You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/259318540

Behavioral and neurochemical effects of dietary methyl donor deficiency


combined with unpredictable chronic mild stress in rats

Article  in  Behavioural Brain Research · December 2013


DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.047 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

21 164

10 authors, including:

Hervé Javelot Jean-François Bisson


Etablissement Public de Santé Alsace Nord, Brumath, France ETAP-Lab
96 PUBLICATIONS   2,066 CITATIONS    51 PUBLICATIONS   2,419 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Catherine Strazielle Robert Lalonde


University of Lorraine Université de Rouen
130 PUBLICATIONS   2,710 CITATIONS    330 PUBLICATIONS   10,239 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

personal research View project

Centre de Ressource & d'Expertise en PsychoPharmacologie (CREPP) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Hervé Javelot on 03 November 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Behavioural Brain Research 261 (2014) 8–16

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr

Research report

Behavioral and neurochemical effects of dietary methyl donor


deficiency combined with unpredictable chronic mild stress in rats
H. Javelot a,b,c,∗ , M. Messaoudi b , C. Jacquelin a , J.F. Bisson b , P. Rozan b , A. Nejdi b ,
C. Lazarus d , J.C. Cassel d , C. Strazielle a,e , R. Lalonde f
a
Laboratoire de Nutrition Génétique et Exposition aux Risques Environnementaux, INSERM U954, Faculté de Médecine de Nancy – UHP,
Vandœuvre-lès-Nancy, France
b
ETAP-Applied Ethology – Neuropsychopharmacology Department, Vandœuvre-lès-Nancy, France
c
Clinical Pharmacy Service – Mental Health Establishment (EPSAN), Brumath, France
d
Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives et Adaptatives, UMR 7363, Université de Strasbourg, CNRS, Strasbourg, France
e
Laboratoire de Microscopie Electronique, Faculté de Médecine de Nancy – UHP, Vandœuvre-lès-Nancy, France
f
Université de Rouen, Dépt. Psychologie, Laboratoire ICONES (EA 4699), Mont-Saint-Aignan, France

h i g h l i g h t s

• Methyl donor deficient diet caused disinhibiton.


• Chronic stress exacerbated methyl donor deficiency effects on locomotors activity.
• Hyperhomocysteinemia may contribute to monoaminergic and behavioral disturbances.

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Methyl donor deficiencies and chronic stress cause depression independently, but their interaction has
Received 15 May 2013 never been thoroughly evaluated. In our study, methyl donor deficient diet and chronic stress condition
Received in revised form consisted respectively of a B2, B9, B12, and choline-free diet and a chronic mild stress procedure. Rats were
22 November 2013
randomly assigned to six groups with three “diet” conditions (free-feeding, pair-fed and methyl donor
Accepted 26 November 2013
Available online 9 December 2013
deficient diet) and two “stress” conditions (no-stress and stress) and were evaluated in the open-field,
the elevated plus-maze and the forced swimming test. After the behavioral evaluation, corticosterone
and homocysteine plasma levels were measured and dopamine, DOPAC, serotonin, 5HIAA concentra-
Keywords:
Rat tions were evaluated in several brain areas. Rats given a methyl donor deficient diet for 11 weeks causing
Behavior elevated plasma homocysteine levels were compared to pair-fed and free-feeding rats with or without
Depression unpredictable chronic mild stress. Regardless of stress environmental conditions, the methyl donor defi-
Chronic mild stress cient diet decreased plasma corticosterone levels and caused disinhibition in the elevated plus-maze
B-vitamin condition relative to both control groups. However, stress potentiated the effects of the deficient regi-
Methyl donors men on rearing in the open-field and climbing in the forced swim test. The dietary changes involved in
behavior and plasma corticosterone could be caused by homocysteine-induced decreases in dopamine
and 5-hydroxytryptamine metabolites in selective brain regions and it can be noted that regardless of
stress-conditions, methyl donor deficient diet decreases DOPAC/dopamine and 5HIAA/serotonin ratios in
striatum and hypothalamus and selectively 5HIAA/serotonin ratio in the sensorimotor cortex. Our exper-
imental data is particularly relevant in the context of neuropsychiatric disorders frequently associated
with folate deficiency and hyperhomocysteinemia.
© 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

The neuropsychiatric consequences of folate and vitamin B12


deficiencies have been extensively demonstrated [1,2]. Despite
some degree of overlap, folate deficiency has been more fre-
quently associated with depression and cobalamin deficiency
∗ Corresponding author at: Clinical Pharmacy Service – Mental Health Establish-
with psychosis [3]. More generally, methyl donor deficiencies
ment (EPSAN), Brumath, France. Tel.: +33 0388646170; fax: +33 0388646158.
E-mail address: herve.javelot@ch-epsan.fr (H. Javelot).
lead to several psychiatric disorders [4]. Genetic data shows

0166-4328/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.047
H. Javelot et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 261 (2014) 8–16 9

a close link between folate metabolism and depression [4,5]. 2. Materials and methods
According to a meta-analysis, the common C677T polymorphism
of methylenetetrahydrofolatereductase (MTHFR), catalyzing the 2.1. Animals
conversion of 5,10-methylenetetrahydrofolate to the bio-active 5-
methyltetrahydrofolate (5-MTHF), decreased 5-MTHF production, One-month-old Wistar rats (Harlan, The Netherlands) were
decreased serum folate levels, and increased the risk of depression housed in groups of four inside 48 cm × 27 cm × 20 cm poly-
[6]. carbonate cages (UAR, Epinay-sur-Orge, France) under controlled
Today, a large number of converging data suggests that conditions with respect to ambient humidity (50 ± 5%) and temper-
there is a relationship between folate deficiency and depression, ature (22 ± 1 ◦ C). The rats were maintained on a 12 h light/12 h dark
and hyperhomocysteinemia might be its metabolic consequence cycle (on at 20.00 pm-08.00 am). After a 7-day adaptation period,
[4,5]. Two major metabolites could be involved in the neuro- rats were randomly assigned to one of three diet groups with or
chemical hypothesis of folate-related depression: (1) 5-MTHF, without UCMS (n = 8). The deficient diet lacking vitamins B2, B9,
which influences tetrahydrobiopterin (BH4) levels and activity B12, and choline (Special Diet Services, Saint-Gratien, France) was
and (2) S-adenosylmethionine (SAMe), a methyl donor in numer- given for eight weeks without behavioral testing and then for three
ous biochemical reactions, both consistent with the monoamine more weeks with behavioral testing and compared with appro-
hypothesis of depression, since BH4 is an essential cofactor for priate pair-fed and free-feeding controls. At the same time each
5-hydroxytryptamine (5HT), dopamine, and noradrenaline syn- week, the rats were weighed and their food and water intake mon-
thesis and methylation is a limiting step in the latter [4,5]. itored, the final measure taken on day 77. The deficient animals
Hyperhomocysteinemia could counteract optimal biosynthesis of were compared with two pair-fed groups receiving a standard diet
neurotransmitters via oxidative stress [4,7]. 5-MTHF and SAMe (M20, Special Diet Services). Food eaten by a vitamin-B-deprived rat
possess antidepressant-like activity [5,7,8]. Low serum folate was weighed each day at the same hour and the means obtained
levels may be involved in resistant-depression episodes which were then used for feeding the pair-fed rats. The free-feeding
present with poor responses to fluoxetine, whereas folates and group ate the same diet as the pair-fed animals but ad libitum.
their derivatives have antidepressant properties even when there The experiment follow the guidelines provided by the ASAB Eth-
is no measurable folate deficiency, highlighting their action as ical Committee for the use of animals in research (Anim. Behav.
monoamine enhancers [4,9,10]. 2006, 71, pp. 245–254) and the Canadian Council on Animal Care
Few studies have evaluated the effect of folate derivatives (Guide to the Care and Use of Experimental Animals), 2nd ed., vol. 2
or SAMe on mood-related behaviors in rodent models [11–18], (1993); 1st ed., vol. 1 (1984). All procedures were also in compliance
and even fewer data is available on the effects of methyl donor with regulations provided by the European Community Council
deficient diets on mood [19]. Brocardo et al. [16] demonstrated Directive of 24 November 1986 (86/609/EEC).
antidepressant-like effects of folate in the forced swim test in
mice, which seemed to involve glutamatergic n-methyl-d-asparate
(NMDA) receptors, the l-arginine-nitric oxide-cyclic guanosine 2.2. Unpredictable chronic mild stress
monophosphate pathway, or opioids [17,18]. Benelli et al. [12]
showed that SAMe had a better antidepressant-like action than The three non-stressed groups remained in the same room,
imipramine in the unpredictable chronic mild stress (UCMS), a while the three stressed groups were moved from Monday to Fri-
well-documented model of depression [20]. Moreover, Genedani day to a separate one and exposed to four different stressors in a
et al. [13] showed that the antidepressant action of SAMe was different order each week: intermittent white noise (85 dB; 10.0
partially related to its ability to restore polyamine levels follow- apm–16.00 pm), cage tilt (10.0 apm–16.00 pm), continuous light-
ing UCMS in nucleus accumbens and hippocampus. Lalonde et al. ing (24 h), and exposure to soiled bedding inside the cage (200 ml
[19] described hyperactivity without change in anxiety levels, of spilled water).
motor coordination, and spatial learning after five-month of dietary
restriction of the methyl donors vitamins B2, folate, B12, and 2.3. Behavioral methods and procedures
choline. In the context of depressive subtypes, Fraguas et al. [21]
showed that in major depressive disorder with aggressive behavior, 2.3.1. Open-field
homocysteine levels were positively correlated with the severity The rats were first tested after 8 weeks of diet exposure (day
and the duration of the depressive episode, and this was not found 56) in the open-field. Each rat was individually placed for 3 min in
in major depression without aggressive symptoms. Mineur et al. the center of a circular arena, 60 cm in diameter, with clear plex-
[21] consider the UCMS model as being particularly relevant in iglas walls at a height of 40 cm and a plexiglas floor divided into
depressed patients presenting with aggressive behavior. 9 equal areas: 8 black areas at the periphery and a white one at
In the present study, we assessed the separate and combined the center. The number of squares crossed and the number of rears
effects of UCMS with the same methyl donor deficient diet used were recorded under dim white light, as well as the time spent in
previously in mice [19], lacking vitamin B2, folate, vitamin B12, the central square (s). The floor was washed with a water–alcohol
and choline, similar to the one used to analyze rat exploratory solution and dried before each subject to diffuse odor cues.
activity in the elevated plus-maze and other tests [23]. In addition
to the elevated plus-maze [24], rats explored the open-field [25]
and were then exposed to the forced swim test of depression-like 2.3.2. Elevated plus-maze
behavior [26]. We hypothesized that mild stress over several weeks The elevated plus-maze was made of black plasticized wood,
would potentiate the effects of the methyl donor deficient diet, consisting of four cross-shaped arms, two open (50 cm × 12 cm) and
possibly increasing anxiety and depression-like behaviors. At the two enclosed on opposite sides, with the walls of the enclosed arms
end of the behavioral evaluation, corticosterone and homocysteine reaching 40 cm in height, together with a common central platform
plasma levels were measured along with dopamine and 5HT con- (12 cm × 12 cm). On day 58, each rat was placed in the center of the
centrations in several brain areas. We hypothesized that mild stress maze with its head facing an open arm and left to explore it in
would also potentiate the effects of a methyl donor deficient diet a single 5-min session. The test was conducted under dim white
at the biochemical level, possibly increasing plasma corticosterone light and behaviors recorded with a CCD-TV camera. The numbers
and homocysteine levels as well as brain monoamine levels. of entries (4-paw criterion) inside open and enclosed arms were
10 H. Javelot et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 261 (2014) 8–16

measured together with the time spent inside each arm and the different compounds were determined with data analysis software
open/total arm duration ratio. (Baseline 810, Waters) and expressed in ng/mg microwaved tissue.

2.3.3. Forced swim test 2.6. Statistical analyses


During the pre-test session on day 75, the rats were placed inside
a plexiglas cylinder (50 cm height and 20 cm diameter) filled with For non-repeated measures, 3 × 2 analyses of variance
water at a temperature of 25 ◦ C to a depth of 30 cm for 15 min. (ANOVA)s were conducted with diet at 3 levels and stress at
The water was changed on every trial to eliminate urine, feces, and 2 levels as the main factors. For repeated measures, three-way
other possible odor clues left by the previous rat. After the swim ANOVAs with repeated measures on the final factor were con-
session, the rats were removed from the cylinder, gently dried with ducted, with diet as one main factor, stress as the second main
towels, and placed under a red light (30 ◦ C) for 20 min before being factor, and time as the repeated measure. Whenever the interac-
returned to their home cage. The following day, the rats were sub- tion was significant, Fisher’s least significant difference (LSD) test
mitted to the same swim stress condition for 5 min. The 5-min test was used to determine between-group differences.
session was recorded with a CCD-TV camera, and the duration of
immobility, swimming, and climbing behaviors were scored later 3. Results
on. Immobility was defined as the absence of any movement, with
the body inclined forward, passively floating, with immobile paws. 3.1. Exploratory activity and forced swimming
Swimming was defined as horizontal movements in the water, and
climbing as vertical movements similar to escape attempts. 3.1.1. Open-field (see Table 1)
Stress exerted an effect on rearing in the open-field
2.4. Plasma concentrations of homocysteine and corticosterone [F(1,42) = 4.08; P = 0.05] and interacted with diet for this measure
as well as time spent inside the central square [F(2,42) = 4.24;
On day 77, the animals were euthanized with halothane. Blood P = 0.02; F(2,42) = 5.19; P = 0.01, respectively]. Among stressed rats,
was rapidly removed by cardiac puncture and sampled into tubes the methyl donor deficient group reared more often than either
with EDTA, then centrifuged (4 ◦ C, 2500 ␮g, 15 min) to retrieve pair-fed or free-feeding groups [P < 0.05 and P < 0.01, respectively,
the plasma fraction. To reduce the effects of diurnal variations on Fisher’s test]. This result was not found among non-stressed rats
titration, the rats in each group were killed alternatively within a [P > 0.05]. Regarding the time spent inside the central square, the
4 h time-frame and blood samples were collected between 14:00 stressed pair-fed group had higher values than the other two
PM and 18:00 PM. Plasma was stored at −80 ◦ C until homocys- stressed groups [P < 0.05], whereas no difference was found among
teine and corticosterone concentrations were respectively assessed the non-stressed.
by fluorescent polarization immunoassay (IMX system; Abbott,
Fornebu, Norway, [27]) and radioimmunoassay. Plasma was first 3.1.2. Elevated plus-maze (see Table 1)
deproteinized by ethanol extraction. Regarding corticosterone, In the elevated plus-maze, there were significant diet effects
the working range of the assay was 20–10,000 pg/tube and con- on time spent inside open arms [F(2,35) = 5.06; P = 0.01] and
centrations were estimated from the parameters of a standard enclosed arms [F(2,35) = 4.82, P = 0.01] as well as open arm
curve linearized by logit-log transformation [28]. Corticosterone entries [F(2,35) = 6.76, P = 0.003] and open/total arm duration
and homocysteinemia were respectively expressed in mg/ml and [F(2,35) = 5.06; P = 0.01]. Specific comparisons revealed that methyl
ng/ml. donor deficient rats entered more often and spent more time inside
open arms and had higher open/total arm durations than either
pair-fed or free-feeding rats [P < 0.05, Fisher’s test] and conversely
2.5. Brain and spinal concentrations of biogenic amines and
metabolites spent less time inside enclosed arms [P < 0.05]. Methyl donor defi-
cient rats also tended to spend more time inside the central square
Immediately after cardiac puncture, the rats were decapi- than these groups, but the comparison with the free-feeding group
did not quite reach the required level of significance [P = 0.06].
tated and their brains removed. Brain tissue samples were put
Stress had no impact on any of these measures (Table 2).
in nitrogen liquid before being weighed and kept at a −80 ◦ C
temperature; hippocampus, limbic cortex, sensorimotor cortex,
striatum, thalamus, hypothalamus, cerebellum, and medulla were 3.1.3. Forced swim test (see Fig. 1)
then analyzed at a neurochemical level. Left and right struc- During the test period, diet effects were found on swimming and
tures from each rat were pooled for bilateral brain regions. The climbing duration as well as climbing frequency [F(2,42) = 22.56,
concentrations of dopamine and one of its major metabolites, P < 0.0001; F(2,42) = 17.05, P < 0.0001; F(2,42) = 10.4, P = 0.0002,
3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC), along with 5HT and its respectively]. There were also diet × stress interactions for all three
principal metabolite, 5-hydroxyindolacetic acid (5-HIAA), were measures [F(2,42) = 10.41, P = 0.0002; F(2,42) = 8.33, P = 0.0009;
measured by means of high performance liquid chromatography F(2,42) = 10.75, P = 0.0002, respectively]. Among stressed rats, the
(HPLC) with electrochemical detection. Tissue samples were pre- methyl donor deficient group spent less time swimming, more
pared for HPLC by homogenization in 1 N formic acid/acetone time climbing, and climbed more often than the other two groups
(18:8.5, v/v), and the formic extracts used for monoamine deter- [P < 0.0001, Fisher’s test]. No such result was found among non-
minations without further purification. The HPLC system consisted stressed rats and no effect was found for immobility time.
of an ESA liquid chromatography pump (ESA Inc., Bedford) coupled
to an ESA Coulochem II detector (Eurosep Instruments) equipped 3.2. Plasma homocysteine and corticosterone levels (see Fig. 2A
with a 5014 high performance analytic cell (ESA Inc., Bedford) set and B)
at +0.4 V and analyzed on a C18 Spherisorb ODS2 reverse phase
column (5 ␮m pore size, 4.6 mm in diameter, 25 cm in length). 3.2.1. Homocysteine
The mobile phase consisted of 0.1 M NaH2 PO4 , pH = 3, containing There were diet, stress, and interaction effects between
0.1 mM/l of EDTA, 1.7 mM/l 1-octane sulfonic acid sodium salt, and these factors on homocysteine levels [F(2,30) = 70.71, P < 0.0001;
10% acetonitrile. The flow rate was 1 ml/min. Concentrations of the F(1,30) = 4.94, P = 0.03 and F(2,30) = 5.59, P = 0.009, respectively].
H. Javelot et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 261 (2014) 8–16 11

Table 1
Effects of UCMS and methyl donor deficiency on exploratory activity in the open-field and plus-maze.

Free-feeding Free-feeding Pair-fed Pair-fed Methyl donor Methyl donor


Non-stress Stress Non-stress Stress deficient deficient
Non-stress Stress

Open-field
Squares crossed 51.9 ± 3.6 44.1 ± 3.6 51.4 ± 1.6 56 ± 4.4 50.5 ± 2.2 58.1 ± 4.4
Rears 8 ± 1.8 6.2 ± 6.9 6.1 ± 1 8.1 ± 1.6 6.6 ± 1.6 15.9 ± 2.6* , ##
Central square duration (s) 3.4 ± 0.8 2.7 ± 1 2.4 ± 0.6 8.5 ± 2.5 4.4 ± 1.3 2.5 ± 1.8*

Elevated plus-maze
Central square visits 13.4 ± 2.5 13.5 ± 2.4 13.7 ± 0.9 20.2 ± 1.5 12.9 ± 1.5 15.5 ± 1.7
Central square duration (s) 68.8 ± 16.4 66.5 ± 10.7 61.9 ± 4.7 70 ± 11.2 72.25 ± 11.5 100.6 ± 4.7
Open arm visits 0.8 ± 0.4 0.7 ± 0.3 1.2 ± 0.4 0.7 ± 0.5 2.4 ± 0.6† , ‡ 2.7 ± 0.7† , ‡
Open arm duration (s) 5.8 ± 3.3 8.1 ± 3.9 8.2 ± 3.7 6.2 ± 3.7 21.6 ± 5.5†† , ‡‡ 22.4 ± 8†† , ‡‡
Enclosed arm duration (s) 225.4 ± 17.8 225.4 ± 13.7 229.9 ± 7.9 223.7 ± 12.7 206.1 ± 15.2† , ‡ 177 ± 9.8† , ‡
Open/total arm duration 2.68 ± 1.61 3.96 ± 1.96 3.65 ± 1.66 2.82 ± 1.77 10.13 ± 2.62† , ‡ 11.34 ± 11.76† , ‡

All data were expressed as the mean ± SEM.


*
P < 0.05 methyl donor deficient vs pair-fed in stressed rats.
##
P < 0.01 vs free-feeding.

P < 0.05.
††
P < 0.01 methyl donor deficient vs pair-fed.

P < 0.05.
‡‡
P < 0.01 vs free-feeding (stressed and non-stressed rats pooled); all comparisons with Fisher’s test.

Fig. 1. Effects of UCMS and methyl donor deficiency on the duration of immobility (A) and swimming (B) and the duration and frequency of climbing (C and D, respectively)
in the forced swimming test during the test session. Results are expressed in mean ± SEM. ***P < 0.001 and *P < 0.05 methyl donor deficient vs pair-fed; ### P < 0.001 and
#
P < 0.05 vs free-feeding, in stressed groups (Fisher’s test).
12 H. Javelot et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 261 (2014) 8–16

Methyl donor deficient rats expressed a higher level of homo-


cysteinemia than pair-fed and free-feeding rats independently of
stress levels [P < 0.0001, Fisher’s test], though the effect was ampli-
fied in non-stressed rats.

3.2.2. Corticosterone
There were diet [F(2,30) = 7.84, P = 0.002] and stress
[F(1,30) = 6.04, P = 0.02] effects on corticosterone levels. Methyl
donor deficient rats expressed a lower level of corticosterone
levels than pair-fed and free-feeding rats [P < 0.01 and P < 0.05,
respectively, Fisher’s test], while stress increased these levels.

3.3. Biogenic amines and metabolites in brain (see Table 3)

3.3.1. DOPAC
Two-way ANOVAs revealed diet effects on DOPAC levels in the
thalamus, the hypothalamus, and the cerebellum [F(2,42) = 5.21,
P = 0.009; F(2,41) = 3.41, P = 0.04; F(2,42) = 3.66, P = 0.03, respec-
tively] as well as stress effects on the medulla [F(1,41) = 5.34,
P = 0.03]. Methyl donor deficient rats pooled for the stress variable
had lower DOPAC levels than the other groups of rats in hypothala-
mus [P < 0.05], but in thalamus and cerebellum their levels were
only lower than free-feeding rats [P < 0.01 and P < 0.05, respec-
tively]. The stress effect on the medulla is due to higher DOPAC
levels in UCMS-exposed rats.
Fig. 2. Effects of methyl donor deficiency on plasma corticosterone (ng/ml)
and homocysteine levels (␮mol/l). (A) Corticosterone levels (ng/ml). Results are
3.3.2. Dopamine expressed in mean ± SEM. †† P < 0.01 methyl donor deficient vs pair-fed; ‡ P < 0.05 vs
free-feeding (non-stressed and stressed rats pooled; comparisons with Fisher’s test).
There were diet effects on dopamine levels in the medulla
(B) Homocysteine levels (␮mol/l). Results are expressed in mean ± SEM. ***P < 0.001
[F(2,41) = 4.22, P = 0.02] and a significant diet × stress interaction methyl donor deficient vs pair-fed; #### P < 0.05 vs free-feeding, in stressed groups
in the sensorimotor cortex [F(2,42) = 4.97, P = 0.01]. Methyl donor (Fisher’s test).
deficient rats expressed higher dopamine levels in medulla only
with respect to pair-fed rats [P < 0.05]. In the sensorimotor cortex,

Table 2
Effects of UCMS and methyl donor deficiency on dopamine and DOPAC levels (in ng/mg) and the ratio DOPAC/dopamine in different brain regions and cervical spinal cord.

Free-feeding Free-feeding Pair-fed Pair-fed Methyl donor Methyl donor


Non-stress Stress Non-stress Stress deficient deficient
Non-stress Stress

DA
Hippocampus 0.128 ± 0.039 0.069 ± 0.058 0.064 ± 0.036 0.105 ± 0.036 0.099 ± 0.027 0.136 ± 0.053
Limbic cortex 0.075 ± 0.004 0.065 ± 0.006 0.062 ± 0.003 0.059 ± 0.004 0.060 ± 0.009 0.064 ± 0.007
Sensorimotor cortex 0.341 ± 0.048 0.290 ± 0.027 0.189 ± 0.062 0.377 ± 0.043 0.316 ± 0.045 0.225 ± 0.054
Striatum 1.141 ± 0.067 1.135 ± 0.044 1.169 ± 0.043 1.054 ± 0.138 1.175 ± 0.081 1.314 ± 0.121
Thalamus 0.114 ± 0.032 0.095 ± 0.012 0.058 ± 0.005 0.118 ± 0.035 0.086 ± 0.015 0.068 ± 0.011
Hypothalamus 0.510 ± 0.101 0.595 ± 0.101 0.642 ± 0.144 0.696 ± 0.157 0.546 ± 0.113 0.534 ± 0.085
Cerebellum 0.008 ± 0.001 0.008 ± 0.001 0.007 0.006 0.006 0.008
Medulla 0.057 ± 0.004 0.061 ± 0.004 0.050 ± 0.003 0.062 ± 0.005 0.064 ± 0.004† 0.068 ± 0.004†,

DOPAC
Hippocampus 0.034 ± 0.008 0.022 ± 0.006 0.024 ± 0.006 0.034 ± 0.006 0.025 ± 0.004 0.027 ± 0.003
Limbic cortex 0.038 ± 0.005 0.037 ± 0.006 0.031 ± 0.002 0.030 ± 0.002 0.029 ± 0.003 0.035 ± 0.003
Sensorimotor cortex 0.081 ± 0.019 0.064 ± 0.009 0.049 ± 0.015 0.106 ± 0.022 0.078 ± 0.013 0.057 ± 0.020
Striatum 0.554 ± 0.044 0.572 ± 0.024 0.589 ± 0.021 0.466 ± 0.075 0.503 ± 0.047 0.455 ± 0.027
Thalamus 0.042 ± 0.04 0.043 ± 0.004 0.058 ± 0.005 0.042 ± 0.004 0.035 ± 0.003‡‡ 0.029 ± 0.003‡‡
Hypothalamus 0.127 ± 0.016 0.159 ± 0.022 0.152 ± 0.027 0.149 ± 0.024 0.096 ± 0.016†,‡ 0.104 ± 0.016†,‡
Cerebellum 0.013 ± 0.001 0.015 ± 0.002 0.014 ± 0.002 0.012 ± 0.001 0.010 ± 0.001‡ 0.012 ± 0.001‡
Medulla 0.015 ± 0.001 0.016 ± 0.001 0.012 ± 0.001 0.017 ± 0.002 0.015 ± 0.001 0.016 ± 0.001

DOPAC/DA
Hippocampus 0.471 ± 0.144 0.535 ± 0.142 0.660 ± 0.136 0.515 ± 0.116 0.671 ± 0.287 0.607 ± 0.181
Limbic cortex 0.507 ± 0.059 0.581 ± 0.083 0.497 ± 0.033 0.535 ± 0.060 0.523 ± 0.049 0.577 ± 0.067
Sensorimotor cortex 0.225 ± 0.027 0.221 ± 0.034 0.299 ± 0.031 0.272 ± 0.025 0.262 ± 0.030 0.294 ± 0.067
Striatum 0.483 ± 0.020 0.505 ± 0.014 0.507 ± 0.024 0.430 ± 0.042 0.426 ± 0.022††,‡‡‡ 0.360 ± 0.025††,‡‡‡
Thalamus 0.469 ± 0.067 0.473 ± 0.043 0.584 ± 0.041 0.502 ± 0.089 0.448 ± 0.043 0.459 ± 0.041
Hypothalamus 0.272 ± 0.031 0.278 ± 0.019 0.250 ± 0.014 0.232 ± 0.014 0.184 ± 0.010†,‡ 0.208 ± 0.022†,‡
Cerebellum 1.732 ± 0.111 2.004 ± 0.282 2.016 ± 0.266 1.879 ± 0.082 1.803 ± 0.163 1.436 ± 0.125
Medulla 0.269 ± 0.016 0.269 ± 0.016 0.244 ± 0.012 0.269 ± 0.017 0.245 ± 0.017 0.245 ± 0.007

All data were expressed as the mean ± SEM.


Specific comparisons with non-stressed and stressed rats pooled with Fisher’s test: † P < 0.05 and †† P < 0.01 methyl donor deficient vs pair-fed; ‡ P < 0.05, ‡‡ P < 0.01 and ‡‡‡ P < 0.001
vs free-feeding.
ANOVA:  P < 0.05 stressed rats vs non-stressed rats.
H. Javelot et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 261 (2014) 8–16 13

Table 3
Effects of CMS and methyl donors deficiency on 5-HT and 5HIAA levels (in ng/mg) and the ratio 5HIAA/5-HT in different brain regions and cervical spinal cord.

Free-feeding Free-feeding Pair-fed Pair-fed Methyl donor deficient Methyl donor deficient
Non-stress Stress Non-stress Stress Non-stress Stress

5HT
Hippocampus 0.204 ± 0.013 0.184 ± 0.015 0.246 ± 0.025 0.222 ± 0.016 0.204 ± 0.025 0.204 ± 0.011
Limbic cortex 0.452 ± 0.015 0.418 ± 0.016 0.448 ± 0.006 0.483 ± 0.021 0.442 ± 0.049 0.479 ± 0.036
Sensorimotor cortex 0.214 ± 0.016 0.160 ± 0.019 0.175 ± 0.018 0.221 ± 0.021 0.207 ± 0.012 0.204 ± 0.029
Striatum 0.344 ± 0.038 0.397 ± 0.024 0.417 ± 0.020 0.330 ± 0.036 0.418 ± 0.031 0.339 ± 0.016
Thalamus 0.417 ± 0.012 0.404 ± 0.021 0.378 ± 0.016 0.400 ± 0.019 0.385 ± 0.026 0.340 ± 0.028
Hypothalamus 0.764 ± 0.050 0.766 ± 0.031 0.779 ± 0.021 0.725 ± 0.132 0.750 ± 0.050 0.688 ± 0.053
Cerebellum 0.026 ± 0.003 0.031 ± 0.003 0.032 ± 0.003 0.020 ± 0.003 0.031 ± 0.002 0.035 ± 0.004
Medulla 0.353 ± 0.017 0.332 ± 0.010 0.336 ± 0.020 0.373 ± 0.021 0.403 ± 0.038††,‡‡‡ 0.407 ± 0.016††,‡‡‡

5HIAA
Hippocampus 0.184 ± 0.013 0.166 ± 0.012 0.218 ± 0.022 0.195 ± 0.014 0.190 ± 0.023 0.185 ± 0.006
Limbic cortex 0.219 ± 0.007 0.201 ± 0.011 0.216 ± 0.008 0.216 ± 0.010 0.197 ± 0.022 0.197 ± 0.011
Sensorimotor cortex 0.153 ± 0.010 0.128 ± 0.010 0.139 ± 0.012 0.139 ± 0.009 0.131 ± 0.008 0.122 ± 0.011
Striatum 0.263 ± 0.018 0.298 ± 0.012 0.342 ± 0.024 0.237 ± 0.025 0.282 ± 0.018 0.239 ± 0.010
Thalamus 0.322 ± 0.008 0.292 ± 0.014 0.326 ± 0.017 0.310 ± 0.013 0.288 ± 0.011††,‡ 0.250 ± 0.024††,‡
Hypothalamus 0.424 ± 0.024 0.423 ± 0.014 0.432 ± 0.029 0.405 ± 0.055 0.381 ± 0.036 0.350 ± 0.033
Cerebellum 0.032 ± 0.002 0.029 ± 0.002 0.030 ± 0.003 0.028 ± 0.001 0.027 ± 0.002 0.054 ± 0.024
Medulla 0.145 ± 0.007 0.150 ± 0.006 0.146 ± 0.008 0.156 ± 0.011 0.169 ± 0.005‡ 0.162 ± 0.005‡

5HIAA/5HT
Hippocampus 0.906 ± 0.039 0.917 ± 0.057 0.917 ± 0.084 0.883 ± 0.042 0.940 ± 0.054 0.922 ± 0.042
Limbic cortex 0.485 ± 0.010 0.482 ± 0.023 0.483 ± 0.016 0.449 ± 0.016 0.433 ± 0.029 0.417 ± 0.016
Sensorimotor cortex 0.723 ± 0.021 0.821 ± 0.036 0.803 ± 0.032 0.648 ± 0.037$$ 0.633 ± 0.022***,# 0.618 ± 0.022*,###
Striatum 0.798 ± 0.048 0.761 ± 0.042 0.820 ± 0.039 0.725 ± 0.023 0.680 ± 0.020†,‡ 0.708 ± 0.016†,‡
Thalamus 0.774 ± 0.014 0.730 ± 0.038 0.880 ± 0.074 0.776 ± 0.021 0.759 ± 0.029 0.739 ± 0.038
Hypothalamus 0.561 ± 0.026 0.556 ± 0.019 0.556 ± 0.036 0.585 ± 0.033 0.502 ± 0.021†,‡ 0.507 ± 0.025†,‡
Cerebellum 1.545 ± 0.406 0.973 ± 0.033 0.952 ± 0.071 1.826 ± 0.435 0.872 ± 0.066 1.719 ± 0.835
Medulla 0.412 ± 0.012 0.453 ± 0.012 0.434 ± 0.009 0.416 ± 0.012$$ 0.391 ± 0.016* 0.400 ± 0.005###

All data were expressed as the mean ± SEM.


Specific comparisons in non-stressed or stressed rats with Fisher’s test: * P < 0.05 and *** P < 0.001 methyl donor deficient vs pair-fed; # P < 0.05 and ### P < 0.001 vs free-feeding
and $$ P < 0.01 pair-fed vs free-feeding.
Specific comparisons with non-stressed and stressed rats pooled with Fisher’s test: † P < 0.05 and †† P < 0.01 methyl donor deficient vs pair-fed; ‡ P < 0.05 and ‡‡‡ P < 0.001 vs
free-feeding.

methyl donor deficient rats had lower dopamine levels than among non-stressed groups, and, also in the free-feeding rats com-
pair-fed rats among stressed groups and pair-fed rats had lower pared to methyl donor deficient ones among stressed groups. In
levels than free-feeding rats among non-stressed groups. the cerebellum, levels were higher for methyl donor deficient and
free-feeding rats compared to pair-fed ones among stressed groups.
3.3.3. DOPAC/dopamine ratio
There were diet effects on the DOPAC/dopamine ratio in the 3.3.6. 5HIAA/5HT ratio
striatum and the hypothalamus [F(2,42) = 8.39, P = 0.0009; F(2,41) There were effects of diet conditions on 5HIAA/5HT ratio in
=8, P = 0.001, respectively]. Methyl donor deficient rats had a lower the sensorimotor cortex, the striatum, the hypothalamus, and
ratio than either pair-fed or free-feeding rats in the striatum the medulla [F(2,42) = 13.11, P < 0.0001; F(2,42) = 4.44, P = 0.02;
[P < 0.01 and P < 0.001, respectively] and the hypothalamus [P < 0.05 F(2,41) = 3.67, P = 0.03; F(2,41) = 6.38, P = 0.004, respectively], as
and P < 0.001, respectively]. well as diet × stress interactions in the sensorimotor cortex and
in the medulla [F(2,42) = 8.37, P = 0.0009; F(2,41) = 3.77, P = 0.03,
3.3.4. 5HIAA respectively]. Methyl donor deficient rats showed a lower ratio
Measurements of 5HIAA levels revealed a diet effect on the than pair-fed and free-feeding rats in the hypothalamus [P < 0.05].
thalamus and the medulla [F(2,42) = 5.51, P = 0.007; F(2,41) = 3.24, Methyl donor deficient rats had a lower ratio than pair-fed rats
P = 0.05, respectively]. Methyl donor deficient rats showed lower in the sensorimotor cortex and the medulla, among non-stressed
5HIAA levels than the others in the thalamus [P < 0.01 and P < 0.05, groups [P < 0.001 and P < 0.05, respectively] and also lower than
respectively] but higher levels than free-feeding rats in the medulla free-feeding rats, but only in the sensorimotor cortex [P < 0.05].
[P < 0.05]. Among stressed groups, methyl donor deficient rats had a lower
ratio than pair-fed rats in the sensorimotor cortex [P < 0.05], and
also had a lower ratio relative to free-feeding rats in the sensorim-
3.3.5. 5HT
otor cortex and the medulla [P < 0.001]. Pair-fed rats also showed
There was a diet effect on 5-HT levels in the medulla
a lower ratio than free-feeding rats in the sensorimotor cortex and
[F(2,41) = 10.87, P = 0.0002] and diet × stress interactions in
medulla [P < 0.01].
the sensorimotor cortex, the striatum, and the cerebellum
[F(2,42) = 3.33, P = 0.04; F(2,42) = 3.24, P = 0.05; F(2,42) = 4.83,
P = 0.01, respectively]. In the medulla, methyl donor deficient rats 4. Discussion
had higher 5HT levels than the others [P < 0.01 and P < 0.0001,
respectively]. In the sensorimotor cortex, levels were higher in 4.1. Plasma homocysteine and corticosterone
methyl donor deficient and free-feeding rats compared to the pair-
fed among non-stressed groups, and the pair-fed rats compared to The methyl donor deficient diet given over the course of
free-feeding ones among stressed groups. In the striatum, levels 11 weeks led to a 6-fold increase in plasma homocysteine lev-
were higher in the pair-fed rats compared to the free-feeding ones els of Wistar rats relative to pair-fed and free-feeding controls
14 H. Javelot et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 261 (2014) 8–16

independently of exposure to UCMS. The same diet given to mice activity. In particular, the UCMS-exposed methyl donor deficient
for weeks led to a 2-fold increase, also significantly different from group spent less time swimming, more time climbing, and climbed
each control group [19]. Since homocysteinemia was observed after more often than the two control groups, but this result was
selective folate deficiency from 4 to 8 weeks in rats [29–32], this not found among non-UCMS-exposed rats. These results are very
may be due to the absence of this vitamin alone, even though it is similar to those found in the open-field, in which only the UCMS-
likely that this effect was amplified by the additional depletion of exposed methyl donor deficient group reared more often. Alcaro
vitamins B2 and B12 as well as choline. et al. [45] observed increased immobility time in C57BL/6 mice
In addition to homocysteine, the methyl donor deficient diet during forced swimming after 12 days of food restriction, and this
decreased plasma corticosterone concentrations in non-stressed effect was observed in our pair-fed or deficient groups. Although
pair-fed and free-feeding rats. UCMS-induced higher plasma corti- our UCMS-exposed methyl donor deficient group spent less time
costerone levels were similar to those reported by Bielajew et al. swimming, it did not spend more time immobile, but rather more
[33] and Grippo et al. [34], who used this stress paradigm in rats time climbing, which seemed like escape attempts.
during 3 and 4 weeks, respectively. There seems to be a relationship In the forced swimming test, increased climbing time is
between higher serum cortisol concentrations and homocysteine- a characteristic of antidepressant medication. Excessive and
mia in human subjects, although it is dependent on several factors persistent climbing may be due to low corticosterone levels
such as age and co-morbidities [35,36]. Nevertheless, despite of the found in methyl donor deficient rats. Thus, lower activation
reduction of serum folate and vitamin B12 levels, subchronic injec- of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis observed in methyl
tions of either adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) or cortisol donor deficiency rats may have induced an adaptative and
during four days had no impact on plasma homocysteine levels in antidepressant-like behavior. However, hyperhomocysteinemia
human subjects [37], such as UCMS in rats. In contrast, the reduc- could also explain this overactivity in the forced swimming
tion of plasma corticosterone levels in rats exposed to the methyl test through a neurotoxic effect. Indeed, homocysteinic acid, an
donor deficient diet may be due to changes in brain monoamine oxidized derivative of homocysteine, acts as an excitatory neuro-
levels and may have influenced behaviors in open-field, elevated transmitter and is known to induce flight reactions in rats when
plus-maze, and forced swim tests. injected into the dorsal periaqueductal gray area [46]. Increased
climbing in our hyperhomocysteinemic rats may have been caused
4.2. Exploratory activity and forced swimming by glutamatergic overactivity or altered monoamine transmission.

In the open-field, the methyl donor deficient group reared more 4.3. Biogenic amines and metabolites in brain
often than either pair-fed or free-feeding groups among stressed
but not among non-stressed rats. This effect was not found on Diet-induced alterations in biogenic amine concentrations may
ambulatory activity and time spent inside the central square. UCMS be responsible for different plasma corticosterone levels and behav-
interacted with diet on the rearing measure with no effect on iors. Relative to pair-fed and free-feeding rats and independently
ambulatory activity, unlike the reduction of ambulatory activity of UCMS, methyl donor deficient rats had lower DOPAC levels and a
previously found in rats exposed to this paradigm [38]. Conversely, lower DOPAC/dopamine ratio in the hypothalamus, the ratio being
stress-resistant rats increased novelty-seeking behaviors in the also lower in the striatum. Although decreased dopamine turnover
open-field and elevated plus-maze tests following exposure to estimated by its major metabolite DOPAC in the hypothalamus has
UCMS in previous studies [39]. Diet had no effect on rearing in non- been noted during restricted food intake and 30% of body loss [47],
stressed rats, which is consistent with data found in non-stressed this result was found only in our deficient group. A decrease in the
mice [19]. However, the use of this diet in mice increased open-field DOPAC/dopamine ratio in the hypothalamus may be responsible
ambulation and enclosed arm entries. Besides species differences for reduced plasma corticosterone concentrations, since dopamine
which might explain these results, diet exposure began earlier (1 exerts a positive control on ACTH release [48]. Folate deficiency
month versus 3) and lasted longer (8 weeks versus 5) in rats than and homocysteinemia are liable to disrupt monoamine synthesis
Balb/c mice, known for its low levels of activity relative to other [4,5]. Conversely, folic acid administration is sometimes of benefit
strains such as C57BL/6 [40]. Food restriction alone has long been on human hyperactivity symptoms, seemingly characterized by a
shown to induce hyperactivity in rats [41], but this result was not hypodopaminergic state [49]. The offspring of dams given a methyl
found in our study, except for an increase in rearing behaviors in donor deficient diet accumulated homocysteine in striatal neu-
stressed rats exposed to methyl donor deficiency. rons and astrocytes [22]. Homocysteinemia causes neuroexcitatory
There was an increased vertical exploration and open arm explo- effects, notably in the striatum, mediated by N-methyl-d-aspartate
ration in the elevated plus-maze: the methyl donor deficient group (NMDA) or metabotropic glutamate receptors [50], leading to
entered more open arms and spent more time in the open arms than neurotoxicity [51,52]. Therefore, changes in dopaminergic trans-
pair-fed or free-feeding rats regardless of UCMS, which is consistent mission in striatum may be responsible for increased rearing
with decreased anxiety. In studies with C57BL/6 and Balb/c mice, in open-field and increased climbing during forced swimming.
rearing activity was positively correlated with open and enclosed Indeed, peripherally administered dopamine-stimulating drugs
arm entries [40]. However, these measures were not correlated can increase rearing in the open-field and climbing in the forced
in Wistar rats selectively bred for high rearing activity, who were swim test [53]. The diet effect superseded any stress effect on
more active than those with low rearing activity, but whose behav- dopaminergic striatal activity, although UCMS may also decrease
ior did not differ from typical Wistar rats in open arm exploration this activity [54].
[42]. Inoue et al. [43] reported that acute and subchronic (10 days) Independently of UCMS and in parallel to dopamine-related
food restriction in rats increased time spent in the open arms, but, mechanisms, methyl donor deficient rats had lower 5HIAA levels
in the present conditions, this result was observed only in the defi- and a lower 5HIAA/5HT ratio in the thalamus compared to either
cient groups. The decreased anxiety rates may be due to reduced control group. The ratio was also lower in the sensorimor cortex, but
corticosterone levels found in these groups, because, when the hor- only in the subgroup exposed to UCMS. Moreover, the methyl donor
mone was injected in the amygdala of rats, open arm exploration deficient group also increased 5HT levels in the medulla. Since
decreased [44]. Ashcroft and Sharman’s pioneering work [55], decreased levels of
In addition to exploratory activity, the vitamin B-deficient diet 5HIAA in CSF of depressive patients has been well established and
influenced the forced swim test, which reflects depression-like could reflect a general decrease in 5HT turnover, but this result was
H. Javelot et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 261 (2014) 8–16 15

found only in our deficient group and only in the thalamus and the [4] Stahl SM. Novel therapeutics for depression: l-methylfolate as a tri-
sensorimotor cortex. Subnormal levels in the methyl donor defi- monoamine modulator and antidepressant-augmenting agent. CNS Spectr
2007;12(10):739–44.
cient group may be due to folate or vitamin B12 deficiency. Indeed, [5] Miller AL. The methylation, neurotransmitter, and antioxidant connections
either folate or vitamin B12 deficiency diminished CSF levels of between folate and depression. Altern Med Rev 2008;13(3):216–26.
5HIAA in humans [56,57] and folate deficiency diminished whole- [6] Gilbody S, Lewis S, Lightfoot T. Methylenetetrahydrofolatereductase (MTHFR)
genetic polymorphisms and psychiatric disorders: a HuGE review. Am J Epi-
brain 5HT levels in rats [56]. This decrease is likely responsible for demiol 2007;165:1–13.
depressive symptoms often observed in folate-deprived patients [7] Guaraldi GP, Fava M, Mazzi F, la Greca P. An open trial of methyltetrahydrofolate
[4,5]. In addition to the possible influence of B vitamins, 5HIAA- in elderly depressed patients. Ann Clin Psychiatry 1993;5:101–5.
[8] Kagan BL, Sultzer DL, Rosenlicht N, Gerner RH. Oral S-adenosylmethionine in
related changes may have been caused by low corticosterone levels,
depression: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Am J Psychi-
as adrenalectomy causes regionally selective changes in 5HT lev- atry 1990;147:591–5.
els or turnover that are reversed following administration of the [9] Coppen A, Bailey J. Enhancement of the antidepressant action of fluoxe-
tine by folic acid: a randomised, placebo controlled trial. J Affect Disord
hormone [58].
2000;60:121–30.
Although it also interacts with diet, the impact of UCMS on [10] Alpert JE, Mischoulon D, Rubenstein GE, Bottonari K, Nierenberg AA, Fava M.
monoamine levels was minimal. Nevertheless, stressed rats had Folinic acid (Leucovorin) as an adjunctive treatment for SSRI-refractory depres-
higher DOPAC levels in medulla, but did not differ from non- sion. Ann Clin Psychiatry 2002;14(1):33–8.
[11] Czyrak A, Rogóz Z, Skuza G, Zajaczkowski W, Maj J. Antidepressant activity
stressed rats regarding 5HT or 5HIAA. Medial and dorsal raphe of S-adenosyl-l-methionine in mice and rats. J Basic Clin Physiol Pharmacol
neurons projecting to substantia nigra, ventral tegmentum, and 1992;3(1):1–17.
neocortex are usually activated during stressful episodes [59], and [12] Benelli A, Filaferro M, Bertolini A, Genedani S. Influence of S-adenosyl-l-
methionine on chronic mild stress-induced anhedonia in castrated rats. Br J
sometimes 5HT turnover increases during chronic stress [60–62]. In Pharmacol 1999;127(3):645–54.
contrast, UCMS has been described as lowering 5HT and 5HIAA lev- [13] Genedani S, Saltini S, Benelli A, Filaferro M, Bertolini A. Influence of SAMe on
els, or the 5HIAA/5-HT ratio, particularly in the frontal cortex, more the modifications of brain polyamine levels in an animal model of depression.
Neuroreport 2001;12(18):3939–42.
variably in the striatum, the hippocampus or the hypothalamus [14] Molina-Hernández M, Téllez-Alcántara NP. Fluoxetine, 17-␤ estradiol or folic
[54,63,64]. However, this result was not found here. acid combined with intra-lateral septal infusions of neuropeptide Y produced
In sum, these data indicate that MDD stress-related behaviors antidepressant-like actions in ovariectomized rats forced to swim. Peptides
2011;32(12):2400–6.
are potentially similar to those reported in agitated depression [15] Molina-Hernández M, Téllez-Alcántara NP, Olivera-López JI, Jaramillo MT. The
with psychomotor agitation and anxiety [65]. These symptoms are folic acid combined with 17-␤ estradiol produces antidepressant-like actions
observed in melancholic depression with weight loss and reduced in ovariectomized rats forced to swim. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psy-
chiatry 2011;35(January (1)):60–6.
folate levels [66,67], in major depressive disorders with burst of
[16] Brocardo PS, Budni J, Lobato KR, Santos AR, Rodrigues AL. Evidence for the
anger and hyperhomocysteinemia [21] or in geriatric depression, involvement of the opioid system in the antidepressant-like effect of folic acid
highly associated with hyperhomocyteinemia, whose agitated- in the mouse forced swimming test. Behav Brain Res 2009;200(1):122–7.
form include irritability, pacing and unstoppable worries [68–70]. [17] Brocardo PS, Budni J, Lobato KR, Kaster MP, Rodrigues AL. Antidepressant-like
effect of folic acid: involvement of NMDA receptors and l-arginine-
nitric oxide-cyclic guanosine monophosphate pathway. Eur J Pharmacol
2008;598(1–3):37–42.
5. Conclusions [18] Brocardo PS, Budni J, Kaster MP, Santos AR, Rodrigues AL. Folic acid
administration produces an antidepressant-like effect in mice: evidence for
the involvement of the serotonergic and noradrenergic systems. Neuro-
Psychiatric disorders are often due to multiple factors result- pharmacology 2008;54(2):464–73.
ing from behavioral and neurobiological disturbances. Deficiencies [19] Lalonde R, Barraud H, Ravey J, Guéant JL, Bronowicki JP, Strazielle C. Effects
in B-vitamins and chronic stress could be causative agents act- of a B-vitamin-deficient diet on exploratory activity, motor coordination, and
spatial learning in young adult Balb/c mice. Brain Res 2008;1188:122–31.
ing together in depression and other mood-related disorders.
[20] Willner P. Chronic mild stress (CMS) revisited: consistency and behavioural-
Our results show that, on one hand, chronic stress exacerbated neurobiological concordance in the effects of CMS. Neuropsychobiology
the effects of methyl donor deficiency in open-field and forced 2005;52(2):90–110.
swim tests. On the other hand, methyl donor deficiency alone [21] Fraguas Jr R, Papakostas GI, Mischoulon D, Bottiglieri T, Alpert J, Fava M. Anger
attacks in major depressive disorder and serum levels of homocysteine. Biol
decreased plasma corticosterone as well as DOPAC and 5HIAA levels Psychiatry 2006;60(3):270–4.
and caused disinhibition in the elevated plus-maze. Our exper- [22] Mineur YS, Prasol DJ, Belzung C, Crusio WE. Agonistic behavior and unpre-
imental data may therefore have relevance to neuropsychiatric dictable chronic mild stress in mice. Behav Genet 2003;33(5):513–9.
[23] Blaise SA, Nédélec E, Schroeder H, Alberto JM, Bossenmeyer-Pourié C, Guéant
disorders frequently associated with folate deficiency, hyperhomo- JL, et al. Gestational vitamin B deficiency leads to homocysteine-associated
cysteinemia and psychomotor agitation, such as geriatric agitated brain apoptosis and alters neurobehavioral development in rats. Am J Pathol
depression. 2007;170(2):667–79.
[24] Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M. Validation of open:closed arm entries in
an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J Neurosci Methods
1985;14:149–67.
Acknowledgments [25] Crawley JN. Exploratory behavior models of anxiety in mice. Neurosci Biobehav
Rev 1985;9(1):37–44.
We thank Sophie Hidalgo and Valérie Demade from ETAP [26] Porsolt RD, Epichon M, Jalfre M. Depression: a new animal model sensitive to
antidepressant treatments. Nature 1977;266:730–2.
Laboratory – Vandoeuvre-Lès-Nancy for technical assistance in [27] Amouzou EK, Chabi NW, Adjalla CE, Rodriguez-Guéant RM, Feillet F, Villaume
behavioral testing. We are grateful to Elise Jeannesson and Philippe C, et al. High prevalence of hyperhomocysteinemia related to folate deficiency
Gérard (LNGERE, INSERM U954 – Vandoeuvre-Lès-Nancy) for help and the 677C→T mutation of the gene encoding methylenetetrahydrofolate
reductase in coastal West Africa. Am J Clin Nutr 2004;79(4):619–24.
in determining homocysteine and corticosterone concentrations in [28] Bats S, Thoumas JL, Lordi B, Tonon MC, Lalonde R, Caston J. The effects
plasma samples. of a mild stressor on spontaneous alternation in mice. Behav Brain Res
2001;118(1):11–5.
[29] Durand P, Prost M, Blache D. Folic acid deficiency enhances oral
contraceptive-induced platelet hyperactivity. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol
References
1997;17(10):1939–46.
[30] Nieman KM, Hartz CS, Szegedi SS, Garrow TA, Sparks JD, Schalinske KL. Folate
[1] Frankenburg FR. The role of one-carbon metabolism in schizophrenia and status modulates the induction of hepatic glycine N-methyltransferase and
depression. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2007;15(4):146–60. homocysteine metabolism in diabetic rats. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab
[2] Kronenberg G, Colla M, Endres M. Folic acid, neurodegenerative and neuropsy- 2006;291(6):E1235–42.
chiatric disease. Curr Mol Med 2009;9(3):315–23. [31] Kim JM, Lee H, Chang N. Hyperhomocysteinemia due to short-term folate
[3] Hutto BR. Folate and cobalamin in psychiatric illness. Compr Psychiatry deprivation is related to electron microscopic changes in the rat brain. J Nutr
1997;38(6):305–14. 2002;132(11):3418–21.
16 H. Javelot et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 261 (2014) 8–16

[32] Ebbesen LS, Ingerslev J. Folate deficiency-induced hyperhomocysteinemia [51] Lipton SA, Kim WK, Choi YB, Kumar S, D’Emilia DM, Rayudu PV, et al. Neurotox-
attenuates, and folic acid supplementation restores, the functional activities icity associated with dual actions of homocysteine at the N-methyl-d-aspartate
of rat coagulation factors XII, X, and II. J Nutr 2005;135(8):1836–40. receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1997;94(11):5923–8.
[33] Bielajew C, Konkle AT, Merali Z. The effects of chronic mild stress on male [52] Jara-Prado A, Ortega-Vazquez A, Martinez-Ruano L, Rios C, Santamaria A.
Sprague-Dawley and Long Evans rats: I. Biochemical and physiological analy- Homocysteine-induced brain lipid peroxidation: effects of NMDA receptor
ses. Behav Brain Res 2002;136(2):583–92. blockade, antioxidant treatment, and nitric oxide synthase inhibition. Neurotox
[34] Grippo AJ, Francis J, Beltz TG, Felder RB, Johnson AK. Neuroendocrine and Res 2003;5(4):237–43.
cytokine profile of chronic mild stress-induced anhedonia. Physiol Behav [53] Vieira C, De Lima TC, Carobrez Ade P, Lino-de-Oliveira C. Frequency of climbing
2005;84(5):697–706. behavior as a predictor of altered motor activity in rat forced swimming test.
[35] Cascalheira JF, Parreira MC, Viegas AN, Faria MC, Domingues FC. Serum homo- Neurosci Lett 2008;445(2):170–3.
cysteine: relationship with circulating levels of cortisol and ascorbate. Ann Nutr [54] Bekris S, Antoniou K, Daskas S, Papadopoulou-Daifoti Z. Behavioural and neu-
Metab 2008;53(1):67–74. rochemical effects induced by chronic mild stress applied to two different rat
[36] Terzidis K, Panoutsopoulos A, Mantzou A, Tourli P, Papageorgiou G, Saltiki K, strains. Behav Brain Res 2005;161(1):45–59.
et al. Cortisol levels and metabolic parameters in middle- and advanced-age [55] Ashcroft GW, Sharman DF. 5-Hydroxyindoles in human cerebrospinal fluids.
subjects: associations with age. J Endocrinol Invest 2011;34(11):e398–402. Nature 1960;186:1050–1.
[37] Berg AL, Rafnsson AT, Johannsson M, Hultberg B, Arnadottir M. The effects of [56] Botez MI, Young SN, Bachevalier J, Gauthier S. Folate deficiency and decreased
adrenocorticotrophic hormone and cortisol on homocysteine and vitamin B brain 5-hydroxytryptamine synthesis in man and rat. Nature 1979;278:182–3.
concentrations. Clin Chem Lab Med 2006;44(5):628–31. [57] Botez MI, Young SN, Bachevalier J, Gauthier S. Effect of folic acid and vitamin
[38] Li LF, Lu J, Li XM, Xu CL, Yang J, Qu R, et al. Antidepressant-like effects of the B12 deficiencies on 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in human cerebrospinal fluid.
saponins extracted from Chaihu-jia-longgu-muli-tang in a rat unpredictable Ann Neurol 1982;12:479–84.
chronic mild stress model. Fitoterapia 2012;83(1):93–103. [58] Biegon A. Effects of steroid hormones on the serotonergic system. Ann N Y Acad
[39] Li Y, Zheng X, Liang J, Peng Y. Coexistence of anhedonia and anxiety- Sci 1990;600:427–34.
independent increased novelty-seeking behavior in the chronic mild stress [59] Morgan WW, Rudeen PK, Pfeil KA. Effect of immobilization stress on serotonin
model of depression. Behav Processes 2010;83(3):331–9. content and turnover in regions of the rat brain. Life Sci 1975;17(1):143–50.
[40] Lalonde R, Strazielle C. Relations between open-field, elevated plus-maze, and [60] Adell A, Garcia-Marquez C, Armario A, Gelpi E. Chronic stress increases sero-
emergence tests as displayed by C57BL/6J and BALB/c mice. J Neurosci Methods tonin and noradrenaline in rat brain and sensitizes their responses to a further
2008;171:48–52. acute stress. J Neurochem 1988;50(6):1678–81.
[41] Routtenberg A, Kuznesof AW. Self-starvation of rats living in activity wheels [61] Petty F, Kramer G, Larrison A. Neurochemistry of stress: regional brain levels of
on a restricted feeding schedule. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1967;64(3):414–21. biogenic amines and metabolites with ten different stressors. Biogen Amines
[42] Görisch J, Schwarting RK. Wistar rats with high versus low rear activity differ 1996;12:377–94.
in radial maze performance. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2006;86:175–87. [62] Blanc S, Somody L, Gharib A, Gauquelin G, Gharib C, Sarda N. Coun-
[43] Inoue K, Zorrilla EP, Tabarin A, Valdez GR, Iwasaki S, Kiriike N, et al. Reduction teraction of spaceflight-induced changes in the rat central serotonergic
of anxiety after restricted feeding in the rat: implication for eating disorders. system by adrenalectomy and corticosteroid replacement. Neurochem Int
Biol Psychiatry 2004;55(11):1075–81. 1998;33(4):375–82.
[44] Myers B, Greenwood-Van Meerveld B. Elevated corticosterone in the amygdala [63] Yi LT, Li JM, Li YC, Pan Y, Xu Q, Kong LD. Antidepressant-like behavioral
leads to persistent increases in anxiety-like behavior and pain sensitivity. Behav and neurochemical effects of the citrus-associated chemical apigenin. Life Sci
Brain Res 2010;214:465–9. 2008;82(13–14):741–51.
[45] Alcaro A, Cabib S, Ventura R, Puglisi-Allegra S. Genotype- and experience- [64] Ahmad A, Rasheed N, Banu N, Palit G. Alterations in monoamine levels and
dependent susceptibility to depressive-like responses in the forced-swimming oxidative systems in frontal cortex, striatum, and hippocampus of the rat brain
test. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;164(2):138–43. during chronic unpredictable stress. Stress 2010;13(4):355–64.
[46] Miguel TL, Pobbe RL, Spiacci Jr A, Zangrossi Jr H. Dorsal raphe nucleus reg- [65] Koukopoulos A, Koukopoulos A. Agitated depression as a mixed state and the
ulation of a panic-like defensive behavior evoked by chemical stimulation problem of melancholia. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1999;22(3):547–64.
of the rat dorsal periaqueductal gray matter. Behav Brain Res 2010;213(2): [66] Fava M, Borus JS, Alpert JE, Nierenberg AA, Rosenbaum JF, Bottiglieri T. Folate,
195–200. vitamin B12, and homocysteine in major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry
[47] Broocks A, Liu J, Pirke KM. Semistarvation-induced hyperactivity com- 1997;154(3):426–8.
pensates for decreased norepinephrine and dopamine turnover in the [67] Gold PW, Chrousos GP. The endocrinology of melancholic and atypical depres-
mediobasal hypothalamus of the rat. J Neural Transm Gen Sect 1990;79(1–2): sion: relation to neurocircuitry and somatic consequences. Proc Assoc Am
113–24. Physicians 1999;111(1):22–34.
[48] Jezová D, Jurcovicová J, Vigas M, Murgas K, Labrie F. Increase in plasma ACTH [68] Alexopoulos GS, Vrontou C, Kakuma T, Meyers BS, Young RC, Klausner E, et al.
after dopaminergic stimulation in rats. Psychopharmacology 1985;85:201–3. Disability in geriatric depression. Am J Psychiatry 1996;153(7):877–85.
[49] Patrick LR. Restless legs syndrome: pathophysiology and the role of iron and [69] Dimopoulos N, Piperi C, Salonicioti A, Psarra V, Gazi F, Papadimitriou A, et al.
folate. Altern Med Rev 2007;12(2):101–12. Correlation of folate, vitamin B12 and homocysteine plasma levels with depres-
[50] Ziemińska E, Stafiej A, Łazarewicz JW. Role of group I metabotropic sion in an elderly Greek population. Clin Biochem 2007;40(9–10):604–8.
glutamate receptors and NMDA receptors in homocysteine-evoked acute [70] Kim JM, Stewart R, Kim SW, Yang SJ, Shin IS, Yoon JS. Predictive value of folate,
neurodegeneration of cultured cerebellar granule neurons. Neurochem Int vitamin B12 and homocysteine levels in late-life depression. Br J Psychiatry
2003;43(4–5):481–92. 2008;192(4):268–74.

View publication stats

You might also like