or above with only two of the HEXACO‐PI‐R
the less religious countries. As an example, Honesty‐Humility facets—Fairness (r = 0.11) and Altruism (r = 0.13). Overall,
showed correlations of 0.09, 0.03, 0.02, −0.02, −0.01, and religious upbringing correlated with the personality variables

T A B L E 2   Means and standard


Fairness Sentimentality Forgivingness Altruism
deviations of scores on four HEXACO‐PI‐R How religious
facet scales by responses on the are you? N M SD M SD M SD M SD
religiousness scale
1 (Not at all) 76,447 3.30 1.09 3.11 0.92 2.42 0.86 3.61 0.80
2 26,995 3.40 1.02 3.28 0.84 2.49 0.81 3.73 0.70
3 17,917 3.45 1.01 3.32 0.83 2.51 0.83 3.77 0.69
4 37,344 3.59 1.01 3.38 0.82 2.55 0.84 3.85 0.68
5 21,170 3.79 0.96 3.42 0.81 2.67 0.84 3.96 0.65
6 13,254 4.06 0.88 3.47 0.81 2.80 0.85 4.09 0.62
7 (Extremely 6,076 4.27 0.85 3.54 0.86 2.97 0.90 4.22 0.63
religious)
Note. Personality variables are computed as means across items having a 1‐to‐5 response scale.
ASHTON and LEE   
|
   9

about half as strongly as religiousness did; this fact, in con- religiousness and religiousness‐of‐upbringing. When we com-
junction with the fairly high correlation between religious puted these correlations separately among persons who were
upbringing and religiousness (r = 0.47), suggests that reli- (a) more than 0.5 SD above the sample mean and (b) more
gious upbringing has little association with personality inde- than 0.5 SD below the sample mean on Unconventionality,
pendent of religiousness itself. the obtained values were 0.37 and 0.60, respectively. That
To illustrate this situation, we conducted a two‐way is, religiousness was better predicted by religiousness of
ANOVA using religiousness and religious upbringing as in- upbringing among persons low in Unconventionality than
dependent variables and the HEXACO‐PI‐R factor and facet among persons high in Unconventionality.6
scales as dependent variables. The results of this analysis We also conducted some analyses in relation to the find-
showed that religious upbringing had little independent as- ing by McCullough et al. (2003) that religious upbringing
sociation with the personality variables, with no η2 values was more strongly associated with religiousness among less
exceeding 0.001. By contrast, religiousness showed much emotionally stable persons, in their sample of adults who
stronger independent associations with the personality vari- had been identified during childhood as intellectually gifted.
ables, with the η2 values reaching 0.030 for Altruism and We began by computing an ad hoc HEXACO Neuroticism
0.034 for Fairness. (Note that, because of the very large sam- (vs. Emotional Stability) composite scale using a combina-
ple size, even an η2 value of 0.002 reaches statistical signifi- tion of facet scales according to the formula given in Lee
cance at the 0.001 level.) and Ashton (2013). This variable did not show any appre-
This analysis also allows an examination of the interac- ciable religiousness‐by‐religious upbringing interactions
tion between religiousness and religious upbringing in rela- (η2 < 0.001). When we compared the correlations of reli-
tion to the personality variables. Recall that if there are any giousness with religiousness‐of‐upbringing between partici-
personality features that characterize persons of the “amaz- pants who were (a) more than 0.5 SD units above the sample
ing believer” or “amazing apostate” categories, then this mean and (b) more than 0.5 SD units below the sample mean
should be revealed in nontrivial interaction effects. We found on this Neuroticism composite, the obtained values were
that the interaction between religiousness and religious up- very similar (r = 0.46 and r = 0.48, respectively; cf. r = 0.56
bringing was essentially zero for most personality factors vs. r = 0.33 in McCullough et al., 2003). Thus, the finding by
and facets (η2 values below 0.003) but that there were con- McCullough et al. (2003) that religious upbringing was more
siderable interaction effects for the Openness to Experience strongly associated with religiousness among more neurotic
factor (η2 = 0.011) and for all four of its facets, particularly (i.e., less emotionally stable) persons was not well replicated
Unconventionality (η2 = 0.013). (Note that the interaction in our sample.
for the Unconventionality scale was not driven by any single
item; the four items of that scale separately showed η2 values
3.6  |  Religiousness and pro‐out‐
ranging from 0.003 to 0.009.)
group attitudes
The interaction between religiousness and religious up-
bringing in relation to Unconventionality is illustrated by The positive association of religiousness with Altruism might
Figure 1, which shows the mean Unconventionality scores suggest that religiousness should be related to political at-
of persons having each combination of religiousness levels titudes that are associated with Altruism; however, it is also
and religious upbringing levels. Persons having the lowest known that religiousness correlates with a right‐wing politi-
level of religiousness despite the highest level of religious cal orientation, which is actually negatively correlated with
upbringing had a mean Unconventionality score of 4.11 Altruism (Lee et al., 2018). To the extent that political at-
(SD = 0.63), which was considerably higher than that ob- titudes associated positively with Altruism might also be as-
served for persons having the lowest religiousness and low- sociated negatively with right‐wing political orientation, we
est religious upbringing (M = 3.79, SD = 0.68), the highest might expect religiousness to be roughly uncorrelated with
religiousness and lowest religious upbringing (M = 3.84, such attitudes.
SD = 0.68), and (especially) the highest religiousness and Table 4 shows the correlations of pro‐out‐group attitudes
highest religious upbringing (M = 3.62, SD = 0.75). In other with right‐wing political orientation, religiousness, Altruism,
words, religiousness and Unconventionality were negatively and the six HEXACO factor scales. As seen in that table,
related among persons whose upbringing was very religious, pro‐out‐group attitudes were correlated negatively with right‐
but were roughly unrelated among persons whose upbringing wing political orientation (r = −0.49) and positively with
was very nonreligious. altruism (r = 0.43). Religiousness, which was positively cor-
As another way of examining the associations among related with both right‐wing political orientation and altru-
Unconventionality, religiousness, and religious upbringing, ism (in this subsample, r = 0.24 and r = 0.23, respectively),
we compared the subsamples of persons scoring low and was virtually uncorrelated with pro‐out‐group attitudes
high in Unconventionality in terms of the correlation between (r = −0.02).
T A B L E 3   Religiousness descriptive statistics and correlations with HEXACO‐PI‐R factor‐ and (selected) facet‐level scales, by country
|

Religiousness
10      

N M SD H E X A C O H‐Fair E‐Sent A‐Forg O‐Unco Altruism


Pakistan 366 4.27 1.40 0.25 0.00 0.17 0.17 0.16 −0.08 0.31 0.10 0.16 −0.14 0.25
Philippines 1739 4.08 1.64 0.24 0.18 0.30 0.25 0.22 −0.11 0.35 0.25 0.29 −0.26 0.39
Indonesia 407 4.03 1.46 0.28 0.22 0.22 0.23 0.17 −0.12 0.49 0.32 0.22 −0.15 0.33
Malaysia 613 3.57 1.64 0.24 0.21 0.06 0.16 0.11 −0.06 0.36 0.24 0.16 −0.15 0.30
India 2,503 3.45 1.80 0.10 0.26 0.13 0.09 0.02 −0.17 0.20 0.29 0.13 −0.24 0.24
US 84,438 3.20 1.91 0.14 0.09 0.14 0.10 0.09 −0.09 0.24 0.14 0.15 −0.17 0.22
Greece 2,643 3.14 1.78 0.10 0.18 0.11 0.07 0.08 −0.09 0.24 0.21 0.10 −0.15 0.25
Singapore 2,147 3.10 1.72 0.14 0.15 0.11 0.08 0.03 −0.06 0.22 0.17 0.14 −0.15 0.25
Romania 1,040 2.89 1.69 0.14 0.14 0.14 0.06 0.11 −0.04 0.22 0.18 0.11 −0.13 0.22
Slovakia 481 2.81 1.70 0.08 0.12 0.04 0.12 0.04 0.12 0.15 0.10 0.18 −0.03 0.18
Serbia 426 2.79 1.69 0.04 0.22 0.06 −0.10 0.02 0.01 0.16 0.16 −0.02 −0.05 0.15
Mexico 735 2.74 1.72 0.04 0.10 0.15 0.06 0.08 −0.06 0.11 0.19 0.12 −0.14 0.22
Canada 12,774 2.64 1.77 0.10 0.09 0.10 0.09 0.05 −0.05 0.19 0.13 0.13 −0.10 0.21
Hungary 297 2.64 1.73 0.08 0.24 0.16 0.01 −0.02 −0.01 0.16 0.32 0.14 −0.15 0.27
Poland 664 2.61 1.66 0.05 0.14 0.05 0.09 0.01 0.05 0.16 0.15 0.13 0.00 0.18
Croatia 379 2.60 1.67 −0.05 0.03 0.19 −0.04 0.01 −0.05 0.07 0.08 0.00 −0.07 0.19
Brazil 921 2.59 1.73 0.11 0.13 0.17 0.08 −0.03 −0.05 0.19 0.21 0.10 −0.12 0.18
Ireland 1986 2.53 1.62 0.09 0.13 0.14 0.11 0.08 −0.06 0.18 0.12 0.11 −0.10 0.21
New Zealand 2,725 2.50 1.70 0.11 0.09 0.05 0.04 0.06 −0.01 0.19 0.11 0.09 −0.08 0.19
Australia 10,824 2.48 1.71 0.10 0.11 0.06 0.09 0.05 0.01 0.18 0.13 0.11 −0.05 0.18
Italy 1908 2.44 1.64 0.01 0.11 0.12 0.07 0.07 0.02 0.14 0.12 0.10 −0.02 0.14
Japan 309 2.43 1.53 0.10 0.05 0.10 0.03 0.10 0.14 0.09 0.14 0.03 −0.01 0.04
Austria 599 2.38 1.58 0.03 0.11 0.11 −0.01 0.06 0.05 0.17 0.12 0.00 −0.04 0.12
France 1801 2.36 1.60 −0.02 0.06 0.11 0.05 0.06 0.04 0.14 0.09 0.06 0.00 0.15
Czech 340 2.36 1.58 0.07 0.14 0.16 0.05 0.03 0.12 0.13 0.27 0.10 −0.07 0.20
Switzerland 768 2.33 1.58 0.02 0.14 0.04 0.02 0.10 0.03 0.14 0.14 0.05 −0.08 0.13
Portugal 649 2.30 1.61 0.12 0.11 0.14 0.06 0.06 −0.01 0.26 0.13 0.15 −0.10 0.22
UK 40,340 2.30 1.60 0.10 0.11 0.08 0.09 0.06 0.02 0.19 0.13 0.11 −0.03 0.19
Germany 3,331 2.29 1.56 0.06 0.13 0.10 0.06 0.04 0.05 0.19 0.16 0.09 −0.02 0.20
Netherlands 3,771 2.19 1.58 0.10 0.11 0.03 0.07 0.04 0.04 0.17 0.13 0.08 −0.03 0.20
(Continued)
ASHTON and LEE
ASHTON and LEE   
|
   11

Notes. Countries are listed in descending order of mean religiousness. H‐Fair = Honesty‐Humility: Fairness; E‐Sent = Emotionality: Sentimentality; A‐Forg = Agreeableness: Forgivingness; O‐Unco = Openness: Unconventionality.
Altruism
0.18
0.14
0.14
0.13
0.14
0.17
0.76
4.20

4.10
O‐Unco

4.00
−0.01
−0.08
−0.03
−0.04

−0.04
−0.77
0.00

Unconventionality
3.90

3.80

3.70
A‐Forg
0.07
0.06
0.03
0.09
0.00
0.08
0.68 3.60

3.50

Religious Upbringing
7
3.40
E‐Sent

5
0.12
0.18
0.08
0.09
0.10
0.15
0.49

3.30 3
1 2 3 4 1
5 6 7
Religiousness
H‐Fair
0.18
0.09
0.05
0.08
0.14
0.16
0.78

F I G U R E 1   Interaction between religousness and religious


upbringing: unconventionality
−0.01

−0.67
0.00

0.03
0.01
0.06
0.02

The pattern of the correlations described above appears


O

to suggest that religiousness has two different, opposing


paths in influencing pro‐out‐group attitudes, which to-
−0.01

−0.11
0.05
0.11

0.06
0.09
0.57

gether produce a near‐zero association between religious-


C

ness and pro‐out‐group attitudes. To examine this aspect


of the results, we assessed the direct and indirect effects
−0.01

−0.02
0.11
0.00

0.04

0.08
0.67

of religiousness on pro‐out‐group attitudes in a path model


A

where religiousness was set to influence pro‐out‐group at-


titudes via two mediators, namely, right‐wing political ori-
entation and Altruism. The indirect effects of religiousness
0.03
0.15

0.55
0.08
0.06
0.03
0.12
X

were −0.102 via right‐wing political orientation and 0.079


via Altruism; the direct path coefficient was 0.005. Thus,
these results suggest the possibility that religiousness influ-
0.12
0.12
0.09
0.10
0.07
0.12
0.30
E

ences both Altruism and right‐wing political orientation,


which in turn have opposing influences on pro‐out‐group
attitudes, leaving religiousness approximately uncorrelated
−0.01
−0.02
0.08

0.02
0.03
0.09
0.78
H

with those attitudes.

4  |  DISCUSSION
1.46
1.53
1.50
1.41
1.48
1.42
Religiousness

SD

religiousness and personality/religiousness

4.1  |  Comparisons with previous findings


Religiousness item has a 1-to-7 response scale.
2.15
2.15
2.14
2.08
2.05
1.98
M

The sizes of the associations observed in this study between


Correlations between country‐level

religiousness and the broad personality factors were gener-


ally similar to those reported by Gebauer et al. (2014) and
1,262
972
990
882
645

955
T A B L E 3   Continued

Schmitt and Fuller (2015) in their worldwide surveys. For


N

example, we found a zero‐order correlation of 0.09 between


HEXACO Conscientiousness and religiousness, whereas
for Big Five Conscientiousness the correlation was 0.13 in
Schmitt and Fuller (2015) and the beta weight (with other Big
relations
Denmark

Belgium

Norway
Sweden
Finland

Five factors also included as predictors) was 0.10 in Gebauer


Spain

et al. (2014). Likewise, we found that the HEXACO factors


|
12       ASHTON and LEE

related to Big Five Agreeableness—namely, Agreeableness, with religiousness. This latter result suggests that the previ-
Honesty‐Humility, and Emotionality—all correlated mod- ous findings of associations between Big Five Agreeableness
estly with religiousness (rs of 0.11, 0.13, and 0.11, respec- and religiousness might be driven largely by the specifically
tively), whereas for Big Five Agreeableness the correlation “altruistic” content (and perhaps to a lesser extent the spe-
was 0.16 in Schmitt and Fuller and the corresponding beta cifically “forgiving” content) of the Big Five Agreeableness
weight was 0.15 in Gebauer et al. (2014). (Moreover, if scales.
we derive a HEXACO‐based approximation to Big Five
Agreeableness, the resulting scale correlates 0.18 with reli-
4.2  |  The origins of religiousness/personality
giousness in this sample.)7 The present results, and also those
associations
of Gebauer et al. and of Schmitt and Fuller, are thus similar to
those of Saroglou’s (2010) meta‐analysis for the associations The data of this report were based entirely on self‐reports,
of (Big Five) Agreeableness with religiousness (meta‐ana- and therefore cannot resolve the issue of whether the associa-
lytic r = 0.19). However, Saroglou’s meta‐analysis showed tions between religiousness and prosociality represent a real
a somewhat stronger Conscientiousness/religiousness corre- trait difference, such that religious persons truly tend to be
lation (r = 0.16) than that found in the present study and in more prosocial, as opposed to a difference in self‐report bi-
Gebauer et al.’s study, with the Schmitt and Fuller results ases, such that religious persons systematically tend to over-
intermediate. We do not know the reasons for the latter dif- estimate their own prosociality. As noted in the Introduction,
ferences, but even those differences are rather small. however, previous research evidence—as based on cross‐
As noted at the beginning of this report, one novel feature source, self/observer designs and on objectively recorded
of the present research is that it combined a very large in- behavior—suggests that such results do reflect, at least in
ternational sample with personality assessment of narrower, considerable part, a real trait difference. In addition, we note
facet‐level traits as well as broader, factor‐level traits. Our that the procedure of our data collection is unlikely to have
results showed considerable heterogeneity of personality/ induced an artifactual association of the kind that could occur
religiousness associations involving facets belonging to if the religiousness self‐ratings would induce prosocial per-
the same broad factor. Most notably, the Fairness facet (of sonality self‐reports, or vice versa: specifically, we obtained
Honesty‐Humility) correlated 0.24 with religiousness even the religiousness self‐rating in the middle of a demographic
though the corresponding correlations for the other three questionnaire that was administered after the omnibus self‐
facets of Honesty‐Humility ranged only from 0.02 to 0.08, report personality questionnaire. Therefore, there was no
with the overall Honesty‐Humility factor scale yielding a “priming” of religious identity prior to the personality self‐re-
correlation of 0.13. Likewise, the Altruism facet—which ports, and there is little prospect that participants’ responses
occupies an “interstitial” location, loading moderately on to the prosocial personality items would have substantially
Honesty‐Humility, on Agreeableness, and on Emotionality— influenced their subsequent religiousness self‐ratings.
correlated 0.23 with religiousness even though only 3 of the To the extent that there is some real association between
12 facets assigned to those 3 factors correlated above 0.10 religiousness and prosocial personality traits, this association

T A B L E 4   Correlations among pro‐out‐group attitudes, religiousness, political orientation, and personality characteristics

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Pro‐out‐group 4.92 1.48
attitude
2. Religiousness 2.80 1.81 −0.02
3. Right‐wing political 3.64 1.33 −0.49 0.24
orientation
4. Altruism 3.80 0.74 0.43 0.23 −0.19
5. Honesty‐Humility 3.32 0.70 0.25 0.11 −0.13 0.46
6. Emotionality 3.16 0.67 0.22 0.10 −0.19 0.35 0.11
7. Extraversion 3.19 0.73 0.10 0.16 0.05 0.25 −0.01 −0.14
8. Agreeableness 2.99 0.65 0.18 0.11 −0.03 0.41 0.37 −0.11 0.22
9. Conscientiousness 3.50 0.62 0.03 0.13 0.08 0.17 0.17 −0.06 0.26 0.11
10. Openness 3.61 0.63 0.16 −0.03 −0.17 0.15 0.14 −0.09 0.09 0.12 0.08
Notes. N = 7,119. Pro‐out‐group attitude variable is computed as mean across two items having a 1‐to‐7 response scale. Religiousness and right‐wing political orientation
items have a 1‐to‐7 response scale. Personality variables are computed as means across items having a 1‐to‐5 response scale.
ASHTON and LEE   
|
   13

could result from (a) a tendency for religiousness to promote which correlated negatively with pro‐out‐group attitudes
prosociality, or (b) a tendency for prosocial personality traits (and also with Altruism). That is, religiousness was cor-
to predispose persons to be religious. Previous reviews of related positively with both Altruism and right‐wing po-
longitudinal research (e.g., Saroglou, 2010; see also Ashton litical orientation, despite the negative correlation between
& Lee, 2014) have tended to favor the latter explanation. those two variables, and the near‐zero association of reli-
However, some features of the present results seem to be at giousness with pro‐out‐group attitudes fell about halfway
least as consistent with the former explanation as with the in between the associations shown by Altruism (positive)
latter. In particular, we found that religiousness was associ- and right‐wing political orientation (negative) with those
ated specifically with the Fairness facet scale (r = 0.24) but attitudes. Presumably, religions generally encourage their
much less so with any of the other three facet scales within adherents to behave altruistically, but to direct this altru-
Honesty‐Humility (rs ranging from 0.02 to 0.08). This pat- ism toward in‐group members in good standing rather than
tern of results suggests that religiousness is mainly associated toward out‐group members. In this way, the altruistic in-
with the specific variance of Fairness, that is, the variance not clinations of religious persons are neutralized when inter-
attributable to a common Honesty‐Humility dimension (or group altruism is considered (recall the earlier citation to
any other broad dimension). We think that the most plausible Saroglou, 2013, p. 446).
explanation of these findings is that religiousness tends to
increase persons’ levels of the specific variance of Fairness,
4.4  |  Religiousness/personality associations
insofar as the more religious persons would internalize more
across religious affiliations and countries
strongly their religions’ norms against behaviors such as
stealing and cheating. (This account is consistent with experi- A noteworthy feature of our results was that the personal-
mental evidence suggesting that religious priming can induce ity correlates of religiousness were broadly similar across the
some forms of prosocial behavior among religious persons; various religious groups. In particular, religiousness showed
see review by Preston, Salomon, & Ritter, 2014.) small‐to‐medium positive correlations with two “prosocial”
It is also possible that persons who are strongly predis- facet‐level traits—Fairness and Altruism—within each of
posed toward adhering to prosocial norms are consequently those groups. In keeping with our suggested interpretation
motivated to become or to remain religious. However, if above, we interpret this finding as suggesting that all of the
this were entirely responsible for the observed correlation religions considered here share a common tendency to pro-
between religiousness and Fairness, then one would expect mote these prosocial tendencies in their adherents, presum-
the correlations of religiousness with the other Honesty‐ ably through some combination of scriptures, sermons, and
Humility facet scales to be larger than those observed here, broader social norms. Alternatively, it is also possible that
because the shared as well as the unique variance of Fairness the various religions share a common tendency to attract and
would be implicated.8 Nevertheless, it remains plausible that retain prosocial persons as adherents.
Fairness has at least some role in predisposing persons to- The strength of the associations between these person-
ward religiousness. (Similar observations regarding possible ality variables and religiousness did differ somewhat across
directions of influence would also apply to the religiousness/ religions, but the interpretation of those differences is not
Altruism correlations.) obvious. For example, the Fairness/religiousness link was
stronger in Muslims (r = 0.31), in Protestant Christians (r
= 0.27), and in “Other” Christians (r = 0.25) than in Asian
4.3  |  Religiousness, prosocial
Buddhists (r = 0.13) and especially non‐Asian Buddhists (r
personality, and pro‐out‐group attitudes
= 0.07). In the case of non‐Asian Buddhists, we suspect that
An additional issue involves the extent to which any real for this group more than the others, high levels of self‐rated
association between religiousness and prosocial personal- religiousness tend to reflect intense personal religious striv-
ity traits would be manifested in the endorsement of al- ings rather than intense commitment to religious teachings
truistic actions at the intergroup level. We addressed this concerning morality.
issue by examining the associations of religiousness and of Another modest difference in personality/religiousness
prosocial personality traits with attitudes toward foreign aid relations across religions involved the Forgivingness facet:
and toward multicultural immigration. We found that even the Forgivingness/religiousness correlation was higher in
though religiousness correlated positively with Altruism, Christians (r = 0.17; except for Eastern Orthodox Christians,
which in turn correlated positively with these pro‐out‐ r = 0.09) and in Muslims (r = 0.14) than in the other religious
group attitudes, religiousness was uncorrelated with those groupings (all rs < 0.10). The somewhat higher correlations
attitudes. This near‐zero correlation can be understood by for Christians might be interpreted in terms of the emphasis
considering additionally our finding that religiousness also in Christianity on unconditional forgiveness (as opposed to
correlated positively with right‐wing political orientation, forgiveness contingent upon contrition), but this would not
|
14       ASHTON and LEE

explain the similarly high correlations for Muslims or the In contrast to our results for “amazing apostates,” above,
lower correlation for Eastern Orthodox Christians. we did not find any personality variables that predicted re-
With regard to national‐level differences in the person- ligiousness only for persons whose upbringing was nonreli-
ality/religiousness relations, our results showed consistently gious. We suggest that the process of religious conversion
stronger associations in the more religious countries than in among persons of nonreligious background operates largely
the less religious countries. In particular, within relatively reli- independently of personality, and instead depends on a va-
gious countries, the more religious persons tended to be more riety of personal situations such as those documented by
“prosocial” (i.e., higher Fairness, Altruism, Forgivingness, Altemeyer and Hunsberger (e.g., death of a loved one, sub-
Sentimentality) and to some extent more “conforming” (i.e., stance abuse problems, etc.). In other words, “amazing be-
lower Unconventionality) than did the less religious persons, lievers” might emerge as a function of their circumstances
but these tendencies were greatly attenuated within nonreli- rather than of their personalities. Nevertheless, it would be
gious countries. These results thus replicate those of Gebauer interesting to examine whether religious conversion among
et al. (2014), with even sharper differences between coun- persons of nonreligious background could be predicted by
tries, and are consistent with the “sociocultural motives per- other individual difference variables not considered here. For
spective” of those authors. example, one might expect that such “amazing conversions”
would be much less likely among persons who firmly disbe-
lieve in the existence of any supernatural phenomena.
4.5  |  Personality and religiousness as a
An additional result of this study is the non‐replication
function of religious upbringing
of McCullough et al.’s (2003) finding that religiousness
As reported above, self‐rated religiousness was correlated was more strongly predicted by religious upbringing among
rather strongly (r = 0.47) with self‐rated religiousness of up- less emotionally stable persons than among more emotion-
bringing. For most personality characteristics, religiousness ally stable persons. Given that the interaction reported by
showed stronger associations than did religiousness of up- McCullough et al. was based on a large participant sample
bringing, which was not independently associated with those and therefore seems unlikely to be a chance result, we spec-
characteristics. These results are consistent with the possibil- ulate that it might be specific to the particular population
ity that certain personality traits predispose people to develop investigated (i.e., 20th‐century US adults who had been iden-
a higher or lower level of religiousness than that of their up- tified as intellectually gifted during childhood).
bringing (as considered in the subsection on The Origins of
Religiousness/Personality Associations). Moreover, these
4.6  | Limitations
results also suggest that any influence of religious upbring-
ing on personality trait levels does not long outlast any later Several limitations of the present research should be noted.
abandonment of religion. For example, the participants in our study who belong to a
For most personality characteristics, the associations particular religious group will not be representative of the
with religiousness did not vary as a function of religious population of members of that religious group. Also, the
upbringing. However, for Openness to Experience and its (rather small) differences between religious groups observed
facets—especially Unconventionality—the associations in this study will be confounded somewhat with differences
with religiousness did depend on religious upbringing. In between countries. In addition, our reliance on a single reli-
particular, the associations of Openness (and especially giousness item will somewhat attenuate the personality/reli-
Unconventionality) with religiousness were clearly nega- giousness associations (recall footnote 2) and also precludes
tive for persons whose upbringing was very religious, but any further exploration of the contrasts between a spiritual,
close to zero for persons whose upbringing was very non- mystical style of religion and a traditional, fundamental-
religious. (Framed another way, the correlations of reli- ist style of religion (see, e.g., Saroglou, 2010; Saucier &
giousness with religious upbringing were stronger among Skrzypińska, 2006).
persons lower in Unconventionality than among persons
higher in Unconventionality.) That is, the “amazing apos-
tates” (Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1997) as a group appear 5  |  CONCLUSIONS
to be characterized by Unconventionality and Openness,
especially in comparison with their counterparts who have Our study of personality and religiousness in a large online
maintained the high religiousness of their upbringing. We sample suggests that personality/religiousness associations,
suggest that the most plausible interpretation of this result as assessed through self‐reports, tend to be rather modest.
is that a predisposition to be unconventional makes people The strongest associations involve facet‐level personal-
who are raised to be very religious less likely to maintain ity variables associated with prosocial tendencies, particu-
that religiousness in adulthood. larly Fairness and Altruism, on which the most religious
ASHTON and LEE   
   15
|
participants averaged about 1 SD higher than did the least 6
An anonymous reviewer suggested conducting a moderated multiple
religious participants. Also, among persons whose upbring- regression analysis regressing religiousness on religious upbringing,
ing was very religious, religiousness was negatively related unconventionality, and their cross‐product term. The interaction term
increased the squared multiple correlation from 0.233 to 0.243 (i.e.,
to Unconventionality, a result not observed among persons
ΔR2 = 0.010). See the Supplementary Analysis for the full results,
whose upbringing was very nonreligious. Religiousness/
including a figure.
personality associations showed little variation between reli- 7
We computed this variable as the unit‐weighted sum of the four fac-
gious affiliations (Hinduism, Islam, Judaism, Buddhism, and
ets from Honesty‐Humility, the four facets from Agreeableness, the
several branches of Christianity) but did tend to be stronger Sentimentality facet from Emotionality, and the Altruism interstitial
within countries having higher average levels of religious- facet.
ness. Despite the positive association of religiousness with 8
In this sample, Fairness correlated 0.45 with Sincerity, 0.39 with Greed
Altruism, religiousness was not associated with pro‐out‐ Avoidance, and 0.41 with Modesty, but the correlations of religious-
group attitudes (i.e., support for multicultural immigration ness with those latter three facets were only 33%, 7%, and 25% as large
and for foreign aid). as the correlation of religiousness with Fairness.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
ORCID
The authors thank Mike Edmonds for assistance with online
Michael C. Ashton  https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4711-8199
data collection.

CONFLICT OF INTERESTS R E F E R E NC E S
Aghababaei, N., Blachnio, A., Arji, A., Chiniforoushan, M., Tekke, M.,
The authors have received royalties for the nonacademic use
& Merhabadi, A. F. (2016). Honesty‐Humility and the HEXACO
of the HEXACO‐PI‐R. structure of religiosity and well‐being. Current Psychology, 35,
421–426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9310-5
Aghababaei, N., Wasserman, J. A., & Nannini, D. (2014). The re-
ENDNOTES
ligious person revisited: Cross‐cultural evidence from the
1
Considerable research has examined the associations of (trait) forgiv- HEXACO model of personality structure. Mental Health, Religion
ingness with religiousness, typically outside the context of general and Culture, 17, 24–29. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2012.7
personality/religiousness associations. This line of research has shown 37771
self‐rated religiousness to be correlated with self‐reported forgivingness Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (1997). Amazing conversions: Why
(Davis, Worthington, Hook, & Hill, 2013), though not necessarily to some turn to faith and others abandon religion. New York, NY:
self‐reported forgivingness for specific past transgressions. This contrast, Prometheus Books.
sometimes referred to as a “forgiveness‐religion discrepancy,” seems to Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2014). Personality and religiousness. In V.
be attributable in large part to the psychometric limitations of single‐ Saroglou (Ed.), Religion, personality, and social behavior (pp. 31–
transgression measures of forgivingness (McCullough & Worthington, 45). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
1999; Tsang, McCullough, & Hoyt, 2005). Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2016). Age trends in HEXACO‐PI‐R self‐re-
2
We expect that the reliabilities of this single item would likely be at least ports. Journal of Research in Personality, 64, 102–111. https://doi.
0.70, given the findings of previous research. For example, Saucier & org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.08.008
Skrzypińska (2006) reported that a single‐item religiousness measure (a Davis, D. E., Worthington Jr., E. L., Hook, J. N., & Hill, P. C. (2013).
self‐rating on the adjective, “religious”) correlated 0.78 with a multi‐item Research on religion/spirituality and forgiveness: A meta‐analytic
religiousness measure that was administered nine years later. review. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 5, 233–241.
3 Gebauer, J. E., Bleidorn, W., Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., Lamb, M.
When we regressed religiousness on the 6 factor‐level scales and on the 25
E., & Potter, J. (2014). Cross‐cultural variations in Big Five relation-
facet‐level scales, the squared multiple correlation achieved by the facet‐
ships with religiosity: A sociocultural motives perspective. Journal
level scales was noticeably larger than that achieved by the factor‐level
of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 1064–1091. https://doi.
scales (0.115 vs. 0.053).
org/10.1037/a0037683
4
Correlations of the HEXACO‐100 items with religiousness are provided Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2013). Prediction of self‐and observer re-
in the Supplementary Table. port scores on HEXACO‐60 and NEO‐FFI scales. Journal of
5
At the request of an anonymous reviewer, we conducted a series of mod- Research in Personality, 47, 668–675. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
erated multiple regression analyses in which religiousness was predicted jrp.2013.06.002
by a personality variable, by dummy variables representing countries, Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2018). Psychometric properties of
and by their interaction terms (i.e., dummy variables × personality). the HEXACO‐100. Assessment, 25, 543–556. https://doi.
The ΔR2 associated with the interaction term block was largest for the org/10.1177/1073191116659134
Unconventionality facet (0.005), followed by the Openness factor and Lee, K., Ashton, M., Griep, Y., & Edmonds, C. (2018). Personality,
the Fairness facet (both 0.003), with all the others falling below 0.002. Religion, and Politics: An investigation in 33 countries. European
|
16       ASHTON and LEE

Journal of Personality, 32, 100–115. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Saroglou, V. (2017). Culture, personality, and religiosity. In A. T.
per.2142 Church (Ed.), The Praeger handbook of personality across cultures:
McCullough, M. E., Tsang, J. A., & Brion, S. (2003). Personality Vol. 2. Culture and characteristic adaptations (pp. 153–184). Santa
traits in adolescence as predictors of religiousness in early adult- Barbara, CA: Praeger.
hood: Findings from the Terman Longitudinal Study. Personality Saucier, G., & Skrzypińska, K. (2006). Spiritual but not religious?
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 980–991. https://doi. Evidence for two independent dispositions. Journal of Personality,
org/10.1177/0146167203253210 74, 1257–1292. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00409.x
McCullough, M. E., & Worthington, E. L. Jr (1999). Religion and Schmitt, D. P., & Fuller, R. C. (2015). On the varieties of human sexual
the forgiving personality. Journal of Personality, 67, 1141–1164. experience: Cross‐cultural links between religiosity and human mat-
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.00085 ing strategies. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 7, 314–326.
Preston, J. L., Salomon, E., & Ritter, R. S. (2014). Religious prosoci- Tsang, J. A., McCullough, M. E., & Hoyt, W. T. (2005). Psychometric
ality: Personal, cognitive, and social factors. In V. Saroglou (Ed.), and rationalization accounts of the religion‐forgiveness dis-
Religion, personality, and social behavior (pp. 149–169). New crepancy. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 785–805. https://doi.
York, NY: Psychology Press. org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00432.x
Saroglou, V. (2010). Religiousness as a cultural adaptation of basic traits:
A five‐factor model perspective. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 14, 108–125. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309352322 SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Saroglou, V. (2011). Believing, bonding, behaving, and belong-
Additional supporting information may be found online in
ing: The big four religious dimensions and cultural variation.
Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 42, 1320–1340. https://doi.
the Supporting Information section at the end of the article.  
org/10.1177/0022022111412267
Saroglou, V. (2012). Is religion not prosocial at all? Comment on
Galen (2012). Psychological Bulletin, 138, 907–912. https://doi. How to cite this article: Ashton MC, Lee K.
org/10.1037/a0028927 Religiousness and the HEXACO personality factors
Saroglou, V. (2013). Religion, spirituality, and altruism. In K. I. and facets in a large online sample. Journal of
Pargament (Ed.), APA handbook of psychology, religion, and Personality. 2019;00:1–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/
spirituality: Vol. 1. Context, theory, and research (pp. 439–457). jopy.12459
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

You might also like