You are on page 1of 13

Literacy Volume Number xxxx 2023 1

The roots of reading for pleasure:


Recollections of reading and current habits
Manzar Zare , Stephanie Kozak , Monyka L. Rodrigues and
Sandra Martin-Chang

Abstract The Matthew effect in reading


Children’s early literacy experiences are critical, yet it The Matthew effect (Stanovich, 1986) has been
remains unclear whether memories of early reading
summarised as the rich get richer and the poor get poorer.
instruction continue to be associated with reading
habits into adulthood. We examined the association
A ‘fast start’ (Sparks et al., 2014, p. 207) into the literate
between recollections of reading experiences and world is associated with reading for pleasure, the
present-day reading habits in an adult population. benefits of which accrue over the lifetime (Allington
University students responded in writing to three and McGill-Franzen, 2021; Martin-Chang et al., 2021).
open-ended prompts asking about their memories of However even when children get off to a ‘slow start’
reading during early childhood, elementary school (characterised by difficult decoding and/or unsuccess-
and high school. They also completed two question- ful comprehension), those that can be enticed to read
naires inquiring about reading enjoyment and during their free time will show steeper trajectories in
frequency in elementary school and high school. For skill development (Cunningham and Stanovich, 1997).
the concurrent measures of reading, participants Students’ reading abilities ultimately lie at the
described their current reading habits in an
crossroads between their aptitudes and their encoun-
open-ended prompt and completed an author
recognition test. Results showed positive links
ters with texts and other expert readers (Carreker
between favourable memories of reading during et al., 2007). Thus far, the bulk of the research has
elementary and high school years and present-day focused on children’s innate potential and their
reading habits. Conversely, unfavourable memories exposure to print (Torppa et al., 2019). Although it is
during high school were associated with unenthusias- recognised that perceived instructional quality is also
tic present-day reading habits. We found that reading paramount to reading motivation (Guay et al., 2019;
instruction in school forms long-lasting memories, Locher et al., 2021), few studies have examined
and these memories are linked in meaningful ways whether adults remember their previous classroom
with print exposure during adulthood. literacy experiences. Here, we aimed to better
understand the role of students’ lasting subjective
Key words: Autonomy Support, Reading Instruction,
experiences across school levels in developing their
Reading Motivation, Print Exposure
desire to read.
The scientific community has known for over three
decades that recreational reading is associated with a
host of positive outcomes (Stanovich and West, 1989; Classroom practices
Martin-Chang et al., 2021). It is also well established
that implementing research-based classroom practices There is no question that the home literacy
in schools is critical for developing reading habits environment significantly impacts children’s literacy
during childhood (e.g. De Naeghel et al., 2014). development (see Sénéchal and Lefevre, 2014).
However, it is less clear whether these early reading Similarly, the school literacy environment curated by
experiences leave lasting impressions and whether teachers also carries great sway over children’s reading
they are associated with the tendency to read for skills and motivation (Daisey, 2010). Applegate and
pleasure into adulthood. Here, we examined relations Applegate (2004) found that teachers who are not avid
between concurrent reading habits of university readers themselves, sometimes lack the enthusiasm
students and their most salient memories from learn- that is instrumental in sharing a love of reading with
ing to read to reading in high school (Garces-Bacsal their students. They called this the Peter effect, referring
et al., 2018; Vanden Dool and Simpson, 2021). to the parable about being unable to give what one is

© 2023 The Authors. Literacy published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of United Kingdom Literacy Association.
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any medium, provided the
original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
2 RECOLLECTIONS OF READING

inherently lacking. When viewed through the Recollections of past experiences


opposite lens, individuals can liberally share what they
hold in abundance. From this perspective, teachers Students who benefit from having enthusiastic and
who have positive attitudes and pedagogical knowl- knowledgeable teachers might be expected to
edge are better positioned to nurture leisure reading have higher levels of print exposure later in life
in their students (Vanden Dool and Simpson, 2021). (Binks-Cantrell et al., 2012; Applegate et al., 2014).
For example, Garces-Bacsal et al. (2018) noted that One method of examining possible links between
teachers, who described themselves as being ‘devoted’ formative school reading experiences and adult leisure
readers, were far more likely to respond to questions reading habits is to ask students what they remember
about motivating students to read in their classrooms. about past reading-related events. Notably, some
They were also more likely to share books and recom- scholars (e.g. Norman, 2009; Kahneman, 2010) have
mend their favourite titles with students. suggested that memories (what we recall) should be
Binks-Cantrell et al. (2012) discuss the Peter effect in distinguished from actual past experiences (what
relation to content knowledge, pointing out that actually happened) because even if memories are
teachers with more content knowledge have stronger not verbatim factual reports, people nevertheless
pedagogical skills to foster reading development in inform their decisions based on their recollections
their students. Thus far, much of the work in teacher of the past, irrespective of what transpired (Braun
knowledge has been focused on the basic building et al., 2018).
blocks of literacy, but recently more inquiry has been Emotions are essential for shaping memories;
aimed at understanding teachers’ preparation to people remember experiences that include strong
increase reading motivation in their students (Kozak emotions more vividly and with more details than
and Martin-Chang, 2019). events without strong emotional ties (Strijbosch
Self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 2000) et al., 2019). Having happy or unpleasant emotions
dictates that to be intrinsically motivated, students about experiences affects new choices. When individ-
need to feel connected, competent and autonomous. uals have positive memories of an experience, they
Thus, recreational reading should increase when are more likely to repeat it. The opposite is also true.
individuals feel connected to other readers and/or Activities that are connected to negative or unpleasant
fictional characters, capable of understanding the text, memories are likely to be avoided (Redelmeier and
and self-directed in their reading choices or Kahneman, 1996).
assignments. Accordingly, De Naeghel et al. (2016) Therefore, listening to adults’ positive and negative
found that reminding teachers to increase time for experiences can help researchers and educators
students to talk (i.e. relatedness), allow more indepen- determine which classroom practices leave lasting
dent work (i.e. autonomy) and provide students with impressions on students, both good and bad. Knowing
optimal challenges (i.e. competence) was associated which activities have impacted students’ memories
with increased student reading motivation. These can inform classroom practices to create memories of
findings suggest that classroom practices are related enjoyable reading experiences. In doing so, they might
to students’ ongoing reading habits as they develop also be able to prevent negative experiences that
into adolescents. discourage students from future reading.
In a similar vein, Fisher and Frey (2018) conducted a Here, we used a retrospective correlational design
teacher intervention where they stressed the impor- to examine whether memories of learning to read and
tance of giving students access to books, choice of reading practices in school are associated with reading
reading materials, time for book discussions and the habits into adulthood.
opportunity to learn about new books through book
talks. Twelve weeks later, 93% of the teachers in
Grades 1, 3 and 5 indicated that the programme Method
significantly impacted students’ extracurricular
reading. In addition, participating families reported Research questions
reading more books compared to the past, and library
circulation increased from the previous year. The This study sought to answer three research questions:
authors concluded that teachers could influence (1) Do memories of reading in elementary school and
students’ exposure to print by adopting targeted in high school correlate with reading habits in
practices within their classrooms. However, it is still adulthood? (2) Are university students’ levels of print
unknown whether these strategies are remembered exposure associated with how frequently they read?
by students beyond the school years, and if so, (3) Are university students’ levels of print exposure
whether they are associated with concurrent print associated with how much they report enjoying
exposure into adulthood. reading?

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Literacy Volume Number xxxx 2023 3

Participants Materials
Memories of reading. Participants were asked to provide
The host university granted ethical approval for the a written response to four open-ended prompts re-
study. Social media posts were used to recruit garding their memories of reading. The prompts asked
participants. Surveys were collected with LimeSurvey about participants’ memories of reading in chronolog-
GmbH (Version 3.23.1, 2020) over 3 weeks in 2020 ical order from learning to read to current reading
when all data were being collected online due to habits. Prompt 1 (learning to read) stated:
COVID-19. Note that Limesurvey assigns a participant
number each time the link is accessed. Thus, an initial Please take a few minutes to write down your most
triage was done to separate surveys from the piloting salient memories of learning to read. Feel free to answer
and revision stages; then, another triage was done to with the first thing that comes to mind. (This should take
isolate the completed surveys from those that had 2–3 minutes. A good guideline is 1–2 paragraphs. Don’t
merely been opened. According to this criterion, worry about spelling or grammar.)
77 surveys were completed. Surveys were excluded
if they were duplications of other submissions In the subsequent prompts, ‘Learning to read’ was
(e.g., participants’ exact information was included replaced with ‘Reading during elementary school’
twice) or if they were nonsensical (e.g., filled with (Prompt 2) and ‘Reading during high school’
random letter strings). The resulting sample consisted (Prompt 3), respectively. For current reading habits
of 67 surveys that were complete and unique. (Prompt 4), participants were asked, ‘How would
The final sample was drawn from 12 universities you describe your leisure reading habits now?’ These
across North America. A power analysis was open-ended questions inquired only about partici-
conducted a priori with G*Power (Faul et al., 2007); it pants’ most salient memories and resulted in a wide
is a software used to estimate the sample size based range of responses ranging between highly positive
on the required power to detect an effect between and highly negative.
variables of interest. An estimated sample size of Deductive coding (Kuckartz, 2014) was used to
42 participants was required to achieve a medium analyse the written responses to the prompts. Each
effect size of .30 with .80 power and alpha set to .05. submission was examined by phrase or meaningful
Therefore, our sample size of 67 was deemed statement (a group of words forming a conceptual
sufficient. At the time of data collection, 4.5% of the unit). Statements were sorted based on their valence,
participants were pursuing a diploma, 58.2% a as being either Favourable or Unfavourable and then in
bachelor’s degree, 32.8% a master’s degree, and 4.5% relation to the three components of self-determination
a doctoral degree. Approximately 73% of participants theory: Autonomy (the perception of choice and
were enrolled in the humanities and social sciences, willingly endorsing one’s behaviour), Competence
while the remainder were enrolled in natural science (experience of developing mastery in one’s activity)
and engineering. and Relatedness (feeling connected and a sense of
Most of our sample identified as female (77%), and belongingness with others, in this case to both real
the remaining identified as male. Participants’ age people and fictional characters) (Deci and Ryan, 2000).
ranged from 19 to 41 years (M = 24.94, SD = 4.72). Statements that did not contain enough information to
The majority of participants (65.7%) identified as sort into the self-determination categories were
White (e.g. Italian, Irish, Greek), 29.2% identified as categorised as General. After these eight categories
Asian (e.g. Chinese, Indian, Pakistani, Syrian, Arab, were established, no new categories emerged (Fusch
Lebanese, Iranian), and the remaining (5.1%) identified and Ness, 2015; for data saturation, see also Francis
as Black (e.g. Caribbean, African American). et al., 2010; Vasileiou et al., 2018). Table 1 contains
All participants reported English as their dominant further details and examples.
language. More than 80% of the participants reported Additional coding units for each subcategory
speaking a second language. As is typical in the region included the use of modifiers, repetition and emphasis.
where the study took place, French was listed as the For example, ‘I really liked reading’ was counted as
most common second language spoken, consisting of two instances of General Favourable: one for ‘liked
83.6% of all second languages spoken. A quarter of reading’ and an additional tally for ‘really’. No
the participants reported speaking more than two further categorisation was possible because the
languages. Other languages spoken were Arabic participant did not indicate why they liked it, only that
(6%), Mandarin (6%), Urdu (3%), Romanian (3%), they did.
Constance (1.5%), Farsi (1.5%), Greek (1.5%), Gujarati Notably, any statement that referred to having or
(1.5%), Italian (1.5%), Tamil (1.5%) and Naval building autonomy, competence or relatedness were
Chaudhary (1.5%). coded as being favourable, whereas statements that

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
4 RECOLLECTIONS OF READING

Table 1: Coding Categories and Valence for Statements Written in Response to the Prompts with Description and Examples.
Category and Valence Description Example

Competence
Favourable - Descriptions of practicing reading ‘I was learning to make metaphors…’
- Noting reading instruction or materials ‘I could read thick novels’
- Positively mentioning own reading ‘I was one of the best’
skills ‘We had many book essays’
- Descriptions of competence ‘I thought my world widened’
- Discussing benefits of reading
Unfavourable - Negatively mentioning own reading ‘Even though I was in grade 3 or 4 I was
skills probably at a kindergarten level’
- Feeling insecure about reading abilities ‘Everyone in my class was so much
- Mentioning reading difficulties better than me and that did impact my
- Lacking reading comprehension confidence’
‘It was always something about the text
that I probably didn’t understand’
Autonomy
Favourable - Engaging in reading-related activities ‘Most of my spare time at home was
by choice spent reading’
- Having choice in selecting reading ‘I wanted to read the books that I was
materials interested in …’
‘I remember having free time to read in
school’
Unfavourable - Lack of choice in reading activities ‘I remember trying to do anything to
- Choosing not to read avoid reading the books assigned in
- Lack of choice in reading materials class’
- Lack of time to read due to work/ ‘We definitely didn’t read enough books
school that suited our genre tastes as teenagers’
Relatedness
Favourable - Positive relationship between ‘I remember learning to read with my
participant and other people shaped mom’
because of reading activities ‘I often shed tears for the joys and
- Feeling attached to books or characters sorrows of the protagonists in the novels’
Unfavourable - Negative relationships between ‘I remember really not liking the special
students and others around reading ed teacher who would work one on one
activities with me’
- Feeling disconnected from books, ‘I haven’t found a book I can identify
reading, or stories with in a long time’
General
Favourable - Demonstrated positive affection or ‘loved reading’, ‘obsessed with reading’,
emotions ‘reading is my favourite’
Unfavourable - Demonstrated negative feelings and ‘hated reading’, ‘reading was stressful’,
emotions ‘reading was boring’

referenced being denied or lacking these qualities were statement that amplified the first. In contrast, ‘I found
coded as unfavourable. For example, ‘We would reading difficult’ indicated lacking skill and was there-
practice reading every single day. I found reading fore coded as Unfavourable Competence. In this sense,
difficult’ would be coded as two instances of Favourable and Unfavourable can be conceptualised as
Favourable Competence and one instance of Unfavourable being favourable or unfavourable conditions for foster-
Competence. ‘We would practice reading’ was deemed ing autonomy, competence or relatedness.
as building skill and was therefore coded as compe- The categories (Autonomy, Competence, Relatedness,
tence, and ‘every single day’ was counted as a second General) were summed to form a favourable and

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Literacy Volume Number xxxx 2023 5

unfavourable score for each of the four-time prompts. averaged to create the elementary reading frequency
A higher score indicated responses that contained score and the elementary reading enjoyment score.
more favourable or unfavourable coded statements in The highest score that could be obtained in each
relation to the period being remembered. measure was 5 (e.g. 5 × 9/9).
A second rater independently coded 30% of the The format was repeated for high school reading
responses to the prompts. Cohen’s κ indicated frequency and enjoyment but with an additional five
substantial agreement between the two raters, questions about non-fiction books. This resulted in a
κw = .78, P < .001 (Laerd Statistics, 2019). No new total of 14 questions. The same scale and procedure
categories emerged during the secondary coding was used for scoring the frequency and enjoyment
process. portions (e.g. 5 × 14/14).
Print exposure. Participants’ print exposure was The data were inspected to ensure that the inclusion
assessed using a version of the Author Recognition of zeros for ‘I don’t remember’ was not artificially
Test (ART; Stanovich and West, 1989), adapted by lowering the scores; less than 3% of all responses fell
Kozak and Martin-Chang (2019) to include authors of in this category. Running the analyses excluding the
children’s and young adult literature. The ART is a participants who selected this response did not impact
proxy of print exposure over the lifespan (Moore and any of the findings; thus, all participants were
Gordon, 2014), which has come to be an indication of included in the analyses to follow.
leisure reading or reading for pleasure (Stanovich
and West, 1989). It has repeatedly demonstrated
high internal consistency ranging from α = .75 to .89 Procedure
(e.g. Mol and Bus, 2011). Participants were given a list
of names and asked to select those they knew to be real On average, participants completed all tasks in a
authors. Embedded foils served to detect guessing. 20-min session. The order of the tasks was held
The proportion of incorrectly identified foils was constant: Consent, Prompt 1, Demographics, Prompt 2,
subtracted from the proportion of correctly identified ART, Prompt 3, the Elementary School Reading
authors to determine the final score. Frequency and Enjoyment Questionnaire, the High
Reading Frequency and Enjoyment Questionnaire. The School Reading Frequency and Enjoyment Question-
Reading Enjoyment and Frequency Questionnaire naire and Prompt 4. It was suggested that the prompts
elicited responses to activities during elementary be answered in 2–3 min using one or two paragraphs;
school and high school. These complemented the however, no time or word limits were set.
responses to the prompts because rather than being
open-ended, the questionnaire gave participants the
chance to respond to a set of uniform experiences that Results
may not have come to mind immediately as being
salient. Preliminary analyses
Nine items asked about reading enjoyment and
frequency during elementary school. Participants The average word length for the prompts was 77.43
reported whether they remembered: teachers reading words; each of the 67 participants completed all
to them, reading themselves in school, reading in their prompts resulting in approximately 20,636 words to
free time, talking about books in small groups, having be analysed. Within these responses, there were 1781
classroom discussions about books, teachers suggest- statements coded as being either favourably (1394) or
ing fiction books, teachers suggesting non-fiction unfavourably (387) related to literacy (see Figure 1).
books, teachers sharing their own reading preferences To gain a sense of whether the statements were
and teachers giving them the choice over what they divided equally among the four self-determination
read.1 Participants also rated how frequently these derived categories, we ran two repeated measure
activities occurred (1 = never to 5 = very often) and ANOVAs for the favourable and unfavourable
how much they enjoyed them (1 = disliked a lot to recollections.
5 = liked a lot). Overall, each of the nine questions were For the favourable statements, a 3 (Prompt: learning
asked twice: once to assess frequency and once to assess to read, elementary, high school) × 4 (Category:
enjoyment. An option of ‘I don’t remember’ (0) was also competence, autonomy, relatedness, general)
included for the frequency measure because of the within-subject ANOVA showed main effects for
retrospective nature of the question. Responses were both prompt, F(2,132) = 3.82, P = .02, η2p = .06, and
1
category, F(3,198) = 33.43, P < .001, η2p = .34. The
We also asked about having access to books in school, but this ques-
tion was uniformly answered positively and was hence excluded Prompt × Category interaction was also significant,
from all further analyses. F(6,396) = 5.14, P < .001, η2p = .07. Post hoc comparisons

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
6 RECOLLECTIONS OF READING

Figure 1: Mean Number of Positive Statments Written in Response to the Prompts as a Function of Category and Time Period
Being Recalled.

with Bonferroni corrections revealed that participants Turning to the unfavourable statements, we ran a 3
shared more positive statements about competence (Prompt) × 4 (Category) within-subjects ANOVA.
while learning to read compared to what they It revealed a significant main effect for prompt,
said about high school (P = .04). Participants F(2,132) = 7.60, P < .001, η2p = .10, no significant effect
wrote more favourable comments about having
for category, F(3,198) = 2.23, p = .09, η2p = .03, and a
autonomy in elementary school and in high school
compared to when they were learning to read (both significant Prompt × Category interaction,
P’s < .001). No other pairwise comparisons were F(6, 396) = 3.31, P = .003, η2p = .05. Post hoc comparisons
significant. with Bonferroni corrections showed that participants

Figure 2: Mean Number of Negative Statements Written in Response to the Prompts as a Function of Category and Time Pe-
riod Being Recalled.

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Literacy Volume Number xxxx 2023 7

shared more negative statements about autonomy (Prompt 1–3). The main effect of the valence was
while remembering high school compared to remem- significant, F(1,64) = 85.70, P < .001, η2p = .57, indicating
bering their experiences learning to read (P < .001) that participants were more likely to share favourable
and more negative experiences regarding autonomy statements about past reading experiences compared
when remembering high school than elementary to unfavourable statements. By contrast, the main
school (P = .03). Finally, with regard to the statements effect of prompt was not significant, F(2,128) = 1.99,
that could not be divided according to P = .14, η2p = .032. This was qualified by a significant
self-determination subcategories and were thus put in interaction between Valence and Prompt,
the general subcategory, participants wrote fewer
F(2,128) = 7.89, P < .001, η2p = .11. Post hoc comparisons
negative comments about learning to read than either
elementary school (P = .005) or high school (P = .009). with Bonferroni corrections in place showed that
No other pairwise comparisons were significant favourable statements significantly decreased from
(see Figure 2). elementary school to high school (P = .014). There
was also a significant increase in unfavourable
We were interested in students’ lasting subjective
statements from learning to read to high school
experiences when prompted about salient memories
(P < .001).
from various school levels; thus, the categories were
summed to create a favourable and unfavourable
composite for learning to read, elementary school and
high school (see Table 2). A two-way repeated- Memories about past reading experiences and
measures ANOVA was conducted with Valence present-day reading habits
(Favourable and Unfavourable) and Prompts
The first research question sought to determine
whether memories of learning to read or reading in el-
Table 2: The Number of Statements Written in Response to ementary school and high school correlated with read-
the Prompts as a Function of Valance and Time Period, the ing habits in adulthood. Because not all variables were
ART, Reading Frequency, Reading Enjoyment during normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk’s test, P = .05),
Elementary and High School. Kendall’s tau correlation (rτ), a non-parametric
Measures M Min–Max SD correlation test, was used to assess links between the
variables of interest.
Learning to read (Prompt 1)a Positive associations were found between the ART
Favourable 6.22 0–19 4.46 scores and participants’ favourable statements
Unfavourable 0.72 0–10 1.68 about reading in elementary school and high school
Elementary (Prompt 2)a (see Table 3). This suggests that the more fondly adults
Favourable 6.58 0–22 4.83 wrote about their reading experiences during elemen-
Unfavourable 1.56 0–15 2.58 tary and high school, the more authors they recognised
High school (Prompt 3)a on the ART. In contrast, participants’ ART scores were
Favourable 5.06 0–16 3.60 not correlated with the number of unfavourable state-
Unfavourable 2.49 0–17 3.86 ments they made in response to any of the time points
Current reading habits or to the favourable statements they made about learn-
(Prompt 4)a ing to read.
Favourable 2.57 0–14 2.52 We were also interested in whether statements
Unfavourable 0.93 0–7 1.50 made about reading experiences across schooling
Print exposureb,c (ART) 0.16 .08.64 .19 levels correlated with participants self-described read-
Elementary school ing habits (Prompt 4). First, examining the favourable
Reading frequency 3.11 1–4.78 .80 responses, results indicated positive correlations
School reading enjoyment 3.60 1–5.00 .80 between each schooling level and responses about
High school current, self-described reading habits. These findings
Reading frequency 3.05 1–4.64 .82 suggest that individuals who described their current
School reading enjoyment 3.29 1–4.86 .72 reading habits favourably also wrote more compli-
mentary statements about their memories from all
ART, Author Recognition Test. other time periods.
a
Based on the number of coded statements. Regarding the unfavourable statements, only two
b
Higher scores indicate higher exposure to print. significant associations were noted. First, a significant
c
The pattern of the results remained the same when positive correlation was observed between responses
participants with the negative ART scores were to Prompt 4 (unfavourable current reading habits)
removed. and responses to Prompt 3 (unfavourable high school).

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
8 RECOLLECTIONS OF READING

Table 3: Correlations Between Favourable and Unfavourable Statements Written in Response to the Prompts and Current
Reading Habits.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. ART -
2. Favourable: Learning to read (Prompt 1) .11 -
3. Favourable: Elementary (Prompt 2) .18* .36** -
4. Favourable: High School (Prompt 3) .18* .33** .32** -
5. Favourable: Current (Prompt 4) .32** .21* .29** .23* -
6. Unfavourable: Learning to read (Prompt 1) .09 .17 .11 .01 .05 -
7. Unfavourable: Elementary (Prompt 2) .14 .06 .08 .01 .00 .27* -
8. Unfavourable: High School (Prompt 3) .01 .06 .29** .03 .09 .39** .22* -
9. Unfavourable: Current (Prompt 4) .14 .04 .05 .00 .34** .21* .14 .39**

ART, Author Recognition Test.


*
P < .05.
**
P < .01.
***
P < .001.

Second, as expected, a negative association was found wrote more fondly about their elementary reading
between the favourable and unfavourable statements recollections were more likely to voice discontent
made in responses to Prompt 4 (current reading about their reading experiences in high school.
habits). Interestingly, favourable responses to all three
prompts regarding past reading experiences (i.e. learn-
ing to read, elementary school and high school) were The ART and reading frequency and enjoyment in
intercorrelated. In other words, participants who had school
more favourable early recollections about learning to
read (Prompt 1) also remembered more favourable The second research question addressed whether
reading experiences in elementary school (Prompt 2) associations would be noted between participants’
and high school (Prompt 3). Surprisingly, there was a ART scores and their reported reading frequency
positive association between favourable elementary and reading enjoyment. As illustrated in Table 4, the
school statements (Prompt 2) and unfavourable high correlation between reading frequency and reading
school statements (Prompt 3), suggesting that those enjoyment was positive for both elementary and high

Table 4: Correlations between the ART, Statements from the Prompts, Reading Frequency and Reading Enjoyment Separated
by Elementary and High School.
Elementary school High school

1 2 3 4 5

1. ART – .18* .01 .15 .21* 1. ART


2. Favourable .18** – .03 .17 .23** 2. Favourable
3. Unfavourable .14 .08 – .31** .15 3. Unfavourable
4. Reading frequency .33** .01 .09 – .43** 4. Reading frequency
5. Reading enjoyment .40** .07 .05 .46** – 5. Reading enjoyment

Note: Correlations for elementary school are below the diagonal in black whereas, the correlations for high school
are above the diagonal in grey ink.
ART = Author Recognition Test. Favourable = Favourable Statements and Unfavourable = Unfavourable State-
ments written in response to the prompts.
*
P < .05.
**
P < .01.

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Literacy Volume Number xxxx 2023 9

school. There was also a positive correlation between were based on an open-ended question about learning
the ART and both reading frequency and enjoyment to read, not storybook reading with parents. It seems
in elementary school. In other words, the more that early family communication around books is a
frequently adults reported that they remembered salient event that creates lasting impressions into
participating in, and enjoying literacy activities during adulthood.
elementary school, the better they fared on the ART. By Relatedly, during the prompt about learning to read,
contrast, a positive link between the ART and high individuals were most likely to talk about building
school reading enjoyment was found, but no links competence and feeling connected to the people they
were noted with reading frequency. In fact, the were reading with. During this prompt, it was rare
participants who responded that they did a lot of for individuals to discuss wanting or enjoying auton-
reading activities during high school tended to write omy. When they did discuss autonomy, it was often
more unfavourable comments about their most salient in relation to choosing a favourite book repeatedly, as
memories of that time. In contrast, participants who re- was highlighted in the quote above. Indeed, many of
ported enjoying those classroom activities responded memories people shared were about participating in
more positively to the high school writing prompt literacy events with others rather than wanting per-
and scored higher on the ART. sonal choice. It makes sense that before individuals
are competent readers themselves, they might be con-
tent to rely on the choices made by others. Perhaps it
Discussion is not surprising then, that there were no unfavourable
statements made about a lack of autonomy during the
The Matthew effect states that early reading success prompts about this time period. Thus, it would appear
paves the way to future reading habits and that that individuals’ most salient memories of learning to
well-maintained reading habits increase facility with read are tipped in favour towards activities that build
language over time (Stanovich, 1986). Recently much competence and relatedness.
discussion has centred on how early reading When prompted to remember salient aspects of
proficiency and genetic make-up influence reading reading during school years, we found a positive
propensity later in life (e.g. Keenan et al., 2006; Olson association between favourable statements made
et al., 2017; van Bergen et al., 2018; Torppa about elementary and high school and later print expo-
et al., 2019). Here, we asked a different but related sure (i.e. the ART). We also found that participants
question, namely, whether associations would be who were inclined to share positive comments about
found between adults’ memories of early reading reading during elementary and high school were more
experiences and their current recreational reading likely to be enthusiastic when asked to describe their
behaviours. We were also interested in the types of current reading habits. These findings suggest that
memories people shared when they were asked to re- recollections of reading experiences throughout the
call events that were personally significant from three school years are related to reading practices years later.
different developmental time spans. In contrast, only unfavourable memories of reading in
To begin, we found that favourable early memories high school were linked to negative descriptions of
of learning to read were significantly correlated with reading habits in adulthood. For example, one
positive responses to the prompts inquiring about all participant wrote about a teacher insisting that they
later time periods, including describing reading habits read aloud in front of the class. When asked about
in adulthood. This indicates that those who recalled their current reading habits, that same participant
more warm experiences from early childhood were wrote: ‘Reading would be the last thing I would do
more likely to describe themselves as avid readers [for pleasure] in this life for sure’ (Participant 122).
throughout their schooling. Interestingly, when asked However, others remembered their teachers helping
about their memories of learning to read, more than half in various ways, including improving their basic
of the participants (60%) recalled shared reading with reading skills, teaching effective comprehension
family members, specifically mothers. For example, in strategies and incorporating captivating books into
the current study, one participant responded by classroom activities. Indeed, our participants
saying: ‘My mom used to read me books before bed. highlighted the use of engaging books as being a very
It was one of my favourite activities that my mom positive motivational force. Previous research has
and I would do. I remember I would ask her to read shown certain characteristics are more likely to be
me the same storybook over and over again’ considered ‘high-interest’ to children and preteens.
(Participant 41). Our results are in line with Tremblay For example, children seem to be especially interested
et al. (2020), who found that shared storybook reading in literature containing same-aged protagonists, and
with parents in childhood correlated with print expo- futuristic settings, as well as plots that included
sure and literacy skills in adolescence. These responses danger, monsters and action (Jensen et al., 2016). Thus,

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
10 RECOLLECTIONS OF READING

in line with self-determination theory (Deci and during high school (see also Hughes-Hassell and
Ryan, 2000), participants positively described teachers Rodge, 2007).
who introduced them to engaging literature and made In sum, the literature shows that providing
them feel connected to the characters, as well as to classroom discussion (Woodford, 2016; Fisher and
their classmates. This can be highlighted by one partic- Frey, 2018), emphasising students’ choices (Ivey and
ipant who wrote: Johnston, 2013; Miller, 2015), using engaging literature
(Kozak and Martin-Chang, 2019; Locher et al., 2019)
… when reading plays such as Hamlet and Dracula. and providing a safe and entertaining environment
We would often get in groups in front of the class and for students to discuss difficult topics (Fletcher
reenact what was happening in the play and play et al., 2012; Cushing and Carter, 2021) are effective
different characters. It made it easier to understand. After classroom practices. Our data extend these findings
reading a chapter for homework the following class we by showing that the more frequently participants
would take 10 minutes to discuss what happened. It reported activities like listening to stories, reading in
would be in small groups or the whole class together. school, talking to teachers and other students about
(Participant 106). books and having the choice to pick the materials they
wanted to read during elementary school years, the
Often it was not the activity per se (e.g. reading in higher they scored on the ART as adults. Interestingly,
front of the class) that influenced whether the memory in high school, reading enjoyment rather than reading
was held in a positive or negative light, but rather the frequency was linked to later print exposure. This sug-
perception of choice (or lack of choice) and connected- gests that years later, remembering the reading activi-
ness (or isolation) that was reported along with the ties per se seemed to be less critical than remembering
experience. This lends support to research highlighting that the reading activities were enjoyable.
the power teachers hold to create positive classroom
literacy environment—a good teacher can help shape
readers, while a lesser teacher may inadvertently de-
rail students’ desire to read (Martin-Chang et al., 2021). Limitations
Participants’ recollections support the idea that
students’ psychological needs for competence, As with any investigation of this nature, our study has
autonomy and relatedness must be met for students some limitations that merit consideration. First, when
to feel motivated to read (Deci and Ryan, 2000). Our asked a neutral question, participants might feel
data suggest that classroom practices combining all more comfortable sharing positive memories with
three elements of the self-determination theory were researchers, rather than negative ones, which might
vividly recalled as positive. In contrast, participants account for the larger number of positive recollections
expressed feelings of frustration when these in our data set. Alternatively, our participants were all
psychological needs were not met. For example, university students and were, therefore, presumably,
several participants commented on the lack of choice skilled readers. Perhaps in a more representative
when selecting reading materials in high school and sample, an increased number of negative memories
compared it to the autonomy-supportive practices in would have emerged.
elementary school. Indeed, in response to a neutral However, it has been argued that reading skill leads
prompt, people were more likely to share negative to recreational reading, rather than the other way
memories about being forced to read specific books around (Torppa et al., 2019). Thus, it is interesting that
in high school compared to in elementary school. differences in print exposure (i.e. the ART) and
Although speculative, the perceived imbalance in self-described reading habits (Prompt 4) were noted
autonomy possibly contributed to the association even within a highly literate sample.
between the favourable memories of reading in Second, the current study was correlational. There-
elementary school and the unfavourable memories of fore, we cannot comment on the direction of the associ-
reading in high school. Reviewing the responses ations between the participants’ recollections of their
showed that many participants, even those with early reading experiences and current print exposure.
positive memories from elementary school, disliked However, at this juncture, we can conclude that people
reading the assigned books during high school. Similar with higher print exposure (as measured by the ART)
findings were observed by Locher et al. (2019), who and who report positive current reading habits (as
found a negative association between mandatory measured by Prompt 4) are more likely to remember
reading of classic literature and ninth graders’ past reading experiences positively compared to those
reading motivation. Our results add to this by indicat- who have lower print exposure and reading habit
ing the importance of increasing autonomy to support scores. There are three possible directions that this
reading activities during the school years, especially association could take. It could be that (1) positive

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Literacy Volume Number xxxx 2023 11

educational experiences lead to a proclivity to read for classroom activities. They also remembered negative
pleasure throughout the lifetime, or (2) that partici- experiences, such as uninteresting books, being unable
pants who read for pleasure might be more likely to re- to choose their own reading materials and tedious
call their past reading experiences fondly, or (3) that a classroom practices. Parents and teachers might find
separate variable, such as reading ability, influences solace in the fact that what they do remains salient to
both reading experiences and reading habits in adult- children even into their university years.
hood. Only a longitudinal research design would be
able to unravel these three competing hypotheses
(e.g. see Erbeli et al., 2019). However, if the correlations
were entirely driven by either current attitudes or by a Conclusion
higher-order variable such as reading ability, we
would not expect the positive link between favourable This is one of the first studies in the field to survey
elementary and unfavourable high school memories. adults about their formative impressions of reading.
In other words, if current attitudes or reading ability We asked neutral, open-ended prompts that provided
were driving the effects, we would expect to see simi- opportunity for participants to share memories
larities between responses on the prompts rather than about reading that stood out as being impactful. We
differences. Thus, the unexpected result of having did not sway the participants towards favourable or
more positive memories of reading in elementary unfavourable memories or to memories about certain
school related to reporting more negative memories activities. Nevertheless, when asked about reading at
from high school makes us more confident that partic- various points across development; not one participant
ipants were able to discriminate between good and indicated that they had no memories related to read-
bad memories from all three time points somewhat in- ing. Said differently, every participant recalled some-
dependently and that they did not simply view their thing, be it good or bad, from the three time periods.
past experiences through one monochromatic lens. The reading frequency and enjoyment question-
naire, which was completed after the reading prompts
to avoid cross-contamination, provided a different
vantage point by asking participants to report on a
Implications uniform set of classroom practices selected based on
motivational theory (i.e. self-determination theory;
The findings of the current study have important Deci and Ryan, 2000). On the questionnaire, only 3%
implications for parents and teachers in terms of of responses indicated that the participants ‘did not
meeting the fundamental psychological needs of remember’ classroom activities. While we are not
competence, relatedness and autonomy. First, we claiming that the participants’ memories were
noted a shift where the most salient memories from accurate, nevertheless, they were able to share their
early life revolved around meeting the needs for subjective experiences, indicating that learning to read
competency and relatedness and the need for and subsequent reading activities seem to occupy a
autonomy-supportive practices seemed to be more special place in childhood and adolescence that is
critical later. Thus, parents and educators of young remembered into adulthood. Our participants
children should provide opportunities to build connec- remembered what their teachers did (or did not do),
tions to and develop a solid foundation of skills reinforcing the push to encourage reading for pleasure
around book reading. While we recommend staying in primary school classrooms (Cremin and Moss, 2018),
sensitive to children’s book preferences, the memories which calls into question the notion raised by some
adults shared with us were mainly about the people teachers that reading motivation cannot be taught
who read with them and taught them. When recalling (Garces-Bacsal et al., 2018).
the events that took place in high school, participants Together, the associations between the prompts, the
still voiced a need for connection and competence, questionnaire and the ART underscore the value of
but they also talked about times where autonomy creating positive past and present reading experiences.
and choice were either granted or withheld. High Participants’ memories were linked in meaningful
school teachers might benefit by leveraging this desire ways to how many authors they recognised on the
for autonomy to make reading in their classes more ART, which is a proxy for reading over the lifetime.
appealing. Findings from the present research have significant
Thus, our findings suggest that reading habits implications for understanding how memories of
formed in childhood and throughout school years positive and negative experiences may align with
have long-lasting effects. The undergraduate students future reading habits. Our results stress the importance
surveyed here remembered positive reading practices of increasing teachers’ knowledge about motivational
such as enjoying books and participating in engaging strategies that satisfy students’ autonomy, competence

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
12 RECOLLECTIONS OF READING

and relatedness (Deci and Ryan, 2000), as many of the area instruction. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 53.8,
reported memories were related to these three basic pp. 678–687. https://doi.org/10.1598/JAAL.53.8.6
DE NAEGHEL, J., VALCKE, M., DE MEYER, I., WARLOP, N., VAN
needs, rather than being generally positive or negative
BRAAK, J. and VAN KEER, H. (2014) The role of teacher behavior
in nature. in adolescents’ intrinsic reading motivation. Reading and Writing,
27.9, pp. 1547–1565. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9506-3
DE NAEGHEL, J., VAN KEER, H., VANSTEENKISTE, M.,
HAERENS, L. and AELTERMAN, N. (2016) Promoting
Conflicts of interest elementary school students’ autonomous reading motivation:
effects of a teacher professional development workshop. The
The authors declare that the research was conducted in Journal of Educational Research, 109.3, pp. 232–252. https://doi.
the absence of any commercial or financial relation- org/10.1080/00220671.2014.942032
DECI, E. L. and RYAN, R. M. (2000) The “what” and “why” of goal
ships that could be construed as a potential conflict pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior.
of interest. Psychological Inquiry, 11.4, pp. 227–268. https://doi.org/10.1207/
S15327965PLI1104_01
ERBELI, F., BERGEN, E. and HART, S. A. (2019) Unraveling the
relation between reading comprehension and print exposure. Child
Funding Development, 91.5, pp. 1548–1562. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.
13339
This research was funded by a grant to S. FAUL, F., ERDFELDER, E., LANG, A.-G. and BUCHNER, A. (2007)
GPower 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the
Martin-Chang from the Social Sciences and Humani-
social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research
ties Research Council of Canada (435-2020-1228). Methods, 39.2, pp. 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
FISHER, D. and FREY, N. (2018) Raise reading volume through
access, choice, discussion, and book talks. The Reading Teacher,
References 72.1, pp. 89–97. https://doi.org/10.1002/trtr.1691
FLETCHER, J., GRIMLEY, M., GREENWOOD, J. and PARKHILL, F.
ALLINGTON, R. L. and MCGILL-FRANZEN, A. M. (2021) Reading (2012) Motivating and improving attitudes to reading in the
volume and reading achievement: a review of recent research. final years of primary schooling in five New Zealand schools.
Reading Research Quarterly, 56, pp. 231–238. https://doi.org/10. Literacy, 46.1, pp. 3–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-4369.2011.
1002/rrq.404 00589.x
APPLEGATE, A. J. and APPLEGATE, M. D. (2004) The Peter effect: FRANCIS, J. J., JOHNSTON, M., ROBERTSON, C., GLIDEWELL, L.,
reading habits and attitudes of preservice teachers. The Reading ENTWISTLE, V., ECCLES, M. P. and GRIMSHAW, J. M. (2010)
Teacher, 57.6, pp. 554–563. https://doi.org/10.2307/20205399 What is an adequate sample size? Operationalising data saturation
APPLEGATE, A. J., APPLEGATE, M. D., MERCANTINI, M. A., for theory-based interview studies. Psychology & Health, 25.10,
MCGEEHAN, C. M., COBB, J. B., DEBOY, J. R., MODLA, V. B. pp. 1229–1245. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440903194015
and LEWINSKI, K. E. (2014) The Peter effect revisited: reading FUSCH, P. and NESS, L. (2015) Are we there yet? Data saturation in
habits and attitudes of college students. Literacy Research and qualitative research. The Qualitative Report, 20.9, pp. 1408–1416.
Instruction, 53.3, pp. 188–204. https://doi.org/10.1080/19388071. https://doi.org/10.46743/2160-3715/2016.228i
2014.898719 GARCES-BACSAL, R. M., TUPAS, R., KAUR, S., PACULDAR, A. M.
BRAUN, E. K., WIMMER, G. E. and SHOHAMY, D. (2018) and BAJA, E. S. (2018) Reading for pleasure: whose job is it to
Retroactive and graded prioritizatoin of memory by reward. build lifelong readers in the classroom? Literacy, 52.2, pp. 95–102.
Nature Communucations, 9, p. 4886. https://doi.org/10.1038/ https://doi.org/10.1111/lit.12151
s41467-018-07820-0. GUAY, F., STUPNISKY, R., BOIVIN, M., JAPEL, C. and DIONNE, G.
BINKS-CANTRELL, E., WASHBURN, E. K., JOSHI, R. M. and (2019) Teacher’s relatedness with students as a predictor of stu-
HOUGEN, M. (2012) Peter effect in the preparation of reading dent’s intrinsic motivation, self-concept, and reading achievement.
teachers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16.6, pp. 526–536. https:// Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 48.3, pp. 215–225. https://doi.
doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2011.601434 org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.03.005
CARREKER, S. H., NEUHAUS, G. F., SWANK, P. R., JOHNSON, P., HUGHES-HASSELL, S. and RODGE, P. (2007) Leisure reading habits
MONFILS, M. J. and MONTEMAYOR, M. L. (2007) Teachers with of urban adolescents. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literature, 51.1,
linguistically informed knowledge of reading subskills are associ- pp. 22–33. https://doi.org/10.1598/JAAL.51.1.3
ated with a Matthew effect in reading comprehension for monolin- IVEY, G. and JOHNSTON, P. H. (2013) Engagement with young
gual and bilingual students. Reading Psychology, 28.2, pp. 187–212. adult literature: outcomes and processes. Reading Research
https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710601186456 Quarterly, 48.3, pp. 255–275. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.46
CREMIN, T. and MOSS, G. (2018) Reading for pleasure: supporting JENSEN, J. D., CHRISTY, K., KRAKOW, M., JOHN, K. and
reader engagement. Literacy, 52.2, pp. 59–61. https://doi.org/10. MARTINS, N. (2016) Narrative transportability, leisure reading,
1111/lit.12156 and genre preference in children 9-13 years old. The Journal of
CUNNINGHAM, A. E. and STANOVICH, K. E. (1997) Early reading Educational Research, 109.6, pp. 666–674. https://doi.org/10.
acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 1080/00220671.2015.1034351
10 years later. Developmental Psychology, 33.6, pp. 934–945. KAHNEMAN, D. (2010). The riddle of experience vs. memory.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934 PsycEXTRA Dataset. https://doi.org/10.1037/e623702010-001
CUSHING, I. and CARTER, A. (2021) Using young adult fiction to KEENAN, J. M., BETJEMANN, R. S., WADSWORTH, S. J., DEFRIES,
interrogate raciolinguistic ideologies in schools. Literacy, 56, J. C. and OLSON, R. K. (2006) Genetic and environmental influ-
pp. 106–119. https://doi.org/10.1111/lit.12277 ences on reading and listening comprehension. Journal of Research
DAISEY, P. (2010) Secondary preservice teachers remember their in Reading, 29.1, pp. 75–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.
favorite reading experiences: insights and implications for content 2006.00293.x

© UKLA.
17414369, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lit.12315 by Readcube (Labtiva Inc.), Wiley Online Library on [01/04/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Literacy Volume Number xxxx 2023 13

KOZAK, S. and MARTIN-CHANG, S. (2019) Preservice teacher STANOVICH, K. E. (1986) Matthew effects in reading: some conse-
knowledge, print exposure, and planning for instruction. Reading quences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.
Research Quarterly, 54.3, pp. 323–338. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Reading Research Quarterly, 21.4, pp. 360–407. https://doi.org/10.
rrq.240 1598/rrq.21.4.1
KUCKARTZ, U. (2014) Qualitative Text Analysis: A Guide to Methods, STANOVICH, K. E. and WEST, R. F. (1989) Exposure to print and or-
Practice & Using Software. SAGE Publications Ltd 10.4135/ thographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24.4,
9781446288719. pp. 402–433. https://doi.org/10.2307/747605
Laerd Statistics (2019) Fleiss’ kappa using SPSS Statistics. Statistical STRIJBOSCH, W., MITAS, O., VAN GISBERGEN, M., DOICARU,
tutorials and software guides. Retrieved October 6, 2022, from M., GELISSEN, J. and BASTIAANSEN, M. (2019) From experience
https://statistics.laerd.com/spss-tuorials/fleiss-kappa-in-spss- to memory: on the robustness of the peak-and-end-rule for
statistics.php complex, heterogeneous experiences. Frontiers in Psychology, 10,
LimeSurvey GmbH [An Open Source Survey Tool]. (2020). http:// p. 1705. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01705ers.11
www.limesurvey TORPPA, M., NIEMI, P., VASALAMPI, K., LERKKANEN, M.,
LOCHER, F. M., BECKER, S. and PFOST, M. (2019) The relation TOLVANEN, A. and POIKKEUS, A. (2019) Leisure reading (but
between students’ intrinsic reading in recreational and school not any kind) and reading comprehension support each other—a
contexts. AERA Open., 5.2, pp. 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/ longitudinal study across grades 1 and 9. Child Development, 91.3,
2332858419852041 pp. 876–900. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13241
LOCHER, F. M., BECKER, S., SCHIEFER, I. and PFOST, M. (2021) TREMBLAY, B., RODRIGUES, M. L. and MARTIN-CHANG, S.
Mechanisms mediating the relation between reading self-concept (2020) From storybooks to novels: a retrospective approach linking
and reading comprehension. European Journal of Psychology of Edu- print exposure in childhood to adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology,
cation, 36, pp. 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-020-00463-8 11, p. 2520. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.571033
MARTIN-CHANG, S., KOZAK, S., LEVESQUE, K. C., CALARCO, VAN BERGEN, E., SNOWLING, M. J., ZEEUW, E. L.,
N. and MAR, R. A. (2021) What’s your pleasure? Exploring the BEIJSTERVELDT, C. E. M., DOLAN, C. V. and BOOMSMA, D. I.
predictors of leisure reading for fiction and nonfiction. Reading (2018) Why do children read more? The influence of reading
and Writing, 34, pp. 1387–1414. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145- ability on voluntary reading practices. Journal of Child Psychology
020-10112-7 and Psychiatry, 59.11, pp. 1205–1214. https://doi.org/10.1111/
MILLER, R. (2015) Learning to love reading: a self-study on fostering jcpp.12910
students’ reading motivation in small groups. Studying Teacher VANDEN DOOL, C. and SIMPSON, A. (2021) Reading for pleasure:
Education, 11.2, pp. 103–123. https://doi.org/10.1080/17425964. exploring reading culture in an Australian early years classroom.
2015.1045771 Literacy, 55.2, pp. 113–124. https://doi.org/10.1111/lit.12247
MOL, S. E. and BUS, A. G. (2011) To read or not to read: a VASILEIOU, K., BARNETT, J., THORPE, S. and YOUNG, T. (2018)
meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Characterising and justifying sample size sufficiency in
Psychological Bulletin, 137.2, pp. 267–296. https://doi.org/10. interview-based studies: systematic analysis of qualitative health
1037/a0021890 research over a 15-year period. BMC Medical Research Methodology,
MOORE, M. and GORDON, P. C. (2014) Reading ability and print 18.1, p. 148. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-018-0594-7
exposure: item response theory analysis of the author recognition WOODFORD, H. (2016) Engaging young adolescents with reading.
test. Behavior Research Methods, 47.4, pp. 1095–1109. https://doi. Literacy Learning: The Middle Years, 24.2, pp. 43–54 https://www.
org/10.3758/s13428-014-0534-3 questia.com/library/journal/1G1-458564957/engaging-young-
NORMAN, D. A. (2009) THE WAY I SEE ITMemory is more impor- adolescents-with-reading
tant than actuality. Interactions, 16.2, pp. 24–26. https://doi.org/
10.1145/1487632.1487638
OLSON, R. K., KEENAN, J. M., BYRNE, B. and SAMUELSSON, S.
(2017) ‘Genetic and environmental influences on the development
CONTACT THE AUTHORS
of reading and related skills’, in Studies in Written Language and
Literacy. John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 33–54 Manzar Zare, Department of Education,
10.1075/swll.15.03ols. Concordia University, Montreal, QC Canada
REDELMEIER, D. A. and KAHNEMAN, D. (1996) Patients’ memo-
ries of painful medical treatments: real-time and retrospective Stephanie Kozak, Department of Education,
evaluations of two minimally invasive procedures. Pain, 66.1,
pp. 3–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(96)02994-6
Concordia University, Montreal, QC Canada
SÉNÉCHAL, M. and LEFEVRE, J.-A. (2014) Continuity and change
in the home literacy environment as predictors of growth in Monyka L. Rodrigues, Department of Education,
vocabulary and reading. Child Development, 85.4, pp. 1552–1568. Concordia University, Montreal, QC Canada
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.1222
SPARKS, R. L., PATTON, J. and MURDOCH, A. (2014) Early reading
success and its relationship to reading achievement and reading
Sandra Martin-Chang, Department of Education,
volume: replication of ‘10 years later.’. Reading and Writing, 27.1, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada.
pp. 189–211. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9439-2 email: s.martin-chang@concordia.ca

© UKLA.

You might also like