You are on page 1of 29

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/303322636

A questionnaire-based validation of multidimensional models of


self-regulated learning strategies

Article  in  Modern Language Journal · May 2016


DOI: 10.1111/modl.12339

CITATIONS READS

167 4,051

2 authors:

Lin Sophie Teng Lawrence Jun Zhang


University of Auckland University of Auckland
20 PUBLICATIONS   732 CITATIONS    334 PUBLICATIONS   6,233 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Feedback Effects on Learner Development in Second Language Learning and Teaching View project

teacher education View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Lin Sophie Teng on 12 December 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


A Questionnaire-Based Validation
of Multidimensional Models
of Self-Regulated Learning Strategies
LIN SOPHIE TENG LAWRENCE JUN ZHANG*
The University of Auckland The University of Auckland
Faculty of Education and Social Work Faculty of Education and Social Work
School of Curriculum and Pedagogy School of Curriculum and Pedagogy
Private Bag 92601, Symonds Street Private Bag 92601, Symonds Street
Auckland 1150 Auckland 1150
New Zealand New Zealand
Email: lin.teng@auckland.ac.nz Email: lj.zhang@auckland.ac.nz

This study aimed to validate a newly-developed instrument, The Writing Strategies for Self-Regulated
Learning (SRL) Questionnaire, with respect to its multifaceted structure of SRL strategies in English as
a foreign language (EFL) writing. A total of 790 undergraduate students from 6 universities in Northeast
China volunteered to be participants. Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) through structural equation
modeling (SEM) were applied to evaluate 3 hypothesized models. The results of the CFA validated a
9-factor correlated model of second language (L2) writing strategies for SRL with satisfactory psychome-
tric characteristics. Model comparisons confirmed a hierarchical, multidimensional structure of SRL as
the best model, in which self-regulation, as a higher order construct, accounted for the correlations of
the 9 lower-order writing strategies, pertaining to cognitive, metacognitive, social–behavioral, and moti-
vational regulation aspects. Multiple regression analysis revealed that 6 out of 9 SRL strategies had sig-
nificant predictive effects on EFL writing proficiency. The empirical evidence lends preliminary support
to a transfer of SRL theory from educational psychology to the field of L2/EFL education, particularly
L2/EFL writing. Implications of these findings are discussed.
Keywords: self-regulated learning (SRL); writing strategies for SRL; Chinese university learners; question-
naire validation; language learning strategies (LLSs); multidimensional models of SRL

THERE IS GROWING RECOGNITION THAT motivation and behavior” (Pintrich, Wolters, &
one of the fundamental goals of education is Baxter, 2000, p. 453). In the field of second and
to teach students to become self-regulated learn- foreign language (L2) education, studies reveal
ers, who can actively and efficiently manage their the primary role of SRL strategies in fostering stu-
own learning by deploying various self-regulated dents’ self-regulated capacity to improve their lan-
learning (SRL) strategies in the learning process guage proficiency (e.g., Oxford, 2013). It appears
(Zimmerman & Risemberg, 1997). In the edu- that self-regulation is pivotal to student success in
cational psychology literature, SRL is defined as L2 learning in academic settings.
“an active, constructive process whereby learners Dörnyei (2005) posits that self-regulation is “a
set goals for their learning and then attempt to multidimensional construct, including cognitive,
monitor, regulate, and control their cognition, metacognitive, motivational, behavioral, and envi-
ronmental processes that learners can apply to en-
*corresponding author. hance academic achievement” in different learn-
The Modern Language Journal, 100, 0, (2016) ing contexts (p. 101). To date, most empirical
DOI: 10.1111/modl.12339 studies have documented cognitive and metacog-
0026-7902/16/1–28 $1.50/0 nitive strategies in first language (L1) reading and

C 2016 The Modern Language Journal
writing (e.g., Harris et al., 2011; Zimmerman &
2 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
Bandura, 1994) as well as L2 reading (N. J. Ander- goals through the effective deployment of learn-
son, 1991; Zhang, 2008; Zhang, Gu, & Hu, 2008). ing strategies.
By contrast, there has been little explicit discus- Researchers such as Oxford (2013) and Zim-
sion of L2 writing strategies based on SRL theo- merman (2011) have acknowledged that SRL
ries, especially in English as a foreign language strategies build on a multifaceted structure that
(EFL) settings, nor has the interplay of the multi- includes cognition, metacognition, social be-
ple dimensions of SRL strategies been empirically havior, and motivational regulation. Learners
examined in L2 contexts. deploy different dimensions of SRL strategies to
Furthermore, little attention has been given to help them actively control their internal mental
the development of instruments for evaluating states, beliefs, observable behaviors, and environ-
SRL strategies in EFL contexts. Despite an abun- ments in the learning process (Andrade & Evans,
dance of research on evaluating SRL strategies in 2012; Zimmerman, 2013). During the past three
L1 settings (e.g., Pintrich et al., 1991; Weinstein, decades, extensive research on SRL strategies has
Schulte, & Palmer, 1987), these instruments can- been conducted in order to find ways for improv-
not be generalized across settings, especially with ing students’ academic achievement and fostering
regard to EFL writing, which has been acknowl- their learning capacity. A first step was to classify
edged as a complex, situational, and multifaceted the SRL strategies used in general learning con-
process, fluctuating in a wide range of contexts texts. Zimmerman and Martinez–Pons (1986),
(Byrnes, 2014a; Manchón, 2009; Zhang, 2013). for example, working with elementary and sec-
These lacunae point to a need to develop a theo- ondary students from both high achievement
retically robust instrument with vigorous psycho- and low achievement tracks, endeavored to iden-
metric properties to evaluate students’ perceived tify different categories of SRL strategies. They
use of SRL strategies in EFL writing. developed a semi-structured interview guide, the
Our study attempts to conceptualize, develop, Self-Regulated Learning Interview Scale (SRLIS),
and validate a new questionnaire framed within to investigate students’ reported use of SRL strate-
the multidimensional structure of SRL theory for gies in the contexts of class, homework, and self-
evaluating EFL students’ perceived use of writing study. They elicited 14 categories of SRL strate-
strategies. We propose a multifaceted portrayal gies, which included self-evaluation, organization
of EFL writing strategies that includes cognition, and transformation, goal setting and plan-
metacognition, social behavior, and motivational ning, information seeking, record keeping, self-
regulation, all of which are essential to fostering monitoring, environmental structuring, giving
self-regulated writers. We also explore how stu- self-consequences, rehearsing and memorizing,
dents’ reported use of writing strategies for SRL seeking social assistance, and reviewing. Their
affects their writing proficiency. study revealed that of the three factors, gender,
social economic status, and SRL strategies, SRL
SELF-REGULATED LEARNING (SRL) measures demonstrated the best prediction of
students’ standardized achievement test scores.
SRL theory originates from educational psy- Another strand of SRL research gave primary
chology and has exhibited exponential develop- attention to how individual differences influence
ment since the 1970s. As a construct, SRL refers to the deployment of SRL strategies. These stud-
the degree to which individuals are active partici- ies on individual variations investigated constructs
pants in their own learning; it is a more dynamic such as motivations and goals (e.g., Zimmerman,
concept than learning strategies in that the learn- 2008), attributions (e.g., Schunk & Rice, 1986),
ers’ “strategic efforts to manage their own achieve- self-efficacy (e.g., Schunk & Ertmer, 2000), and
ment through specific beliefs and processes” are emotions (e.g., Boekaerts, 2011). The empirical
pivotal to their success in learning (Zimmerman findings so far have confirmed that the use of SRL
& Risemberg, 1997, p. 105). SRL involves “cogni- strategies is influenced by a range of individual
tive, affective, motivational, and behavioral com- differences.
ponents that provide the individual with the ca- In sum, research in the field of general ed-
pacity to adjust his or her actions and goals to ucation presents SRL strategies as multifaceted,
achieve desired results in light of changing en- incorporating metacognition, affect, and diverse
vironment conditions” (Dörnyei, 2005, p. 191). social factors in general or specific learning con-
Zimmerman and Schunk (2011) have argued that texts. On that basis it is now possible to explore,
in the self-regulating process, learners intention- both from theoretical and empirical perspectives,
ally activate, sustain, and adjust cognitions, af- how different SRL strategies influence students’
fects, and behaviors to achieve their learning learning outcomes in the context of EFL writing.
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 3
LANGUAGE LEARNING STRATEGIES (LLS) reorient LLSs as goal-directed activities in rela-
AND SRL tion to specific L2 contexts. Such a perspective
enables LLSs to be further explored within the
When discussing SRL strategies for L2 learn- framework of SRL, which is interpreted as “an
ing it is important to refer to the already well- active, constructive process whereby learners
developed field of language learning strategies set goals for their learning and then attempt to
(LLSs). Emanating from the seminal work of monitor, regulate and control their cognition,
Rubin (1975; see also Stern, 1975), a rich body of motivation and behavior, guided and constrained
studies has underlined the important relationship by their goals and the contextual features in the
between LLSs and students’ academic achieve- environment” (Pintrich, 2000, p. 453).
ment (e.g., Cohen & Macaro, 2007; O’Malley & An SRL perspective would be especially con-
Chamot, 1990). However, that research on LLSs ducive to promoting active and productive
has encountered a barrage of criticisms, including learning in specific L2 language skill areas and
definitional fuzziness, contentious taxonomies, learning contexts. Tseng et al. (2006), for ex-
insufficient theorizing, and a lack of a psycho- ample, devised the Self-Regulation Capacity in
metrically sound instrument for measuring LLSs Vocabulary Learning Scale to assess the capacity
(Dörnyei, 2005; Tseng, Dörnyei, & Schmitt, 2006; of strategic learning within an SRL framework.
Woodrow, 2005). Confirmatory factor analysis validated a 5-factor
Such criticisms would seem to indicate that LLS model that included commitment control, emo-
research has run its course. However, researchers tional control, metacognitive control, satiation
have recently called for a more differentiated control, and environment control. Subsequent
analysis and evaluation of LLS research (e.g., exploratory factor analysis revealed the unidi-
Cohen & Griffiths, 2015). It is worth recalling mensionality of the 5 indicators in the instrument
that O’Malley and Chamot (1990) derived their with satisfactory psychometric properties. Their
process-oriented theoretical understanding of findings provided support for the appropriate-
LLSs from J. R. Anderson’s (1983, 1985) well- ness of using the construct of self-regulation in
established principles for the adaptive control the specific case of vocabulary learning.
of thought (ACT*). While their approach to Encouraging as those findings are, what re-
LLSs is well aligned with understanding about mains to be accounted for is the multidimension-
learning strategies in both general and educa- ality of the SRL construct, which also includes
tional psychology, it may have overextended itself metacognition, as Gao (2007) and Zhang (2010)
by lumping together all strategies to cover all have argued, and its use in different educational
language skill areas, thereby setting up numerous contexts. For example, Zhang et al.’s (2008) inves-
definitional hurdles. Just as problematic is the fact tigation of SRL strategies of Singaporean primary
that, as Cohen and Macaro (2007), Gao (2007), school students in a bilingual/biliteracy learning
and Rose (2011), among others, have pointed context showed a significant relationship between
out, LLSs have actually not been well researched, students’ use of SRL strategies and their language
leading to inappropriate over-generalizations re- proficiency. However, their study only reported
garding their use (see also, Zhang, 2010). Indeed, cognitive strategies in bilingual reading contexts
by examining LLS use with reference to specific without reference to the interplay of other SRL di-
skill areas (e.g., listening, reading, vocabulary, mensions, such as metacognition, motivation, and
or writing), it has been possible to address the social behavior.
criticisms of strategy taxonomies and to begin to Based on this research evidence, we postu-
operationalize strategy use more firmly (Cohen late that a particular strength of SRL, just like
& Macaro, 2007; Macaro, 2006; Woodrow, 2005). for LLS research, is its heavy emphasis on the
In an additional step, Dörnyei and associates learning process and learners’ pivotal role in it.
(e.g., Dörnyei, 2005; Tseng et al., 2006) have In addition, both SRL and LLS are overarch-
suggested rejuvenating LLS research in terms ing terms that include cognitive, metacognitive,
of a self-regulation mechanism, which concep- social–behavioral, and motivational components.
tualizes learning strategies from the perspective This makes it possible to incorporate the control
of students’ capacity to manage their own learn- mechanism of cognition, behavior, environment,
ing. In fact, some researchers (e.g., Cumming, and motivation and to begin to explore various
Busch, & Zhou, 2002) have proposed that en- dimensions of learners’ development of SRL ca-
hancing learners’ self-regulatory capacity with a pacity in specific contexts, such as L2 writing.
goal orientation is the central tenet of the SRL We therefore suggest that L2 writing as a process
process. Their proposal has helped L2 scholars be usefully examined from a multidimensional
4 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
perspective that includes an understanding of Sasaki, 2000) or focusing on one specific type
how learners set goals; attempt to monitor, regu- such as planning (Victori, 1999), composing
late, and control their cognition, motivation, and (Chenoweth & Hayes, 2001), or revision (Berg,
behavior in the learning process; and consider 1999). Some researchers explored specific ac-
how these aspects, in turn, are often guided and tions related to writing processes, such as orga-
constrained by learners’ goals and diverse contex- nizing the content for planning, translating in
tual features. the writing process, editing after draft completion
(Cumming, 1989; Victori, 1999); or they focused
L2 WRITING RESEARCH on metacognitive strategies with the tripartite
distinction of planning, monitoring, and evalua-
L2 writing research has been strongly influ- tion (Wenden, 1991; Zhang, Aryadoust, & Zhang,
enced by L1 writing theories with different foci 2016). Finally, yet other researchers applied learn-
(Silva & Matsuda, 2010). The effects of cognitive ing strategy classifications to L2 writing to ex-
manipulation on the quality of written texts (e.g., plore multiple dimensions of L2 students’ writing
Ong & Zhang, 2013), the influence of the L1 strategies (see Oxford et al., 2014, for more in-
on the L2 (e.g., Roca de Larios, Martin, & Mur- formation). Not surprisingly, such a classification
phy, 2001), the function of written feedback (e.g., of writing strategies was strongly influenced by
Hyland & Hyland, 2006; Lee, 2014), the role of two prominent taxonomies of general LLSs: (a)
learner differences (e.g., Kormos, 2012), and the O’Malley & Chamot’s (1990) three broad types
effects of genre or task-based writing instruction of learning strategies: cognitive, metacognitive,
(e.g., Byrnes, 2014b; Hyland, 2007) have all been and social-affective; and (b) Oxford’s (1990) six
explored. The next section discusses in greater de- factor strategies, which distinguish direct (mem-
tail yet another prominent research area, that of ory, cognitive, and compensation) from indirect
L2 writing strategies. (metacognitive, affective, and social) strategies.
While these classifications have been widely used
Classifications of L2 Writing Strategies (see, e.g., Oxford, 2013), they have not been
sufficiently validated with specific populations or
Driven by the proliferation of research on pro- learning contexts. Only Hsiao and Oxford (2002)
cess writing in L1 English in the 1980s in North evaluated different functional classifications of
America, L2 writing researchers (e.g., Leki, 1995; LLSs with EFL students in Taiwan through confir-
Roca de Larios et al., 2008) turned to investigat- matory factor analyses; they found that Oxford’s
ing and classifying specific strategies on the part (1990) six part taxonomy had the best fit when
of L2 writers. However, their efforts were ham- compared with other models (e.g., O’Malley &
pered by the “shaky theoretical foundation of Chamot, 1990).
research on learner strategies” (Manchón, Roca As social dimensions have increasingly been
de Larios, & Murphy, 2007, p. 230), well mani- adduced in order to illuminate the composing
fested in the lack of an agreed-upon operational process, many researchers have reclassified writ-
classification of writing strategies. At present, ing strategies in terms of the sociocognitive na-
the classification of writing strategies is grounded ture of writing activities. For example, Leki (1995)
in an array of theoretical paradigms, among them reclassified writing strategies into 10 categories
cognitive models of L1 writing with a focus on that included clarifying and focusing strategies for
process writing (Flower & Hayes, 1981), empiri- completing a task, using previous knowledge or
cal research on language learning and language experience, making use of social contexts such
use strategies in general contexts (O’Malley & as using training or others’ feedback, taking a
Chamot, 1990; Oxford, 1990), a sociocognitive ap- stance toward teachers’ demands, and managing
proach (Leki, 1995), and goal theories in educa- time and learning efforts. She highlighted the
tional psychology (Cumming et al., 2002). need to depict “the fullest range possible of strate-
The process writing approach itself originated gies employed” (Leki, 1995, p. 240), an emphasis
from cognitive theory based on the three pri- that was echoed by Riazi (1997), who concluded
mary cognitive processes: planning, translating, that literacy production is “an interactive social-
and reviewing (Flower & Hayes, 1981). Accord- cognitive process in that production of the texts
ingly, writing strategies were generally classified required extensive interaction between the indi-
in terms of three phases: pre-writing, drafting vidual’s cognitive process and social/contextual
(composing), and after writing (revising). A pro- factors in different ways” (p. 105).
fusion of research studies followed, either ad- In another attempt to provide a more solid
dressing all three processes (e.g., Cumming, 1989; ground for strategies research, researchers such
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 5
as Cumming et al. (2002) have used goal theories Another strand of research has investigated
in educational psychology as a frame of reference how L1 strategies and language proficiency in-
and have defined writing strategies as “the level fluenced the use of L2 writing strategies. Some
of strategic operations for the activity of writing” studies (Cumming, 1989; Silva, 1993) found that
directed by goals (p. 193). They favored a re- L2 writing strategies are similar to L1 writing
search path in which strategies “are analyzed in strategies but strategy deployment is influenced
reference to the goals people have to motivate by learners’ L1/L2 language proficiency. More-
and guide their task performance as well as other over, the transfer of L1 strategies to the L2 is in-
essential aspects of these activity structures and fluenced by cultural and contextual factors and
the contexts in which they are embedded” (Cum- writing processes (see Manchón et al., 2007, for a
ming et al., 2002, p. 193). They grouped writing summary).
strategies into five categories while integrating A majority of these studies adopted qualitative
self-regulation as an essential component that in- methods that drew on concurrent or retrospec-
cluded peer learning from others, self-regulation, tive introspection techniques (e.g., think-aloud
stimulation, and use of tools for resourcing. protocols and interviews) and worked with small
Because this line of classification integrates social samples of participants in order to solicit situa-
and emotional regulation in order to capture tional and individualized writing strategies (e.g.,
qualities of writing strategies, it shows important Raimes, 1985; Sasaki, 2000, see also Griffiths &
similarities with the categories of SRL strategies Oxford, 2014, for a review). However, only a few
proposed by Zimmerman and Martinez–Pons studies (e.g., Cumming, 1989) reported the inter-
(1986). In other words, research on L2 writing rater and intra-rater reliabilities of the qualita-
strategies has shifted from a purely cognitive tive coding and analysis, a fact that makes it dif-
to a sociocognitive orientation, acknowledging ficult to interpret the data (see, e.g., Manchón
the cultural and contextual factors to reveal the et al. 2007). Some studies used self-developed or
multidimensional nature of the writing process modified questionnaires to depict the pattern of
(Silva & Matsuda, 2010). Even so, an empiri- writing strategies used by learners (e.g., Gordon,
cal evaluation of an appropriate way to classify 2008). Unfortunately, such questionnaires were
learning strategies still remains to be presented largely ad hoc creations, without being theoreti-
and validated for the specific context of L2 cally validated in relation to specific sociocultural
writing. contexts. One way to overcome some of these
shortcomings is through the use of validated ques-
Research on L2 Writing Strategies tionnaires with clear reliabilities, an approach ad-
vocated by Petrić and Czárl (2003) for being able
As with general learning strategies, research on to provide holistic information on a large scale for
L2 writing strategies, too, has been strongly in- researchers to depict the pattern of students’ use
fluenced and directed by insights gained in L1 of writing strategies in a specific learning context.
contexts. In line with the theoretical understand-
ing of L1 composing processes, numerous studies EXISTING QUESTIONNAIRES FOR
(e.g., Raimes, 1985; Sasaki, 2000, 2004) have ex- EVALUATING LLS AND SRL STRATEGIES
plored how learners’ language proficiency (i.e.,
high proficiency and low proficiency) and learn- O’Malley and Chamot (1990) pointed out that
ing experience (i.e., novice/expert, skilled/less questionnaires are a useful method for solicit-
skilled, successful/less successful) influence the ing individual perceptions and interpretations of
use of certain types of writing strategies (see also students’ own learning experience that can pro-
Manchón et al., 2007, for a review). While ear- vide explanations for behavior (e.g., trait-like fea-
lier studies evaluated different cognitive learning tures). Large-scale questionnaires, in particular,
strategies in the L2 writing process with a focus on enable the use of quantitative methodologies as
planning, composing, and revising, more recent a way of developing a model for understanding
work has foregrounded the writing process as a how writing strategies interact with other factors
“socially situated, cognitive, communicative activ- such as social and psychological variables in L2
ity” (Manchón et al., 2007, p. 229). Then followed contexts (Hsiao & Oxford, 2002; Petrić & Czárl,
a plethora of L2 studies that have taken a more 2003; Zhang et al., 2016).
inclusive view of writing strategies and explored, In the literature, three questionnaires devel-
in particular, how writing proficiency significantly oped to evaluate LLSs in L2 settings and SRL
influences the use of a range of writing strategies strategies in general learning contexts stand out:
(e.g., Gordon, 2008; Zhang et al., 2016). the Strategy Inventory of Language Learning
6 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
(SILL; Oxford, 1990), the Learning and Study of what Pintrich et al. referred to as students’
Strategies Inventory (LASSI; Weinstein et al., value (extrinsic and intrinsic goal orientation
1987), and the Motivated Strategies for Learning and task value), expectancy (control belief and
Questionnaire (MSLQ; Pintrich et al., 1991). The self-efficacy), and affect (test anxiety); the learn-
first, the SILL (Oxford, 1990), has been widely ing strategy scales include students’ use of cog-
used to investigate learning strategies in L2 con- nitive strategies (rehearsal, elaboration, orga-
texts. The SILL is a 5-point scale questionnaire nization, and critical thinking), metacognitive
ranging from 1 (never use it) to 5 (often use it). strategies (planning, monitoring, and regulat-
The instrument has been used to address specific ing strategies), and resource management (ef-
strategic behavior, including six types of strate- fort management, time and environment man-
gies broken down into: (a) memory strategies, agement, and help-seeking). The motivation and
(b) cognitive strategies, (c) compensation strate- learning strategy sections correspond to the three
gies, (d) metacognitive strategies, (e) affective elements in the definition of SRL: motivation,
strategies, and (f) social strategies. Some re- metacognition, and behavior. The instrument is
searchers have developed their versions of inven- intended as a coherent framework in which the
tories based on the SILL to investigate writing 15 different subscales on the MSLQ could be
strategies (e.g., Petrić & Czárl, 2003). Although utilized together or singly by taking the mean
this instrument has been extensively used in vari- of the items corresponding to each factor. The
ous contexts, especially in general language learn- MSLQ has undergone extensive psychometric
ing environments, it is not above criticism. As testing and the overall internal consistency reli-
Tseng et al. (2006) argued, the scales of specific ability (Cronbach’s alpha) was found to be ade-
strategies in the SILL are not cumulative, which quate for a motivation scale (α = .78) and a learn-
makes it impossible to “assume a linear relation- ing strategy scale (α = .71), respectively. How-
ship between individual item scores and the to- ever, confirmatory factor analysis did not gener-
tal item scores” (p. 83). That shortcoming has ate satisfactory goodness-of-fit indices for the mo-
marred the application of the SILL to specific con- tivation sections (GFI = .77; AGFI = .73; RMR
texts with robust psychometric properties. = .07), or for the learning strategy subscales
The second instrument, the LASSI, was de- (GFI = .78; AGFI = .75; RMR = .08) (Pintrich
veloped by Weinstein et al. (1987) as a diag- et al., 1991). The goodness-of-fit indices were
nostic and prescriptive instrument within SRL lower than the recommended benchmark values
theory to evaluate students’ awareness about (Hu & Bentler, 1999), indicating that the MSLQ’s
and use of learning strategies in terms of skill, construct validity might not be reliable enough,
will, and self-regulation. It is an 80-item self- particularly if it is applied in other learning
report inventory with a 5-point scale ranging contexts.
from 1 (not at all typical of me) to 5 (very Findings from these instruments clearly sup-
much typical of me). Of the 10 scale strategies, port utilizing questionnaires to evaluate learning
the Skill categories include concentration, se- strategies. Also, the popularity of the MSLQ and
lecting main ideas, and information processing; the LASSI produced clear evidence that SRL was
the Will categories include motivation, attitudes, an important construct that merits further re-
and anxiety; the Self-regulation categories com- search. However, the utility of these instruments
prise time management, study aids, self-testing, has been subject to criticism. As noted earlier,
and test strategies. Coefficient alphas of the 10 critics initially argued against the broad-brush in-
scale strategies range from.76 to .87. The inter- vestigation of general learning strategies, which
nal correlations between these scales range from intrinsically marred the conceptual framework
.13 (between information process and anxiety) and classification of these instruments. In addi-
to .59 (between time management and motiva- tion, contextual and cultural differences have not
tion), revealing the discriminant validity of the been fully considered and explored. For exam-
measurement. ple, the LASSI and the MSLQ inventories were
Pintrich et al. (1991) developed a self-report in- widely applied in educational contexts, such as
strument, the MSLQ, with 7-point Likert scales sports, music, and L1 reading and writing, with-
ranging from 1 (not at all true of me) to 7 out sufficient empirical validity in L2 specific
(very true of me). The MSLQ was designed contexts.
to evaluate two distinct constructs: motivation In sum, how to evaluate writing strategies for
(31 items) and learning strategies (50 items) SRL in EFL settings is far from resolved and
of college students in classroom environments. needs to be addressed in relation to specific
The motivational scales include the assessment learning contexts. Instruments that were initially
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 7
developed for L1 or L2 learners in general learn- RESEARCH ON WRITING STRATEGIES
ing contexts cannot be directly applied to EFL IN CHINA
writing contexts, nor can their data be treated as
reliable for providing insights into specific writ- Incorporating aspects of process-oriented writ-
ing issues. Such a lacuna in the research literature ing instruction as Western pedagogies, research
calls for the development of a theoretically more on writing strategies in China has focused on de-
robust instrument with strong psychometric prop- scriptive studies, the transfer of L1 writing strate-
erties to evaluate the use of writing strategies for gies to L2 writing, individual differences in the
SRL in EFL environments. use of strategies, and the effect of strategies-based
writing instruction (e.g., Lei, 2008; Nguyen & Gu,
2013; Wang & Wen, 2002). Previous studies have
WRITING INSTRUCTION IN CHINA foregrounded individual differences in terms of
Although writing is a required component in age, motivation, self-efficacy, and language profi-
the English language teaching curriculum in Chi- ciency and have found that Chinese students re-
nese universities, it is commonly regarded as the ported using lower level writing strategies. Also,
most challenging language skill and one in which they have tended to explore writing strategies un-
Chinese students find it difficult to develop com- der the optic of a typical process approach with
petence above low performance levels (Lei, 2008; multiple drafting, revising, and teacher or peer
Zhang, 2013). Despite over six years of learning feedback. In other cases they have focused on
English in schools and even with continuous prac- some cognitive and/or metacognitive strategies
tice in university, Chinese students’ improvement without systematic instructional guidance or the-
in writing tends to be limited (Wang, 2014; Zhao, oretical foundation. By comparison, little atten-
2010). Students’ slow progress in writing skills is tion has been given to exploring other categories
also related to how English writing is taught. Be- of writing strategies (e.g., social and affective
cause the teaching of EFL writing is test-driven strategies) from SRL theory in EFL writing (Teng
and product-oriented, teachers pay little attention & Zhang, 2016). In addition, as Zhang (2016)
to cultivating students’ own interest in and moti- observed, EFL practitioners often have limited
vation for writing; nor do they have time for fos- knowledge of their students’ strategy repertoires,
tering different learning strategies to help them the role of individual differences influencing stu-
to learn to write (Teng & Zhang, 2016). dents’ use of writing strategies, and the ways to
The pedagogy for undergraduates favors mas- integrate task-based writing strategies into their
tery of comprehensive knowledge of English in teaching.
terms of grammar, vocabulary, and reading, while Given the essential role of writing strategies
speaking and writing courses are usually offered in influencing students’ learning outcomes (see
as electives. In turn, writing instruction in univer- Plonsky, 2011, for a meta-analysis), it is critical
sities aims to develop students’ declarative knowl- to know what strategies EFL Chinese students ac-
edge ranging from “micro-skills such as orthogra- tually do use and which strategies help improve
phy and sentence level writing to macro skills such their writing performance. Assuming that a large-
as paragraph and whole text writing” (Woodrow, scale inquiry is preferable, a reliable and valid in-
2011, p. 511). Although teachers do ask students strument for targeting specific EFL writing con-
to practice writing in order to pass the writing texts with a sound theoretical framework needs to
test as required in the national examinations such be developed.
as the CET-Band 4 or the CET-Band 6 in the
classroom, they rarely give writing assignments for A MULTIDIMENSIONAL MODEL OF L2
students to complete after class. Scholars such WRITING STRATEGIES FOR SRL
as Zhao (2010) and Teng (2016) have pointed
out that the curricula, teaching syllabuses, in- Informed by Oxford’s (2013) definition of
structional materials, and forms of evaluation are LLSs, we redefine L2 writing strategies for SRL as
prescribed by authorities such as the faculty or deliberate, goal-directed attempts to make writing
administrative committees who are in charge of enjoyable, less challenging, and more effective.
a particular specialty. This product-oriented and According to Zimmerman & Risemberg (1997),
teacher-centered approach allows for little in- self-regulation of writing refers to self-initiated
structional variety or student involvement in the thoughts, feelings, and actions that writers use
learning process and is itself a consequence of to attain various goals, including improving their
teachers’ poor training in the teaching of L2 writ- writing skills and knowledge; enhancing the qual-
ing (Lee, 2014; Zhang, 2016). ity of the text they create; and sustaining learning
8 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
effects in either learning-to-write or composing levels of academic achievement (e.g., Pintrich &
processes. While applied linguistics often makes De Groot, 1990; Winne, 2011; Zhang et al., 2008).
a functional distinction between strategies for In turn, metacognitive strategies refer to the
learning and strategies for using the language skills used to control and regulate learners’ own
(Wenden, 1991, see also A. D. Cohen, 2014), our cognition and the cognitive resources they can
study takes an inclusive and comprehensive view apply to meet the demands of particular tasks
of writing strategies for SRL in that it encompasses (Winne, 2011). Many studies have confirmed
a range of strategies used by L2 writers for devel- the significant role of metacognitive strategies
oping linguistic knowledge, for producing writ- such as goal setting, planning, monitoring, and
ten language, or for completing a task. To us, evaluating in cultivating L2 proficiency (e.g.,
both purposes play a critical role in fostering self- Zhang, 2010). According to Oxford (2013), “the
regulated learners who “can approach challeng- cognitive and metacognitive strategies facilitate
ing tasks and problems by choosing from a reper- understanding, increase meaningful mental
toire of tactics those they believe best suited to associations, and are the most useful strategies
the situation, and applying those tactics appropri- for long-term retention of information” (p. 30);
ately” (Winne & Perry, 2000, pp. 533–534). all contribute to deep processing. By including
For the purposes of this study, we follow Zim- cognitive and metacognitive strategies it should
merman’s (1989) understanding of SRL as a be possible to ascertain students’ active role in
dynamic, multidimensional process in which learning to write; in turn, having this kind of eval-
learners are “metacognitively, motivationally, and uative instrument might raise students’ awareness
behaviorally” active in regulating their own learn- of what cognitive/metacognitive strategies can
ing (p. 329). Informed by sociocognitive theory, facilitate their learning, thus contributing to
our approach to self-regulation emphasizes the better academic performance.
reciprocal determinism of the environment on
the person, mediated through behavior (Ban- Social Behavioral Strategies
dura, 1991). In writing contexts, environmental
processes refer to writers’ self-regulation of the As a key aspect of self-regulation, social–
social setting in which they write, behavioral pro- behavioral strategies involve individuals’ attempts
cesses pertain to writers’ self-regulation of overt to control their learning behavior under the in-
motoric activities associated with writing, and fluence of contextual and environmental aspects
personal processes involve writers’ self-regulation (Zimmerman, 1989, 2011). Although there is
of cognitive beliefs and affective states associated a wide range of strategies subsumed by such a
with writing (Dinsmore, Alexander, & Loughlin, construct, we only focus on two subcategories
2008). Following Zimmerman and Risemberg involved in feedback-handling and peer learning.
(1997), we hold that “each of these triadic forms SRL models emphasize the importance of feed-
of self-regulation interacts reciprocally via a cyclic back loops in which learners monitor, evaluate,
feedback loop through which writers self-monitor and adjust strategies, goals, and motivational
and self-react to feedback about the effective- factors in a given task (Zimmerman, 2013).
ness of specific self-regulatory techniques or Therefore, how learners handle others’ feedback
processes” (p. 73). Therefore, explanations for mediates the use and adjustment of other strate-
self-regulation development will need to address gies, and, in turn, affects learning outcomes.
the role of cognitive and metacognitive, social– Also, the development of SRL relies on the social
behavioral, and motivational processes (see also mediation and interactive support from teach-
Alexander, Graham, & Harris, 1998; Andrade & ers and peers, which is beneficial to learners’
Evans, 2012). active learning and enhancement of motivation
(Schunk & Rice, 1986). Self-regulated learners do
Cognitive and Metacognitive Strategies not work in isolation. When faced with a complex
task, they either seek help from others who are
Cognitive strategies refer to skills students knowledgeable such as their peers, family mem-
use to process the information or knowledge in bers, and teachers, or consult written resources
completing a task (Pintrich et al., 1991). They (Zimmerman & Risemberg, 1997).
help learners construct, transform, and apply Although previous studies have confirmed
L2 knowledge (Oxford, 2013). A plethora of the essential role of social behavior strategies
research has confirmed the essential role of (e.g., Zimmerman & Martinez–Pons, 1986), little
text processing, organization, and rehearsal in attention has been paid to the evaluation of
fostering active engagement in learning and high the construct in EFL writing through empirical
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 9
research. In this study, we propose that social influenced by “cognitive, affective, and social con-
behavior strategies, as a distinct construct of ditions” (Hayes, 1996, p. 5). Harris et al. (2011),
the SRL mechanism, interact with cognition, among other scholars, have regarded writing as
metacognition, and motivational regulation, the most complex challenge in developing lan-
which, taken together, contribute to EFL writing guage proficiency due to its “recursive, strategic,
performance. Ferris and Robert’s (2001) study and multi-dimensional” characteristics (p. 188).
has shown how important teachers’ feedback This view is echoed by scholars in L2/EFL writing
on earlier drafts of work can be in influencing (e.g., Byrnes, 2014a; Manchón, 2009; Silva &
learners’ writing processes. Peer response is also Matsuda, 2010; Zhang, 2013).
valuable in providing feedback and extending
the writer’s audience beyond just the teacher. THE STUDY
Indeed, it has been argued that developing self-
regulated learners in classroom activities requires This study aims to validate the Writing Strate-
peer interaction; an additional benefit is that it gies for Self-Regulated Learning Questionnaire
also contributes to constructing a cooperative (WSSRLQ) with regard to the proposed multi-
learning environment (Zimmerman, 2013). faceted structure of SRL strategies and the
predictive effect of the elicited SRL strategies
on EFL writing proficiency. Framed within the
Motivational Regulation Strategies
SRL theory, the WSSRLQ was designed to be
Motivational regulation strategies are defined multidimensional, measuring the constructs
as the procedure or thoughts that students of cognition, metacognition, social behavior,
apply purposefully to sustain or increase their and motivational regulation. The instrument
willingness to engage in a task (Wolters, 1999). was subjected to a series of confirmatory factor
According to Zimmerman & Risemberg (1997), analyses (CFA) through structural equation mod-
writing “is a social cognitive process wherein eling (SEM) in order to examine its factorial
writers must be aware of readers’ expectations structure. We first hypothesized that the writing
and must be willing to devote the personal time strategies for SRL would be reclassified into 9
and effort necessary to revise text drafts until they correlated factors probing the four dimensions of
communicate effectively” (p. 76). In other words, self-regulation: cognition, metacognition, social
writing achievement is contingent on the degree behavior, and motivational regulation. We then
of individuals’ motivational control to use the explored whether there would be a higher order
strategies to regulate their writing performance. construct explaining the variance of the 9 sub-
Previous studies, albeit limited, have found that strategies. For evaluating predictive validity, the
motivational regulation strategies interact with study further examined how the elicited 9 SRL
cognitive, behavioral, and contextual variables in strategies predicted EFL students’ writing pro-
SRL models (Pintrich, 2004). For that reason, mo- ficiency. Two research questions motivated the
tivational regulation strategies play a mediating study:
role in influencing students’ choice, effort, cogni-
tive engagement, and academic performance in RQ1. What structural model best represents
educational contexts, such as psychology, math, the dimensions of EFL writing strategies
science, and L1 English (e.g., Wolters & Mueller, for SRL?
2010). However, whether motivational regulation RQ2. Do the EFL writing strategies for SRL
strategies also constitute a key component of SRL predict EFL writing proficiency?
strategies in EFL writing as yet awaits empirical
exploration. Based on the tenets of SRL and In order to address them, we proposed three
existing research, for this study we propose that structural models to evaluate the dimensions of
motivational regulation strategies be included SRL strategies in EFL writing:
in the spectrum of SRL strategies so as to better Model 1: A 9-Factor Correlated Model of EFL Writ-
characterize the writing process. ing Strategies for SRL. This model specified 40 items
To sum up, on the one hand, the choice of the into nine distinct but correlated writing strategies
four dimensional strategies corresponds to the framed within SRL theory;
sociocognitive view of SRL, which acknowledges Model 2: A 4-Factor Second-Order Model of EFL Writ-
the regulation of behavior, person, and environ- ing Strategies for SRL. Grounded in SRL theory, we
ment. On the other hand, this model is in line proposed Model 2 to examine whether the nine
with the interpretation of writing from cognitive SRL strategies exhibited a hierarchical structure.
theory, which postulates that writing behavior is We hypothesized that the 9 SRL strategies were
10 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
conceptualized into four second-order correlated Given the wide range of SRL strategies in the
factors, including cognition, metacognition, proposed four dimensions, the EFL writing strate-
social behavior, and motivational regulation. gies in our questionnaire were selective and syn-
Model 3: A One-Factor Second-Order Model of EFL thetic, rather than fully inclusive based on the
Writing Strategies for SRL. As a competing hierar- following criteria: (a) EFL students in the focus
chy model we postulated that a single higher or- group interviews mentioned that they had used
der common factor, self-regulation, might be suf- these writing strategies in completing a writing
ficient to account for the correlations of the 9 task inside and outside the classroom, (b) the
lower order strategies. writing strategies reflected any of the four dimen-
sions of cognition, metacognition, social behav-
ior, and motivational regulation, and (c) the writ-
Participants ing strategies were essential to promoting SRL, as
supported by the research literature.
A total number of 790 undergraduate students
A list of 45 items pertaining to EFL writing
were recruited from six universities in Northeast
strategies was generated. Three experts in the
China. This was a convenience sample, with par-
field of LLSs and SRL were invited to examine
ticipants selected from the first year and the sec-
the initial list. Specifically, they scrutinized the
ond year (first year N = 427, 54%; second year N =
theoretical rationale, checked the questions for
363, 46%). All participants were volunteers from
the constructs being measured, and rated the de-
six majors: Economics (15%, N = 119), Civil En-
gree to which the survey questions matched the
gineering (16%, N = 126), Visual Arts (17%, N =
constructs as defined in the study. This procedure
134), Psychology (14%, N = 111), English (25%,
resulted in the elimination of items that had re-
N = 197), and Law (13%, N = 103). They were
ceived the lowest rating. The revised list was then
aged between 18 and 22 (M = 20.04, SD = 1.21);
given to 15 EFL students who checked the items
40% were male (N = 316), 60% female. They re-
for clarity and readability. As a result, irrelevant
ported an average of 9 years of formal English lan-
and double-barreled items were eliminated and
guage learning (M = 9.12, SD = 1.04).
related statements were combined. The final in-
strument containing 40 items was sequenced log-
Questionnaire Development ically and organized according to the clusters of
subcategories. In line with Pintrich et al. (1991)
The WSSRLQ was developed through a three- and Tseng et al. (2006), a 7-point Likert scale with
phase process: item generating, initial piloting, gradation rating from 1 (not at all true of me) to
and psychometric evaluation. The process of item 7 (very true of me) was adopted to explore the
generation for the WSSRLQ began with focus trait features of SRL strategies; the mean scores
group interviews. As Dörnyei (2010) argued, in- of these items were made cumulative.
volving targeted learners in the item-generating Given the level of English language proficiency
process adds to the credibility and quality of the of these participants, the English questionnaire
items used in the questionnaire. To this end, 10 was translated into Chinese by the first author and
undergraduate students were assigned to each fo- then verified by the second author, both of whom
cus group, with each group showing diversity in are L1 Chinese speakers. Back translation was ap-
terms of gender, year level, and disciplinary major. plied to ensure parallelism of bilingual versions
During the focus group interviews, participants of the instrument. The questionnaire was then
were invited to describe what strategies they used subjected to more statistical scrutiny to determine
while performing a writing task or learning writ- construct validity using CFA.
ing knowledge inside and outside the classroom.
The analysis of the focus group data helped gen- Writing Test
erate the initial items (see Appendix A for a list of
guided interview questions). In order to evaluate students’ writing profi-
We also consulted the relevant literature and ciency, we adopted a given-topic argumentative
critically examined established instruments for writing test selected from the International En-
evaluating SRL strategies (e.g., Pintrich et al., glish Language Testing System (IELTS) Task 2,
1991; Wolters, 1999; Zimmerman & Martinez– which is one of the most widely used English-as-
Pons, 1986). As argued by many researchers (e.g., a-second-language (ESL) tests around the world.
Dörnyei, 2010; Petrić & Czárl, 2003), such an The writing task requires an extended compo-
item-generation procedure lends construct valid- sition in response to a proposition or question.
ity to a questionnaire. The argumentative writing test has been proven
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 11
to be effective in evaluating students’ academic Therefore, we enlisted other fit indices that in-
achievements in light of linguistic compe- cluded the Goodness of Fit Index (GFI), the Root
tence, critical thinking, and articulation of ideas Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA)
(Varghese & Abraham, 1998). The production with its corresponding 90% confidence interval,
of an argumentative essay requires students’ the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual
active use of a range of strategies such as using (SRMR), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and
knowledge to generate ideas, monitoring and the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI). The GFI is a
evaluating their progress, revising the written es- commonly reported fit index to measure the hy-
say, or actively regulating their emotion to sustain pothesized model and the observed covariance,
their learning efforts throughout the composing with values of over .90 generally indicating an ac-
process. Active use of these writing strategies in ceptable model fit. The RMSEA is a critical value
the composing process reflects students’ proac- in this study because it assesses the model fit while
tive engagement with a writing task (Zimmerman taking into the account the complexity of the
& Risemberg, 1997). The topics or contexts of model structure. A value of .05 or less is indicative
language in the IELTS are designed to avoid bias of good model fit. Unlike the chi-square statistic,
against any group of candidates of a particular both the SRMR and the CFI are not sensitive to
background. sample size. Values for the SRMR less than .08
In our study, all participants were required to and values of CFI larger than .90 are generally
produce a written argument on a given topic indicative of acceptable model fit. TLI analyzes
with at least 250 words within an hour in class- the discrepancy between the chi-squared value
room environments. Then their writing scripts of the hypothesized model and the chi-squared
were marked based on the standard IELTS Task value of the null model. It is not sensitive to
2 analytic scales, which focus on task response, sample size but depends on the average size of
coherence and cohesion, lexical resource, and the correlations in the data. The recommended
grammatical range and accuracy. Two evaluators values of TLI are equal to or more than .90
who were certified IELTS examiners were invited indicating an acceptable level of model fit (see
to evaluate these essays. The interrater reliabil- Hu & Bentler, 1999, for more information).
ity and intrarater reliability were at .88 and .92, In this study, chi-square statistics were also used
respectively. to help us select the appropriate structural model
when comparing the hypothesized nested mod-
Data Analysis els. The difference in chi-square as a ratio of the
difference in df was examined with the significant
The data were subjected to CFAs using SEM p-value indicating that the reference model is a
through the IBM SPSS AMOS computer program, better fit for the data (Kline, 2011).
Version 22 (Arbuckle, 2013). CFA is a useful sta- To evaluate the predictive validity of the instru-
tistical measure to test a theoretical model and a ment, a simultaneous multiple regression analysis
tighter specification of multiple hierarchies by uti- was used to investigate how the 9 SRL strategies
lizing the factor, correlation, and covariance pat- predicted EFL learners’ writing performance. As-
terns, and residual or error values within a data sumptions of multiple regressions were first exam-
matrix (Kline, 2011). In this study, the maximum ined. Given that the variables (9 SRL strategies) in
likelihood (ML) estimation method was applied this set of analysis were correlated, a Bonferroni
to evaluate the three hypothesized models. While adjustment was adopted to set the alpha level for
there are no golden rules for assessing model fit, the overall test of each regression to be a more
we interpreted the CFA data based on the sev- conservative value.
eral omnibus fit statistics to test whether a spe-
cific model of interest adequately fits the data be- Procedures
cause different indices reflect different aspects of
model fit. As suggested by Kline (2011), we first The questionnaire was given to participants af-
reported a chi-square statistic, along with its de- ter a writing course to elicit authentic context-
grees of freedom (df) and associated p value. The based strategies. Instructions were reviewed and
ratio of chi-square x 2 divided by the df < .30 indi- clarified first, and any doubts and comments were
cates the best fit with the nonsignificant p value. recorded and addressed. Participants spent on av-
However, due to the sample-size dependency of erage 10–15 minutes completing the Chinese ver-
the chi-square test statistic, significant values of sion of the WSSRLQ. On the following day, the
chi-square are usually found when large sample students were given an in-class writing test, which
sizes are involved in CFA. was used to elicit their writing proficiency.
12 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
Data collected from the WSSRLQ were variables was not able to affect the proposed
screened and cleaned first. Missing responses, paths’ significance.
normality, and homogeneity for multivariate anal-
yses were examined thoroughly. Data collected
from the completed WSSRLQ were subjected Evaluating A Nine-Factor Correlated Model of EFL
to a series of CFAs to evaluate the hypothesized Writing Strategies for SRL
dimensions of EFL writing strategies for SRL.
Based on the theoretical design, we first tested
They were then correlated with the participants’
a 9-Factor Correlated Model (Model 1) as we had
writing scores in a regression model to check how
hypothesized (see Figure 1).
the elicited SRL strategies affected students’ EFL
This model specified 40 items into 9 distinct
writing proficiency.
but correlated writing strategies framed within
SRL theory. In Figure 1, rectangles represent
observed variables (items in a questionnaire)
RESULTS and ovals indicate unobserved variables (latent
variable/factor). A one-headed arrow indicates a
Descriptive Statistics and Normality Check hypothesized one-way direction, whereas a two-
headed arrow indicates a correlation between
Descriptive statistical analyses showed that the
two variables. In this model, the 9 latent factors’
mean scores of the 40 items ranged from 3.13 to
loadings were each fixed to be one and did not
5.72 with standard deviations ranging from .94 to
have to be estimated. The 40 observed variables
1.73. The values for skewness were between −1.23
had measurement errors. Each indicator was
and .35 and the values for kurtosis were between
constrained to load only on the factor it was
−1.03 and 2.17. They were far less than the cutoff
designed to measure. Factor covariances were
values of ± 3.0 and ± 8.0 for skewness and kur-
free to be estimated and error terms associated
tosis, respectively, indicating the univariate nor-
with each indicator were uncorrelated. Each item
mality of the response (Kline, 2011). Appendix B
pair measure had a nonzero loading on a specific
shows the means, standard deviations, skewness,
writing strategy that the questionnaire was de-
and kurtosis of the 40–item questionnaire.
signed to measure, and a zero loading on all other
Initially, four cases with systematic response
factors.
bias (e.g., same response for the entire question-
Results of a CFA revealed an acceptable model
naire) were eliminated. In addition, six cases
fit (x 2 780 = 1676; df = 743; p < .001; x 2 /df =
with missing values were removed without impu-
2.25; GFI = .91; TLI = .91; CFI = .92; RMSEA
tation because the total proportion of missing
= .045 [.042, .049], SRMR = .056), with 40 items
values was far less than the cutoff value of 5%
loading on 9 correlated factors as designed. Fig-
(Enders, 2010). Thus, a final sample size of
ure 2 shows the standardized results for the 9-
780 participants for a 40-item scale met the de-
factor correlated model. The parameter estimates
sired cases-to-variables ratio (5:1) analysis (Field,
presented here are all standardized as this facili-
2009). The assumptions of linearity, singularity,
tates the interpretation of parameters along with
and homogeneity of the sample were satisfied
standard errors. Standardized estimates loadings
and no outlying cases were detected.
(factor loading) from the factors to the observed
Multivariate normality was examined using
variables are higher than the benchmark value
Mardia’s normalized multivariate kurtosis value
.50, thereby suggesting an acceptable effect size
(equivalent to a z score). Normalized coefficients
(Raykov & Marcoulides, 2008). In this model, all
greater than 3.00 are indicative of nonnormality
40-item parameter estimates were statistically sig-
(Field, 2009). In the AMOS software program, the
nificant at p < .001.
multivariate kurtosis value of 355.42 represented
Mardia’s coefficient of multivariate kurtosis, the Inter-Correlations of the 9 EFL Writing Strategies for
critical ratio of which was 33.72, so these data SRL. The CFA results also confirmed the dis-
were multivariate nonnormal. In order to test criminant validity of the 9 EFL writing strategies as
whether nonnormality inflated the significance evidenced by the small to moderately strong cor-
of the regression paths, Bootstrap ML estimates relations of the 9 factors. As depicted in Table 1,
were performed to provide bias-corrected con- inter-correlation coefficients ranged from r =
fidence intervals for each bootstrap estimate. .12 between peer learning (PL) and emotional
Findings showed that all of the significant paths control (EC) to r = .56 between goal-oriented
kept the same significance as the original re- monitoring and evaluating (GME) and peer
sults, indicating that the nonnormality of these learning (PL). All 9 factors were significantly
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 13
FIGURE 1
9-Factor Correlated Model of EFL Writing Strategies for SRL (N = 780).

Note. GME = Goal-Oriented Monitoring and Evaluating; IP = Idea Planning; PL = Peer Learning; FH = Feedback
Handling; IE = Interest Enhancement; EC = Emotional Control; MST = Motivational Self-Talk; TP = Text Processing;
Course Memory = CM.

correlated with each other at p < .01. Our re- had a small correlation with course memory
sults revealed that these 9 factors of EFL writing (CM, r = .29) but were moderately correlated
strategies for SRL were clearly correlated but also with idea planning (IP, r = .35) and goal-oriented
distinct constructs. monitoring and evaluating (GME, r = .38). In the
As shown in Table 1, goal-oriented monitoring motivational regulation dimension, motivational
and evaluating strategies (GME) of the metacog- self-talk (MST) had medium correlations with
nitive component were strongly correlated with 7 SRL strategies and a small correlation with
interest enhancement (IE, r = .52) and peer peer learning. All these correlation coefficients
learning (PL, r = .56). As regards the cognitive indicated that cross-loadings of the 9 factors were
dimension, text processing strategies (TP) only likely to exist.
14 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
FIGURE 2
A 9-Factor Correlated Model of EFL Writing Strategies for SRL With Standardized Regression Weight
(N = 780).

Note. All 40-item parameter estimates were statistically significant at p < .001. GME = Goal-Oriented Monitoring and
Evaluating; IP = Idea Planning; PL = Peer Learning; FH = Feedback Handling; IE = Interest Enhancement; EC =
Emotional Control; MST = Motivational Self-Talk; TP = Text Processing; Course Memory = CM.
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 15
TABLE 1
Inter-Correlations for the 9-Factor Correlated Model of EFL Writing Strategies for SRL

Dimensions Strategies TP CM IP GME PL FH IE MST EC

Cognition TP 1
CM .29** 1
Metacognition IP .35** .25** 1
GME .38** .44** .39** 1
Social Behavior PL .25** .27** .25** .56** 1
FH .20** .17** .22** .13** .33** 1
Motivational Regulation IE .35** .21** .32** .52** .25** .24** 1
MST .31** .44** .31** .41** .21** .31** .43** 1
EC .23** .26** .26** .19** .12** .37** .33** .49** 1

Note. TP = Text Processing; CM = Course Memory; IP = Idea Planning; GME = Goal-Oriented Monitoring and
Evaluating; PL = Peer Learning; FH = Feedback Handling; IE = Interest Enhancement; MST = Motivational Self-
Talk; EC = Emotional Control; ** = All correlations are significant at p < .01.

TABLE 2 9 SRL strategies conceptualized into 4 higher


Means, Standard Deviations, and Internal order correlated factors, including cognition,
Reliabilities of the 9 EFL Writing Strategies for SRL metacognition, social behavior, and motivational
(N = 780) regulation. Figure 3 shows the 4-Factor Second-
Order Model of EFL Writing Strategies for SRL
Dimensions Writing Strategies M SD α (Model 2).
Cognition TP (6 items) 4.67 1.50 .80 Given the cross-loading of the 9 SRL strategies,
CM (3 items) 4.47 1.12 .76 we further proposed a One-Factor Second-Order
Metacognition IP (3 items) 4.61 0.91 .73 Model (Model 3), which postulated that a single
GME (6 items) 3.76 1.26 .86 common factor, self-regulation, as a higher order,
Social behavior PL (3 items) 3.60 1.11 .80 was sufficient to account for the correlations of
FH (4 items) 5.61 1.17 .79 the 9 lower order strategies (see Figure 4).
Motivational IE (4 items) 4.61 1.02 .84 In both Model 2 and Model 3, each indicator
regulation MST (8 items) 4.97 1.43 .87 was constrained to load only on the factor it was
EC (3 items) 5.17 1.33 .75
designed to measure, factor covariances were free
Note. TP = Text Processing; CM = Course Memory; IP = to be estimated, and error terms associated with
Idea Planning; GME = Goal-Oriented Monitoring and each indicator were uncorrelated. Table 3 shows
Evaluating; PL = Peer Learning; FH = Feedback Han- the fit indices of the competing models.
dling; IE = Interest Enhancement; MST = Motivational Model comparisons showed that both Model 2
Self-Talk; EC = Emotional Control. and Model 3 had acceptable model fit indices. We
conducted further comparisons between Model 1
and Model 2 (x M1 2
− x M2
2
= 18; d f M1 − d f M2
Internal Reliability. Scale reliability tests (Cron- = 13; p = .16). There was no significant im-
bach’s alpha) were computed for each of the provement between the two models. However,
9 SRL scales. Table 2 shows that Cronbach’s al- the indices of Model 3 (one high order factor
pha coefficient of the 9 SRL strategies was much model) improved significantly in fit over Model 2
higher than the benchmark value .70, suggesting
2
(x M2 − x M3
2
= 14; d f M2 − d f M3 = 4; p = .007)
a robust internal reliability of each scale. Table 2 and Model 1 (x M1 2
− x M3
2
= 32; d f M1 − d f M3 =
presents the means, standard deviations, and 17; p = .015). The significant difference between
internal reliabilities of the 9 factors. chi-square values suggests that Model 3 was signif-
icantly better than the other two competing mod-
Model Comparisons els. On this basis, we retained Model 3 with self-
regulation as a hierarchical construct explaining
In order to evaluate the hierarchical structure the 9 SRL strategies as the model of best fit in this
underlying the 9 writing strategies, we proposed study. For this model, all item parameter estimates
two high-order models based on the theoretical were statistically significant at p < .001. The 9 SRL
framework of SRL. First, we postulated that the strategies had loaded on the hypothesized high
16 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
FIGURE 3
A 4-Factor Second-Order Model of EFL Writing Strategies for SRL

Note. GME = Goal-Oriented Monitoring and Evaluating; IP = Idea Planning; PL = Peer Learning; FH = Feedback
Handling; IE = Interest Enhancement; MST = Motivational Self-Talk; EC = Emotional Control; TP = Text Processing;
Course Memory = CM.

order latent construct with standardized estimates As shown in Figure 5, idea planning (IP), goal-
over the recommended value of .50 (Raykov & oriented monitoring and evaluating (GME), mo-
Marcoulides, 2008). The structure coefficients of tivational self-talk (MST), and interest enhance-
the 9 subcategories ranged between .53 and .79 ment (IE) had large loadings on self-regulation
on the construct of self-regulation, supporting compared with other writing strategies, revealing
convergent validity (Kline, 2011). Figure 5 shows the essential role of metacognition and motiva-
the standardized regression weight of Model 3. tional regulation in the SRL process.
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 17
FIGURE 4
A 1-Factor Second-Order Model of EFL Writing Strategies for SRL

Note. GME = Goal-Oriented Monitoring and Evaluating; IP = Idea Planning; PL = Peer Learning; FH = Feedback
Handling; CM = Course Memory; IE = Interest Enhancement; EC = Emotional Control; TP = Text Processing; MST
= Motivational Self-Talk.

Predictive Effect of SRL Strategies on Writing 9 SRL strategies as a whole, explained approxi-
Proficiency mately 37% of the variance in students’ writing
A simultaneous multiple regression analysis was scores, F(9,745) = 45.251, p < .001, R2 = .37,
conducted, in which the 9 SRL strategies were en- adjusted R2 = .36. The effect size (R2 = .37)
tered as a group in one step. Adjusted Bonfer- indicates that the 9 SRL strategies as a whole
roni value was at .006. Results showed that the were a strong factor predicting students’ writing
18 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
TABLE 3
Goodness-of-Fit Indices for Competing Models

Model x2 df x 2 /df CFI TLI GFI RMSEA RMSEA 90% CI SRMR


*
Model 1 1676 743 2.25 .92 .91 .91 .045 .042–.049 .056
Model 2 1658* 730 2.27 .91 .90 .91 .047 .043–.051 .055
Model 3 1644* 726 2.26 .93 .92 .92 .044 .039–.048 .054

Note. df = degree of freedom; * = p < .001; CFI = Comparative Fit Index; TLI = Tucker–Lewis Index; GFI = Goodness-
of-Fit Index; RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; RMSEA 90% CI = RMSEA 90% confidence
interval, SRMR = Standardized Root Mean Square Residual.

performance (strong effect size = R2 >. 35; sociocognitive view of the SRL process, requiring
Cohen, 1992). Table 4 presents the standardized the deployment of a range of writing strategies
regression coefficient (β), t-statistic, p-value, and in relation to cognition, metacognition, social
confidence interval (95% CI). behavior, and motivational regulation (Zimmer-
As shown in Table 4, individual predictors of man, 2013). In addition, these four dimensions
text processing (TP), idea planning (IP), goal- are not completely distinct but interact with one
oriented monitoring and evaluating (GME), feed- another in the SRL process. As Bandura (1986)
back handling (FH), motivational self-talk (MST), explained, human function of self-regulation is a
and emotional control (EC) yielded significant cyclical, triadic, and reciprocal process, in which
predictions for students’ writing proficiency. In- “behavior, cognitive and other personal factors,
terest enhancement (IE), course memory (CM), and environmental events all operate as interact-
and peer learning (PL) were not identified as ing determinants of each other” (p. 18). That
significant predictors of students’ writing profi- might help explain the cross-loading of the 9 SRL
ciency. strategies across the four dimensions. Thus, scores
of the 9 subcategories can be calculated separately
DISCUSSION to reveal the level of students’ perceived use of
each of the writing strategies. The averaged sums
The overarching aim of this study was to vali- of the 9 substrategies collectively represent stu-
date a self-report instrument, the WSSRLQ, for dents’ overall level of self-regulation capacity.
evaluating the multifaceted structure of writing
strategies used by EFL students for SRL. The Multiple Dimensions of EFL Writing Strategies for SRL
40 items in the questionnaire were measured
using a 7-point scale, ranging from 1 (not at all In this study, the 9 factors confirmed by CFA
true of me) to 7 (very true of me) to probe into were conceptually interpreted with reference to
the trait features of self-regulation. The results the four core paradigms of SRL, namely cogni-
of CFAs provided substantial evidence for the tion, metacognition, social behavior, and motiva-
factorial structure of the instrument, entailing tional regulation.
motivational self-talk, interest enhancement, The cognitive dimension of writing strategies
emotional control, goal-oriented monitoring and for SRL comprised text processing and course
evaluating, idea planning, text processing, course memory. Text processing with six items reflected
memory, feedback handling, and peer learning. students’ use of linguistic, rhetorical, and dis-
In general, not only did the findings support the course knowledge to generate a written text (e.g.,
utility of the WSSRLQ as a measure of EFL writing When writing, I check the structure for logical coher-
strategies for SRL with satisfactory psychometric ence). Course memory with three items referred
properties, but they also revealed that the 9 SRL to students’ actively remembering writing knowl-
strategies were reliably distinguished on both edge taught in the writing course (e.g., I write use-
conceptual and empirical grounds. ful words and expressions taught in writing courses to
Model comparisons revealed that the one- help me remember them).
factor second order model (Model 3) had the The metacognitive dimension included idea
best model fit over the other two competing mod- planning and goal-oriented monitoring and
els (Model 1 and Model 2). This means that self- evaluating, reflecting the triadic aspects of the
regulation, as an integrated construct, is sufficient metacognitive regulation (Pintrich et al., 2000).
to account for the correlations of the lower order Idea planning with three items referred to spe-
writing strategies. Our results lend support to the cific idea-generating behavior before writing
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 19
FIGURE 5
One-Factor Second-Order Factor Model of EFL Writing Strategies for SRL (N = 780)

Note. All parameter estimates were statistically significant at p < .001. GME = Goal-Oriented Monitoring and Eval-
uating; IP = Idea Planning; PL = Peer Learning; FH = Feedback Handling; CM = Course Memory; IE = Interest
Enhancement; EC = Emotional Control; TP = Text Processing; MST = Motivational Self-Talk.
20 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
TABLE 4
Simultaneous Multiple Regression Model of the 9 SRL Strategies on EFL Writing Scores (N = 745)

95% CI

SRL Dimensions Predictor β t p LL UL

Cognition CM .12 .78 .67 −1.74 2.65


TP .31 3.64 < .001 −.23 5.27
Metacognition IP .21 2.35 .004 −1.88 2.28
GME .25 2.98 .001 −5.96 .38
Social Behavior PL .11 −.71 .472 −3.10 1.45
FH .17 1.75 .003 −1.43 4.09
Motivational Regulation IE .12 .71 .223 −.92 3.86
MST .24 2.87 < .001 −1.91 5.12
EC −.16 −1.99 .002 −5.93 .16

Note. CM = Course Memory; TP = Text Processing; IP = Idea Planning; GME = Goal-Oriented Monitoring and Eval-
uating; PL = Peer Learning; FH = Feedback Handling; IE = Interest Enhancement; MST = Motivational Self-Talk;
EC = Emotional Control. The dependent variable was writing score. R2 = .37, Adjusted R2 = .36; CI = confidence
interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit.

(e.g., Before writing, I use the Internet to search for trol with three items measured learners’ efforts to
related information to help me plan). Goal-oriented reduce distraction when completing a writing task
monitoring and evaluating with six items in- or learning to write (e.g., I find ways to regulate my
cluded an arsenal of strategies such as setting up mood when I want to give up writing).
goals to direct writing activities (e.g., When learn-
ing to write, I set up goals for myself in order to direct my
learning activities) or monitoring and evaluating Correlations of the 9 EFL Writing Strategies for SRL
the mastery of knowledge and performance in Results of the inter-correlation coefficient pro-
writing courses (e.g., I monitor my learning process in vided a clear picture of how the 9 EFL writing
writing courses; I evaluate the mastery of the knowledge strategies correlated with each other in the SRL
or skills learned in writing courses). process. The significant and moderate correla-
Feedback handling and peer learning were in- tions indicate that the 9 SRL strategies are dis-
terpreted together to reflect how learners uti- tinct but interrelated and interwoven during the
lized social–behavioral factors to promote their learning-to-write process.
self-regulatory capability. Feedback handling with Goal-oriented monitoring and evaluating
four items embraced students’ attitude toward strategies were strongly correlated with peer
teacher and peer feedback (e.g., I try to improve learning of the social dimension and interest
my English writing based on teachers’ feedback). Peer enhancement of the motivational regulation
learning with three items involved the procure- dimension. The significant correlations reflect
ment of help from peers in the learning environ- the essential role of metacognition in the SRL
ment, and as such it is also a social interaction process (Winne & Hadwin, 2010). This means
(e.g., I discuss with my peers to have more ideas to that students who have awareness about realiz-
write). ing and monitoring their task goals might also
The motivational regulation dimension in- exert effort to regulate their social behavior and
cluded three types of writing strategies: motiva- intrinsic motivation to maintain or increase their
tional self-talk, interest enhancement, and emo- engagement with tasks.
tional control. Motivational self-talk with eight Also notable are the moderate correlations of
items included the self-encouragement in knowl- motivational regulation strategies, particularly
edge mastery (e.g., I persuade myself to work hard in motivational self-talk and interest enhancement
writing courses to improve my writing skills and knowl- with the other dimensions of SRL strategies.
edge) and academic performance (e.g., I remind This means that EFL students who are active in
myself about how important it is to get good grades in regulating their motivation tend to deploy a rich
writing courses). Interest enhancement with four repertoire of cognitive, metacognitive, and social
items reflected students’ tendency to make learn- strategies. Our findings are consistent with some
ing more enjoyable (e.g., I look for ways to bring previous studies, which reported small to medium
more fun to the learning of writing). Emotional con- correlations of motivational regulation strategies
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 21
with cognitive and metacognitive strategies in L1 Results suggest that EFL learners who relied on
contexts (Cooper & Corpus, 2009; Wolters, 1999). deeper processing strategies were more likely to
As Wolters (1999) explained, motivational regu- perform better in writing tests. As Harris et al.
lation strategies are helpful to “increase students’ (2011) explained, metacognitive strategies con-
level of cognitive engagement, overall level of tributed to learners’ cognitive maturity, which in
effort, and subsequent achievement within an aca- turn had a positive effect on their academic per-
demic setting” (p. 285). The positive correlations formance. Our study, along with the previous find-
between motivational regulation strategies and ings, corroborates the arguments for the essential
social behavior strategies (e.g., peer learning and role of metacognitive strategies in optimizing writ-
feedback handling) lend support to the argument ing performance (e.g., Pintrich et al, 2000; Zhang
that “all aspects of self-regulation, including moti- & Zhang, 2013).
vational regulation, are developed through social Of the motivational regulation strategies, both
and cultural interaction/influences” (Wolters emotional control and motivational self-talk were
& Mueller, 2010, p. 633). Our study theoreti- significant predictors of students’ writing profi-
cally supports an increasingly strong argument ciency. As discussed earlier, the statements on
for viewing motivational regulation as a promi- emotional control strategies in the questionnaire
nent facet for developing self-regulated learners were directly related to how students actively reg-
(Zimmerman, 2008). ulated their negative emotions such as anxiety or
worries in task-based environments (e.g., taking a
Predictive Effect of SRL Strategies on Writing test). Our findings revealed that positive emotions
Proficiency about the learning situation encouraged students
to become more committed to the task, thus con-
The 9 writing strategies for SRL as a whole pro- tributing to better writing outcomes. This corrob-
duced a large effect on students’ EFL writing pro- orates Boekaerts’s (2011) argument that emotion
ficiency, supporting the validity of the one high control in the service of one’s goal is “a promi-
order model, in which self-regulation as an inte- nent capacity that is key to having success in all
grated construct affects students’ learning perfor- areas of life” (p. 409). In addition, the signifi-
mance. Although only six types of writing strate- cant prediction of motivational self-talk on writing
gies for SRL had significant predictive effects on scores indicates that students who used intrinsic
writing proficiency, our findings provide some and extrinsic reasons (performance and mastery
preliminary support for the claim that L2 writing self-talk) to motivate them to learn and/or sus-
achievement is contingent upon the use of dif- tain their learning efforts tended to perform bet-
ferent dimensions of SRL strategies, which play ter in writing tests. Our finding mirrors some pre-
an essential role in mobilizing, directing, and sus- vious studies conducted in other contexts such as
taining learning efforts, therefore affecting stu- in the United States (Wolters, 1999) and Germany
dents’ academic performance (Manchón et al., (Schwinger, Steinmayr, & Spinath, 2009). For ex-
2007). ample, Wolters (1999) found that performance
Within the cognitive dimension, only text pro- self-talk produced a weak significant prediction
cessing strategies significantly predicted writing on students’ grade-point average. Schwinger et al.
scores with a large effect size. This suggests that (2009) reported that mastery self-talk had an
learners’ use of linguistic and writing knowledge indirect effect on examination performance in
is a critical factor affecting their writing perfor- German contexts. Although no consistent results
mance, as revealed in many other studies (e.g., have been found, these empirical studies together
Winne, 2011; Zhang et al., 2008). The result also provide evidence in support of using motiva-
supports studies that have argued for the essen- tional self-talk strategies for improving learners’
tial role of cognitive processes in fostering active task performance. However, interest enhance-
engagement and enhancing students’ writing out- ment did not generate any significant effects on
comes (e.g., Flower & Hayes, 1981; Ong & Zhang, writing proficiency. This might be due to the test-
2013). Course memory, however, was not a signif- like environment, in which EFL students did not
icant predictor of writing scores. This means that have time to use interest enhancement strategies
remembering learning certain materials or course to facilitate their task performance.
knowledge, as kinds of surface strategies, did not Of the social behavior strategies, peer learn-
have a direct effect on individuals’ writing scores. ing did not generate any predictive effect on writ-
Of the metacognitive strategies, both idea plan- ing performance. Similarly, Pintrich et al.’s (1991)
ning and goal-oriented monitoring and evaluat- study found that peer learning and help-seeking
ing were significant predictors of writing scores. were not significantly related to course grades.
22 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
However, feedback handling yielded a significant, the social cognitive view of SRL, which emphasizes
positive effect on students’ writing scores. As Zim- the proactive engagement of SRL processes under
merman and Schunk (2011) have asserted, within the triadic interplay of individuals, behaviors, and
the triadic cyclical model of SRL, feedback aids environments (Zimmerman, 2013). The findings
individuals’ monitoring and self-evaluation in the collectively reveal how human behavior and the
SRL process, contributing to positive academic regulation of cognition and motivation as inter-
outcomes. acting determinants of each other influence stu-
Taken together, our findings confirm the direct dents’ academic performance (Bandura, 1991).
effect of some SRL strategies in promoting EFL Practically, the newly developed questionnaire
students’ writing performance. The results lend (WSSRLQ) might be applied as a self-evaluation
support to some earlier studies on LLSs, for tool for students to appraise the degree of aware-
positive predictive relationships between writ- ness of SRL writing strategies and cultivate a habit
ing strategies and language learning outcomes of using them in developing writing skills in EFL
in general or in specific writing settings (see contexts. Although it does not provide an exhaus-
Manchón et al., 2007; Plonsky, 2011, for more tive illustration of writing strategies across the four
information). A less consistent but perhaps more dimensions, the evaluating process may neverthe-
interesting pattern was the varying predictions less give learners a sense of the utility of the nine
of motivational regulation strategies and social elicited SRL strategies from cognitive, metacog-
behavior strategies on students’ writing scores. In nitive, social–behavioral, and motivational regula-
line with Pintrich’s (2004) study, our data lend tion aspects. Students may further adjust or try to
preliminary support to the view that individuals’ deploy other SRL strategies to achieve their learn-
learning achievement is directly influenced by ing goals.
their self-regulation of cognition, motivation, and In addition, the instrument might be useful in
behavior, which mediates the relations among the classrooms as a pedagogical tool for evaluating
person, the environment, and the achievement. students’ preferences for using different dimen-
The salient role of SRL strategies also reveals sions of SRL strategies when teachers are inter-
that in promoting active and efficient learning, ested in engaging their students with strategies-
students need strategies-based instruction from based writing instruction for effective learning in
a multidimensional perspective when they learn authentic contexts. As Wolters and Benzon (2013)
how to write (see e.g., Zhang et al., 2016). As Zim- posited, “knowing what strategies are preferred or
merman and Bandura (1994) argued, “students used most often by students within more authen-
needed to be taught skills and strategies for man- tic academic contexts provides insight into which
aging not only the cognitive aspects of managing ones might best be used as the target of instruc-
learning but also methods in which to motivate tional interventions” (p. 201).
themselves for academic pursuits in the face of The significant predictive power of some writ-
difficulties or attractive alternatives” (p. 857). ing strategies suggests that teachers might want to
provide guidance on which strategies should be
CONCLUSION AND IMPLICATIONS taught to the targeted EFL students in classroom
environments. For example, if some motivation
This article has reported the validation of a regulation strategies continue to be linked posi-
self-report questionnaire, the WSSRLQ, to evalu- tively to students’ academic outcomes, then one
ate the perceived use of writing strategies for SRL basic implication is that these strategies should be
in EFL learning environments. The CFA results taught directly to students.
confirmed that the nine EFL writing strategies for
SRL represented reliably distinguishable but cor- LIMITATIONS
related aspects under an overarching construct of
self-regulation. The moderate correlations of the Like many studies, our study is not exempt from
nine writing strategies across the four conceptual limitations. First, the single method of strategy
dimensions reveal that during the self-regulating evaluation through self-report data might fail to
process EFL students’ use of writing strategies provide rich and accurate information of what
relating to cognition, metacognition, social be- writing strategies learners use in reality, as argued
havior, and motivational regulation is interwoven. by some researchers (Cohen & Macaro, 2007; Ox-
Theoretically, our results render preliminary ford et al., 2014). For example, participants may
evidence for transferring educational psychology have forgotten some strategies they have used in
theory to the field of L2/EFL education, particu- the past or they may have reported using some
larly EFL writing. The study lends some support to strategies that they have never used before, or they
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 23
may have misunderstood the items in the ques- Andrade, M. S., & Evans, N. W. (2012). Principles
tionnaire. Therefore, we recommend that multi- and practices for response in second language writing:
methods for data collection (e.g., stimulated re- Developing self-regulated learners. New York: Rout-
call after completing a task, reflection journals, ledge/Taylor & Francis.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2013). IBM SPSS Amos 22 user’s guide.
among others) be used in future studies to offer
Crawfordville, FL: Amos Development Corpora-
more comprehensive evaluations of SRL strate-
tion.
gies. However, we should be aware of the inherent Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and ac-
shortcomings of all these self-report measures, as tion: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
they can only access part of the writing process, of Prentice–Hall.
which learners are consciously aware. Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-
Second, we recruited only university students in regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human
China. This prevents generalization of our find- Decision Processes, 50, 248–287.
ings to other populations, such as younger stu- Berg, E. C. (1999). The effects of trained peer response
dents from schools. Further studies are needed on ESL students’ revision types and writing quality.
Journal of Second Language Writing, 8, 215–241.
for expanding the sampling methods and partic-
Boekaerts, M. (2011). Emotions, emotion regulation,
ipant pool. Students of different age groups or
and self-regulation of learning. In B. J. Zimmer-
other ethnicities should be included. man & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-
Third, although this study investigated stu- regulation of learning and performance (pp. 408–425).
dents’ reported use of SRL strategies from four New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.
dimensions, our instrument solicited only nine Byrnes, H. (2014a). Theorizing language development
specific strategies, without providing all possible at the intersection of ‘task’ and L2 writing: Re-
strategies used in the learning-to-write or com- considering complexity. In H. Byrnes & R. M.
posing processes in L2 settings. More studies are Manchón (Eds.), Task-based language learning: In-
needed to specifically tap into other essential as- sights from and for L2 writing (pp. 79–103). Philadel-
phia/Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
pects of SRL (e.g., self-efficacy and environmental
Byrnes, H. (2014b). Linking task and writing for
control).
language development: Evidence from a genre-
Fourth, this study did not distinguish writing based curricular approach. In H. Byrnes & R.
strategies for using and strategies for learning L2 M. Manchón (Eds.), Task-based language learning:
writing, but instead included both broad cate- Insights from and for L2 writing (pp. 237–263).
gories for promoting SRL. Based on the predic- Philadelphia/Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
tive effects, it might be interesting to separate the Chenoweth, N. A., & Hayes, J. R. (2001). Fluency in writ-
questionnaire in future research with a focus on ing: Generating text in L1 and L2. Written Commu-
learning to write or using writing for learning con- nication, 18, 80–98.
tent area knowledge. Cohen, A. D. (2014). Strategies in learning and using
a second language (2nd ed.). New York: Rout-
Finally, given that our study only found six types
ledge/Taylor & Francis.
of SRL strategies having direct effects on EFL writ-
Cohen, A. D., & Griffiths, C. (2015). Revisiting LLS re-
ing proficiency, it might be interesting to explore search 40 years later. TESOL Quarterly, 49, 414–
whether other types of writing strategies such 429.
as peer learning and interest enhancement have Cohen, A. D., & Macaro, E. (Eds.). (2007). Language
indirect effects on EFL writing scores. learner strategies: Thirty years of research and practice.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin,
REFERENCES 112, 155–159.
Cooper, C. A., & Corpus, J. H. (2009). Learners’ devel-
oping knowledge of strategies for regulating mo-
Alexander, P. A., Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (1998). tivation. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,
A perspective on strategy research: Progress and 30, 525–536.
prospects. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 129– Cumming, A. (1989). Writing expertise and second-
154. language proficiency. Language Learning, 39, 81–
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cam- 135.
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Cumming, A., Busch, M., & Zhou, A. (2002). Inves-
Anderson, J. R. (1985). Cognitive psychology and its impli- tigating learners’ goals in the context of adult
cations (2nd ed.). New York: Freeman. second-language writing. In S. Ransdell & M. L.
Anderson, N. J. (1991). Individual differences in strat- Barbier (Eds.), New directions for research in L2 writ-
egy use in second language reading and testing. ing (pp. 189–208). Dordrecht, the Netherlands:
Modern Language Journal, 75, 460–472. Kluwer.
24 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
Dinsmore, D. L., Alexander, P. A., & Loughlin, S. Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural
M. (2008). Focusing the conceptual lens on equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford.
metacognition, self-regulation, and self-regulated Kormos, J. (2012). The role of individual differences in
learning. Educational Psychology Review, 20, 391– L2 writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 21,
409. 390–403.
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). Language learning strategies and Lee, I. (2014). Revisiting teacher feedback in EFL writ-
student self-regulation. In Z. Dörnyei (Ed.), The ing from sociocultural perspectives. TESOL Quar-
psychology of the language learner: Individual dffer- terly, 48, 201–213.
ences in second language acquisition (pp. 162–196). Lei, X. (2008). Exploring a sociocultural approach to
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. writing strategy research: Mediated actions in writ-
Dörnyei, Z. (2010). Questionnaires in second language re- ing activities. Journal of Second Language Writing, 17,
search: Construction, administration, and processing. 217–236.
New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis. Leki, I. (1995). Coping strategies of ESL students in writ-
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New ing tasks across the curriculum. TESOL Quarterly,
York: Guilford. 29, 235–260.
Ferris, D., & Roberts, B. (2001). Error feedback in Macaro, E. (2006). Strategies for language learning and
L2 writing classes: How explicit does it need to for language use: Revising the theoretical frame-
be? Journal of Second Language Writing, 10, 161– work. Modern Language Journal, 90, 320–337.
184. Manchón, R. (2009). Introduction: Broadening the per-
Field, A. P. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (and spective of L2 writing scholarship: The contribu-
sex, drugs and rock ’n’ roll) (3rd ed.). London: tion of research on foreign language writing. In R.
SAGE. Manchón (Ed.), Writing in foreign language contexts:
Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1981). A cognitive process the- Learning, teaching, and research (pp. 1–19). Cleve-
ory of writing. College Composition and Communica- don, UK: Multilingual Matters.
tion, 32, 365–387. Manchón, R., Roca de Larios, J., & Murphy, L. (2007).
Gao, X. (2007). Has language learning strategy research A review of writing strategies: Focus on conceptu-
come to an end? A response to Tseng et al. Applied alization and impact of first language. In A. D. Co-
Linguistics, 28, 615–620. hen & E. M. Macaro (Eds.), Language learner strate-
Gordon, L. (2008). Writing and good language learn- gies: Thirty years of research and practice (pp. 229–
ers. In C. Griffiths (Ed.), Lessons from good lan- 250). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
guage learners (pp. 244–254). Cambridge: Cam- Nguyen, T. C. L., & Gu, Y. (2013). Strategy-based in-
bridge University Press. struction: A learner-focused approach to develop-
Griffiths, C., & Oxford, R. L. (2014). The twenty-first ing learner autonomy. Language Teaching Research,
century landscape of language learning strate- 17, 9–30.
gies: Introduction to this special issue. System, 43, O’Malley, J. M., & Chamot, A. U. (1990). Learning strate-
1–10. gies in second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cam-
Harris, K. R., Graham, S., MacArthur, C., Reid, R., & bridge University Press.
Mason, L. H. (2011). Self-regulated learning pro- Ong, J., & Zhang, L. J. (2013). Effects of the manipu-
cesses and children’s writing. In B. J. Zimmerman lation of cognitive processes on EFL writers’ text
& D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of quality. TESOL Quarterly, 47, 375–398.
learning and performance (pp. 187–202). New York: Oxford, R. L. (1990). Language learning strategies: What
Routledge/Taylor & Francis. every teacher should know. Boston, MA: Heinle &
Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new framework for understand- Heinle.
ing cognition and affect in writing. In C. M. Levy Oxford, R. L. (2013). Teaching and researching lan-
& S. Randsdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theo- guage learning strategies (2nd ed.). Harlow, UK:
ries, methods, individual differences, and applications Pearson.
(pp. 1–27). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Oxford, R. L., Rubin, J., Chamot, A. U., Schramm, K.,
Hsiao, T. Y., & Oxford, R. L. (2002). Comparing theo- Lavine, R., Gunning, P., & Nel, C. (2014). The
ries of language learning strategies: A confirma- learning strategy prism: Perspectives of learning
tory factor analysis. Modern Language Journal, 86, strategy experts. System, 43, 30–49.
368–383. Petrić, B., & Czárl, B. (2003). Validating a writing strat-
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit egy questionnaire. System, 31, 187–215.
indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conven- Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation
tional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural in self-regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. R.
Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-
Hyland, K. (2007). Genre pedagogy: Language, literacy regulation (pp. 451–502). San Diego, CA: Aca-
and L2 writing instruction. Journal of Second Lan- demic Press.
guage Writing, 16, 148–164. Pintrich, P. R. (2004). A conceptual framework for as-
Hyland, K., & Hyland, F. (2006). Feedback on second sessing motivation and self-regulated learning in
language students’ writing. Language Teaching, 39, college students. Educational Psychology Review, 16,
83–101. 385–407.
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 25
Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motiva- Silva, T. (1993). Toward an understanding of the distinct
tional and self-regulated learning components of nature of L2 writing: The ESL research and its im-
classroom academic performance. Journal of Edu- plications. TESOL Quarterly, 27, 657–677.
cational Psychology, 82, 33–40. Silva, T., & Matsuda, P. K. (Eds.). (2010). Practicing theory
Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, in second language writing. West Lafayette, IN: Par-
W. J. (1991). A manual for the use of the Motivated lor Press.
Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Ann Stern, H. H. (1975). What can we learn from the good
Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press. language learner. The Canadian Modern Language
Pintrich, P. R., Wolters, C. A., & Baxter, G. P. (2000). As- Review, 31, 304–318.
sessing metacognition and self-regulated learning. Teng, L. S. (2016). Changes in teachers’ beliefs after
In G. Schraw & J. C. Impara (Eds.), Issues in the mea- a professional development project for teaching
surement of metacognition (pp. 43–97). Lincoln, NE: writing: Two Chinese cases. Journal of Education for
Buro Institute of Mental Measurements. Teaching, 42, 106–109.
Plonsky, L. (2011). The effectiveness of second lan- Teng, L. S., & Zhang, J. L. (2016). Fostering strate-
guage strategy instruction: A meta-analysis. Lan- gic learning: The development and validation of
guage Learning, 61, 993–1038. the Writing Strategies for Motivational Regulation
Raimes, A. (1985). What unskilled ESL students do Questionniare (WSMRQ). The Asia-Pacific Educa-
as they write: A classroom study of composing. tion Researcher, 25, 123–134.
TESOL Quarterly, 19, 229–258. Tseng, W. T., Dörnyei, Z., & Schmitt, N. (2006). A new
Raykov, T., & Marcoulides, G. A. (2008). An introduc- approach to assessing strategic learning: The case
tion to applied multivariate analysis. New York: Rout- of self-regulation in vocabulary acquisition. Ap-
ledge. plied Linguistics, 27, 78–102.
Riazi, A. (1997). Acquiring disciplinary literacy: A social- Varghese, S. A., & Abraham, S. A. (1998). Undergradu-
cognitive analysis of text production and learning ates arguing a case. Journal of Second Language Writ-
among Iranian graduate students of education. ing, 7, 287–306.
Journal of Second Language Writing, 6, 105–137. Victori, M. (1999). An analysis of writing knowledge
Roca de Larios, J., Manchón, R., Murphy, L., & Martín, in EFL composing: A case study of two effec-
J. (2008). The foreign language writer’s strategic tive and two less effective writers. System, 27, 537–
behaviour in the allocation of time to writing pro- 555.
cesses. Journal of Second Language Writing, 17, 30– Wang, J. (2014). A synthesis of the studies on L2 writ-
47. ing in the Chinese context. In J. Wang & D. Lin
Roca de Larios, J., Martin, J., & Murphy, L. (2001). A (Eds.), Teaching and researching EFL writing from
temporal analysis of formulation processes in L1 psycho-cognitive and socio-cultural perspectives: Proceed-
and L2 writing. Language Learning, 51, 497–538. ings of the 8th International Conference on Teaching
Rose, H. (2011). Reconceptualizing strategic learning and Researching EFL Writing in China (pp. 37–69).
in the face of self-regulation: Throwing language Beijing: FLTRP.
learning strategies out with the bathwater. Applied Wang, W., & Wen, Q. (2002). L1 use in the L2 compos-
Linguistics, 33, 92–98. ing process: An exploratory study of 16 Chinese
Rubin, J. (1975). What the “good language learner” can EFL writers. Journal of Second Language Writing, 11,
teach us. TESOL Quarterly, 9, 41–51. 225–246.
Sasaki, M. (2000). Toward an empirical model of EFL Weinstein, C. E., Schulte, A., & Palmer, D. R. (1987).
writing processes: An exploratory study. Journal of LASSI: Learning and Study Strategies Inventory. Clear-
Second Language Writing, 9, 259–291. water, FL: Heinle and Heinle.
Sasaki, M. (2004). A multiple-data analysis of the 3.5- Wenden, A. (1991). Learner strategies for learner autonomy.
year development of EFL student writers. Lan- New York: Prentice Hall.
guage Learning, 54, 525–582. Winne, P. H. (2011). A cognitive and metacognitive anal-
Schunk, D. H., & Ertmer, P. A. (2000). Self-regulation ysis of self-regulated learning. In B. J. Zimmerman
and academic learning: Self-efficacy enhancing & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation
interventions. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & of learning and performance (pp. 15–32). New York:
M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation Routledge/Taylor & Francis.
(pp. 631–649). San Diego, CA: Academic Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (2010). Self-regulated
Press. learning and socio-cognitive theory. In P. Pene-
Schunk, D. H., & Rice, J. M. (1986). Extended attribu- lope, B. Eva, & B. McGaw (Eds.), International en-
tional feedback: Sequence effects during remedial cyclopedia of education (pp. 503–508). Oxford, UK:
reading instruction. Journal of Early Adolescence, 6, Elsevier.
55–66. Winne, P. H., & Perry, N. E. (2000). Measuring self-
Schwinger, M., Steinmayr, R., & Spinath, B. (2009). regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich,
How do motivational regulation strategies affect & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation
achievement: Mediated by effort management (pp. 531–566). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
and moderated by intelligence. Learning and Indi- Wolters, C. A. (1999). The relation between high school
vidual Differences, 19, 621–627. students’ motivational regulation and their use of
26 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)
learning strategies, effort, and classroom perfor- ological developments, and future prospects.
mance. Learning & Individual Differences, 11, 281– American Educational Research Journal, 45, 166–
299. 183.
Wolters, C. A., & Benzon, M. B. (2013). Assessing and Zimmerman, B. J. (2011). Motivational sources and
predicting college students’ use of strategies for outcomes of self-regulated learning and perfor-
the self-regulation of motivation. Journal of Experi- mance. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk
mental Education, 81, 199–221. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and per-
Wolters, C. A., & Mueller, S. A. (2010). Motivation regu- formance (pp. 49–64). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erl-
lation. In P. P. B. McGaw (Ed.), International encyclo- baum.
pedia of education (3rd ed.; pp. 631–635). Oxford, Zimmerman, B. J. (2013). From cognitive modeling to
UK: Elsevier. self-regulation: A social cognitive career path. Ed-
Woodrow, L. (2005). The challenge of measuring lan- ucational Psychologists, 48, 135–147.
guage learning strategies. Foreign Language Annals, Zimmerman, B. J., & Bandura, A. (1994). Impact of
38, 90–98. self-regulatory influences on writing course attain-
Woodrow, L. (2011). College English writing affect: Self- ment. American Educational Research Journal, 31,
efficacy and anxiety. System, 39, 510–522. 845–862.
Zhang, L. J. (2008). Constructivist pedagogy in strate- Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez–Pons, M. (1986). De-
gic reading instruction: Exploring pathways to velopment of a structured interview for assess-
learner development in the English as a second ing student use of self-regulated learning strate-
language (ESL) classroom. Instructional Science, 36, gies. American Educational Research Journal, 23, 614–
89–116. 628.
Zhang, L. J. (2010). A dynamic metacognitive systems ac- Zimmerman, B. J., & Risemberg, R. (1997). Becoming
count of Chinese university students’ knowledge a self-regulated writer: A social cognitive perspec-
about EFL reading. TESOL Quarterly, 44, 320–353. tive. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 73–
Zhang, L. J. (2013). Second language writing as and for 101.
second language learning. Journal of Second Lan- Zimmerman, B. J., & Schunk, D. H. (2011). Self-
guage Writing, 22, 446–447. regulated learning and performance: An intro-
Zhang, L. J. (2016). Reflections on pedagogical imports duction and an overview. In B. J. Zimmerman
of Western practices for professionalizing second- & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation
language writing and writing-teacher education. of learning and performance (pp. 1–12). New York:
Writing & Pedagogy, 8. Routledge/Taylor & Francis.
Zhang, L. J., Aryadoust, V., & Zhang, D. (2016). Tak-
ing stock of the effects of strategies-based instruc-
tion on writing in Chinese and English in Sin-
gapore primary schools. In R. E. Silver & W.
APPENDIX A
Bokhorst–Heng (Eds.), Quadrilingual education in
Singapore: Pedagogical innovation in language educa-
tion (pp. 103–126). New York: Springer. Guided Interview Questions
Zhang, L. J., Gu, Y., & Hu, G. (2008). A cognitive per-
spective on Singaporean primary school pupils’ 1. What strategies do you use in the writing
use of reading strategies in learning to read in En-
course to help you learn?
glish. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78,
2. What strategies do you use pre, during and
245–271.
Zhang, L. J., & Zhang, D. (2013). Thinking metacog- after the writing process?
nitively about metacognition in second and for- 3. Do you plan before writing? If yes, please
eign language learning, teaching, and research: explain the process.
Toward a dynamic metacognitive systems perspec- 4. Do you revise after writing? If yes, please ex-
tive. Contemporary Foreign Languages Studies, 369, plain the process.
111–121. 5. Can you monitor and evaluate your writ-
Zhao, H. (2010). Investigating learners’ use and under- ing process and performance in the writing
standing of peer and teacher feedback on writing: course?
A comparative study in a Chinese English writing
6. How do you motivate yourself in the com-
classroom. Assessing Writing, 15, 3–17.
posing or learning-to-write process?
Zimmerman, B. J. (1989). A social cognitive view of self-
regulated academic learning. Journal of Educational 7. How do you solve learning challenges in the
Psychology, 81, 329–339. writing course?
Zimmerman, B. J. (2008). Investigating self-regulation 8. Would you seek help from others in the
and motivation: Historical background, method- learning-to-write process?
Lin Sophie Teng and Lawrence Jun Zhang 27

APPENDIX B

Descriptive Statistics of the Writing Strategies for Self-Regulated Learning Questionnaires (WSSRLQ)
(40 items, N = 780)

Items M SD Skewness Kurtosis

Text Processing (TP)


1. When writing, I use some literary devices to make the 3.39 1.58 0.29 –0.83
composition more interesting.
2. When writing, I check grammar mistakes. 5.05 1.59 –0.84 –0.01
3. When writing, I check spelling and punctuation. 4.32 1.65 –0.28 –0.97
4. When writing, I check the structure for logical coherence. 4.25 1.63 –0.28 –0.83
5. When writing, I check the cohesiveness or connection among 4.68 1.53 –0.58 –0.45
sentences.
6. When writing, I check whether the topic and the content 5.12 1.42 –0.86 0.21
have been clearly expressed.
Course Memory (CM)
1. I write useful words and expressions taught in writing courses 4.40 1.73 –0.28 –0.98
to help me remember them.
2. I speak out useful words and expressions taught in writing 4.70 1.58 –0.54 –0.59
courses to help me remember them.
3. I read my class notes and the course material over and over 4.31 1.54 –0.21 –0.85
again to help me remember them.
Idea Planning (IP)
1. Before writing, I read related articles to help me plan. 4.66 1.63 –0.58 –0.61
2. Before writing, I use the internet to search for related 4.68 1.65 –0.60 –0.54
information to help me plan.
3. Before writing, I think about the core elements of a good 4.50 1.54 –0.39 –0.66
composition I have learned to help me plan.
Goal-Oriented Monitoring and Evaluating (GME)
1. When learning to write, I set up goals for myself in order to 3.32 1.58 0.34 –0.67
direct my learning activities.
2. When learning to write, I check my progress to make sure I 3.76 1.52 0.01 –0.77
achieve my goal.
3. I evaluate my mastery of the knowledge and skills learned in 4.01 1.52 –0.06 –0.71
writing courses.
4. I monitor my learning process in writing courses. 3.80 1.53 –0.02 –0.72
5. When writing, I tell myself to follow my plan. 4.16 1.61 –0.28 –0.70
6. When learning to write, I set up a learning goal to improve
my writing.
Peer Learning (PL)
1. I brainstorm with my peers to help me write. 3.13 1.51 0.35 –0.83
2. I discuss with my peers to have more ideas to write with. 3.78 1.62 0.03 –1.03
3. I work with my peers to complete a writing task. 3.79 1.70 0.09 -0.93
Feedback Handling (FH)
1. I am open to peer feedback on my writing. 5.52 1.24 –1.12 1.41
2. I am open to teacher feedback on my writing. 5.64 0.94 –1.23 1.60
3. I try to improve my English writing based on peer feedback. 5.31 1.26 –0.95 1.06
4. I try to improve my English writing based on teacher 5.72 1.10 –1.21 2.17
feedback.
Interest Enhancement (IE)
1. I look for ways to bring more fun to the learning of writing. 4.62 1.54 –0.49 –0.48
2. I choose interesting topics to practice writing. 4.72 1.68 –0.56 –0.60
3. I connect the writing task with my real life to intrigue me. 4.45 1.65 –0.32 –0.75
4. I try to connect the writing task with my personal interest. 4.66 1.61 –0.51 –0.51
28 The Modern Language Journal 100 (2016)

Items M SD Skewness Kurtosis

Motivational Self-Talk (MST)


1. I remind myself about how important it is to get good grades 4.88 1.59 –0.68 –0.30
in writing courses.
2. I tell myself that it is important to practice writing to 5.19 1.39 –0.86 0.28
outperform my peers.
3. I compete with other students and challenge myself to do 4.94 1.40 –0.67 –0.14
better than them in writing courses.
4. I tell myself to practice writing to get good grades. 4.90 1.48 –0.64 –0.26
5. I tell myself that I need to keep studying to improve my 4.78 1.49 –0.60 –0.27
writing competence.
6. I persuade myself to work hard in writing courses to improve 4.97 1.39 –0.68 –0.13
my writing skills and knowledge.
7. I persuade myself to keep on learning in writing courses to 4.78 1.37 –0.54 –0.20
find out how much I can learn.
8. I tell myself that I should keep on learning in writing courses 5.34 1.31 –0.86 0.37
to become good at writing.
Emotional Control (EC)
1. I tell myself not to worry when taking a writing test or 5.15 1.43 –0.85 0.36
answering questions in writing courses.
2. I tell myself to keep on writing when I want to give it up. 5.04 1.37 –0.71 0.25
3. I find ways to regulate my mood when I want to give up 5.33 1.20 –0.82 0.98
writing.

View publication stats

You might also like