You are on page 1of 11

Psychophysiology, 51 (2014), 309–318. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA.

Copyright © 2014 Society for Psychophysiological Research


DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12177

Mechanisms underlying hemodynamic and neuroendocrine stress


reactivity at different phases of the menstrual cycle

JENNIFER L. GORDON and SUSAN S. GIRDLER


Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA

Abstract
This study examined the association of menstrual cycle phase with stress reactivity as well as the hormonal and
neuroendocrine mechanisms contributing to cycle effects. Fifty-seven women underwent a modified Trier Social Stress
Test during the early follicular, late follicular, and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle. Greater increases in cardiac index
(CI) and greater decreases in vascular resistance index (VRI) during speech were observed in the luteal phase relative to
other phases, while greater increases in epinephrine (EPI) was observed during the late follicular and luteal phases
compared to the early follicular phase. Luteal phase estradiol predicted luteal EPI reactivity but not CI or VRI reactivity,
while luteal phase EPI reactivity predicted luteal phase CI and VRI reactivity. Thus, cycle-related changes in EPI
reactivity may be a stronger determinant of cycle effects on hemodynamic reactivity than sex hormones per se.
Descriptors: Menstrual cycle, Estradiol, Progesterone, Stress reactivity, Catecholamines, Hemodynamics, Cortisol,
Blood pressure, Heart rate

Cardiovascular disease (CVD) is the leading cause of mortality for studies suggest that estrogen and progesterone may serve to protect
women in the United States (Center for Disease Control, 2009). premenopausal women from CVD via their positive effect on car-
However, women are relatively protected from CVD until they diovascular and neuroendocrine stress reactivity, with few excep-
reach the menopause transition, after which they experience a tions, the majority of these studies have examined combined
doubling of their CVD risk (Gordon, Kannel, Hjortland, & (estrogen plus progesterone) therapy and therefore cannot differ-
McNamara, 1978). That young women experiencing premature entiate the cardioprotective effects of estrogen from progesterone.
ovarian failure also exhibit an increased risk for CVD (Hu et al., Studies in premenopausal women that have attempted to do so
1999; Jacobsen, Knutsen, & Fraser, 1999) strongly suggests that by examining stress reactivity at different phases of the menstrual
sex steroid hormone withdrawal, independent of advancing age, is cycle have yielded inconsistent findings (Childs, Dlugos, & DeWit,
responsible for this observation. 2010; Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Gaab, Schommer, & Hellhammer,
Female sex hormones may act to confer CVD protection in part 1999; Lustyk, Douglas, Shilling, & Woods, 2012; Lustyk, Olson,
via beneficially modifying physiologic reactivity to stress. Exag- Gerrish, Holder, & Widman, 2010; Manhem, Jern, Pilhall, Shanks,
gerated stress reactivity has been shown to predict the development & Jern, 1991; Polefrone & Manuck, 1988; Pollard, Pearce,
of CVD risk factors, including hypertension, increased left ven- Rousham, & Schwartz, 2007) that may be attributable to at least
tricular mass, carotid atherosclerosis, and coronary calcification two methodological limitations in the existing research. First, these
(Gianaros et al., 2005; Matthews et al., 2004; Treiber et al., 2000) studies have compared stress reactivity in the luteal phase, charac-
as well as future CVD risk itself (Chida & Steptoe, 2010). Among terized by elevated estradiol and progesterone, with reactivity in the
postmenopausal women, combined estrogen and progesterone follicular phase, assumed to reflect a lower hormone state.
therapy lowers blood pressure (BP) reactivity to stress and benefi- However, only the early follicular phase is characterized by both
cially modifies the hemodynamic and neuroendocrine determinants low estradiol and progesterone while the mid- to late follicular
of BP reactivity, namely, vascular resistance stress reactivity, phase is associated with markedly elevated estradiol concentra-
endothelial function, and norepinephrine stress reactivity (Del Rio tions. Thus, the existing menstrual cycle literature, with one excep-
et al., 1998; Girdler et al., 2004; Light et al., 2001). While these tion (Pollard et al., 2007), is unable to differentiate the influence of
estradiol alone from estradiol plus progesterone on stress reactivity.
Pollard et al. (2007) showed that heart rate (HR) reactivity to per-
This research was supported by NIH grant RO1-DA013705, CTSA ceived work stress is greater in the late follicular phase compared to
grant UL1RR025747, and NIH grant T32-MH093315. Dr. Gordon is also the early follicular phase, underscoring the importance of refining
the recipient of a postdoctoral fellowship of the Fonds de la Recherche en the follicular phase window of testing.
Santé du Québec (FRSQ). The second major limitation of many of the referred menstrual
Address correspondence to: Susan S. Girdler, Ph.D., Department of
Psychiatry, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill School of Medicine,
cycle studies is the reliance on a between-subjects design. The
CB# 7160, 101 Manning Drive, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3366, USA. substantial interindividual differences in absolute sex steroid con-
E-mail: Susan_Girdler@med.unc.edu centrations across the menstrual cycle obscures the ability to
bs_bs_banner

309
310 J.L. Gordon and S.S. Girdler

examine menstrual cycle phase effects on stress reactivity with a reactivity across the menstrual cycle. Thus, it is unknown whether
between-subjects design (Gravetter & Forzano, 2011). estradiol and progesterone influence hemodynamic reactivity
These limitations notwithstanding, the majority of studies in directly or indirectly (e.g., through alterations in neuroendocrine
this field find an absence of menstrual cycle effects on BP reactivity reactivity). The aims of the present study were therefore: (a)
to stress (Collins, Eneroth, & Landgren, 1985; Girdler & Light, to examine menstrual cycle effects on hemodynamic and neuro-
1994; Girdler, Pedersen, Stern, & Light, 1993; Kajantie & Phillips, endocrine stress reactivity in the early follicular (low hormones),
2006; Stoney, Langer, & Gelling, 1986; Stoney, Owens, Matthews, late follicular (high estradiol, low progesterone), and luteal (high
Davis, & Caggiula, 1990). That is not to say, however, that the estradiol and high progesterone) phases of the menstrual cycle; and
menstrual cycle does not influence the underlying hemodynamic (b) to explore the hormonal and neuroendocrine predictors of these
determinants of BP reactivity, namely, stroke volume (SV), cardiac effects.
output (CO), and vascular resistance (VR). The relative contribu-
tion that the myocardium versus the vasculature makes to BP
responses, which is relatively stable within an individual over time Method
(Kasprowicz, Manuck, Malkoff, & Krantz, 1990; Sherwood, Participants
Turner, Light, & Blumenthal, 1990), may be an important patho-
genic influence in CVD development (Manuck, 1994). Estradiol Participants included 57 women who were medically healthy non-
has been shown to exert inotropic (contractile) actions on the myo- smokers, aged 18–40, recruited through newspaper advertisements.
cardium, thereby increasing SV (Girdler et al., 2004; Pines et al., Exclusion criteria included blood pressure >160/90 mmHg,
1991; Ren et al., 2003), and to promote greater vasodilation to prescription medication use (including oral contraceptives),
stress, thereby decreasing VR (Girdler et al., 2004; Pines et al., regular over-the-counter medication use (e.g., nonsteroidal
1991). This combination of enhanced myocardial reactivity and antiinflammatory agents), a chronic pain condition (e.g., temporo-
greater decreases in VR is a hemodynamic stress response profile mandibular joint disorder, fibromyalgia, arthritis) or other medical
seen in groups at lower risk for CVD relative to their higher risk illness (e.g., cardiovascular, respiratory, neuroendocrine, and gas-
counterparts (Allen, Stoney, Owens, & Matthews, 1993; Girdler, trointestinal disorders), irregular menstrual cycles (<24 or >32
Turner, Sherwood, & Light, 1990; Light, Turner, Hinderliter, & days), self-reported alcoholism or drug abuse, a history of more
Sherwood, 1993; Treiber et al., 2000). Moreover, greater VR con- than one depressive episode, the presence of a depressive episode
tributes to left ventricular hypertrophy (Levy et al., 1988), an inde- in the last year, or self-reported moderate to severe emotional
pendent predictor of CVD morbidity/mortality (Ghali et al., 1992; premenstrual symptoms. Due to the association of both depression
Koren, Devereux, Casale, Savage, & Laragh, 1991). (Gordon et al., 2011) and anxiety (Watkins, Blumenthal, & Carney,
To date, there have been only four studies examining menstrual 2002) with cardiac-autonomic dysfunction, participants with Ham-
cycle influences on hemodynamic stress reactivity. In two prior ilton Depression Scale scores >7 or Hamilton Anxiety scores >9
within-subjects studies from our group, the luteal phase was asso- (administered by a trained interviewer) were excluded. The study
ciated with greater increases in SV coupled with greater decreases protocol was approved by the institution’s Institutional Review
in VR to mental stress relative to the early follicular phase (Girdler Board. All participants provided informed, written consent prior to
& Light, 1994; Girdler et al., 1993). The third study (McFetridge & participating and received $150 in compensation.
Sherwood, 2000), using a between-subjects design, similarly found
greater decreases in VR to mental stress during the luteal compared Procedure
to the early follicular phase, though SV did not differ. Finally, in the
fourth study (Sita & Miller, 1996), a within-subjects design, there All laboratory sessions began at 8:00 a.m. Participants were asked
were no differences in SV, CO, or VR between the luteal phase and to refrain from all over-the-counter medications 24 h before testing
the late follicular phase. However, the relative equivalence of estra- as well as refrain from caffeine the day of testing. Participants were
diol concentrations in luteal and late follicular phases may have tested at three time points throughout their menstrual cycle
accounted for the absence of a phase effect in that study. (Figure 1): the early follicular phase (days 1–4, when both estradiol
The influence of the endogenous steroid hormones on and progesterone are low), the late follicular phase (days 9–12,
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and sympathoadrenal when estradiol is elevated and progesterone is low), and the luteal
axis reactivity is also relevant to CVD risk (Christensen & phase (5–10 days after ovulation, when both estradiol and proges-
Schultz-Larsen, 1994; Whitworth, Williamson, Mangos, & Kelly, terone are elevated). The timing of the luteal phase was based upon
2005; Zoccali et al., 2002). Where phase effects on HPA axis the detection of the luteinizing hormone surge that occurs 24–36 h
reactivity have been observed, cortisol reactivity, but not prior to ovulation using a home urine ovulation predictor test. All
adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) reactivity, is greater in the cycles were later confirmed to be ovulatory based on serum pro-
luteal than the follicular phase (Childs et al., 2010; Kirschbaum gesterone and estradiol levels taken during the baseline period of
et al., 1999). While fewer studies on catecholamine stress reactivity each testing session (Table 1). The order of menstrual cycle phase
across the menstrual cycle exist, the data suggest greater was randomized for each woman.
catecholamine resting levels (Girdler et al., 2003) and reactivity in Upon arrival, participants were equipped with a BP cuff and
the luteal versus the early to midfollicular phase (Childs et al., impedance band electrodes for later monitoring of cardiovascular
2010; Collins et al., 1985). Exceptions to this general finding do stress reactivity. A catheter was then placed into a forearm vein, to
exist, however; for example, one study by McFetridge & Sherwood be used for blood draws using a nonheparinized, multistop-cock
(2000) found that women in the luteal phase exhibited lower system allowing for blood samples to be taken at precise time
catecholamine responses to laboratory stressors compared to intervals throughout testing. A curtain was drawn to prevent the
women in the follicular phase. subject from viewing the catheter and the blood draws.
Finally, no studies of which we are aware have examined the A secondary goal of the current study was to examine the effect
hormonal and neuroendocrine predictors of hemodynamic stress of the menstrual cycle on pain sensitivity to a short battery of pain
Stress reactivity at different phases of the menstrual cycle 311

Figure 1. A stylized representation of steroid hormone concentrations during the human female menstrual cycle. Original figure.

tests. These results are reported elsewhere (Klatzkin, Mechlin, & findings are based on events 3, 4, and 5 for those who had the TSST
Girdler, 2010). Thus, subjects were exposed to both a mental first, or events 3, 7, and 8 for those who had pain testing first. These
stressor battery and a pain testing battery. The order of stress and events are described below.
pain testing was counterbalanced across subjects but constant
within subjects (see Figure 2). Baseline
Figure 2 depicts the sequence of laboratory events: (1) Instru-
mentation and intravenous set-up; (2) recovery from venipuncture Twenty-five minutes of quiet rest followed the intravenous set-up,
(15 min); (3) baseline (10 min quiet rest); (4) Trier Social Stress the first 15 min being recovery from venipuncture and the final
Test (TSST, 23.5 min); (5) recovery (10 min); (6) pain testing; 10 min constituting baseline. Blood pressure and impedance-
(7) rest control (or TSST if event #4 was rest control, 23.5 min); derived measures were taken at minutes 1, 3, 6, 8, and 10 and
(8) recovery (10 min); and (9) pain testing. The current study’s averaged to yield a mean baseline value. Blood was sampled at
minute 10 for baseline ACTH, cortisol, epinephrine (EPI), norepi-
nephrine (NE), estradiol, and progesterone concentrations.

Table 1. Demographic and Baseline Characteristics The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST)
Mean (SD) or % The TSST used was modified to account for the fact that partici-
Age (years) 27.5 (0.8) pants were not ambulatory and to include a serial addition rather
BMI (kg/m2) 26.9 (0.8) than a serial subtraction task. The TSST is a stress test that has been
Race shown to induce reliably large and consistent activation of the
% Caucasian 60 sympathetic nervous system (SNS) and HPA axes (Kirschbaum,
% African American 31
% Other 9
Pirke, & Hellhammer, 1993). The TSST involves four components:
BDI score 3.0 (0.4) (1) Pretask instructions (5 min) during which participants are intro-
Spielberger Trait Anxiety 31.7 (6.5) duced to the committee who will later listen to their job talk, and
Spielberger State Anxiety 27.8 (5.9) are given instructions for the mental arithmetic task; (2) Speech
Estradiol (ng/ml) preparation period (5 min) during which time participants were left
Early follicular 23.3 (6.4)
Late follicular 44.6 (22.6) alone to prepare their job talk; (3) Job speech (5 min). Immediately
Luteal 54.5 (22.4) following the preparation period, the selection committee returned
Progesterone (ng/ml) to the testing room and asked the participant to deliver his/her job
Early follicular 0.2 (0.1) talk on why they would be the perfect candidate for the job. If the
Late follicular 0.2 (0.2)
Luteal 13.7 (7.8)
participant ended their talk before 5 min, the selection committee
questioned the participant in a systematic fashion to ensure
Note. BMI = body mass index; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory. the participant would speak the entire 5 min. Participants were

Figure 2. Schedule of events.


312 J.L. Gordon and S.S. Girdler

tape-recorded throughout their performance; (4) Paced Auditory Plasma ACTH was determined using RIA techniques from
Serial Addition Test (PASAT; 8.5 min) involves a tape-recorded commercially available kits (Nichols Institute Diagnostics). The
presentation of numbers from 1 to 9 (for greater detail, see sensitivity of this assay is high at 1 pg/ml, and the selectivity is
Tombaugh, 2006). Participants added each number presented on excellent showing only 0–0.02% cross-reactivity with steroid
the tape to the immediately preceding number and stated the compounds.
answer aloud. There are four series of numbers, with progressively Plasma EPI and NE concentrations were determined using the
shorter interdigit intervals. The committee remained in the room to high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) technique. All
monitor performance. HPLC procedures were conducted in the core laboratory of the
Cardiovascular measures were obtained at minutes 1, 3, and 5 of institution’s General Clinical Research Center. The lower limit of
both the speech preparation period and the job speech and once quantification is 25 pg/ml and the intra- and interday coefficients
during each of the four series of the PASAT. These measures were are <10%.
then averaged to obtain a mean preparation, speech, and math task
value for each cardiovascular measure. Plasma EPI and NE were
sampled at the end of minute 2 of both the job speech and PASAT Statistical Analyses
since catecholamines peak within the first 2 min of mental stress
As per Warner (2012), sensitivity analyses were used to identify
(Dimsdale & Ziegler, 1991).
extreme outliers prior to data analysis, defined as values three or
Poststress Cortisol Sampling more interquartile ranges below the first quartile or above the third
quartile (SAS Institute Inc., 2011). Although the analyses presented
Participants rested quietly alone. Plasma cortisol was measured at are those with the outliers removed, analyses with the outliers
the end of the recovery period to capture the delayed plasma retained yielded an identical pattern of results.
cortisol response to the TSST (Kirschbaum, Klauer, Filipp, & For each cardiovascular or neuroendocrine measure, reactivity
Hellhammer, 1995; Kirschbaum, Prussner et al., 1995). was defined as the difference between mean stress level and mean
baseline level. T tests were used to confirm a significant effect of
Physiological Recording Procedures
the TSST on hemodynamic and neuroendocrine measures.
The SunTech Exercise BP monitor, Model 4240 (SunTech Medical Apart from cortisol and ACTH, which were measured only in
Instruments, Inc., Raleigh, NC) provided BP measurements during response to the TSST, reactivity of all other measures was calcu-
baseline and mental stress testing. Five standard stethoscopic and lated separately in response to the job speech and the PASAT in
automated BP measurements were taken simultaneously to ensure consideration of the literature suggesting that women are particu-
correct microphone placement and cuff position. larly responsive to social rejection over performance-type tasks
Impedance cardiography was used to noninvasively monitor (Darnall & Suarez, 2009).
cardiovascular activity (Sherwood, Allen et al., 1990), including For each physiologic dependent measure, a repeated measures
CO, SV, total VR, and HR. A custom-designed impedance cardio- analysis of variance (ANOVA), with cycle phase as the repeated
graph (HIC-100 Bio-Impedance Technology, Inc., Model 100, measure was then used to examine phase effects on baseline and
Chapel Hill, NC) was used in conjunction with a tetrapolar band reactivity values in the early follicular, late follicular, and luteal
electrode configuration to record impedance dZ/dt and Zo signals. phases. Within-subjects paired t tests were then used to explore
Impedance and electrocardiogram signals were processed online significant phase effects. The Huynh-Feldt correction was used
by specialized computer software (BIT, Chapel Hill, NC) with whenever appropriate to correct for sphericity.
subsequent manual editing to improve accuracy. For each minute of Where significant phase effects on stress reactivity were
interest, a 30-s continuous sample of waveforms (obtained concur- observed, stepwise regression analyses were conducted in order to
rently with BP) was processed to generate an ensemble-averaged examine predictors of the phase effects. In stepwise regression,
cardiac cycle, from which SV was determined using the Kubicek, each independent variable specified is entered into the regression
Karnegis, Patterson, Witsoe, and Mattson (1966) equation, and HR one at a time until all variables have been added with the provision
was determined using the mean interbeat interval. CO and VR for that each meets the specified criterion. For this study, the criterion
these same minutes were then calculated using standard formulae used was one of significance level p < .100, a more conservative
(Sherwood, Allen et al., 1990). CO, SV, and VR were adjusted for significance level than the p value of .150 that is conventional for
individual variations in body size by using body surface areas to stepwise regression (SAS Institute, 2011). Furthermore, the
derive cardiac index (CI), stroke volume index (SVI), and vascular stepwise approach involves an additional procedure in which all
resistance index (VRI). variables are reexamined after the addition of other variables to
verify that each remains a significant and independent predictor.
Hormone and Neuroendocrine Assays Thus, this approach helps to circumvent the problem of
multicolinearity of independent variables.
Plasma cortisol, serum estradiol, and progesterone were deter-
All analyses were conducted using SAS v. 9.3.
mined using radioimmunoassay (RIA) techniques (ICN Biomedi-
cal Inc.). The specificity of the antiserum for progesterone is very
high, showing only 0.01–2.5% cross-reactivity with other steroid
compounds. The specificity of the antiserum for estradiol is also Results
high, showing only 0.01–1.45% cross-reactivity with steroid hor- Participant Characteristics
mones with the exception of estrone, for which there is up to 6%
cross-reactivity. For cortisol, the sensitivity of the assay is excellent As reflected in Table 1, the participants were relatively young, had
at 0.07 μg/dl and the specificity high, showing 0.05–2.2% cross- low depression and anxiety scores, and exhibited estradiol and
reactivity with similar compounds, except prednisolone, where progesterone concentrations in each menstrual cycle phase that
94% cross-reactivity is obtained. were consistent with expected values.
Stress reactivity at different phases of the menstrual cycle 313

Table 2. Mean (SD) Baseline Cardiovascular and Neuroendocrine Measures as a Function of Menstrual Cycle Phase

Early follicular Late follicular Luteal


SBP (mmHg) 110.6 (10.4) 109.6 (10.8) 111.3 (10.5)
DBP (mmHg) 65.5 (7.7) 63.7 (8.1) 64.6 (6.8)
HR (bpm) 69.0* (9.4) 68.2# (10.4) 71.2*# (8.1)
CI (l/min per M2) 4.2 (1.2) 4.3 (1.1) 4.4 (1.4)
SVI (ml/beat per M2) 62.7 (19.0) 64.8 (18.0) 62.4 (18.8)
VRI (dyne-s cm−5 M2) 515.0* (167.6) 484.6 (146.0) 484.9* (126.6)
Epinephrine (pg/ml) 10.7 (8.0) 11.1 (7.1) 11.4 (8.4)
Norepinephrine (pg/ml) 172.8 (64.4) 172.3 (68.6) 190.8 (71.3)
Cortisol (pg/ml) 6.8 (2.3) 6.9 (2.3) 6.2 (2.5)
ACTH (pg/ml) 21.4 (10.3) 21.4 (11.2) 19.2 (6.5)

Note. Values in the same row with the same superscript (* or #) are significantly different from each other at the p < .05 level. SBP = systolic blood pressure;
DBP = diastolic blood pressure; HR = heart rate; SVI = stroke volume index; VRI = vascular resistance index; ACTH = adrenocorticotropic hormone.

Menstrual Cycle Effects on Baseline Measures to the speech was greater in the late follicular, t(46) = −2.2,
p = .031, and luteal phases, t(45) = −2.7, p = .009, compared to the
A significant effect of phase was found for baseline HR, early follicular phase (Figure 5).
F(2,98) = 3.91, p = .024, such that HR during the luteal phase was
significantly higher than the early, t(49) = 2.27, p = .027, and late Norepinephrine (NE), ACTH, and cortisol. There were no sig-
follicular phase, t(51) = 2.56, p = .014 (Table 2). Second, a signifi- nificant effects of cycle phase on stress reactivity of these
cant effect of menstrual cycle phase was seen for baseline VRI, neuroendocrine measures.
F(2,94) = 3.06, p = .052, such that VRI was significantly lower
during the luteal phase compared to the early follicular phase, Hormonal and Neuroendocrine Predictors of Menstrual
t(47) = −2.14, p = .038. No other baseline cardiovascular or Cycle Phase Effects
neuroendocrine measures differed according to menstrual cycle
In order to examine predictors of speech stress reactivity measures
phase.
that were influenced by the menstrual cycle (EPI, CI, and VRI

Overall Efficacy of the Stress Protocol Table 3. Mean (SD) Reactivity During Mental Stress as a
Systolic BP, diastolic BP, and HR all significantly increased in Function of Menstrual Cycle Phase
response to the mental stressors, t(57) = 21.1, 24.6, and 19.2,
Early follicular Late follicular Luteal
p < .001, respectively, as did plasma EPI, NE, t(55) = 6.8 and 6.7,
p < .001, respectively, ACTH, t(55) = 2.0, p = .047, and CI, SBP (mmHg)
t(55) = 10.6, p < .001. SVI tended to decrease, t(55) = −1.9, Speech 22.0 (9.7) 23.3 (9.3) 22.3 (9.3)
Math 15.5 (8.2) 15.8 (8.6) 15.0 (8.5)
p = .070, while no effect of the mental stress was seen for VRI or DBP (mmHg)
plasma cortisol. Speech 16.3 (7.2) 16.6 (6.6) 14.8 (5.3)
Math 11.2 (5.6) 11.6 (5.0) 11.1 (5.8)
HR (bpm)
Menstrual Cycle Effects on Stress Reactivity Speech 17.4 (8.7) 18.9 (8.8) 19.2 (10.0)
Math 11.6 (5.7) 12.7 (7.1) 12.3 (6.4)
Blood pressure (BP) and heart rate (HR). No phase effects on CI (l/min per M2)
BP or HR reactivity were observed in response to either mental Speech 1.1* (0.9) 1.2 (1.0) 1.4* (1.0)
stressor (Table 3). Math 0.6 (0.7) 0.6 (0.7) 0.6 (0.7)
SVI (ml/beat per M2)
Speech −0.7 (10.3) −1.6 (10.1) 1.3 (10.8)
Hemodynamic measures. Menstrual cycle phase effects were Math −2.4 (7.4) −4.0 (7.7) −2.6 (7.9)
seen for CI reactivity, F(2,94) = 3.6, p = .033, such that during the VRI (dyne-s cm−5 M2)
speech, reactivity was greater during the luteal phase than the early Speech −10.0 (113.2) 0.8* (86.0) −24.9* (91.2)
follicular phase, t(47) = −2.6, p = .014 (Figure 3). Although the Math 12.6 (85.7) 16.2 (87.3) 13.6 (77.1)
Epinephrine (pg/ml)
omnibus test for a cycle effect on VRI reactivity to speech did not Speech 11.7*& (13.8) 17.7* (26.2) 17.9& (23.2)
reach statistical significance, F(2,94) = 2.2, p = .123, because of Math 6.1 (10.1) 5.7 (10.3) 5.8 (9.8)
the prior literature indicating that such an effect is consistently Norepinephrine (pg/ml)
observed, we compared the individual phases in response to the Speech 31.1 (44.4) 37.8 (45.2) 41.1 (46.8)
speech. Consistent with prior research, VRI decreases in the luteal Math 17.6 (46.5) 17.2 (53.4) 27.3 (42.7)
Cortisol (pg/ml) 0.2 (2.4) 0.0 (2.5) 0.1 (2.2)
phase were significantly different from essentially no change in ACTH (pg/ml) 1.4 (6.2) 1.6 (7.1) 1.0 (4.8)
VRI in the late follicular phase, t(49) = 2.4, p = .019 (Figure 4). No
cycle effects were observed for SVI reactivity. Note. Values in the same row with the same superscript (* or &) are signifi-
cantly different from each other. SBP = systolic blood pressure;
DBP = diastolic blood pressure; HR = heart rate; SVI = stroke volume
Epinephrine (EPI). Consistent with the pattern seen for CI reac- index; VRI = vascular resistance index; ACTH = adrenocorticotropic
tivity, a significant effect of menstrual cycle phase was seen for EPI hormone.
reactivity, F(2,86) = 3.3, p = .048, such that reactivity in response &
p < .01. *p < .05.
314 J.L. Gordon and S.S. Girdler

Figure 3. Mean (+SEM) cardiac index (CI) reactivity during the speech
Figure 5. Mean (+SEM) plasma epinephrine (EPI) reactivity during the
stress as a function of menstrual cycle phase. *p < .05.
speech stress as a function of menstrual cycle phase. *p < .05; **p < .01.

reactivity), stepwise multiple regression analyses were conducted. VRI. A significant predictive model was found for VRI reactivity
Estradiol, progesterone, baseline, and reactivity values for cortisol, during the luteal, but not the early or late follicular phases. In this
EPI, and NE were entered as possible predictors into two separate model, both greater luteal EPI reactivity and baseline NE predicted
models, the first predicting CI reactivity to the speech stress and the greater decreases in VRI. Together, they accounted for 17% of the
second predicting VRI reactivity to the speech stress. These analy- variance in VRI reactivity, F(2,46) = 4.4, p = .019.
ses were conducted separately for each menstrual cycle phase. A
third model, including the same predictors as above, with the EPI. In the early follicular phase, greater baseline NE and greater
exception of baseline and reactivity EPI, was similarly examined as NE reactivity predicted greater EPI reactivity, accounting for 18%
potential predictors of EPI reactivity for each menstrual cycle of its variance, F(2,46) = 4.6, p = .015. In the late follicular phase,
phase (see Table 4). greater NE reactivity predicted greater EPI reactivity and
accounted for 34% of its variance, F(1,52) = 25.9, p < .001.
CI. In the early follicular phase, greater baseline EPI and greater Finally, in the luteal phase, greater concentrations in luteal phase
EPI reactivity predicted greater CI reactivity, accounting for 36% estradiol and greater NE reactivity and cortisol reactivity predicted
of its variance, F(2,45) = 11.9, p < .001. In the late follicular phase, greater EPI reactivity, accounting for 51% of its variance,
greater EPI reactivity and NE reactivity predicted greater CI reac- F(3,47) = 15.4, p < .001.
tivity, accounting for 26% of its variance, F(2,51) = 8.9, p < .001).
Finally, in the luteal phase, greater EPI reactivity predicted greater
Discussion
CI reactivity, accounting for 43% of its variance, F(1,46) = 34.4,
p < .001. The current study aimed to clarify the effects of estradiol versus
progesterone in various phases of the menstrual cycle on stress
reactivity as well as to identify the hormonal and neuroendocrine
mechanisms predicting these effects. Results indicated that EPI,
CI, and VRI reactivity to stress differ by menstrual phase, with the
greatest increases in EPI and CI, and the greatest decreases in VRI
(vasodilatory responses) occurring during the luteal phase, when
both estradiol and progesterone are elevated. While it must be
acknowledged that the overall effect of cycle phase on VRI did not
reach conventional levels of statistical significance, this likely
resulted from a Type II error as suggested by our post hoc tests
comparing the luteal and late follicular phases, where differences in
VRI were substantial and statistically significant.
It was found that EPI reactivity increased in the late follicular
phase and remained elevated during the luteal phase, mirroring
menstrual increases in estradiol rather than progesterone. Further-
more, stepwise regression analyses demonstrated that estradiol, but
not progesterone, was an independent predictor of EPI reactivity
during the luteal phase, as was cortisol reactivity, the latter poten-
tially reflecting the positive feedback effects involving the HPA and
Figure 4. Mean (+SEM) vascular resistance index (VRI) reactivity during SNS axes (Calogero, Gallucci, Chrousos, & Gold, 1988; Valentino,
the speech stress as a function of menstrual cycle phase. *p < .05. Foote, & Aston-Jones, 1983). That EPI but not NE reactivity was
Stress reactivity at different phases of the menstrual cycle 315

Table 4. Predictors of CI, VRI, and EPI Reactivity to Speech Stress on the Basis of Multiple Regression Analyses

Early follicular phase reactivity Late follicular phase reactivity Luteal phase reactivity
Predictor variables CI VRI EPI CI VRI EPI CI VRI EPI
Estradiol (ng/ml) – – – – – – – R = .08
2

β = .32
Progesterone (ng/ml) – – – – – – – – –
Baseline EPI (pg/ml) R2 = .31 – NA – – NA – – NA
β = .02
EPI reactivity (pg/ml) R2 = .05 – NA R2 = .05 – NA R2 = .43 R2 = .10 NA
β = .03 β = .01 β = .03 β = −1.24
Baseline NE (pg/ml) – – R2 = .06 – – – – R2 = .07 –
β = .09 β = −.34
NE reactivity (pg/ml) – – R2 = .12 R2 = .21 – R2 = .34 – – R2 = .39
β = .08 β = .01 β = .33 β = .29
Baseline cortisol (pg/ml) – – – – – – – – –
Cortisol reactivity (pg/ml) – – – – – – – – R2 = .04
β = 2.19
Total model R2 R2 = .36 – R2 = .18 R2 = .26 – R2 = .34 R2 = .43 R2 = .17 R2 = .51
p = <.001 p = .015 p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p = .019 p < .001

associated with menstrual cycle phase may be related to the fact In the case of CI, although reactivity was greater during the
that estradiol and plasma EPI share the adrenal gland as a common luteal phase, when both estradiol and progesterone are elevated, the
origin whereas release of NE is more dispersed throughout the stepwise regression analyses suggest that the elevated luteal phase
body. However, this explanation is inconsistent with the only other CI reactivity does not result from elevated sex steroid concentra-
study measuring catecholamine reactivity to the TSST across the tions, but rather appears to be driven by menstrual cycle increases
menstrual cycle (Childs et al., 2010), which found NE reactivity to in EPI reactivity (which was also greater in the luteal than the early
be greatest in the luteal relative to the early follicular phase. While follicular phase). While EPI reactivity also predicted CI reactivity
both the current study and the study by Childs et al. find greater in the early and late follicular phases, the amount of variance
catecholamine reactivity in the luteal phase, these cycle effects run explained in CI reactivity was substantially lower. The effect of EPI
counter to the literature examining the effect of exogenous estradiol on CI reactivity is consistent with the well-documented effects of
administration in perimenopausal (Komesaroff, Esler, & Sudhir, EPI on hemodynamic responses (Levy et al., 1997; Levy, Perez,
1999) and postmenopausal women (Girdler et al., 2004; Light Perny, Thivilier, & Gerard, 2011), and likely occurs through EPI’s
et al., 2001; Lindheim et al., 1992) and in men (Del Rio et al., stimulation of myocardial β1 adrenergic receptors, which mediate
1994), which find a catecholamine reactivity blunting effect of both inotropic and chronotropic activities of the myocardium to
estradiol. increase CI.
The human and animal literature suggests that estradiol attenu- Similarly for VRI reactivity, the stepwise regression analyses
ates reactivity of the sympathoadrenal nervous system through a did not support an independent role of either estradiol or proges-
variety of mechanisms (Hinojosa-Laborde, Chapa, Lange, & terone in driving luteal phase-related changes in VRI reactivity.
Haywood, 1999), including reduced sensitivity of α2 adrenergic Rather, greater luteal phase EPI reactivity and baseline NE con-
receptors to inhibitory signals (Del Rio, Verlardo, Zizzo, Marrama, centrations predicted the vasodilatory responses to stress seen in
& Della Casa, 1993) and decreased release and increased degrada- the luteal phase. However, the independent modulatory role of
tion of catecholamines at the adrenal medulla (Fernandez-Ruiz, catecholamines was weaker for VRI than CI. This is consistent
Bukhari, Martinez-Arrieta, Tresguerres, & Ramos, 1988). with the results of McFetridge & Sherwood (2000), who
However, the estradiol-mediated effects on sympathetic mecha- observed exaggerated luteal-phase decreases in VR in the absence
nisms that are observed when exogenous estradiol is delivered in of increased catecholamine reactivity, though that study was
isolation may be less evident in the presence of other hormones or limited by the use of a between-subjects design. Thus, other
processes occurring in the late follicular or luteal phases of the mechanisms not directly related to absolute catecholamine con-
menstrual cycle. For example, relative to control and estradiol centrations may alter VRI reactivity in the luteal phase. One pos-
treatment, combined estradiol and progesterone treatment sibility relates to the α-adrenoceptors. Catecholamine-induced
in postmenopausal women has been associated with greater stimulation of vascular α-adrenoceptors mediates vasoconstrictive
catecholamine reactivity to mental stress (Burleson et al., 1998). responses (Ahlquist, 1976). Alpha 2-adrenoceptor sensitivity has
However, our study found increased EPI reactivity in the late been found to be reduced in the luteal phase of the menstrual
follicular phase relative to the early follicular phase, suggesting cycle, at least in White women, though the mechanisms through
that the association between elevated estradiol concentrations and which this occurs are unknown (Freedman & Girgis, 2000). If
increased catecholamine reactivity is not solely explained by con- this mechanism were at play, although EPI release may increase
currently elevated progesterone concentrations. Clearly, the context in the luteal phase, its α-adrenoceptor-mediated vasoconstrictive
in which reproductive steroid hormones act on the sympathetic effects may be lessened while its β-adreneroceptor-mediated
nervous system, including the age, sex, and menopausal status of vasodilatory effects remain intact, thereby resulting in a net
the individual, is important. Furthermore, the acute actions of an vasodilatory tone. Future studies testing the effect of adrenergic
exogenously administered hormone may differ from the effects of receptor blockade on stress reactivity throughout the menstrual
long-term endogenous hormone exposure. cycle could help clarify this issue.
316 J.L. Gordon and S.S. Girdler

Of note, a menstrual cycle phase effect was found for the speech may limit the generalizability of the current findings to other repro-
task but not the math task. Though participants’ neuroendocrine ductive phases when estradiol and progesterone are particularly
and cardiovascular responses were in the same direction for both dynamic (e.g., puberty and during the menopause transition). Addi-
tasks (Table 2), reactivity was greatest during the speech. The fact tionally, because we did not assess subjective mood during the
that the math task was modified to involve an addition rather than TSST, we could not assess the effects of subjective responses to
a subtraction exercise, making the task easier than the traditional stress on physiologic stress reactivity. Lastly, because half of the
TSST, may be one explanation for the reduced physiological sample was exposed to pain testing prior to mental stress testing,
responses during the math task. The nature of the tasks themselves the possibility exists that there may have been carryover effects of
may also be important: traditionally, speech stressors have been pain-induced stress reactivity that could have influenced stress
viewed as more gender relevant for women, while mental arithme- reactivity to the TSST. However, because the order of pain versus
tic stressors may be more gender relevant for men (Darnall & stress testing was held constant for each subject, there is no reason
Suarez, 2009). Thus, the potency and nature of the stressor may to believe that the menstrual cycle effects on stress reactivity would
unveil a menstrual cycle effect on stress reactivity, as suggested by have been confounded by the order of events. Moreover, the 15-min
previous research using stressors varying greatly in intensity recovery between pain and stress testing was imposed to minimize
(Kajantie & Phillips, 2006). The current study addresses several carryover effects.
gaps in the literature on menstrual cycle and stress reactivity. It In conclusion, while this study showed that myocardial and
used a within-subjects design and is the only laboratory-based vascular determinants of BP reactivity vary with menstrual cycle
study examining stress reactivity at three key times throughout the phase, it also suggests that these hemodynamic determinants are
menstrual cycle, thereby examining menstrual cycle effects during more directly influenced by EPI reactivity to stress than by
three distinct hormonal milieus. Finally, our simultaneous meas- endogenous sex hormones. By and large, cardiovascular stress
urement of both hemodynamic and neuroendocrine reactivity in the reactivity in these medically and psychologically healthy women
same individuals allowed for the advancement of our current is remarkably stable throughout the menstrual cycle. The
understanding of neuroendocrine and hormonal mechanisms con- hemodynamic shifts in CI and VRI reactivity that were observed
tributing to myocardial and vascular stress reactivity in women across phases may represent an adaptive mechanism aimed at
across the reproductive cycle. keeping BP reactivity relatively constant. Taken together, the
Despite these strengths, the current study should be interpreted hemodynamic response profile observed during the luteal phase
in light of its limitations. The absence of stress or cycle effects on of the menstrual cycle, of enhanced CI reactivity but greater vaso-
the HPA axis measures may have resulted from (a) our choice to dilation to preserve stable BP reactivity, a profile seen in groups
conduct all test sessions in the morning when circadian effects on at lower risk for CVD (Allen et al., 1993; Girdler et al., 1990;
the HPA axis may override stress and cycle effects (Kudielka, Light et al., 1993; Sherwood et al., 2010; Treiber et al., 2000),
Schommer, Hellhammer, & Kirschbaum, 2004); (b) the absence of may be one mechanism by which the female sex hormones confer
a measure of free (biologically available) cortisol, which may have cardiac protection in premenopausal women. This is not to say,
obscured our ability to observe a menstrual cycle influence on however, that there may not be subgroups of women for whom
cortisol since estradiol influences corticotrophin-binding globulin hemodynamic mechanisms fail to maintain stable BP reactivity
levels (Kirschbaum et al., 1999); and (c) since cortisol and ACTH across the menstrual cycle. Future studies examining menstrual
were measured only once after the task, this may have limited our cycle influences on cardiovascular and neuroendocrine reactivity
ability to capture the dynamic activation of the HPA axis during and in women at higher risk for CVD, such as women who suffer
following mental stress. It should also be acknowledged that the from obesity or who have a strong family history of CVD, are
relatively young and narrow age range of the study participants warranted.

References

Ahlquist, R. P. (1976). Present state of alpha- and beta-adrenergic Childs, E., Dlugos, A., & De Wit, H. (2010). Cardiovascular, hormonal, and
drugs I. The adrenergic receptor. American Heart Journal, 92, 661–664. emotional responses to the TSST in relation to sex and menstrual cycle
Allen, M. T., Stoney, C. M., Owens, J. F., & Matthews, K. A. (1993). phase. Psychophysiology, 47, 550–559. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.
Hemodynamic adjustments to laboratory stress: The influence 00961.x
of gender and personality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 505– Christensen, N. J., & Schultz-Larsen, K. (1994). Resting venous plasma
517. adrenalin in 70-year-old men correlated positively to survival in a popu-
Burleson, M. H., Malarkey, W. B., Cacioppo, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., lation study: The significance of the physical working capacity. Journal
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Berntson, G. G. (1998). Postmenopausal of Internal Medicine, 235, 229–232. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.1994.
hormone replacement: Effects on autonomic, neuroendocrine, and tb01064.x
immune reactivity to brief psychological stressors. Psychosomatic Collins, A., Eneroth, P., & Landgren, B. M. (1985). Psychoneuroendocrine
Medicine, 60, 17–25. stress responses and mood as related to the menstrual cycle. Psychoso-
Calogero, A. E., Gallucci, W. T., Chrousos, G. P., & Gold, P. W. (1988). matic Medicine, 47, 512–527.
Catecholamine effects upon rat hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing Darnall, B. D., & Suarez, E. C. (2009). Sex and gender in psycho-
hormone secretion in vitro. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 82, 839– neuroimmunology research: Past, present and future. Brain, Behavior,
846. doi: 10.1172/JCI113687 and Immunity, 23, 595–604. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.02.019
Center for Disease Control. (2009). Women and heart disease fact sheet. Del Rio, G., Velardo, A., Menozzi, R., Zizzo, G., Tavernari, V., Venneri,
Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/dhdsp/data_statistics/fact_sheets/ M. G., . . . Petraglia, F. (1998). Acute estradiol and progesterone admin-
docs/fs_women_heart.pdf istration reduced cardiovascular and catecholamine responses to mental
Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2010). Greater cardiovascular responses to labora- stress in menopausal women. Neuroendocrinology, 67, 269–274. doi:
tory mental stress are associated with poor subsequent cardiovascular 10.1159/000054322
risk status: A meta-analysis of prospective evidence. Hypertension, 55, Del Rio, G., Velardo, A., Zizzo, G., Avogaro, A., Cipolli, C., & Della Casa,
1026–1032. L. (1994). Effect of estradiol on the sympathoadrenal response to mental
Stress reactivity at different phases of the menstrual cycle 317

stress in normal men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabo- Kirschbaum, C., Klauer, T., Filipp, S. H., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1995).
lism, 79, 836–840. doi: 10.1210/jc.79.3.836 Sex-specific effects of social support on cortisol and subjective
Del Rio, G., Verlardo, A., Zizzo, G., Marrama, P., & Della Casa, L. (1993). responses to acute psychological stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57,
Sex differences in catecholamine response to clonidine. International 23–31.
Journal of Obesity Related Metabolic Disorders, 17, 465–469. Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., &
Dimsdale, J. E., & Ziegler, M. E. (1991). What do plasma and urinary Hellhammer, D. H. (1999). Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase,
measures of catecholamines tell us about human response to stressors? and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-
[Supplement 4]. Circulation, 83, II36–II42. adrenal axis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 154–162.
Fernandez-Ruiz, J. J., Bukhari, A. R., Martinez-Arrieta, R., Tresguerres, Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The “Trier
J. A., & Ramos, J. A. (1988). Effects of estrogens and progesterone on Social Stress Test”—A tool for investigating psychobiological stress
the catecholaminergic activity of the adrenal medulla in female rats. Life responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 76–81. doi:
Science, 42, 1019–1028. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(88)90432-8 10.1159/000119004
Freedman, R. R., & Girgis, R. (2000). Effects of menstrual cycle and race Kirschbaum, C., Prussner, J. C., Stone, A. A., Federenko, I., Gaab, J., &
on peripheral vascular alpha-adrenergic responsiveness. Hypertension, Lintz, D. (1995). Persistent high cortisol responses to repeated psycho-
35, 795–799. logical stress in a subpopulation of healthy men. Psychosomatic Medi-
Ghali, J. K., Liao, Y., Simmons, B., Castaner, A., Cao, G., & Cooper, R. S. cine, 57, 468–474.
(1992). The prognostic role of left ventricular hypertrophy in patients Klatzkin, R. R., Mechlin, B., & Girdler, S. S. (2010). Menstrual cycle phase
with or without coronary artery disease. Annals of Internal Medicine, does not influence gender differences in experimental pain sensitivity.
117, 831–836. European Journal of Pain, 14, 77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.
Gianaros, P. J., Salomon, K., Zhou, F., Owens, J. F., Edmundowicz, D., & 01.002
Kuller, L. H. (2005). A greater reduction in high-frequency heart Komesaroff, P. A., Esler, M. D., & Sudhir, K. (1999). Estrogen supplemen-
rate variability to a psychological stressor is associated with tation attenuates glucocorticoid and catecholamine responses to mental
subclinical coronary and aortic calcification in postmenopausal stress in perimenopausal women. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology
women. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 553–560. doi: 10.1097/ and Metabolism, 84, 606–610. doi: 10.1210/jc.84.2.606
01.psy.0000170335.92770.7a Koren, M. J., Devereux, R. B., Casale, P. N., Savage, D. D., & Laragh, J. H.
Girdler, S. S., Hinderliter, A. L., Wells, E. C., Sherwood, A., Grewen, (1991). Relation of left ventricular mass and geometry to morbidity and
K. M., & Light, K. C. (2004). Transdermal versus oral estrogen therapy mortality in uncomplicated essential hypertension. Annals of Internal
in postmenopausal smokers: Hemodynamic and endothelial effects. Medicine, 114, 345–352.
Obstetrics & Gynecology, 103, 169–180. doi: 10.1097/01.AOG. Kubicek, W. G., Karnegis, J. N., Patterson, R. P., Witsoe, D. A., & Mattson,
0000103998.48122.0b R. H. (1966). Development and evaluation of an impedence cardiac
Girdler, S. S., & Light, K. C. (1994). Hemodynamic stress responses output system. Aerospace Medicine, 37, 1208–1212.
in men and women examined as a function of female menstrual Kudielka, B. M., Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C.
cycle phase. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 17, 233–248. (2004). Acute HPA axis responses, heart rate, and mood changes to
Girdler, S. S., Pedersen, C. A., Stern, R. A., & Light, K. C. (1993). Men- psychosocial stress (TSST) in humans at different times of day.
strual cycle and premenstrual syndrome: Modifiers of cardiovascular Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 983–992. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.
reactivity in women. Health Psychology, 12, 180–192. doi: 10.1037/ 2003.08.009
0278-6133.12.3.180 Levy, B., Bollaert, P. E., Charpentier, C., Nace, L., Audibert, G., & Bauer,
Girdler, S. S., Sherwood, A., Hinderliter, A. L., Leserman, J., Costello, P. (1997). Comparison of norepinephrine and dobutamine to epineph-
N. L., & Straneva, P. A. (2003). Biological correlates of abuse in women rine for hemodynamics, lactate metabolism, and gastric tonometric vari-
with premenstrual dysphoric disorder and healthy controls. ables in septic shock: A prospective, randomized study. Intensive Care
Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 849–856. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY. Medicine, 23, 282–287. doi: 10.1007/s001340050329
0000088593.38201.CD Levy, B., Perez, P., Perny, J., Thivilier, C., & Gerard, A. (2011). Compari-
Girdler, S. S., Turner, J. R., Sherwood, A., & Light, K. C. (1990). Gender son of norepinephrine-dobutamine to epinephrine for hemodynamics,
differences in blood pressure control during a variety of behavioral lactate metabolism, and organ function variables in cardiogenic shock.
stressors. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52, 571–591. A prospective, randomized pilot study. Critical Care Medicine, 39,
Gordon, J. L., Ditto, B., Lavoie, K. L., Pelletier, R., Campbell, T. S., 450–455. doi: 10.1097/CCM.0b013e3181ffe0eb
Arsenault, A., & Bacon, S. L. (2011). The effect of major depression on Levy, D., Anderson, K. M., Savage, D. D., Kannel, W. B., Christiansen,
post-exercise cardiovascular recovery. Psychophysiology, 48, 1605– J. C., & Castelli, W. P. (1988). Echocardiographically detected left
1610. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01232.x ventricular hypertrophy: Prevalence and risk factors. The Framingham
Gordon, T., Kannel, W. B., Hjortland, M. C., & McNamara, P. M. (1978). Heart Study. Annals of Internal Medicine, 108, 7–13.
Menopause and coronary heart disease. The Framingham Study. Annals Light, K. C., Hinderliter, A. L., West, S. G., Grewen, K. M., Steege, J. F.,
of Internal Medicine, 89, 157–161. & Sherwood, A. (2001). Hormone replacement improves hemodynamic
Gravetter, F. J., & Forzano, L.-A. B. (2011). Research methods for the profile and left ventricular geometry in hypertensive and normotensive
behavioral sciences (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Linda Schreiber-Ganster. postmenopausal women. Journal of Hypertension, 19, 269–278. doi:
Hinojosa-Laborde, C., Chapa, I., Lange, D., & Haywood, J. R. (1999). 10.1097/00004872-200102000-00014
Gender differences in sympathetic nervous system regulation. Clinical Light, K. C., Turner, J. R., Hinderliter, A. L., & Sherwood, A. (1993). Race
and Experimental Pharmacology and Physiology, 26, 122–126. doi: and gender comparisons: I. Hemodynamic responses to a series of
10.1046/j.1440-1681.1999.02995.x stressors. Health Psychology, 12, 354–365. doi: 10.1037/0278-
Hu, F. B., Grodstein, F., Hennekens, C. H., Colditz, G. A., Johnson, M., & 6133.12.5.354
Manson, J. E. (1999). Age at natural menopause and risk of cardiovas- Lindheim, S. R., Legro, R. S., Bernstein, L., Stanczyk, F. Z., Vijod, M. A.,
cular disease. Archives of Internal Medicine, 159, 1061–1066. doi: & Presser, S. C. (1992). Behavioral stress responses in premenopausal
10.1001/archinte.159.10.1061 and postmenopausal women and the effects of estrogen. American
Jacobsen, B. K., Knutsen, S. F., & Fraser, G. E. (1999). Age at natural Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology, 167, 1831–1836. doi: 0002-
menopause and total mortality and mortality from ischemic heart 9378(92)91783
disease: The Adventist Health Study. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, Lustyk, M. K., Douglas, H. A., Shilling, E. A., & Woods, N. F. (2012).
52, 303–307. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(98)00170-X Hemodynamic and psychological responses to laboratory stressors in
Kajantie, E., & Phillips, D. I. (2006). The effects of sex and hormonal status women: Assessing the roles of menstrual cycle phase, premenstrual
on the physiological response to acute psychosocial stress. symptomatology, and sleep characteristics. International Journal of
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 151–178. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen. Psychophysiology, 86, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.10.009
2005.07.002 Lustyk, M. K., Olson, K. C., Gerrish, W. G., Holder, A., & Widman, L.
Kasprowicz, A. L., Manuck, S. B., Malkoff, S. B., & Krantz, D. S. (1990). (2010). Psychophysiological and neuroendocrine responses to labora-
Individual differences in behaviorally evoked cardiovascular response: tory stressors in women: Implications of menstrual cycle phase and
Temporal stability and hemodynamic patterning. Psychophysiology, 27, stressor type. Biological Psychology, 83, 84–92. doi: 10.1016/
605–619. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb03181.x j.biopsycho.2009.11.003
318 J.L. Gordon and S.S. Girdler

Manhem, K., Jern, C., Pilhall, M., Shanks, G., & Jern, S. (1991). Sherwood, A., Turner, J. R., Light, K. C., & Blumenthal, J. A. (1990).
Haemodynamic responses to psychosocial stress during the menstrual Temporal stability of the hemodynamics of cardiovascular reactivity.
cycle. Clinical Science, 81, 17–22. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 10, 95–98.
Manuck, S. B. (1994). Cardiovascular reactivity in cardiovascular disease. Sita, A., & Miller, S. B. (1996). Estradiol, progesterone and cardiovascular
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 1, 4–31. response to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 21, 339–346. doi:
Matthews, K. A., Katholi, C. R., McCreath, H., Whooley, M. A., Williams, 10.1016/0306-4530(95)00053-4
D. R., & Zhu, S. (2004). Blood pressure reactivity to psychological Stoney, C. M., Langer, A. W., & Gelling, P. D. (1986). The effects of
stress predicts hypertension in the CARDIA study. Circulation, 110, menstrual cycle phase on cardiovascular and pulmonary responses to
74–78. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000133415.37578.E4 behavioral and exercise stress. Psychophysiology, 23, 393–402. doi:
McFetridge, J. A., & Sherwood, A. (2000). Hemodynamic and sympathetic 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00652.x
nervous system responses to stress during the menstrual cycle. AACN Stoney, C. M., Owens, J. F., Matthews, K. A., Davis, M. C., & Caggiula, A.
Clinical Issues, 11, 158–167. (1990). Influences of the normal menstrual cycle on physiologic func-
Pines, A., Fisman, E. Z., Levo, Y., Averbuch, M., Lidor, A., & Drory, Y. tioning during behavioral stress. Psychophysiology, 27, 125–135. doi:
(1991). The effects of hormone replacement therapy in normal post- 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb00364.x
menopausal women: Measurements of Doppler-derived parameters of Tombaugh, T. N. (2006). A comprehensive review of the paced auditory
aortic flow. American Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology, 164, 806– serial addition test (PASAT). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21,
812. 53–76. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.006
Polefrone, J. M., & Manuck, S. B. (1988). Effects of menstrual phase and Treiber, F. A., Jackson, R. W., Davis, H., Pollock, J. S., Kapuku, G., &
parental history of hypertension on cardiovascular response to cognitive Mensah, G. A. (2000). Racial differences in endothelin-1 at rest and in
challenge. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50, 23–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1469- response to acute stress in adolescent males. Hypertension, 35, 722–
8986.1989.tb01917.x 725. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.35.3.722
Pollard, T. M., Pearce, K. L., Rousham, E. K., & Schwartz, J. E. (2007). Do Valentino, R. J., Foote, S. L., & Aston-Jones, G. (1983). Corticotropin-
blood pressure and heart rate responses to perceived stress vary accord- releasing factor activates noradrenergic neurons of the locus coeruleus.
ing to endogenous estrogen level in women? American Journal of Brain Research, 270, 363–367. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90615-7
Physical Anthropology, 132, 151–157. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20468 Warner, R. (2012). Applied statistics: From bivariate through multivariate
Ren, J., Hintz, K. K., Roughead, Z. K., Duan, J., Colligan, P. B., & Ren, B. techniques. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications Inc.
H. (2003). Impact of estrogen replacement on ventricular myocyte con- Watkins, L. L., Blumenthal, J. A., & Carney, R. M. (2002). Association of
tractile function and protein kinase B/Akt activation. American Journal anxiety with reduced baroreflex cardiac control in patients after acute
of Physiology—Heart and Circulatory Physiology, 284, H1800–H1807. myocardial infarction. American Heart Journal, 143, 460–466. doi:
doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.00866.2002 10.1067/mhj.2002.120404
SAS Institute Inc. (2011). SAS/STAT 9.3 User’s guide. Cary, NC: SAS Whitworth, J. A., Williamson, P. M., Mangos, G., & Kelly, J. J. (2005).
Publishing. Cardiovascular consequences of cortisol excess. Vascular Health Risk
Sherwood, A., Allen, M. T., Fahrenberg, J., Kelsey, R. M., Lovallo, W. R., Management, 1, 291–299.
& van Doornen, L. J. (1990). Methodological guidelines for impedance Zoccali, C., Mallamaci, F., Parlongo, S., Cutrupi, S., Benedetto, F. A., &
cardiography. Psychophysiology, 27, 1–23. Tripepi, G. (2002). Plasma norepinephrine predicts survival and inci-
Sherwood, A., Park, S. B., Hughes, J. W., Blumenthal, J. A., Hinderliter, A., dent cardiovascular events in patients with end-stage renal disease.
& Trivedi, R. (2010). Cardiovascular hemodynamics during stress in Circulation, 105, 1354–1359. doi: 10.1161/hc1102.105261
premenopausal versus postmenopausal women. Menopause, 17, 403–
409. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181b9b061 (Received December 17, 2012; Accepted October 11, 2013)
This document is a scanned copy of a printed document. No warranty is given about the
accuracy of the copy. Users should refer to the original published version of the material.

You might also like