You are on page 1of 19

The Clinical Neuropsychologist

ISSN: 1385-4046 (Print) 1744-4144 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ntcn20

Adult Norms for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex


Figure Test and for Supplemental Recognition and
Matching Trials from the Extended Complex Figure
Test

Philip S. Fastenau , Natalie L. Denburg & Bradley J. Hufford

To cite this article: Philip S. Fastenau , Natalie L. Denburg & Bradley J. Hufford (1999) Adult
Norms for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and for Supplemental Recognition and
Matching Trials from the Extended Complex Figure Test, The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13:1,
30-47, DOI: 10.1076/clin.13.1.30.1976

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1076/clin.13.1.30.1976

Published online: 09 Aug 2010.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 4326

View related articles

Citing articles: 24 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ntcn20
The Clinical Neuropsychologist 1385-4046/99/1301-030$15.00
1999, Vol. 13, No. 1, pp. 30-47 © Swets & Zeitlinger

Adult Norms for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and


for Supplemental Recognition and Matching Trials from the
Extended Complex Figure Test*
Philip S. Fastenau1, Natalie L. Denburg2,3, and Bradley J. Hufford1
1Indiana University – Purdue University Indianapolis (IUPUI), Indianapolis, IN, 2Michigan State University,
East Lansing, MI, and 3Department of Veterans’ Affairs Medical Center, San Diego, CA

ABSTRACT

The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (‘‘the Rey’’; Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941) has accumulated a
considerable literature as a test of visual-spatial perception/construction and memory. The Extended Com-
plex Figure Test (ECFT; Fastenau, 1996a, in press-a; Fastenau & Manning, 1992) supplements the Rey
with Recognition and Matching trials that follow Copy, Immediate Recall, and Delayed Recall. The Rey
and ECFT were administered to 211 healthy adults. Age ranged from 30 years to 85 years (M = 62.9, SD
= 14.2), education ranged from 12 years to 25 years (M = 14.9, SD = 2.6), 55% were women, and over 95%
were Caucasian. Age and education effects were evident on all trials (Multiple R ranged .23 to .50, p < .05),
but education explained minimal variance (usually 2-3%) on copy and memory trials. Gender effects were
negligible, if present. Age-appropriate norms are presented using Osterrieth’s 36-point scoring, overlap-
ping cells, and convenient tables for converting raw scores to scaled scores.

The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (‘‘the perceptual organization is inferred from the con-
Rey’’; Rey, 1941; Osterrieth, 1944; Rey & Os- structive procedure employed by the individual
terrieth, 1993) was designed to measure both during the copy production. The examiner may
perceptual organization and visual memory in also note the time that lapses between start and
brain-injured persons. It has become a standard finish. Immediately following the copy trial, the
component in many neuropsychological batter- stimulus and copy are removed. The examiner
ies (e.g., Kaplan, 1988; Lezak, 1983, 1995; Or- provides a new blank sheet and asks the person
sini, Van Gorp, & Boone, 1988; Squire, 1986; to reproduce the image from memory (immedi-
Weintraub & Mesulam, 1985). ate recall). After a delay of 15 to 60 min, the
In the most popular administration of this test memory trial is repeated to measure delayed
(Knight, Kaplan, & Ireland, 1994; Lezak, 1995), recall; within this time span, the length of the
a person is asked to copy a complex geometric delay appears to be inconsequential to recall
stimulus on a blank sheet of paper, often using (Berry, Allen, & Schmitt, 1991).
five or six colored pens which are presented one The Rey uses an intricate stimulus that is
at a time by the examiner. The examiner can asymmetrical in its design. The complexity of
track the person’s constructional approach by this stimulus seems to tax the upper range of
recording the sequence of lines as they are visual-spatial processing better than other geo-
drawn or by recording the sequence of colors; metric stimuli. As a product of this complexity,

*
This manuscript is an elaboration of a paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the International Neuropsycho-
logical Society in Orlando, FL in February, 1997.
Address correspondence to: Philip S. Fastenau, Department of Psychology (LD 124), 402 N. Blackford Street,
Indiana University – Purdue University Indianapolis (IUPUI), Indianapolis, IN 46202-3275, USA. E-mail:
pfastena@iupui.edu.
Accepted for publication: July 21, 1998.
NORMS FOR THE REY AND ECFT 31

the Rey appears to be more resistant to verbal be compared to Delayed Recall to determine
mediation, even compared to other ‘‘complex’’ how much of the memory trace is retained over
figures such as the Taylor figure (Casey, Win- time (i.e., forgetting rates). Finally, free recall
ner, Hurwitz, & DaSilva, 1991). (both Immediate Recall and Delayed Recall)
The Rey has proven useful in the study of could be compared to Recognition performance;
normal child development (e.g., Karapetsas & if the latter were significantly better than the
Kantas, 1991; Waber & Holmes, 1985, 1986). former, then inefficient retrieval processes
Atypical child development, also, has been in- would be implicated. That is, the memory trace
vestigated using the Rey (e.g., Brandys & Rour- would have been encoded and stored, but the
ke, 1991; Klicpera, 1983; Prior & Hoffmann, person would need memory aids in order to ac-
1990). cess the memory trace.
The Rey is useful with adults, as well. It has ECFT Recognition was first designed by
proven to be sensitive to a wide range of Fastenau and Manning (1992) using clinical re-
nervous system insults, including solvent expo- cords, developmental literature, and theoretical
sure (Milanovic, Spilich, Vucinic, & Knezevic, literature. The original version was subjected to
1990), hippocampal sclerosis (Miller, Muñoz, & pilot study and expert appraisal, after which it
Finmore, 1993), Wernicke’s encephalopathy was revised into its final version which included
(Parkin, Dunn, Lee, O’Hara, & Nussbaum, a matching trial (Fastenau, 1996a, in press-a).
1993), asymptomatic HIV-seropositive status Reliability and validity for the new instrument
(McManis, Brown, Zachary, & Rundell, 1993), have been examined and reported in detail else-
AIDS (Hamby, Wilkins, & Barry, 1993), Alz- where (Fastenau, 1996a, in press-a, in press-b;
heimer’s-type dementia (Berry et al., 1991; Fastenau & Denburg, 1994; Fastenau & Fisk,
Brouwers, Cox, Martin, Chase, & Fedio, 1984), 1997a, 1997b; Fastenau & Manning, 1992). In
mild head injury (Leininger, Gramling, Farrell, addition to showing strong reliability and valid-
Kreutzer, & Peck, 1990), lateralized strokes ity, the Rey and ECFT have been shown to be
(Binder, 1982), and lateralized seizure foci robust with respect to several variations in ad-
(Fastenau & Fisk, 1997a, 1997b; Jones-Gotman, ministration (Berry et al., 1991; Fastenau, Ben-
1986; Loring, Lee, & Meador, 1988; Taylor, nett, & Fisk, 1996; Fastenau & Denburg, 1997;
1969). Fastenau, Denburg, & Domitrovic, 1997) and
Although the Rey has proven to be a very with respect to different scoring systems for the
useful tool, it lacks recognition and matching Rey figure drawings (Fastenau, Bennett, & Den-
trials, which can help clarify contributions of burg, 1996).
perception and memory retrieval to defective The ECFT has been used with several clinical
memory recall performances. Fastenau (Fas- populations. Recognition and matching scores
tenau, 1996a, in press-a; Fastenau & Manning, discriminate between patients with Alzheimer’s
1992) expanded the administration of the Rey by and matched controls (Fastenau, Unverzagt,
adding recognition and matching trials to the Koop, & Hufford, 1998). In addition, ECFT
existing copy, immediate recall, and delayed Recognition effectively discriminates patients
recall trials of the Rey. This elaborated adminis- with right focal temporal lobe epilepsy from
tration is collectively called the ‘‘Extended those with left epileptic foci (Fastenau & Fisk,
Complex Figure Test’’ (ECFT). 1997b). The ECFT has discriminated also be-
Fastenau (in press-b) described how a five- tween patients with severe depression and
trial administration could be used for diagnostics matched controls (Fastenau, Smet, et al., 1998).
and rehabilitation planning. In the interpretive Data collection is in progress with people who
process, the first step would be to examine have sustained lateralized strokes, particularly
Matching to verify accurate perception of the for determining the sensitivity and specificity of
stimulus. If perception were intact, the Copy the Left- and Right-Detail Subscales of the
trial could then be examined to assess the level ECFT (D. Johnson-Greene, personal communi-
of construction ability. Immediate Recall could cation, July 15, 1997). Data collection has also
32 PHILIP S. FASTENAU ET AL.

begun with people who have schizophrenia cumbersome than the equally reliable, equally
(Smet, 1997), in whom retrieval has been shown valid, and more widely used Osterrieth scoring
to be more affected than encoding (McClain, system (Fastenau et al., 1996; Knight et al.,
1983; Paulsen et al., 1995). 1994; Osterrieth, 1944; Rey & Osterrieth, 1993).
Adult norms are available for the Rey (e.g., In the present study, data from a much larger,
Berry et al., 1991; Boone, Lesser, Hill-Gutier- healthier sample were scored using the popular
rez, Berman, & D’Elia, 1993; Roselli & Ardila, and efficient Osterrieth scoring system. Norms
1991), but none of these include recognition or are derived and tabulated in convenient conver-
matching trials. Optimally, clinicians would sion tables.
want to compare all trial performances on the
same norms to make direct and equitable com-
parisons among trials. METHOD
Meyers and Meyers (1995) introduced a rec-
ognition trial for the Rey shortly after develop- Participants
ment began on the ECFT; they co-normed the The participants for this study were recruited at
Rey with their recognition trial. Although theirs three different sites, as part of other studies. For all
three sites, a stratified sampling procedure re-
appears to be a very useful recognition task, sev-
cruited community-dwelling adults with approxi-
eral differences between that test and the ECFT mately equal numbers of men and women in every
limit the applicability of those norms for inter- age band. Participants were financially compen-
pretation of the ECFT. First, the Meyers’ task sated for their participation. Volunteers with un-
does not provide a matching trial. Second, the corrected visual or hearing impairment or with
Meyers’ recognition trial uses a different format impaired use of the preferred hand were not in-
from the ECFT for assessing recognition. The cluded. Based on a structured interview, volunteers
were excluded for history of cerebrovascular insult
patient is shown 12 pieces of the Rey figure all (stroke or TIA), head injury with loss of con-
at once, together with an equal number of foils; sciousness exceeding 5-min duration, and chronic
the patient is instructed to circle the units that substance abuse. Volunteers were excluded also
were part of the stimulus. Because the patient is because of incomplete protocols.
allowed to examine all of the items at once, An educational cutoff was also implemented.
some pieces can be used to cue the recall of Even though a concerted effort was made to recruit
other units. Finally, the ‘‘immediate’’ recall trial people with lower education, there were very few
people (2%) who had fewer than 12 years of edu-
is administered after a 3-min delay followed by cation after all three samples were assembled. Peo-
yet another recall trial 30 min later. This admin- ple with fewer than 12 years of education score
istration is not typical of the common practices significantly lower than those with 12 or more
of neuropsychologists using the Rey (Knight et years of education on a variety of neuropsycholog-
al., 1994). ical tests (e.g., Bornstein & Suga, 1988; Hawkins
Preliminary norms for the ECFT were pro- et al., 1993; Ross & Lichtenberg, 1997). Fastenau
and colleagues (Fastenau, in press-c; Fastenau,
vided by Fastenau (1996a), but those, too, are
Denburg, & Mauer, in press) have argued that this
limited for several reasons. First, only 88 people can have dramatic effects on norms. Because the
comprised the initial sample. Second, that sam- least-educated population would be severely
ple retained individuals who were living inde- underrepresented in the present sample, it would
pendently in the community, even if they had be misleading to include these participants and to
sustained significant neurological insults; that suggest that these norms would serve that popula-
inclusion criterion may be sufficient for treat- tion equally well. Therefore, individuals with less
than 12 years education were excluded.
ment planning purposes (i.e., discriminating be-
The first sample was selected from a larger
tween people who can vs. cannot care for them- sample of 90 community-dwelling adults, who
selves), but is not optimal for diagnostic pur- were recruited from four religious organizations in
poses. As a final limitation to Fastenau’s a city in the Midwestern U.S. The larger sample
(1996a) preliminary norms, the data were pub- was used in other published papers (Fastenau,
lished using a scoring system that was more 1996a, 1996b; Fastenau & Denburg, 1994; Faste-
NORMS FOR THE REY AND ECFT 33

nau, Denburg, & Abeles, 1996a, 1996b; Fastenau, mated IQ for the total sample based on reading or
Denburg, & Domitrovic, 1997), and part of the vocabulary indices was 107, which is in the Aver-
larger sample was used by Fastenau, Bennett, and age range. This sample was predominantly Cauca-
Denburg (1996). Of that sample, 81 met inclusion sian (over 95%).
criteria for the present study. Age for the selected Scores for the recall drawings were generated
portion of that sample ranged from 30 years to 79 using Osterrieth’s criteria (Osterrieth, 1944; Rey &
years (M = 53.1, SD = 13.6), education ranged Osterrieth, 1993). Fastenau, Bennett, and Denburg
from 12 years to 25 years (M = 15.6, SD = 2.8), (1996) compared this relatively simple scoring
and 59% were women. WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981) system to one that was much more elaborate. The
Vocabulary subtest scores were obtained on this Osterrieth system was more efficient to learn, con-
subsample as an index of intellectual functioning. siderably easier to apply, and at least equally reli-
Age-corrected scaled scores ranged from 7 to 19; able for novice and experienced raters from differ-
the mean was 12.7 (SD = 2.3), which is roughly ent institutions. Correlations between recall and
equivalent to High Average IQ (approximately 110 recognition were equally strong for both scoring
– 115 estimated IQ). systems; the same was true for correlations be-
In another Midwestern sample, 100 participants tween copy and matching. The use of the Os-
were recruited through two local churches and an terrieth system for the normative data is further
older adult organization. Of those, 84 were re- supported by survey data showing that most Rey
tained for the present sample. The ECFT data from examiners use these scoring criteria (Knight et al.,
this sample have not appeared in any other pub- 1994). The scoring system is reprinted in Lezak
lished paper. Age for the selected portion of that (1995) and in Rey and Osterrieth (1993).
sample ranged from 60 years to 85 years (M = Recently, it has become popular to generate test
72.9, SD = 6.8), education ranged from 12 years to norms using multiple regression to correct for de-
20 years (M = 13.9, SD = 2.3), and 50% were mographic influences statistically. Limits of re-
women. For this subsample, WAIS-R Vocabulary gression-based norms have been argued theoreti-
age-corrected scaled scores ranged from 7 to 15; cally (Fastenau & Adams, 1996) and demonstrated
the mean was 11.1 (SD = 1.9), which is roughly empirically (Fastenau, 1998, in press-c). Thus,
equivalent to Average IQ (estimated IQ of approxi- ECFT norms will not rely on this method. Instead,
mately 105). As an additional index of intellectual salient demographic influences are determined by
functioning on this subsample, WRAT-3 (Wil- statistical analysis, and then the sample is subdi-
kinson, 1993) Reading subtest scores were ob- vided into representative subgroups. Raw scores
tained, ranging from 86 to 117; the mean score (M are converted to T scores and tabulated by age and
= 103.2, SD = 8.4) is roughly equivalent to mid- education groups for easy reference.
Average IQ, consistent with Vocabulary-based
estimates. Procedure
The third sample was recruited in a major met- All participants were administered the Rey and the
ropolitan area on the west coast of the U.S., using ECFT as part of a larger battery of tests. Testing
newspaper advertisements, local churches, and was administered by graduate students in clinical
senior citizen organizations. A total of 49 partici- psychology, all of whom had completed the assess-
pants completed testing in that study. Of those, 46 ment training sequence of an APA-accredited doc-
were retained for the present sample. The ECFT toral training program and had substantial testing
data from this sample have not appeared in any experience.
other published paper. Age for the selected portion
of that sample ranged from 40 years to 81 years (M Analyses
= 62.8, SD = 13.0), education ranged from 12 years Multiple regression (MR) analyses were used to
to 20 years (M = 15.4, SD = 2.1), and 59% were assess the effects of age and education, both as
women. Only WRAT-3 Reading subtest scores main effects and in interaction. MR was preferred
were obtained on this subsample, ranging from 71 over the use of analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
to 117; the mean score (M = 105.7, SD = 8.4) is because the former capitalizes on the full range of
roughly equivalent to an estimated IQ in the Aver- variability in these two continuous independent
age range. variables. Based on earlier studies with the Rey,
A total of 211 participants were retained for the age and education effects have been observed with
present study. For the entire sample, age ranged some consistency; gender effects, on the other
from 30 years to 85 years (M = 62.9, SD = 14.2), hand, have consistently been very small and have
education ranged from 12 years to 25 years (M = explained less than 3% of the variance (see Fas-
14.9, SD = 2.6), and 55% were women. The esti- tenau, in press-b, for more discussion). Therefore,
34 PHILIP S. FASTENAU ET AL.

age and education influences were tested under the For Matching Total Scale, there was an Age ×
hypothesis of lower ECFT performance with in- Education interaction, Multiple R = .43, p
creasing age and with decreasing education (one- .00005. For Matching Left-Detail Subscale,
tailed). Dependent variables were ECFT Copy,
there was a main effect for education only, Mul-
Immediate Recall, Delayed Recall, Recognition,
and Matching, together with all scale and subscale tiple R = .25, p .0005. For Matching Right-De-
scores for the latter two trials. tail Subscale, there was an Age × Education in-
teraction, Multiple R = .36, p .00005.
In post hoc analyses, gender and its interac-
RESULTS tions with age and education were analyzed,
again using MR. There was a main effect for
For Copy, there was a main effect for age (p gender on Copy (p .05); beyond the age and
.05, explaining 3% of the variance) and for edu- education effects, however, very little variance
cation (p .05, explaining an additional 2% of ( < 2%) was explained by gender. For Immedi-
the variance), Multiple R = .23, p .005. For ate Recall, Delayed Recall, and all scales and
Immediate Recall, there was a main effect for subscales of Recognition and Matching, gender
age (p .00005, explaining 14% of the variance) failed to enter into the equation as a main effect
and for education (p .0005, explaining an addi- or in interaction (p > .05, two-tailed).
tional 5% of the variance), Multiple R = .44, p In Tables 1 through 9, the data were tabulated
.00005. For Delayed Recall, there was a main in a manner recommended by Fastenau and Ad-
effect for age (p .00005, explaining 13% of the ams (1996). Because education explained very
variance) and for education (p .005, explaining little variance beyond age on Copy, Immediate
an additional 3% of the variance), Multiple R = Recall, Delayed Recall, and Recognition, these
.40, p .00005. There were no interactions for data were stratified by age only; this observation
Copy, Immediate, or Delayed trials. of very small education influences and the
For Recognition Total Scale, there was a choice to stratify by age alone follows the prece-
main effect for age (p .00005, explaining 22% dent by Meyers and Meyers (1995) for their rec-
of the variance) and for education (p .01, ex- ognition trial. Normative groups were created
plaining an additional 2% of the variance), Mul- using overlapping cells, a method introduced by
tiple R = .49, p .00005. For Recognition Global Pauker (1988). Scores were tabulated for easy
Scale, there was a main effect for age (p .0001, conversion to scaled scores that have a mean of
explaining 8% of the variance) and for education 10 and a standard deviation of 3, using the same
(p .001, explaining an additional 3% of the layout as the WAIS-R tables (Wechsler, 1981)
variance), Multiple R = .32, p .00005. For Rec- and Mayo norms (Ivnik et al., 1992). The tables
ognition Detail Scale, there was a main effect were generated using the method described by
for age (p .00005, explaining 23% of the vari- Ivnik and his colleagues (1992). The sample size
ance) and for education (p .05, explaining an and gender ratio are provided for each age
additional 2% of the variance), Multiple R = .50, group. Within each group, the mean and stan-
p .00005. For Recognition Left-Detail dard deviation are provided for each test score.
Subscale, there was a main effect for age (p To use the tables, the user should first iden-
.00005, explaining 18% of the variance) and for tify the table that has a midpoint age closest to
education (p .05, explaining an additional 2% the examinee’s age. Each test score is tabulated
of the variance), Multiple R = .45, p .00005. in its own column. Locate the examinee’s score
For Recognition Right-Detail Subscale, there in the appropriate column. The value in the left
was a main effect for age (p .00005, explaining margin corresponding to the examinee’s score is
21% of the variance) but not for education (p > the age-corrected scaled score; the value in the
.10), Multiple R = .46, p .00005. There were right margin is the centile rank corresponding to
no interactions for any Recognition scale or that same scaled score.
subscale score.
Table 1. Raw Score to Scaled Score Conversions for the Rey and for the Extended Complex Figure Test Recognition Trial.
Midpoint Age = 40 (Age Range = 30 – 50; n = 48).

Recognition Scales

Scaled Scores Copy Immediate Delayed Total Global Detail Left Right Percentile Range
2 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– <1
3 0–22 0–4 0–4 0–4 –– –– –– 0–1 1
4 22.5–25 4.5–8 4.5–9 5–7 0 0–4 –– 2 2
5 25.5 8.5 –– 8 1 5–6 0 –– 3–5
6 26–28.5 9–11 9.5–10 9 2 –– 1 3 6–10

7 29–29.5 11.5–12.5 10.5–11 10–12 –– 7 –– 4 11–18


8 30–30.5 13–13.5 11.5–13.5 13 3 8–9 2 5 19–28

NORMS FOR THE REY AND ECFT


9 31–32.5 14–17.5 14–16.5 14–15 4 10–11 3 6 29–40

10 33–33.5 18–20.5 17–20 16–19 5 12–14 4 7 41–59

11 34–34.5 21–24 20.5–23 20–21 –– 15 –– 8 60–71


12 –– 24.5–27 23.5–25.5 22–23 6 16–17 5–6 –– 72–81
13 35–35.5 27.5–29.5 26–29.5 –– –– –– –– –– 82–89

14 –– 30–32.5 30–31 24–26 –– 18–19 –– 9 90–94


15 –– 33–33.5 31.5–32.5 –– –– –– 7 10 95–97
16 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– 98
17 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– 99
18 36 34–36 33–36 27–30 7 20–23 8–9 11 > 99

M 32.83 20.3 19.28 17.92 4.88 13.04 4.33 7.00


(SD) (3.10) (7.42) (7.29) (5.63) (1.79) (4.29) (1.95) (2.21)

Note. 54% female.

35
36
Table 2. Raw Score to Scaled Score Conversions for the Rey and for the Extended Complex Figure Test Recognition Trial.
Midpoint Age = 48 (Age Range = 40 – 55; n = 47).

Recognition Scales

Scaled Scores Copy Immediate Delayed Total Global Detail Left Right Percentile Range
2 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– <1
3 0–13 0–4 0–4 0–4 –– 0–2 –– 0–1 1
4 13.5–19.5 4.5–5.5 4.5–5 5 0–1 3 0 2 2
5 20–22 6–8 5.5–7.5 6–7 –– 4 –– –– 3–5
6 22.5–24.5 8.5–10 8–9 8–9 2 5–6 –– –– 6–10

7 25–29.5 10.5–11 9.5–10 10 –– –– 1 3 11–18


8 30–30.5 11.5–12.5 10.5–11.5 11–13 3 7–8 –– 4 19–28
9 31–31.5 13–15 12–13.5 14 –– 9–10 2 5 29–40

PHILIP S. FASTENAU ET AL.


10 32–33.5 15.5–18.5 14–18 15–17 4 11–13 3 6–7 41–59

11 –– 19–20.5 18.5–20 18–20 5 14 –– 8 60–71


12 34–34.5 21–23.5 20.5–23.5 21 –– 15 4–5 –– 72–81
13 35–35.5 24–28.5 24–26.5 22–23 –– 16–17 6 –– 82–89

14 –– 29–29.5 27–30.5 24 6 –– –– –– 90–94


15 –– 30–31.5 31 –– –– 18 7 9 95–97
16 –– 32–33.5 31.5–32.5 25–26 –– 19 –– –– 98
17 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– 99
18 36 34–36 33–36 27–30 7 20–23 8–9 10–11 > 99

M 31.79 17.87 17.13 16.60 4.62 11.98 3.70 6.66


(SD) (4.55) (6.84) (7.04) (5.45) (1.57) (4.37) (1.96) (2.30)

Note. 62% female.


Table 3. Raw Score to Scaled Score Conversions for the Rey and for the Extended Complex Figure Test Recognition Trial.
Midpoint Age = 53 (Age Range = 45– 60; n = 43).

Recognition Scales

Scaled Scores Copy Immediate Delayed Total Global Detail Left Right Percentile Range
2 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– <1
3 0–13 0–3.5 0–4 0–3 –– –– –– 0 1
4 13.5–19.5 4–5.5 4.5–5 4 0 0–2 0 1–2 2
5 20–22 6–8 5.5–7.5 5 1 3 –– –– 3–5
6 22.5–24.5 8.5–10 8–9.5 6–9 2 4–6 –– –– 6–10

7 25–28.5 10.5–11 10 10 –– –– 1 3 11–18


8 29–30.5 11.5–12.5 10.5–13 11–13 –– 7–9 –– 4 19–28

NORMS FOR THE REY AND ECFT


9 31–31.5 13–15.5 13.5 14 3–4 10 2 5 29–40

10 32–33.5 16–19 14–18 15–17 –– 11–13 3 6–7 41–59

11 –– 19.5–20.5 18.5–20.5 18–20 5 14 –– 8 60–71


12 34–34.5 21–25.5 21–24.5 21 6 15 4–5 –– 72–81
13 –– 26–28.5 25–26.5 22–23 –– 16 –– –– 82–89

14 35–35.5 29–29.5 27–30.5 24 –– 17 6 –– 90–94


15 –– 30–31.5 31 –– –– 18 7 9 95–97
16 –– 32–33.5 31.5–32.5 25–26 –– 19 –– –– 98
17 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– 99
18 36 34–36 33–36 27–30 7 20–23 8–9 10–11 > 99

M 31.62 18.07 17.48 16.65 4.74 11.91 3.56 6.67


(SD) (4.61) (7.11) (7.11) (5.61) (1.77) (4.43) (2.00) (2.36)

Note. 53% female.

37
38
Table 4. Raw Score to Scaled Score Conversions for the Rey and for the Extended Complex Figure Test Recognition Trial.
Midpoint Age = 58 (Age Range = 50– 65; n = 57).

Recognition Scales

Scaled Scores Copy Immediate Delayed Total Global Detail Left Right Percentile Range
2 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– <1
3 0–13 0–3.5 0–4 0–3 0 –– –– 0 1
4 13.5–17 4–5 4.5–5 4–5 1 0–2 –– 1–2 2
5 17.5–22 5.5 5.5–7.5 6–8 –– 3–4 0 –– 3–5
6 22.5–24.5 6–8.5 –– 9 2 –– –– –– 6–10

7 25–28.5 9–10 8–10 10 –– 5–6 –– 3 11–18


8 29–30.5 10.5–12.5 10.5–13 11–12 3 7 1 4 19–28
9 31–31.5 13–15 13.5 13 4 8–9 –– 5 29–40

PHILIP S. FASTENAU ET AL.


10 32–32.5 15.5–17.5 14–17.5 14–16 –– 10–11 2 6 41–59

11 33–33.5 18–19.5 18–19 17 5 12–13 3 7 60–71


12 –– 20–21 19.5–22.5 18–20 –– 14 4 8 72–81
13 34–34.5 21.5–23 23–24 21 6 15 5 –– 82–89

14 –– 23.5–25.5 24.5 22 –– 16 –– –– 90–94


15 35–35.5 26–28.5 25–27 23–24 –– 17 6 –– 95–97
16 –– 29–31.5 27.5–31 –– –– 18 7 9 98
17 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– 99
18 36 32–36 31.5–36 25–30 7 19–23 8–9 10–11 > 99

M 31.17 16.55 16.44 15.77 4.82 10.95 3.05 6.32


(SD) (4.43) (6.08) (6.08) (4.76) (1.53) (3.93) (1.89) (2.11)

Note. 61% female.


Table 5. Raw Score to Scaled Score Conversions for the Rey and for the Extended Complex Figure Test Recognition Trial.
Midpoint Age = 63 (Age Range = 55 – 70; n = 77).

Recognition Scales

Scaled Scores Copy Immediate Delayed Total Global Detail Left Right Percentile Range
2 0–17 –– –– 0–3 –– –– –– –– <1
3 17.5–23.5 0–3.5 0–3.5 4–5 –– –– –– –– 1
4 –– –– –– –– 0 0–2 –– 0 2
5 24–24.5 4–5.5 4–7.5 6–7 1 3 –– 1–2 3–5
6 25–27.5 6–8.5 8–8.5 8 2 4 0 –– 6–10

7 28–29.5 9–10 9–10 9 –– 5–6 –– 3 11–18


8 30–30.5 10.5–11 10.5–12.5 10 3 7 1 –– 19–28

NORMS FOR THE REY AND ECFT


9 31–31.5 11.5–13.5 13–14 11–12 –– –– –– 4 29–40

10 32–33.5 14–16.5 14.5–17 13–14 4 8–9 2 5 41–59

11 –– 17–19 17.5–18.5 15–16 5 10–11 3 6 60–71


12 –– 19.5–20.5 19–20.5 17 –– –– –– –– 72–81
13 34–34.5 21–22.5 21–23 18–19 6 12–13 4 7 82–89

14 35–35.5 23–24.5 23.5–24.5 20–21 –– 14 5 8 90–94


15 –– 25–26.5 25–26.5 22–24 –– 15–17 –– –– 95–97
16 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– 98
17 –– 27–29 27–32 –– –– –– 6–7 –– 99
18 36 29.5–36 32.5–36 25–30 7 18–23 8–9 9–11 > 99

M 31.94 15.52 15.91 14.38 4.71 9.66 2.70 5.39


(SD) (3.37) (5.82) (5.91) (4.43) (1.64) (3.52) (1.73) (2.07)

Note. 55% female.

39
40
Table 6. Raw Score to Scaled Score Conversions for the Rey and for the Extended Complex Figure Test Recognition Trial.
Midpoint Age = 68 (Age Range = 60 – 75; n = 96).

Recognition Scales

Scaled Scores Copy Immediate Delayed Total Global Detail Left Right Percentile Range
2 0–17 –– –– 0–2 –– –– –– 0 <1
3 17.5–19 0–3.5 0–3.5 3 0 –– –– 1 1
4 19.5–21 4–5 4–5.5 4 –– 0–2 –– –– 2
5 21.5–24.5 5.5 6–6.5 5–6 1 3 –– –– 3–5
6 25–27.5 6–8.5 7–8 7–8 –– 4 –– 2 6–10

7 28–28.5 9–9.5 8.5–9.5 9 2 5 0 –– 11–18


8 29–30.5 10–11 10–11.5 10 –– 6 –– 3 19–28
9 31–31.5 11.5–13 12–13.5 11–12 3 7 1 –– 29–40

PHILIP S. FASTENAU ET AL.


10 32–33.5 13.5–16 14–16 13 4 8–9 2 4–5 41–59

11 –– 16.5–18 16.5–18 14–15 –– 10 –– –– 60–71


12 34–34.5 18.5–20 18.5–19 16–17 5 11 3 6 72–81
13 –– 20.5–22 19.5–22 18 6 12–13 4 –– 82–89

14 35–35.5 22.5–24 22.5–24 19–20 –– 14 5 7 90–94


15 –– 24.5 24.5 21–22 –– 15 –– 8 95–97
16 –– 25–25.5 25–26 –– –– 16–17 –– –– 98
17 –– 26–29 26.5–32 23–24 –– –– 6–7 –– 99
18 36 29.5–36 32.5–36 25–30 7 18–23 8–9 9–11 > 99

M 31.76 15.18 15.29 13.57 4.42 9.16 2.54 5.11


(SD) (3.63) (5.58) (5.57) (4.43) (1.69) (3.45) (1.78) (1.87)

Note. 56% female.


Table 7. Raw Score to Scaled Score Conversions for the Rey and for the Extended Complex Figure Test Recognition Trial.
Midpoint Age = 73 (Age Range = 65–80; n = 102).

Recognition Scales

Scaled Scores Copy Immediate Delayed Total Global Detail Left Right Percentile Range
2 0–14 –– –– 0–2 –– –– –– 0 <1
3 14.5–19 0–3.5 0–3.5 3 0 –– –– 1 1
4 19.5–21 4–5 4 4 –– –– –– –– 2
5 21.5–23.5 5.5 4.5–6 5 –– 0–2 –– –– 3–5
6 24–26.5 6–7.5 6.5–7 6 1 3 –– 2 6–10

7 27–27.5 8–9 7.5–9 7 –– 4–5 0 –– 11–18


8 28–29.5 9.5–11 9.5–11 8–9 2 6 –– 3 19–28

NORMS FOR THE REY AND ECFT


9 30–30.5 11.5–12.5 11.5–12.5 10–11 3 7 1 –– 29–40

10 31–32.5 13–15.5 13–15.5 12–13 4 8 2 4 41–59

11 33–33.5 16–17 16–17 14 –– 9–10 3 5 60–71


12 34–34.5 17.5–19 17.5–19 15 5 11 –– 6 72–81
13 35–35.5 19.5–21.5 19.5–21.5 16–18 –– 12 –– –– 82–89

14 –– 22–24 22–23 19 6 13 4–5 –– 90–94


15 –– 24.5 23.5–25.5 20–21 –– 14–15 –– 7 95–97
16 –– –– 26 22 –– –– –– 8 98
17 –– 25–29 26.5–32 –– –– 16–17 6–7 –– 99
18 36 29.5–36 32.5–36 23–30 7 18–23 8–9 9–11 > 99

M 31.38 14.68 14.61 12.80 4.05 8.75 2.46 4.85


(SD) (4.00) (5.49) (5.58) (4.44) (1.77) (3.35) (1.73) (1.74)

Note. 52% female.

41
42
Table 8. Raw Score to Scaled Score Conversions for the Rey and for the Extended Complex Figure Test Recognition Trial.
Midpoint Age = 78 (Age Range = 70 – 85; n = 83).

Recognition Scales

Scaled Scores Copy Immediate Delayed Total Global Detail Left Right Percentile Range
2 0–14 –– 0–1.5 0–2 –– –– –– 0 <1
3 14.5–19 –– 2–3.5 3 0 –– –– 1 1
4 19.5–21 0–3.5 4 4 –– –– –– –– 2
5 21.5–22.5 4–5.5 4.5–5.5 –– –– 0–2 –– –– 3–5
6 23–25.5 6–7 6–6.5 5 –– 3 –– –– 6–10

7 26–27.5 7.5–8.5 7–7.5 6 1 4 0 2 11–18


8 28–28.5 9–10 8–9.5 7–8 2 5 –– –– 19–28
9 29–30.5 10.5–11.5 10–12 9–10 –– 6 1 3 29–40

PHILIP S. FASTENAU ET AL.


10 31–32.5 12–14 12.5–14 11–13 3 7–8 –– 4 41–59

11 33–33.5 14.5–16.5 14.5–16.5 14 4 9 2 5 60–71


12 –– 17–18.5 17–18 15 –– 10–11 3 6 72–81
13 34–34.5 19–21 18.5–20.5 16–18 5 12 –– –– 82–89

14 35–35.5 21.5–23.5 21–22 19 –– 13–14 4–5 –– 90–94


15 –– 24 22.5–24 20 6 15 –– 7 95–97
16 –– 24.5 –– 21 –– –– –– –– 98
17 –– 25–28 24.5–25.5 22 –– –– –– 8 99
18 36 28.5–36 26–36 23–30 7 16–23 6–9 9–11 > 99

M 30.87 14.01 13.64 12.08 3.80 8.29 2.30 4.63


(SD) (4.16) (5.45) (5.31) (4.68) (1.72) (3.44) (1.69) (1.84)

Note. 53% female.


Table 9. Raw Score to Scaled Score Conversions for the Rey and for the Extended Complex Figure Test Recognition Trial.
Midpoint Age = 80 (Age Range = 75 – 85; n = 49).

Recognition Scales

Scaled Scores Copy Immediate Delayed Total Global Detail Left Right Percentile Range
2 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– <1
3 0–14 –– 0–1.5 0–3 –– –– –– 0 1
4 14.5–19 0–3.5 2–4 4 –– –– –– 1 2
5 19.5–22 4–5 4.5 –– –– 0–2 –– –– 3–5
6 22.5–24 5.5–7.5 5–6.5 5 –– 3 –– –– 6–10

7 24.5–27 8 7–7.5 6 0 –– 0 –– 11–18


8 27.5 8.5–10 8–9 –– 1 4–5 –– 2 19–28

NORMS FOR THE REY AND ECFT


9 28–28.5 10.5–11 9.5–11.5 7–8 2 –– 1 3 29–40

10 29–31.5 11.5–14 12–14 9–13 3 6–8 –– 4 41–59

11 32–33.5 14.5–16.5 14.5–15.5 14 4 9 2–3 5 60–71


12 –– 17–18.5 16–17 –– –– 10 –– –– 72–81
13 34–34.5 19 17.5–19.5 15–16 –– 11 –– 6 82–89

14 35–35.5 19.5–23 20–22 17–19 5 12–13 4 –– 90–94


15 –– 23.5 22.5–24 20 6 14 5 7 95–97
16 –– 24–28 24.5–25.5 –– –– 15 –– –– 98
17 –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– 99
18 36 28.5–36 26–36 21–30 7 16–23 6–9 8–11 > 99

M 30.14 13.67 13.17 11.47 3.45 8.02 2.27 4.51


(SD) (4.52) (5.38) (5.32) (4.80) (1.74) (3.46) (1.62) (1.87)

Note. 51% female.

43
44 PHILIP S. FASTENAU ET AL.

Matching data warranted different treatment the popular Osterrieth scoring system. The data
from the other trials for several reasons. First, are tabulated in a useful format, following many
these subscales showed stronger education influ- of the recommendations by Fastenau and Adams
ences, in addition to age. Second, these distribu- (1996).
tions were very skewed. Finally, two of the three This sample was moderately to well educated
Matching scores were based only on four items. (high school education or higher), although the
Thus, these scores were best treated as screening average estimated IQ was in the Average range.
tools. Table 10 lists each variable, stratified by The participants were predominantly Caucasian,
education and by age, together with the cutoff and all were fluent in English. Applications to
values for the 5th centile and for the 15th per- other ethnic or linguistic groups should be quali-
centile. For the appropriate scale and the appro- fied accordingly. Although gender ratios vary
priate age-education subgroup, a score at or be- considerably among groups, these should be in-
low the tabled value would be abnormally low at consequential given the lack of interaction be-
the specified level. For example, a score of 8 tween gender and age.
would be clearly abnormal for an older adult There are many new prospects for further re-
with a college education (at or below the 5th search. First, although the ECFT was designed
centile) but within normal limits for an elder with children in mind, there are currently no
with a high school education (above the 15th normative data for children or adolescents. Sec-
centile). ond, it would be invaluable to examine the char-
acteristics of the ECFT with theoretically rele-
vant patient groups. For example, people with
DISCUSSION left and right hemi-inattention should show dif-
ferentiation of the Left- and Right-Detail Sub-
Research has been mounting for the Rey and for scales on both Recognition and Matching; peo-
the recognition and matching trials that supple- ple with multiple sclerosis and people with
ment the Rey figure to comprise the ECFT. The pseudodementia should show characteristic re-
present study builds on the rapidly emerging trieval inefficiencies (i.e., poor recall and stron-
literature on these tests by providing norms from ger recognition) whereas those with Alzhei-
a relatively large sample of healthy adults using mer’s-type dementia should show encoding fail-

Table 10. Extended Complex Figure Test Matching Trial Cutoff Scores (Percentile Rank), by Age and Educa-
tion.

Younger Older
(Ages 30 – 65) (Ages 66 – 85)

Subscale Less Educ More Educ Less Educ More Educ


(12 – 14 Years) (15+ Years) (12 – 14 Years) (15+ Years)
(n = 39) (n = 62) (n = 65) (n = 45)
Matching Total
5th centile 7 8 7 8
15th centile 8 9 7 9

Matching Left-Detail
5th centile 2 3 3 3
15th centile 3 4 3 3

Matching Right-Detail
5th centile 2 2 1 3
15th centile 3 3 2 3
NORMS FOR THE REY AND ECFT 45

ure and rapid forgetting (i.e., poor recall and abled children. In B. P. Rourke (Ed.), Neuropsy-
recognition). chological validation of learning disability sub-
types (pp. 73-96). New York: Guilford Press.
Third, it could be very useful to standardize
Brouwers, P., Cox, C., Martin, A., Chase, T., & Fedio,
alternative administrations of the ECFT for pa- P. (1984). Differential perceptual-spatial impair-
tients with manual impairments, develop parallel ment in Huntington’s and Alzheimer’s dementias.
forms for intervention studies and longitudinal Archives of Neurology, 41, 1073-1076.
research, and assess test-retest reliability. Com- Casey, M. B., Winner, E., Hurwitz, I., & DaSilva, D.
(1991). Does processing style affect recall of the
puting risk ratios for various diagnoses or func- Rey-Osterrieth or Taylor Complex Figures? Jour-
tional outcomes is yet another avenue of re- nal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychol-
search with the ECFT (see Bieliauskas, Faste- ogy, 13, 600-606.
nau, Lacy, & Roper, 1998). Conant, L. L., Fastenau, P. S., Giordani, B., Boivin,
Finally, the limited verbal demands of the M. J., Chounramany, C., Xaisida, S., Choula-
mountry, L., Pholsena, P., & Olness, K. (1997).
ECFT would seem to lend it to cross-cultural Relationships among memory span tasks: A cross-
applications. Cross-cultural investigations of cultural, developmental perspective [Abstract]. The
visual-spatial memory development are espe- Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11, 304.
cially intriguing given the apparent role of read- Conant, L. L., Fastenau, P. S., Giordani, B., Boivin,
ing training in the cerebral organization of vi- M. J., Opel, B., & Nseyila, D. D. (1997). Develop-
mental trends in visual and verbal memory span in
sual memory span (Conant, Fastenau, Giordani, Zaïrian children [Abstract]. Journal of the Interna-
Boivin, Chounramany et al., 1997; Conant, Fas- tional Neuropsychological Society, 3, 26.
tenau, Giordani, Boivin, Opel, & Nseyila, 1997, Conant, L. L., Fastenau, P. S., Giordani, B., Boivin,
1998; Fastenau, Conant, & Lauer, 1998). These M. J., Opel, B., & Nseyila, D. D. (1998). Modality
research possibilities and others are considered specificity of memory span tasks among Zaïrian
children: A developmental study. Manuscript sub-
in greater detail by Fastenau (in press-b). mitted for publication.
Fastenau, P. S. (1996a). Development and preliminary
standardization of the ‘‘Extended Complex Figure
REFERENCES Test’’ (ECFT). Journal of Clinical and Experimen-
tal Neuropsychology, 18, 63-76.
Fastenau, P. S. (1996b). An elaborated administration
Berry, D. T. R., Allen, R. S., & Schmitt, F. A. (1991).
of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. The Clini-
Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Psychometric
cal Neuropsychologist, 10, 425-434.
characteristics in a geriatric sample. The Clinical
Fastenau, P. S. (1998). Distortions in regression-based
Neuropsychologist, 5, 143-153.
norms: Effects of age and education corrections
Bieliauskas, L. A., Fastenau, P. S., Lacy, M. A., &
among older adults [Abstract]. Journal of the Inter-
Roper, B. L. (1997). Use of the odds ratio to trans-
national Neuropsychological Society, 4, 54.
late neuropsychological test scores into real-world
Fastenau, P. S. (in press-a). The Extended Complex
outcomes: From statistical significance to clinical
Figure Test (ECFT). Los Angeles: Western Psy-
significance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
chological Services.
Neuropsychology, 19, 889-896.
Fastenau, P. S. (in press-b). Extended Complex Figure
Binder, L. M. (1982). Constructional strategies on
Test (ECFT): Rationale and empirical support for
Complex Figure drawings after unilateral brain
recognition and matching. In J. A. Knight & E. F.
damage. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology,
Kaplan (Eds.), The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure
4(1), 51-58.
Test: Clinical and research applications. Odessa,
Boone, K. B., Lesser, I. M., Hill-Gutierrez, E.,
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Berman, N. G., & D’Elia, L. F. (1993). Rey-
Fastenau, P. S. (in press-c). Validity of regression-
Osterrieth Complex Figure performance in healthy,
based norms: An empirical test of the Comprehen-
older adults: Relationship to age, education, sex,
sive Norms with older adults. Journal of Clinical
and IQ. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 22-28.
and Experimental Neuropsychology.
Bornstein, R. A., & Suga, L. J. (1988). Educational
Fastenau, P. S., & Adams, K. M. (1996). Heaton,
level and neuropsychological performance in
Grant, and Matthews’ Comprehensive Norms: An
healthy elderly subjects. Developmental Neuropsy-
overzealous attempt. Journal of Clinical and Ex-
chology, 4, 17-22.
perimental Neuropsychology, 18, 444-448.
Brandys, C. F., & Rourke, B. P. (1991). Differential
Fastenau, P. S., Bennett, J. M., & Denburg, N. L.
memory abilities in reading- and arithmetic-dis-
(1996). Application of psychometric standards to
46 PHILIP S. FASTENAU ET AL.

scoring system evaluation: Is ‘‘new’’ necessarily Meeting of the International Neuropsychological


‘‘improved’’? Journal of Clinical and Experimen- Society, Boston, MA.
tal Neuropsychology, 18, 462-472. Hamby, S. L., Wilkins, J. W., & Barry, N. S. (1993).
Fastenau, P. S., Bennett, J. M., & Fisk, J. L. (1996). Organizational quality on the Rey-Osterrieth and
Memory consolidation in temporal lobe epilepsy: Taylor Complex Figure Tests: A new scoring sys-
Effects of immediate recall [Abstract]. The Clini- tem. Psychological Assessment, 5, 27-33.
cal Neuropsychologist, 10, 336. Hawkins, K. A., Sledge, W. H., Orleans, J. F.,
Fastenau, P. S., Conant, L. L., & Lauer, R. E. (1998). Quinlan, D. M., Rakfeldt, J., & Hoffman, R. E.
Working memory in young children: Evidence for (1993). Normative implications of the relationship
modality-specificity and implications for cerebral between reading vocabulary and Boston Naming
reorganization in early childhood. Neuropsychol- Test performance. Archives of Clinical Neuropsy-
ogia, 7, 643-652. chology, 8, 525-537.
Fastenau, P. S., & Denburg, N. L. (1994). Reliability Heilbronner, R. L. (1992). The search for a ‘‘pure’’
and validity of the Extended Complex Figure Test visual memory test: Pursuit of perfection? The
(ECFT) [Abstract]. Journal of the International Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 105-112.
Neuropsychological Society, 1, 356. Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E.
Fastenau, P. S., & Denburg, N. L. (1997). Effects of G., Petersen, R. C., Kokmen, E., & Kurland, L. T.
pen color on Rey Complex Figure performances (1992). Mayo’s older American normative studies:
[Abstract]. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, WAIS-R norms for ages 56 to 97. The Clinical
12, 316-317. Neuropsychologist, 6 (Suppl.), 1-30.
Fastenau, P. S., Denburg, N. L., & Abeles, N. Jones-Gotman, M. (1986). Memory for designs: The
(1996a). Age differences in retrieval: Further sup- hippocampal contribution. Neuropsychologia, 24,
port for a resource-reduction hypothesis. Psychol- 193-203.
ogy and Aging, 11, 140-146. Kaplan, E. (1988). A process approach to neuropsy-
Fastenau, P. S., Denburg, N. L., & Abeles, N. chological assessment. In T. Boll & B. K. Bryant
(1996b). The role of attention in aging retrieval: (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and brain func-
Relative sparing with greater visual involvement. tion: Research, measurement, and practice (pp.
Brain and Cognition, 30, 393-396. 125-167). Washington, DC: American Psychologi-
Fastenau, P. S., Denburg, N. L., & Domitrovic, L. A. cal Association.
(1997). Intentional and incidental memory: Order Karapetsas, A., & Kantas, A. (1991). Visuomotor or-
effects in clinical testing. Professional Psychology: ganization in the child: A neuropsychological ap-
Research and Practice, 28, 32-35. proach. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 211-217.
Fastenau, P. S., Denburg, N. L., & Mauer, B. A. (in Klicpera, C. (1983). Poor planning as a characteristic
press). Parallel short forms for the Boston Naming of problem-solving behavior in dyslexic children:
Test: Psychometric properties and elder norms. A study with the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy- Test. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 49, 73-82.
chology. Knight, J. A., & Kaplan, E. F. (Eds., in press). The
Fastenau, P. S., & Fisk, J. L. (1997a). Use of memory Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test: Clinical and
scores to predict side of resection in epilepsy sur- research applications. Odessa, FL: Psychological
gery [Abstract]. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Assessment Resources.
Clinical Neurosciences, 9, 168-169. Knight, J. A., Kaplan, E. F., & Ireland, L. D. (1994,
Fastenau, P. S., & Fisk, J. L. (1997b). Use of visual February). Survey findings of Rey-Osterrieth Com-
memory tests to discriminate between left and right plex Figure use among the INS membership [Ab-
temporal lobe epilepsy [Abstract]. Journal of Neu- stract]. Journal of the International Neuropsycho-
ropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 9, 169. logical Society, 1, 355.
Fastenau, P. S., & Manning, A. A. (1992). Develop- Leininger, B. E., Gramling, S. E., Farrell, A. D.,
ment of a recognition task for the Complex Figure Kreutzer, J. S., & Peck III, E. A. (1990). Neuropsy-
Test [Abstract]. Journal of Clinical and Experi- chological deficits in symptomatic minor head in-
mental Neuropsychology, 14, 43. jury patients after concussion and mild concussion.
Fastenau, P. S., Smet, I. C., Giordani, B., Miller, A. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychia-
C., Tandon, R., & Berent, S. (1998). Spared visual- try, 53, 293-296.
spatial recall and recognition in medically refrac- Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment
tory depression [Abstract]. Journal of the Interna- (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
tional Neuropsychological Society, 4, 5. Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment
Fastenau, P. S., Unverzagt, F. W., Koop, J. I., & (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hufford, B. J. (1998). Validation of the Rey Figure Loring, D. W., Lee, G. P., & Meador, K. J. (1988).
and Extended Complex Figure Test for discrimi- Revising the Rey-Osterrieth: Rating right hemi-
nating Alzheimer’s disease from normal aging. sphere recall. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychol-
Paper submitted for presentation at the Annual ogy, 3, 239-247.
NORMS FOR THE REY AND ECFT 47

McClain, L. (1983). Encoding and retrieval in schizo- Rey, A., & Osterrieth, P. A. (1993). Translations of
phrenics’ free recall. Journal of Nervous and Men- excerpts from André Rey’s ‘‘Psychological exami-
tal Disease, 171, 471-479. nation of traumatic encephalopathy’’ and P. A.
McManis, S. E., Brown, G. R., Zachary, R., & Osterrieth’s ‘‘The complex figure copy test’’ (J.
Rundell, J. R. (1993). A screening test for subtle Corwin & F. W. Bylsma, Trans.). The Clinical
cognitive impairment early in the course of HIV Neuropsychologist, 7, 3-21. (Original works pub-
infection. Psychosomatics, 34, 424-431. lished in 1941 and 1944, respectively).
Meyers, J. E., & Meyers, K. R. (1995). Rey Complex Ross, T. P., & Lichtenberg, P. A. (1997). Expanded
Figure Test and Recognition Trial. Odessa, FL: normative data for the Boston Naming Test in an
Psychological Assessment Resources. urban medical sample of elderly adults [Abstract].
Milanovic, L., Spilich, G., Vucinic, G., & Knezevic, Journal of the International Neuropsychological
S. (1990). Effects of occupational exposure to or- Society, 3, 70.
ganic solvents upon cognitive performance. Neuro- Roselli, M., & Ardila, A. (1991). Effects of age, edu-
toxicity and Teratology, 12, 657-660. cation, and gender on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex
Miller, L. A., Muñoz, D. G., & Finmore, M. (1993). Figure. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 370-
Hippocampal sclerosis and human memory. Ar- 376.
chives of Neurology, 50, 391-394. Smet, I. C. (1997). The neuropsychology of schizo-
Orsini, , D. L., Van Gorp, W. G., & Boone, K. B. phrenia. Colloquium presented at Department of
(1988). The neuropsychology casebook. New Psychology, Indiana University – Purdue Univer-
York: Springer-Verlag. sity Indianapolis (IUPUI), April 18, 1997.
Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test du copie d’une figure Squire, L. R. (1986). The neuropsychology of mem-
complexe. Archives of Psychology (Chicago), 30, ory dysfunction and its assessment. In I. Grant &
206-356. K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assess-
Parkin, A. J., Dunn, J. C., Lee, C., O’Hara, P. F., & ment of neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 268-299).
Nussbaum, L. (1993). Neuropsychological se- New York: Oxford University Press.
quelae of Wernicke’s encephalopathy in a 20-year- Taylor, L. B. (1969). Localisation of cerebral lesions
old woman: Selective impairment of a frontal by psychological testing. Clinical Neurosurgery,
memory system. Brain and Cognition, 21, 1-19. 16, 269-287.
Pauker, J. D. (1988). Constructing overlapping cell Waber, D. P., & Holmes, J. M. (1985). Assessing chil-
tables to maximize the clinical usefulness of nor- dren’s copy productions of the Rey-Osterrieth
mative test data: Rationale and an example from Complex Figure. Journal of Clinical and Experi-
neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical Psychology, mental Neuropsychology, 7, 264-280.
44, 930-933. Waber, D. P., & Holmes, J. M. (1986). Assessing chil-
Paulsen, J. S., Heaton, R. K, Sadek, J. R., Perry, W., dren’s memory productions of the Rey-Osterrieth
Delis, D. C., Braff, D., Kuck, J., Zisook, S., & Complex Figure. Journal of Clinical and Experi-
Jeste, D. V. (1995). The nature of learning and mental Neuropsychology, 8, 563-580.
memory impairments in schizophrenia. Journal of Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence
the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, Scale-Revised manual. New York: Psychological
88-99. Corporation.
Prior, M., & Hoffmann, W. (1990). Neuropsychologi- Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M-M. (1985). Mental
cal testing of autistic children through an explora- state assessment of young and elderly adults in be-
tion with frontal lobe tests. Journal of Autism and havioral neurology. In M-M Mesulam (Ed.), Prin-
Developmental Disorders, 20, 581-590. ciples of behavioral neurology (pp. 71-123). Phila-
Rey, A. (1941). L’examen psychologique dans les cas delphia: F. A. Davis.
d’encephalopathie traumatique. Archives of Psy- Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). Wide Range Achievement
chology (Chicago), 28, 286-340. Test (3rd ed.). Wilmington, DE: Wide Range.

You might also like