You are on page 1of 20

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/326114247

Children's Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-


Agriculture Interface

Article  in  Journal of Ethnobiology · July 2018


DOI: 10.2993/0278-0771-38.2.205

CITATIONS READS

4 517

4 authors, including:

Gisella S. Cruz-Garcia María Elena Chuspe Zans


Oxfam Novib UNIVERSIDAD NACIONAL INTERCULTURAL DE QUILLABAMBA
36 PUBLICATIONS   748 CITATIONS    9 PUBLICATIONS   561 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

José G. Sánchez-Choy
Universidad Nacional Intercultural de la Amazonia
52 PUBLICATIONS   68 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Ethnobotany, Ethnoentomology, Ethnography View project

Proyecto Biodiversidad y Comunidad View project

All content following this page was uploaded by José G. Sánchez-Choy on 06 July 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Children's Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-
Agriculture Interface
Author(s): Gisella S. Cruz-Garcia, Cecilia Caffi, María Elena Chuspe Zans and
José Sanchez-Choy
Source: Journal of Ethnobiology, 38(2):205-222.
Published By: Society of Ethnobiology
https://doi.org/10.2993/0278-0771-38.2.205
URL: http://www.bioone.org/doi/full/10.2993/0278-0771-38.2.205

BioOne (www.bioone.org) is a nonprofit, online aggregation of core research in the


biological, ecological, and environmental sciences. BioOne provides a sustainable online
platform for over 170 journals and books published by nonprofit societies, associations,
museums, institutions, and presses.
Your use of this PDF, the BioOne Web site, and all posted and associated content
indicates your acceptance of BioOne’s Terms of Use, available at www.bioone.org/page/
terms_of_use.
Usage of BioOne content is strictly limited to personal, educational, and non-commercial
use. Commercial inquiries or rights and permissions requests should be directed to the
individual publisher as copyright holder.

BioOne sees sustainable scholarly publishing as an inherently collaborative enterprise connecting authors, nonprofit
publishers, academic institutions, research libraries, and research funders in the common goal of maximizing access to
critical research.
Journal of
Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222

Children’s Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the


Forest-Agriculture Interface

Gisella S. Cruz-Garcia1, 2 *, Cecilia Caffi3, María Elena Chuspe Zans4,


and José Sanchez-Choy5, 6

Abstract. This paper examines the relationship between children’s theoretical knowledge of wild
food plants in relation to sociocultural group, sex, and age in the context of social and environmental
change. Theoretical knowledge was assessed by evaluating the composition of the cultural domain
of “wild food plants,” naming ability, and cognitive salience. Freelistings were conducted with 57
Indigenous Shipibo-Konibo children and 57 mestizo children in Ucayali, one of the regions with the
highest deforestation rates in the Peruvian Amazon. A total of 120 plants were listed by all children,
with 72 listed by Indigenous and 95 by mestizo children. Most species listed as wild food plants have
been classified as domesticated species by scientists. The main factor affecting variation in children’s
theoretical knowledge was sociocultural group. Mestizo children included more introduced crops in
their lists, whereas, for Indigenous children, the most salient species were those that required specific
abilities or knowledge to be consumed. Older children presented statistically significant longer lists
than younger children, but there were no statistical differences in list length in relation to sex. We
conclude by discussing the definition of “wild” versus domesticated plants and the effect of socio-
ecological change on children’s ethnobotanical knowledge as the landscape transforms in the forest-
agriculture interface.
Keywords: cognitive salience, deforestation, ethnobotany, freelisting, traditional ecological
knowledge

Introduction may in fact be changing” (Zarger and Stepp


Theoretical knowledge, including 2004:416). However, there are relatively
species classification and nomenclature, few studies on children’s ethnobotanical
is the basis of ethnobotanical knowledge knowledge (Souto and Ticktin 2012; Young
(Berlin 1992) and could be used as a 2002), although their geographical extent is
proxy to assess knowledge dynamics—the large. Thus, in Asia, ethnobotanical knowl-
process of knowledge creation, acquisi- edge has been studied among children in
tion, transformation, and loss—especially Northeast Thailand (Setalaphruk and Price
in contexts of socio-ecological change 2007); in the Indian Western Ghats (Cruz-
(Zarger and Stepp 2004). In that sense, the Garcia 2006a, 2006b, 2014; Cruz-Garcia
study of children’s ethnobotanical knowl- and Howard 2013); in Africa for the Baka
edge is particularly crucial because “social children from southeastern Cameroon
demand for such expertise and the cultural (Gallois 2015); and in Europe for children
value ascribed to environmental knowledge living in the Biosphere Reserve of Grosses

1
Decision and Policy Analysis Research Area, International Center for Tropical Agriculture, Km 17 Recta Cali-
Palmira, Apartado Aéreo 6713, Cali, Colombia.
2
Botanical Research Institute of Texas, Fort Worth, Texas.
3
Department of Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of Pavia, Italy.
4
Facultad de Ciencias, Escuela Profesional de Biología, Departamento Académico de Biología, Universidad
Nacional de San Antonio Abad del Cusco, Peru.
5
Departamento Agroforestal Acuícola, Universidad Nacional Intercultural de la Amazonía, Ucayali, Peru.
6
Instituto de Investigaciones de la Amazonía Peruana, Pucallpa, Peru.
*
Corresponding author (g.s.cruz@cgiar.org)
206 Cruz-Garcia, Caffi, Zans, and Sanchez-Choy

Walsertal in Austria (Schunko et al. 2012). 1996; González-Insuasti and Caballero


Research on children’s ethnobiology has 2007). The difference, however, might not
also been conducted in Latin America. be so evident in the field. In that sense,
For instance, Quinlan et al. (2016) studied research on children’s knowledge of wild
children’s learning of plants in Dominica, food plants would contribute to a better
West Indies; Zarger and Stepp (2004) understanding of knowledge dynamics in
compared Tzeltal Maya children’s plant- relation to domestication and landscape
naming abilities 30 years ago with those of transformation, which is particularly neces-
a subsequent generation; Wyndham (2010) sary for regions facing increasing social
assessed the influence of schooling on and environmental change.
Rarámuri children’s plant knowledge in the The goal of this work is to document
Sierra Tarahumara, Mexico; Blacutt-Rivero selected aspects of children’s theoretical
et al. (2016) documented children’s knowl- knowledge of wild food plants, including
edge of palm trees among Amerindian and the composition of the cultural domain
mestizo communities in Bolivia; Hynes et of “wild food plants,” naming ability,
al. (1997) compared children’s botanical and cognitive salience. Certainly, chil-
knowledge in relation to the geographic dren’s ability to name local plants is a
isolation of their villages in the subtropical key component of their ethnobotanical
montane forests of northwestern Argen- knowledge (Zarger and Stepp 2004) and a
tina; and Barreau et al. (2016) documented main indicator of theoretical knowledge.
knowledge transmission of wild edible Specifically, this study compares knowl-
plants to younger generations of Mapuche edge of wild food plants among children in
people in Chile. Ucayali, Peru.
To the best of our knowledge, no Previous research shows that knowl-
previous study has focused on children’s edge is distributed according to personal
knowledge of wild food plants in the and social factors and varies spatially and
forest-agriculture interface within a context temporally. For instance, knowledge is
of increasing deforestation. Addressing that influenced by intercultural factors such
topic is important because wild food plants as sociocultural group and ethnicity, and
constitute an essential component of the intracultural factors such as gender and
food basket of rural households around the age (Hopkins 2011; Müller et al. 2015;
world (Bharucha and Pretty 2010; Cruz- Souto and Ticktin 2012). Consequently,
Garcia and Ertug 2014), which contributes we studied knowledge of wild food
to improve the nutritional quality and plants among children from two different
micronutrient content of people’s diet sociocultural groups—Indigenous Shipi-
(Grivetti and Ogle 2000; Heywood 2011). bo-Konibo and mestizo children—while
It is also unknown if children distinguish also considering differences in sex and
between wild and domesticated species in age. Mestizos are migrants, or descen-
contexts of landscape transformation. For dent from migrants, from non-Amazonian
scientists, domesticated species include regions of Peru and usually of mixed Indig-
species that have suffered changes in their enous and European heritage. Additionally,
phenotypic and genotypic characteristics our results explore how deforestation and
as a result of artificial selection, and wild environmental change in Ucayali—one of
food plants are non-domesticated species the regions in the Peruvian Amazon with
that exist along a continuum of different the highest deforestation rates (Porro et al.
intensities of human management ranging 2015)—might relate to the distribution of
from truly wild (absence of management) wild food plant knowledge among different
to semi-domesticated plants (Casas et al. groups of children.

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


Children’s Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-Agriculture Interface 207

Study Site was further facilitated by opening logging


Ucayali is the second largest adminis- roads (Murray 2006). The mestizo villages
trative department of Peru. It is located in the participating in this study were created
Amazon region, and has a humid tropical between the 1970s and 1990s. Mestizo
forest cover and a hot and humid climate. livelihoods depend on agriculture and
The main productive activities of the region, their main income sources come from
including agriculture, livestock farming, palm oil, cacao, plantain, and, to a lesser
and timber production, make up almost degree, livestock. There is no commu-
20% of Ucayali’s gross domestic product nally owned land in mestizo villages and
(GDP) (INEI 2011; MINEM-GOREU 2007). the forest, which is privately owned and
Ucayali is the main center of the Peru- mainly secondary, is fragmented and scat-
vian timber industry (Ramos Delgado and tered across the different household land
Domínguez Torrejón 2009) and is facing a properties. The staple crops in the region
rapid increase in palm oil and cacao plan- are cassava (Manihot esculenta), maize
tations (Salisbury and Fagan 2013), making (Zea mays), plantain (Musa paradisiaca),
it one of the regions with the highest exten- rice (Oryza sativa), and beans (Phaseolus
sions of deforestation (Oliveira et al. 2007). vulgaris).
Land degradation is mainly due to the
expansion of legal and illegal logging, land Methods
clearing, and road construction (Galarza
and La Serna 2005; Miranda et al. 2014). Methods of Data Collection
For instance, by 2010, about nine percent Fieldwork took place between July and
of the original forest area of Ucayali had September 2014 in two mestizo villages,
been cleared (Porro et al. 2015). Pueblo Libre and La Unión, and two Shipi-
About 20% of Ucayali is inhabited bo-Konibo communities, Puerto Belén and
by nearly 300 Indigenous communities, Junín Pablo, in Ucayali, Peru (Figure 1). The
with the Shipibo-Konibo being the largest sites were selected by local experts from the
ethnolinguistic group, having an estimated Instituto de Investigaciones de la Amazonía
population of 35,000 inhabitants by 2002 Peruana (IIAP) as representatives of mestizo
(Tournon 2002). Their livelihoods mainly and Shipibo-Konibo populations in Ucayali.
depend on small-scale agriculture, fishing, Through ongoing research projects in these
and hunting (Pimentel et al. 2004). Their sites, IIAP has built rapport with the local
land, including agricultural fields and populations, which facilitated approaching
forest, is communal property. them. Selected communities are located in
The remaining 80% of the population of the forest-agriculture interface, possessing
Ucayali consists mostly of mestizos who are forested areas alongside agricultural fields.
settled chiefly along the Federico Basadre The mestizo villages presented an average
Highway, which connects Pucallpa with deforestation rate of 28% from 2001 to
Lima (860 km), the capital of the country, 2014, with only 50% of their area having
or along the Ucayali river and its tributaries a 60% or more of canopy cover in 2014.
(Porro et al. 2015). The highway, which During the same period, the Indigenous
was built in 1945 (Pimentel et al. 2004), communities showed an average defor-
allowed for the migration of families from estation rate of 8%, with 82% of their area
the Andean highlands and coastal regions being covered with canopy (see Supple-
placing an increasing demand on Ucayali’s ment 1).
natural resources (particularly timber). The To examine children’s wild food plant
encroachment of mestizos, who cleared knowledge, we conducted written free-
the land for slash and burn agriculture, listings with 146 children from ages seven

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


208 Cruz-Garcia, Caffi, Zans, and Sanchez-Choy

Figure 1. Study sites. The map indicates the changes in land use from January 2004 to December 2013,
according to Terra-i (CIAT 2015). Source: Terra-i; map prepared by Paula Paz. See Supplemental Map 1
for color version showing detailed gradients.

to 14, belonging to different sociocultural the plants in Spanish, the main language
backgrounds (mestizo and Indigenous). spoken in the villages. Indigenous children
Freelistings were conducted at the school, wrote the names in Shipibo-Konibo, with
with permission of the teachers who the Spanish name next to it if they knew
provided prior oral informed consent. All it. The spelling and translation of Shipibo-
children who participated in the activity Konibo names was double-checked
did so voluntarily. Given that there is only according to Tournon (2006). Children were
one school in each village, by working in also asked to indicate their sex and age, but
the school, we expected to engage most not their names. This study was carried out
children living in each village (see Table 1). in accordance with the recommendations
Children were asked to write down on of the guidelines of the International Society
a piece of paper all the wild food plants of Ethnobiology Code of Ethics (ISE 2006).
they know (Borgatti 1999). Mestizo chil- Thirty-two freelistings were excluded
dren wrote down the common names of from the analysis because they were either

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


Children’s Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-Agriculture Interface 209

Table 1. Distribution of the research sample, by sociocultural group, sex, and age, with the number of entries and
plants listed for each group.
% of the Number of Number of plants
Children stratification Sample Populationb population entriesc listed
Sociocultural group:          
Indigenous (total) 57 182 31 465 72
Mestizo (total) 57 97 59 593 95
Sex:          
Girls (total) 64 123 52 629 102
Indigenous girls 28 86 33 236 58
Mestizo girls 36 37 97 393 84
Boys (total) 50 156 32 429 87
Indigenous boys 29 96 30 229 57
Mestizo boys 21 60 35 200 64
Age : a
         
Younger children (total) 64 153 42 549 89
Indigenous younger 31 91 34 205 43
Mestizo younger 33 62 53 344 78
Older children (total) 46 126 37 477 94
Indigenous older 25 91 27 257 64
Mestizo older 21 35 60 220 69
a
Four children (one Indigenous and three mestizo) did not provide information about their age, there are
missing values for the analysis of age.
b
Children from seven to 14 years old (the sample’s age). This information was obtained from the health center
of each village or community.
c
One entry is one plant listed by one child.

empty (7), illegible (12), or did not indicate voucher specimens were collected in situ
a child’s sex (13). This yielded a total of 114 with local experts in one of the villages
lists that were included as part of this study: where the study took place. Herbarium
22 were conducted in Pueblo Libre, 35 in specimens of most identified species are on
La Unión, 29 in Puerto Belén, and 28 in repository in the herbarium at the univer-
Junín Pablo. All entries (where one entry is sity. Information about whether a species
one plant listed by one child) were checked is native or introduced to the Amazon was
for typos and synonyms. When there was obtained from Tropicos (Missouri Botan-
more than one common name for a specific ical Garden 2017), the Natural Resources
plant, the one most frequently mentioned Conservation Service of the United States
was chosen in order to make all lists compa- Department of Agriculture (USDA 2017),
rable. The botanical identification of plant The Plant List (2013), and Clement (1999).
species mentioned by 10% or more Indig- Native species were classified as domesti-
enous or mestizo children was conducted cated or non-domesticated in Amazonia
by a local taxonomist from the Univer- at the time of European contact according
sidad Intercultural de la Amazonía Peruana to Levis et al. (2017), Clement (1999),
in Pucallpa on the basis of plant voucher Clement et al. (2010), and Jones et al.
specimens collected in the study area. The (2013, for breadfruit).

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


210 Cruz-Garcia, Caffi, Zans, and Sanchez-Choy

Methods of Data Analysis Methodological Caveats


Data from freelistings were analyzed The methodology used has several
according to frequency of mention of food limitations. Firstly, information was
plants, particularly in relation to the pres- collected in two different languages, which
ence of wild and domesticated species, as might be a source of error when trans-
well as native and introduced species. The lating a plant name from Shipibo-Konibo
species mentioned by 10% or more of Indig- to Spanish. Secondly, given that data was
enous or mestizo children were selected to collected in written form, the length of the
be compared according to sociocultural lists might be affected by children’s writing
group (Indigenous vs mestizo), sex (girl vs skills and this might cause some lists to be
boy), and age (younger children [i.e., 7–10 empty. Thirdly, the collection of voucher
years old] vs older children [i.e., 11–14 specimens for taxonomic identification
years old]). was conducted with adults knowledgeable
Naming ability was measured with of plant resources but not with children,
the length of the list. We calculated the which might introduce some intrinsic
mean, median, and interquartile range for biases. Fourthly, voucher specimen collec-
the eight different groups: Indigenous boys, tion only took place in one of the mestizo
Indigenous girls, mestizo boys, mestizo study villages but not in the Shipibo-Konibo
girls, Indigenous younger, Indigenous communities, which might be a source of
older, mestizo younger, and mestizo older error, although Shipibo-Konibo children
children. The sample size did not allow us were asked to provide the names of the
to further subdivide these groups (i.e., tabu- plants in Spanish. Finally, factors such as
lating sociocultural group with sex and age education, parents’ occupation, household
in a single division). The Mann-Whitney U composition, and income, which could also
test for non-parametric data was used to explain variation in ethnobotanical knowl-
test the statistical significance of the differ- edge (Giovannini et al. 2011; Quinlan et al.
ences in the list lengths among the different 2016; Reyes-García et al. 2009), were not
groups of children. part of the scope of this study. Despite these
Freelistings were also analyzed caveats, the study yields valuable insights
according to cognitive salience, by into the understanding of children’s theo-
combining both frequency of mention retical knowledge of wild food plants in the
and mean position of a species in the lists forest-agriculture interface.
as part of a single index. The Cognitive
Salience Index (CSI) ranges from 0, repre- Results and Discussion
senting no informant mentioning a species
(least salient), to 1, representing all infor- The Cultural Domain of Wild Food Plants
mants having mentioned a species first A total of 1058 entries were regis-
(most salient), and assumes that the items tered (n = 114 children), with 465 entries
mentioned first in a list are more salient corresponding to Indigenous (n = 57) and
than those mentioned last (Sutrop 2001). 593 to mestizo children (n = 57). A total
CSIs were calculated for the eight different of 120 plants were listed by all children
groups of children and then compared (n = 114), with 72 plants listed by Indige-
using Spearman rank order correlation nous and 95 by mestizo children (Table 1).
coefficients. Venn diagrams were devel- The number of food plants provided by
oped to compare the species with a CSI of children in Ucayali was higher than the
0.05 or higher for the different groups of number reported in studies with adults in
children. the Peruvian Amazon. For instance, Vásque
and Peláez (2015) documented 29 food

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


Children’s Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-Agriculture Interface 211

plants in a village located in Bagua Grande, genus (Inga sp. and Garcinia sp.), one plant
and Cruz-Garcia and Vael (2017) reported was given a sp.1 (species 1) as its scientific
30 wild food plants in Pueblo Libre, one of name because it could not be taxonomi-
the villages where the present study took cally identified, and the rest of the plants
place. However, it is important to note that were identified at the species level. Our
several plants listed by children are intro- results showed that children living in rural
duced crops, which might have increased areas with permanent contact with nature
the length of the lists in this study. mentioned plants that were mainly iden-
Almost two-thirds of the plants were tified at species level, which is aligned to
mentioned by less than 10% of Indigenous data from Berlin and Stevens (1994), who
and by less than 10% of mestizo children reported that children living in areas with
(Table 2). Remarkably, 21% and 35% of less interaction with the natural environ-
the plants mentioned by Indigenous and ment present attrition in naming abilities,
mestizo children, respectively, were listed shifting from species to generic names and
by only one child. This value was higher for life form names in classification. Our find-
older than for younger Indigenous children ings also dovetail with research presenting
(24 and 10 plants respectively), whereas the similar high levels of naming ability among
difference was minimal when comparing rural children, for example among Zapotec
Indigenous girls and boys (18 and 17 plants children (Hunn 2008) and Rarámuri chil-
respectively). dren (Wyndham 2010) in Mexico.
Regarding the species mentioned by When focusing on the plants mentioned
10% or more of Indigenous or mestizo by 10% or more of Indigenous or mestizo
children, 19 were mentioned by both Indige- children, we found that mestizo children
nous and mestizo, nine only by Indigenous, listed more introduced species than Indige-
and 16 only by mestizo children. Children nous children (57% and 46% respectively),
in the study listed food plants that were and all introduced species are domesti-
mainly identified at species level. Thus, only cated (Figure 2). Half of the native species
one plant, cacao del monte, encompassed listed by mestizo children and 58% of
two species (Theobroma obovatum and T. those listed by Indigenous children were
glaucum), two plants (plátano and maduro) not domesticated at the time of European
corresponded to two different varieties of contact (Table 3). These results raise two
the same species (Musa × paradisiaca), two important issues: 1) how “wild” versus
plants were identified only with regard to “domesticated” is conceptualized, and 2)

Table 2. Number of food plants listed by Indigenous and mestizo children.


Number of plants listed by children
Indigenous Mestizo
More than 10% of children (total) 28 35
Less than 10% of children (total) 44 60
five children 3 2
four children 4 7
three children 6 5
two children 16 13
one child 15 33
Total number of plants per sociocultural group 72 95

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


212 Cruz-Garcia, Caffi, Zans, and Sanchez-Choy

Figure 2. Percentage of introduced and native plants listed by Indigenous and mestizo
children. The graph indicates if native species where domesticated or not at the time
of European contact. All introduced species are domesticated. The three plants that
lack information about endemicity and degree of domestication (because they were
not identified at species level) are not presented in the graph. Only information about
plants mentioned by 10% or more of Indigenous or mestizo children is presented (n =
114 children, with four missing values for age).

how knowledge transmission operates in cated landscapes in the Amazon, where


a context of socio-ecological change. We mosaics of different ecozones are situated
discuss these issues in the following para- along a gradient of varying degrees of
graphs. human intervention (Posey 1985; Wiersum
Given that different sociocultural 1997). As children gather plants along this
groups have different ways to interact gradient of environments, it might be diffi-
with nature, the emic perspective, or the cult for them to differentiate between wild
local conceptualization of a given cultural and domesticated.
domain, is the starting point of ethno- Some species dominant across the
botanical studies (Borgatti 1999). Local Amazon forest, like Attalea phalerata,
definitions and classifications might differ were domesticated at the time of European
not only among different cultures and social contact, but nowadays present minimal or
groups, but also from those given by scien- no human management (Levis et al. 2017).
tists (Michon and De Foresta 1997). Our This can be potentially related to the fact
results show that this is the case of wild food that most of the associated traditional
plants in the study site, where this cultural knowledge was lost after the eradication of
domain not only differs according to socio- about 90–95% of the Amazonian popula-
cultural group, sex, and age, but also from tion during the first contact with Europeans
scientific classifications. It could be argued (Clement 1999). Interestingly, a study
that it is not possible to expect children to conducted in one of the mestizo villages
be able to distinguish wild from domesti- participating in this study reported that
cated species, as some differences can only local people gather the fruits of A. phalerata
be assessed at genetic level. In addition, from secondary forests (Cruz-Garcia and
domesticated species native to the region Vael 2017). This study also documented
might present wild populations (Clement et that local inhabitants classified some native
al. 2010), and children might, indeed, be domesticated species as wild, because they
referring to the wild populations of native do not require much care to grow. Several
domesticates. Moreover, there are no clear of these species—such as Inga edulis and
distinctions between wild and domesti- Pouteria caimito—are occurring as feral in

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


Children’s Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-Agriculture Interface 213

Table 3. Food plants listed by Indigenous and mestizo children, indicating species that are native to the Amazonia
and degree of domestication at the time of European contact. The table also indicates frequency of mention (freq)
and Sutrop’s cognitive salience index (CSI). Only information about plants mentioned by 10% or more Indigenous
or mestizo children is presented (n = 114 children, with four missing values for age).
Degree of Indigenous Mestizos
Native to the domestication at (total) (total)
Scientific name Amazoniac European contactd, e freq Sutrop freq Sutrop
Anacardium occidentale  I 9 0.02 7 0.02
Ananas comosus  I 11 0.03 12 0.02
Arachis hypogaea I 6 0.01
Artocarpus altilis  I 8 0.02 7 0.02
Attalea phalerata  N D 7 0.02
Averrhoa carambola  I 7 0.02
Bactris gasipaes  N D 12 0.03 16 0.05
Bertholletia excelsa  N D 6 0.03
Carica papaya I 10 0.02 26 0.08
Cecropia engleriana N ND 9 0.02
Citrullus lanatus  I 12 0.04 11 0.03
Citrus × aurantium  I 17 0.04
Citrus limon  I 9 0.02
Citrus reticulata  I 10 0.03
Cocos nucifera  I 6 0.01 20 0.04
Dioscorea trifida  I 7 0.01
Euterpe precatoria  N D 7 0.01
Garcinia sp. f f
9 0.03
Inga sp. ɸ ɸ
20 0.07 16 0.04
Inga edulis  N SD 19 0.07 35 0.11
Malus domestica  I 7 0.02
Mangifera indica  I 21 0.08 27 0.08
Manihot esculenta  N D 6 0.01
Mauritia flexuosa  N ID 43 0.20 21 0.06
Micropholis egensis  N ND 18 0.11
Musa × paradisiacaa I 6 0.04 17 0.07
Musa × paradisiacab I 10 0.03
Oenocarpus bataua  N ID 32 0.14 15 0.05
Passiflora edulis  N D 11 0.02
Passiflora ligularis  I 26 0.10
Pourouma cecropiifolia  N SD 16 0.05 7 0.01
Pouteria caimito  N D 23 0.10 9 0.03
Psidium guajava  I 12 0.02 16 0.04
Quararibea cordata  N SD 11 0.04 14 0.03
Rollinia mucosa  N D 9 0.02
Saccharum officinarum  I 7 0.01
Solanum sessiliflorum  N D 6 0.03 19 0.05
sp1 f f
9 0.03
Spondias dulcis  I 8 0.02
Syzygium malaccense  I 13 0.04
Theobroma cacao  N SD 23 0.07
Theobroma obovatum 
N ND 10 0.03
Bernoulli, T. glaucum 
Vitis vinifera  I 9 0.04
Zea mays  I   8 0.04 10 0.02
a
Musa x paradisiaca refers to plátano.
b
Musa x paradisiaca refers to banano.
c
N, native; I, introduced
d
D, domesticated; SD, semi-domesticated; ID, with incipiently domesticated populations; ND, not
domesticated but it was not specified if it was SD or ID.
e
Only for species native to the Amazonia.
f
Cannot define the endemicity and degree of domestication because the species has not been determined.

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


214 Cruz-Garcia, Caffi, Zans, and Sanchez-Choy

agricultural fields and home gardens, and area in relation to mestizo villages. We can
present incipient management patterns think of three plausible explanations for
(Cruz-Garcia 2017). this finding. Firstly, although surrounded
Children in our sample listed several by more forest area, the loss of biodiversity
introduced domesticates as wild food is increasing among Indigenous communi-
plants. This might, indeed, also show that ties. In addition, several wild food plants
children have lost or failed to acquire are not necessarily gathered in the forest
knowledge about native non-domesticated but might be collected in other environ-
food plants as the product of historical ments. For instance, the study conducted
processes of sociocultural and ecological in one of the mestizo villages (Cruz-Garcia
change that have occurred in the region, and Vael 2017) showed that 70% of wild
which possess a long history of colo- food plants were gathered in agricultural
nialism. For instance, it is widely known fields and 43% in home gardens. Secondly,
that ecological and social changes bring mestizo children named more species on
about thorough transformation of atti- average than Indigenous children and
tudes and values regarding plants, and that listed many introduced crops, given that
increasing interactions of traditional soci- they are exposed to a great diversity of
eties with modern socio-economic systems plants because of market relations. For
are related to cultural changes that have example, a study conducted by Murray
an effect on ethnobotanical knowledge (2006) comparing mestizo and Indige-
(Reyes-García et al. 2014). Indeed, mestizo nous communities in Ucayali showed that
children listed fewer native wild food plant mestizo households with greater access
species and more introduced and commer- to markets presented longer lists of foods.
cialized crops than Indigenous children. Finally, mestizo children might have had
For example, mestizo children listed as greater ease at writing than Indigenous
wild food plants two crops that do not grow children and, consequently, would have
in the region but are found in the market been more likely to write longer lists.
(Malus domestica [apple], and Vitis vinifera Mestizo girls were the group with
[grape]), whereas all plants that Indigenous longest lists, followed by mestizo boys,
children listed grow in Ucayali. Indigenous Indigenous girls, and Indigenous boys (Table
children have less access to the market, 4). However, list length was not statistically
where most introduced crops are bought by different between girls and boys in general
mestizo families. Certainly, some authors and between girls and boys by socio-cul-
have reported that market integration influ- tural group. This result resembles findings
ences ethnobotanical knowledge (Benz et by Wyndham (2010), who found no statis-
al. 2000; Reyes-García et al. 2005). tically significant differences between girls
and boys regarding naming ability among
Children’s Wild Food Plants Naming Rarámuri children in the Sierra Tarahu-
Ability mara of Mexico. Similarly, Gallois et al.
The length of lists was significantly (2017) did not find gender differences in
different between Indigenous and mestizo relation to identification abilities of wild
children (Mann-Whitney U at 0.05), with edibles among Baka children in southeast
mestizo children presenting longer lists Cameroon. Results, however, differ from
(median = 10, interquartile range = 6 for findings by Setalaphruk and Price (2007)
mestizo children; median = 8, interquar- and by Cruz-Garcia and Howard (2013)
tile range = 5 for Indigenous children). who found that girls knew more wild food
These results might be surprising given plants than boys in northeastern Thailand
that Indigenous communities have a lower and the Indian Western Ghats, respec-
deforestation rate and higher forest cover tively. It should also be noted that a study

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


Children’s Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-Agriculture Interface 215

Table 4. Mean, standard deviation (SD), minimum, and maximum number of plants listed by children, by
sociocultural group, sex, and age.
Mean SD Min Max
Sociocultural group and sex:        
Indigenous girls 8.4 3.3 3 16
Mestizo girls 10.9 4.4 2 22
Indigenous boys 7.9 4.1 1 19
Mestizo boys 9.5 4.9 2 22
Socio-cultural group and age: *        
Indigenous younger 6.6 2.8 1 12
Mestizo younger 10.4 5.0 2 22
Indigenous older 10.3 3.7 4 19
Mestizo older 10.5 4.4 3 18
* Four children did not list their age, which are considered missing values.

conducted among children in the Bolivian Rivero et al. (2016) observed that older
Amazon, Blacutt-Rivero et al. (2016) found children had more knowledge of palm trees
that gender differences existed in relation than younger ones in the Bolivian Amazon,
to practical knowledge of palm trees (e.g., and Quinlan et al. (2016) documented that
palm uses) but not in relation to theoretical children’s knowledge about plants signifi-
knowledge (e.g., naming palms). For these cantly increased with age in a rural village
reasons, we cannot exclude that there might located in Dominica, West Indies. The same
be gender differences in children’s practical finding was reported by Gallois (2015) who
knowledge (e.g., use, management, and found that, in southeastern Cameroon,
preparation) about wild food plants. Baka children’s ecological knowledge
We found that younger children had varied according to age. Indeed, when
shorter lists than older children, the differ- comparing different studies on children’s
ence being significant in statistical terms ethnobiological knowledge, Quinlan et
(Mann-Whitney U at 0.05), particularly al. (2016) concluded that, cross-culturally,
for Indigenous children (Mann-Whitney knowledge increases substantially between
U at 0.05). Such differences could relate nine and 12 years old, thus many children
to writing skills, probably higher among already possess a thorough knowledge of
mestizo than among Indigenous children. their natural environment by the age of 12.
The fact that older children had higher
levels of plant knowledge is not surprising Children’s Cognitive Salience of Wild
and is in line with previous findings. For Food Plants
example, Wyndham (2010) also found that When comparing the complete lists,
older children presented better naming the correlations of CSI values between
scores than younger children, and Hynes Indigenous boys and girls, mestizo boys
et al. (1997) found that older children and girls, Indigenous younger and older
(10–14 years old) from the montane forests children, and mestizo younger and older
of northwestern Argentina could identify children, were all significant (Spearman’s ρ
plants and their uses better than younger at 0.01; see Supplemental Tables 1 and 2).
children (6–9 years old). Similarly, Blacutt- However, the different groups diverged on

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


216 Cruz-Garcia, Caffi, Zans, and Sanchez-Choy

several of the species with high CSI values to easily eat the mucilage of this species
(i.e., CSI equal to or higher than 0.05). without any problems. M. egensis, which
Greater divergence was observed has a very small and sweet fruit, is mainly
between children from the same sex but cultivated for timber and hardly ever for
from different sociocultural groups than food. The consumption of this wild fruit
between children from different sex but the is not common among mestizos, i.e., M.
same sociocultural group (Table 5). Inga egensis was not listed among the wild food
edulis and Mangifera indica (mango) were plants consumed by mestizo adults in one
the only species with high salience for all of the study villages (Cruz-Garcia and Vael
children groups stratified by socio-cul- 2017). Conversely, Carica papaya (papaya),
tural group and sex (Figure 3). These trees Passiflora ligularis, and Theobroma cacao
are commonly present in home gardens, (cacao) had a high salience for mestizo but
where children play around them (and not for Indigenous boys and girls. Certainly,
with them), and their fruits are very easy their fruits are very popular among mestizo
to collect and eat. Pouteria caimito and families, as they are easy to eat and very
Micropholis egensis were among the most common in their diet. These species are
salient species for Indigenous but not cultivated mainly in home gardens by
for mestizo boys and girls. The fruit of P. mestizo families for family consumption,
caimito, which has a very sweet and tasty with the exception of T. cacao, which is
mucilage, requires a specific knowledge cultivated as a source of income. Mestizo
to be eaten. The pericarp has a resin that children like to eat the mucilage of T.
sticks to the lips giving a sensation that both cacao’s fruit, which mainly occurs during
lips would stick to each other, and people the harvesting season when they help their
usually make fun of this. Indigenous chil- parents to separate the seeds from the cob
dren might have developed a better ability (then their parents dry the seeds before

Figure 3. Venn diagrams comparing most salient species for Indigenous and mestizo children (n  114). Each
circle comprises the most salient species for a specific group of children as indicated above the circle. Most salient
species are those with a CSI of 0.05 or higher. Species in the intersection between two circles are those that were
the most salient for both groups of children; whereas species that are not within the intersection were only salient
for one of the two groups. A compares Indigenous boys and girls, B mestizo boys and girls, C Indigenous and
mestizo boys, and D Indigenous and mestizo girls.

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


Children’s Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-Agriculture Interface 217

Table 5. Percentage of agreement regarding the most salient plants listed by children (i.e., CSI equal or higher than
0.05), by sociocultural group, sex, and age.

Agreement regarding the most


salient plants
Sociocultural group and sex:  

Indigenous boys Indigenous girls 41%


Mestizo boys Mestizo girls 41%
Indigenous boys Mestizo boys 18%
Indigenous girls Mestizo girls 29%
Sociocultural group and age:  

Indigenous older Indigenous younger 35%


Mestizo older Mestizo younger 53%
Indigenous older Mestizo older 17%
Indigenous younger Mestizo younger 29%

Figure 4. Diagrams comparing most salient species for Indigenous and mestizo younger and older children (n 
114 children). Each circle comprises the most salient species for a specific group of children as indicated above
the circle. Most salient species are those with a CSI of 0.05 or higher. Species in the intersection between two
circles are those that were the most salient for both groups of children; whereas species that are not within the
intersection were only salient for one of the two groups. A compares younger and older Indigenous children, B
younger and older mestizo children, C younger Indigenous and mestizo children, and D older Indigenous and
mestizo children.

selling them for making chocolate). The Similarly, more differences were found
fruits of T. cacao are also gathered in the between children of the same age but from
forest (Cruz-Garcia and Vael 2017). Indig- different sociocultural groups, than between
enous communities do not cultivate T. children of different ages but from the same
cacao. sociocultural group (Table 5). Inga edulis

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


218 Cruz-Garcia, Caffi, Zans, and Sanchez-Choy

and Oenocarpus bataua were the only their consumption (i.e., fruits that are more
species with high salience for all children difficult to eat were more popular among
groups stratified by sociocultural group and Indigenous children), the environments
age (Figure 4). Inga edulis is very common where they are gathered, the livelihoods of
in home gardens and sometimes is grown Indigenous and mestizos (i.e., small-scale
as living fence. Children like to climb on and commercial agriculture, respectively),
and play around this tree and like to collect and their associated cultural values.
and eat the fruits while playing. Sometimes Certainly, Indigenous and mestizo peoples
children play with I. edulis’s long pods like in Ucayali possess different conceptualiza-
swords. Oenocarpus bataua, which is rich tions of their relationship with nature (see
in oils, is very popular among Indigenous Porro et al. 2015 for mestizos; Tournon 2006
and mestizo families in the region. Inga sp., for Shipibo-Konibo).
M. egensis, and Pourouma cecropiifolia
were the most salient species for younger Conclusions
and older Indigenous children, but not for The results of this study show that
mestizo children. Certainly, these fruits mestizo and Indigenous children in the
are highly appreciated among Indigenous Peruvian Amazon conceptualize wild
children. It is required to have some abil- food plants in a different way than scien-
ities to eat the mucilage of Inga species’s tists do, as most species listed as wild food
pods, and this might be more common plants have been classified as domesticated
among Indigenous children. In contrast by scientists. In addition, children listed
with I. edulis, which has longer pods and several introduced domesticates as wild
bigger seeds, the pods and seeds of Inga food plants. These findings might show
sp. are smaller and, as result, it is more that children have lost or failed to acquire
difficult to separate the edible mucilage knowledge about native non-domesticated
from the seeds. For that, children roll the food plants, potentially originating by
fruit against their teeth with their tongue to changes in the bodies of local knowledge
separate the seeds from the mucilage inside associated with landscape transformations
the mouth, and then spit out the seeds like in the forest-agriculture interface. We also
small bullets. Likewise, the consumption of found that sociocultural group, followed
P. cecropiifolia’s fruit also requires certain by age, was associated with variation in
abilities. The edible mesocarp is very slip- the theoretical knowledge of wild food
pery, thus, sometimes children peel the plants, whereas we did not find differ-
fruit and eat it directly from the peeled skin ences associated with the sex of the child.
with their mouths. Contrarily, C. papaya, For instance, on the one hand, mestizo
P. ligularis, and T. cacao were among the children presented statistically significant
most salient plants for younger and older longer lists than Indigenous children, but
mestizo children but not for Indigenous listed fewer native wild food plant species
children. These results mirror the findings and more introduced and commercial-
documented when comparing boys and ized crops. On the other hand, Indigenous
girls by socio-cultural group (see previous children—in contrast with mestizos—gave
paragraph). priority to species that require specific abil-
It is clear from these results that ities or knowledge to be consumed. These
inter-cultural differences—in contrast with differences might be related to different
intra-cultural differences—were the most preferences, gathering environments,
important factor for differentiating the livelihoods, access to markets, and—ulti-
salience of food plants. The salience of mately—different conceptualizations of
species was mainly related to specific their relationship with nature. The results of
knowledge or abilities that children have for this study open the doors for future inves-

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


Children’s Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-Agriculture Interface 219

tigations on knowledge transmission and Forest? Generational Change in Mapuche


factors affecting the transmission of wild Knowledge of Wild Edible Plants in Andean
food plant knowledge in scenarios of land Temperate Ecosystems of Chile. Journal of
use change along the forest-agriculture Ethnobiology 36:412–432.
interface. Benz, B. F., J. Cevallos E, F. Santana M, J. Rosales
A, and S. Graf M. 2000. Losing Knowledge
Acknowledgments About Plant Use in the Sierra De Manantlan
We would like to thank Paul Peters, Biosphere Reserve, Mexico. Economic
Botany 54:183–191.
who contributed with the organization of
the data, and Madelyn Hancco Carrion for Berlin, B. 1992. Ethnobiological Classification:
contributing to data collection. We would Principles of Categorization of Plants and
Animals in Traditional Societies. Princeton
like to extend our thanks to Paula Paz from
University Press, Princeton, NJ.
the Terra-i team from the International
Center for Tropical Agriculture (CIAT), who Berlin, B., and P. Stevens. 1994. Ethnobiological
Classification: Principles of Categorization
prepared the map and made the deforesta-
of Plants and Animals in Traditional Soci-
tion rate and forest cover calculations; and
eties. Systematic Biology 43:293–295.
to Hub Peters, who revised the English of
the manuscript. We are grateful to Victoria Bharucha, Z., and J. Pretty. 2010. The Roles
and Values of Wild Foods in Agricultural
Reyes-García, Sandrine Gallois, the Journal
Systems. Philosophical Transactions of the
editors, and three anonymous reviewers for Royal Society 365:2913–2926.
their valuable comments on earlier versions
Blacutt-Rivero, E., M. Moraes R, M. Gruca,
of the manuscript. We are indebted to the
and H. Balslev. 2016. Local Knowledge
school teachers who gave us the oppor- About Palms (Arecaceae) among Children
tunity to conduct the freelistings with the in Bolivia. Botanical Journal of the Linnean
students and to the children from Pueblo Society 182:505–516.
Libre, La Unión, Puerto Belén, and Junín
Borgatti, S. 1999. Elicitation Techniques for
Pablo who participated in the study. Cultural Domain Analysis. In Enhanced
This work was associated with the Ethnographic Methods: Audiovisual Tech-
Attaining Sustainable Services from niques, Focussed Group Interviews and
Ecosystems using Tradeoff Scenarios Elicitation Techniques, edited by J. J.
project (ASSETS; http://espa-assets.org/; Schensul, M. D. L. Compte, B. K. Nastasi,
NEJ002267-1), and partly funded with and S. P. Borgatti, pp. 115–151. Altamira
support from the United Kingdom’s Press, Walnut Creek, CA.
Ecosystem Services for Poverty Alleviation Casas, A., M. Del Carmen Vázquez, J. L. Viveros,
(ESPA) program. ESPA receives its funding and J. Caballero. 1996. Plant Management
from the Department for International among the Nahua and the Mixtec in the
Development (DFID), the Economic and Balsas River Basin, Mexico: An Ethnobotan-
Social Research Council (ESRC), and the ical Approach to the Study of Plant Domes-
Natural Environment Research Council tication. Human Ecology 24:455–478.
(NERC). This study was also carried out CIAT (International Center for Tropical Agricul-
with financial and in-kind support from the ture). 2015. Terra-i [website]. http://www.
Instituto de Investigaciones de la Amazonía terra-i.org/terra-i.html. Accessed on May
14, 2018.
Peruana (IIAP) and the International Center
for Tropical Agriculture (CIAT). Clement, C. R. 1999. 1492 and the Loss of
Amazonian Crop Genetic Resources. I.
The Relation between Domestication and
References Cited
Human Population Decline. Economic
Barreau, A., J. T. Ibarra, F. S. Wyndham, A. Rojas, Botany 53:188–202.
and R. A. Kozak. 2016. How Can We Teach
Our Children If We Cannot Access the

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


220 Cruz-Garcia, Caffi, Zans, and Sanchez-Choy

Clement, C. R., M. de Cristo-Araújo, G. Continente Americano. Volumen 2. Investi-


Coppens D’Eeckenbrugge, A. Alves Pereira, gación Y Manejo Sustentable De Recursos
and D. Picanço-Rodrigues. 2010. Origin Genéticos En El Nuevo Mundo, edited by
and Domestication of Native Amazonian A. Casas and F. J. P.-R. Torres-Guevara, pp.
Crops. Diversity 2:72–106. 327–344. Universidad Nacional Autonoma
Cruz-Garcia, G. S. 2006a. Children’s Knowl- de México and Universidad Nacional
edge and Valuation of Wild Food Plants. Agraria La Molina, Morelia, Mexico, and
The Influence of an Educational Program Lima, Peru.
with Tribal and Non-Tribal Children in Galarza, E., and K. La Serna. 2005. Las Conce-
Western Ghats, India. In Proceedings of siones Forestales En El Perú:¿ Cómo
the Fourth International Congress of Ethno- Hacerlas Sostenibles. Universidad del
botany (ICEB 2005) Ethnobotany: At the Pacifico, Lima.
Junction of the Continents and the Disci- Gallois, S. 2015. Dynamics of Local Ecological
plines, edited by F. Ertug, pp. 65–68. Zero Knowledge : A Case Study among the Baka
Prod. Ltd., Istanbul, Turkey. Children from Southeastern Cameroon.
Cruz-Garcia, G. S. 2006b. The Mother - Child 353 Instituto de Ciencia y Tecnología
Nexus. Knowledge and Valuation of Wild Ambiental. Universidad Autónoma de
Food Plants in Wayanad, Western Ghats, Barcelona, Barcelona.
India. Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethno- Gallois, S., R. Duda, and V. Reyes-García. 2017.
medicine 2:39. Local Ecological Knowledge among Baka
Cruz-Garcia, G. S. 2014. Symbol of Poverty? Children: A Case of “Children’s Culture”?
Children’s Valuation of Wild Food Plants Journal of Ethnobiology 37:60–80.
in Wayanad, India. In Plants and People. Giovannini, P., V. Reyes-García, A. Waldstein,
Choices and Diversity through Time, and M. Heinrich. 2011. Do Pharmaceu-
edited by A. Chevalier, E. Marinova, and ticals Displace Local Knowledge and
L. Peña-Chocarro, pp. 421–427. Oxbow Use of Medicinal Plants? Estimates from
Books, Oxford & Philadelphia. a Cross-Sectional Study in a Rural Indige-
Cruz-Garcia, G. S. 2017. Management and nous Community, Mexico. Social Science &
Motivations to Manage “Wild” Food Plants. Medicine 72:928–936.
A Case Study in a Mestizo Village in the Global Forest Watch. 2016. Global Forest
Amazon Deforestation Frontier. Frontiers in Watch. Available at: http://www.global-
Ecology and Evolution 5:127. forestwatch.org/. Accessed on November
Cruz-Garcia, G. S., and F. Ertug. 2014. Intro- 2015.
duction: Wild Food Plants in the Present González-Insuasti, M. S., and J. Caballero. 2007.
and Past. In Plants and People. Choices Managing Plant Resources: How Intensive
and Diversity through Time, edited by A. Can It Be? Human Ecology 35:303–314.
Chevalier, E. Marinova, and L. Peña-Cho-
carro, pp. 211–215. Oxbow Books, Oxford Grivetti, L. E., and B. M. Ogle. 2000. Value of
& Philadelphia. Traditional Foods in Meeting Macro- and
Micronutrient Needs: The Wild Plant
Cruz-Garcia, G. S., and P. L. Howard. 2013. Connection. Nutrition Research Reviews
‘I Used to Be Ashamed’. The Influence 13:31–46.
of an Educational Program on Tribal and
Non-Tribal Children’s Knowledge and Valu- Hansen, M., R. DeFries, J. R. Townshend, and R.
ation of Wild Food Plants. Learning and Sohlberg. 2000. Global Land Cover Classi-
Individual Differences 27:234–240. fication at 1 Km Spatial Resolution Using a
Classification Tree Approach. International
Cruz-Garcia, G. S., and L. Vael. 2017. El Journal of Remote Sensing 21:1331–1364.
Manejo De Plantas Silvestres Alimenti-
cias En Escenarios De Deforestación, Ilus- Heywood, V. 2011. Ethnopharmacology, Food
trado Por Una Comunidad Mestiza De La Production, Nutrition and Biodiversity
Amazonía Peruana. In Domesticación En El Conservation: Towards a Sustainable Future

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


Children’s Knowledge of Wild Food Plants in the Forest-Agriculture Interface 221

for Indigenous Peoples. Journal of Ethno- De Ordenamiento Territorial. Ministerio


pharmacology 137:1–15. de Energía y Minas (MINEM) y Gobierno
Hopkins, A. 2011. Use of Network Centrality Regional de Ucayali (GOREU), Pucallpa.
Measures to Explain Individual Levels Miranda, J. J., L. Corral, A. Blackman, G. Asner,
of Herbal Remedy Cultural Competence and E. Lima. 2014. Effects of Protected
among the Yucatec Maya in Tabi, Mexico. Areas on Forest Cover Change and Local
Field Methods 23:307–328. Communities: Evidence from the Peruvian
Hunn, E. 2008. A Zapotec Natural History: Amazon. Inter-American Development
Trees, Herbs, and Flowers, Birds, Beasts, Bank, Washington.
and Bugs in the Life of San Juan Gbëë. Missouri Botanical Garden. 2017. Topicos [web
University of Arizona Press, Tucson. page]. URL: http://www.tropicos.org/.
Hynes, A. L., A. D. Brown, H. R. Grau, and Accessed on June 1 2017.
A. Grau. 1997. Local Knowledge and the Müller, J. G., R. Boubacar, and I. D. Guimbo.
Use of Plants in Rural Communities in the 2015. The “How” and “Why” of Including
Montane Forests of Northwestern Argen- Gender and Age in Ethnobotanical
tina. Mountain Research and Development Research and Community-Based Resource
17:263–271. Management. Ambio 44:67–78.
INEI (Instituto Nacional de Estadística e Murray, T. P. 2006. Rhythm of the Rivers. An
Informática). 2011. Producto Bruto Interno Ecosystem Approach to Child Nutrition and
Por Departamentos, 2001–2010. Insti- Health on the Amazon Frontier. Unpub-
tuto Nacional de Estadística e Informática lished Doctoral Dissertation, College of
(INEI), Lima. Available at: https://www. Health and Science, University of Western
inei.gob.pe/media/MenuRecursivo/publi- Sydney, Richmond, NSW, Australia.
caciones_digitales/Est/Lib0995/Libro.pdf. Oliveira, P. J., G. P. Asner, D. E. Knapp, A.
Accessed on June 2017. Almeyda, R. Galván-Gildemeister, S.
ISE (International Society of Ethnobiology). Keene, R. F. Raybin, and R. C. Smith. 2007.
2006. International Society of Ethnobi- Land-Use Allocation Protects the Peruvian
ology Code of Ethics (with 2008 additions) Amazon. Science 317:1233–1236.
[website]. URL: http://ethnobiology.net/ Pimentel, L. V., E. B. Rengifo, and R. L. Porto-
code-of-ethics/. Accessed on May 4, 2018. misnski. 2004. The Great Encyclopedia
Jones, A. M. P., S. J. Murch, J. Wiseman, and D. of the Ucayali Region: Peruvian Amazon:
Ragone. 2013. Morphological Diversity in Regional Identity. El Comercio, Lima, Peru.
Breadfruit (Artocarpus, Moraceae): Insights Porro, R., A. Lopez-Feldman, and J. W. Vela-Al-
into Domestication, Conservation, and varado. 2015. Forest Use and Agriculture
Cultivar Identification. Genetic Resources in Ucayali, Peru: Livelihood Strategies,
and Crop Evolution 60:175–192. Poverty and Wealth in an Amazon Frontier.
Levis, C., F. R. Costa, F. Bongers, M. Peña- Forest Policy and Economics 51:47–56.
Claros, C. R. Clement, A. B. Junqueira, E. Posey, D. A. 1985. Indigenous Management of
G. Neves, E. K. Tamanaha, F. O. Figueiredo, Tropical Forest Ecosystems: The Case of the
and R. P. Salomão. 2017. Persistent Effects Kayapó Indians of the Brazilian Amazon.
of Pre-Columbian Plant Domestication on Agroforestry Systems 3:139–158.
Amazonian Forest Composition. Science
355:925–931. Quinlan, M. B., R. J. Quinlan, S. K. Council, and
J. W. Roulette. 2016. Children’s Acquisition
Michon, G., and H. De Foresta. 1997. Agrofor- of Ethnobotanical Knowledge in a Carib-
ests: Pre-Domestication of Forest Trees or bean Horticultural Village. Journal of Ethno-
True Domestication of Forest Ecosystems? biology 36:433–456.
Netherlands Journal of Agricultural Science
45:451–462. Ramos Delgado, N., and G. Domínguez
Torrejón. 2009. Impacto De La Produc-
MINEM-GOREU. 2007. Caracterización Del ción Forestal Maderable En La Economía
Departamento De Ucayali, Con Fines De La Región Ucayali, Perú. Universidad

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222


222 Cruz-Garcia, Caffi, Zans, and Sanchez-Choy

Nacional Agraria La Molina, Lima (Perú). Sutrop, U. 2001. List Task and Cognitive Salience
Escuela de Postgrado. Especialidad de Index. Field Methods 13:263–276.
Bosques y Gestión de Recursos Forestales. The Plant List. 2013. The Plant List. A Working
Reyes-García, V., J. Broesch, L. Calvet-Mir, N. List of All Plant Species. Version 1.1 [web
Fuentes-Peláez, T. W. McDade, S. Parsa, page]. URL: http://www.theplantlist.org.
S. Tanner, T. Huanca, W. R. Leonard, and Accessed on July 1, 2017.
M. R. Martínez-Rodríguez. 2009. Cultural Tournon, J. 2002. La merma mágica: vida e
Transmission of Ethnobotanical Knowledge historia de los shipibo-conibo del Ucayali.
and Skills: An Empirical Analysis from an Centro Amazónico de Antropología y Apli-
Amerindian Society. Evolution and Human cación Práctica, Lima.
Behavior 30:274–285.
Tournon, J. 2006. Las Plantas, Los Rao Y Sus
Reyes-García, V., J. Paneque-Gálvez, M. Gueze, Espíritus:(Etnobotánica Del Ucayali). Gobi-
A. C. Luz, M. J. Macía, M. Orta-Martínez, erno Regional Ucayali, Gerencia Regional
and J. Pino. 2014. Cultural Change and de Desarrollo Social.
Traditional Ecological Knowledge: An
Empirical Analysis from the Tsimane’ in the USDA (United States Department of Agricul-
Bolivian Amazon. Human Organization ture). 2017. Plants Database [web page].
73:162–173. URL: https://plants.usda.gov. Accessed on
July 1, 2017.
Reyes-García, V., V. Vadez, E. Byron, L. Apaza,
W. Leonard, E. Pérez, and D. Wilkie. 2005. Vásque, M. G., and F. P. Peláez. 2015. Especies
Market Economy and the Loss of Ethno- Vegetales Utilizadas Por Pobladores De
botanical Knowledge: Estimates from Berlín, Bagua Grande (Amazonas, Perú)
Tsimane’ Amerindians, Bolivia. Current 2011-2012. Revista REBIOLEST 2:61–75.
Anthropology 46:651–656. Wiersum, K. F. 1997. Indigenous Exploita-
Salisbury, D. S., and C. Fagan. 2013. Coca and tion and Management of Tropical Forest
Conservation: Cultivation, Eradication, and Resources: An Evolutionary Continuum
Trafficking in the Amazon Borderlands. in Forest-People Interactions. Agriculture,
GeoJournal 78:41–60. Ecosystems and Environment 63:1–16.

Schunko, C., S. Grasser, and C. R. Vogl. 2012. Wyndham, F. S. 2010. Environments of Learning:
Intracultural Variation of Knowledge About Rarámuri Children’s Plant Knowledge and
Wild Plant Uses in the Biosphere Reserve Experience of Schooling, Family and Land-
Grosses Walsertal (Austria). Journal of scapes in the Sierra Tarahumara, Mexico.
Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine 8:23. Human Ecology 38:87–99.

Setalaphruk, C., and L. L. Price. 2007. Chil- Young, K. 2002. Minding the Children: Knowl-
dren’s Traditional Ecological Knowledge edge Transfer and the Future of Sustainable
of Wild Food Resources. A Case Study in a Agriculture. Conservation Biology 16:855–
Rural Village in Northeast Thailand. Journal 856.
of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine 3:33. Zarger, R., and J. Stepp. 2004. Persistence of
Souto, T., and T. Ticktin. 2012. Understanding Botanical Knowledge among Tzeltal Maya
Interrelationships among Predictors (Age, Children. Current Anthropology 45:413–
Gender, and Origin) of Local Ecological 418.
Knowledge. Economic Botany 66:149–164.

Journal of Ethnobiology 2018  38(2): 205–222

View publication stats

You might also like