You are on page 1of 13

Current Psychology

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04343-4

The intrinsic and extrinsic factors predicting fake news sharing among
social media users: the moderating role of fake news awareness
Bahiyah Omar1  · Oberiri Destiny Apuke2 · Zarina Md Nor3

Accepted: 27 January 2023


© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2023

Abstract
Research on fake news is growing, yet the relative influence of different factors on fake news sharing and how it can be
reduced are still understudied. To fill this gap, this study treats user motivation and online environment as intrinsic and
extrinsic factors and examines the role of fake news awareness as a prevention against the spread of fake news. This study
describes the results of a Malaysian sample (N = 451) to determine the effects of intrinsic factor (altruism, information
sharing, socialization and status seeking) and extrinsic factor (trust in network, homophily, norm of reciprocity and tie
strength) on fake news sharing using Partial Least Square (PLS). Unlike past research, we treated the two main factors as
higher order-constructs. Our findings revealed a stronger appeal of online environment than user motivation in determining
fake news sharing among social media users in Malaysia. We also found that high fake news awareness determined low
fake news sharing. This result suggests the importance of fake news awareness as an intervention strategy to curtail the
spread of fake news. Future research is needed to build upon our findings to be tested at cross-cultural settings and also
employ time series analysis to better understand the effect of increasing awareness of fake news over time.

Keywords  Fake news · User motivation · Online environment · Fake news awareness · Social media users · Malaysia

Introduction verification and fake news detection tools (Tandoc et al.,


2018), enforcing strict laws and regulations (Roozenbeek
The ubiquity of social media has facilitated the spread of & van der Linden, 2019) and promoting digital literacy or
fake news (Jost et al., 2020) and the increasing reliance on fake news awareness in society to counter fake news spread
mobile devices for news seeking and information sharing (Torres et al., 2018).
has accelerated it further. Fake news, in the form of misin- The rise of fake news in modern times have attracted
formation and disinformation, has received much attention streams of research. These studies could be categorised into
and scrutiny among scholars and policy makers in recent three segments to include studies that focused on fake news
years. This is because the spread of fake news is considered detection using computer science approach, impact of fake
as an information disorder (Castioni et al., 2022; Wang et news on politics, science and society and lastly predictors of
al., 2019) that has social, political, and economic implica- fake news sharing behaviour (Laato et al., 2020). Most stud-
tions. Likewise, policy makers have been looking for the ies that examined fake news predictors relied on Uses and
solutions using various strategies such as developing news Gratification theory (UGT) to demonstrate how different
types of motives predict fake news sharing on social media
(Sampat & Raj, 2022; Laato et al., 2020). There are a few
studies that adopt multiple perspectives, such as a combi-
Bahiyah Omar
bahiyah@usm.my nation of UGT, social network site dependency theory and
social impact theory (Apuke & Omar, 2020), cognitive load
1
School of Communication, Universiti Sains Malaysia, and affordance theory (Islam et al., 2020; Apuke & Omar,
Penang 11800 USM, Malaysia 2021b) to understand the phenomenon but these studies
2
Department of Mass Communication, Taraba State treated the dimension of a factor individually and hence,
University, PMB 1167, Jalingo, Nigeria unable to show the relative influence of different factors on
3
School of Distance Education, Universiti Sains Malaysia, fake news sharing. This study aims to examine the effect
Penang 11800 USM, Malaysia

13
Current Psychology

of user motivation as intrinsic factor and online environ- to seek attention. Meanwhile, the extrinsic factor consists
mental as extrinsic factor on fake news sharing. Unlike past of trust in network, homophily, norm of reciprocity and tie
research, intrinsic and extrinsic factors are treated as higher strength. We define trust in network as the trust a person has
order constructs. Furthermore, the results of this study may on his/her network members. Homophily is the similarities
contribute to efforts aimed at reducing the harm of fake in network members that prompt them to freely share news.
news (Altay et al., 2022; Talwar et al., 2020; Laato et al., Norm of reciprocity is the belief that people will share back
2020). To the best of our knowledge, the role of fake news news, if we share it to them and tie strength is the level
awareness in mitigating the effect of fake news spread is of relationship strength that occurs online. The intrinsic
less well studied. This study intends to address this by treat- and extrinsic factors are derived from the uses and grati-
ing fake news awareness as the moderating variable which fication theory (U&G) and Social Capital Theory (SCT)
we expect to function as a preventive measure to curtail the respectively.
spread of fake news. Despite the growth of research examining fake news and
misinformation, little attention has been paid to develop-
Fake news studies ing countries, especially the Asian contexts (Balakrishnan
et al., 2021). This warrants for further exploration in this
In understanding fake news sharing, many past studies area. Research that appears in the literature mostly focus
have focused on user-related factors. They found various on the link between fake news and politics such as fake
individual factors that lead to fake news sharing on social news governance in Asia-Pacific (Neo, 2022), misinforma-
media, such as confirmation bias (Kim & Dennis, 2019), tion regulation in Asia-Pacific (Carson & Fallon, 2021), and
self-promotion, entertainment, exploration and religiosity fake news public opinion in Japan, South Korea and Thai-
(Islam et al., 2020), altruism, information sharing, informa- land (Cheng et al., 2021). We found very few studies that
tion seeking and passing time (Apuke & Omar, 2021a) and examined why people share fake news in Asian contexts.
deficiency of knowledge, laziness in authenticating infor- For example, Tandoc et al. (2021) found political bias and
mation source(s) and lack of critical thinking (Tsang, 2022). cognitive ability were the significant predictors to fake news
On the other hand, a growing body of studies examined sharing in Singapore and Talwar et al. (2019) found online
other factors apart from the users. Chen et al. (2015) found trust, self-disclosure and fear of missing out were the pre-
that content relevance and believability rather than source, dictors to fake news sharing behaviour in India. Some stud-
contributed to people forwarding misinformation. Other ies examined the spread of fake news related to COVID-19
studies found that information abundance on social media and found altruism, ignorance and entertainment as sig-
contributes to fake news spread (Tandoc & Kim, 2022). nificant predictors to fake news sharing in Malaysia dur-
Information abundance is often associated with informa- ing COVID-19 pandemic (Balakrishnan et al., 2021) while
tion overload which was found to be a significant predictor predictors of self-promotion, exploration, entertainment and
to COVID-19 unverified news sharing (Laato et al., 2020). religiosity were found significant in Bangladesh (Islam et
The results suggest different effects of user-related factors al., 2020). It is important to note that “most of the world’s
across different contexts. This is partly because past studies social media users reside in Asia” (Insider Intelligence,
tested the factors as single predictors rather than as a unified 2022), and hence more research using Asian lens is needed.
predictors to show the influence of user factor in relation to In doing so, we focused on a developing country which is
other factors. Malaysia. As internet penetration in Malaysia reached 88%
The current study aims to fill the gap by developing a in 2020 and is projected to grow to 89.6% in 2025 (Satista
research model that test the factors as higher order constructs Research Department, 2022), it is essential to understand
of intrinsic motivation (user related factors) and extrinsic what drives fake news sharing in one of the countries that
motivation (online environmental factors) to provide a more has high internet penetration rate in Asia. The findings of
comprehensive understanding of fake news spread on social this study, therefore, might speak to a more general pattern
media. For emphasis, the intrinsic factor comprises of altru- of the phenomenon at the global network.
ism, information sharing, socialization and status seeking.
We viewed altruism as users’ act of sharing information on Theoretical background and hypothesis
social media to help others without any thought of recipro- development
cal benefit. Information sharing deals with users’ motive to
inform others on social media. Socialization is the act of We combined Uses and Gratification Theory (UGT) and
sharing information to socialize with other users in the net- Social Capital Theory (SCT) to explain intrinsic (i.e.: user
work for unity and sociality. Status seeking deals with shar- motivation) and extrinsic (i.e.: online environment) factors
ing of information to uphold users’ status on social media for fake news sharing on social media. We argue that UGT

13
Current Psychology

does not capture the external factors that affect sharing, thus, The social capital dimensions used in this current study
this study includes assumptions of SCT to better understand comprised of tie strength, trust, reciprocity, and homophily.
the phenomenon beyond the user factor. Furthermore, this Past studies have used the social capital dimensions to test
study introduced fake news awareness as a moderator to information and knowledge sharing (Dias & Silva 2022),
mitigate the effects of the identifed predictors on online fake information seeking (Zhao et al., 2018) and news sharing
news sharing behaviour in Malaysia. (Al-Emran et al., 2020). We believe that since it has been
successfully used to test news sharing behaviour, it could as
The intrinsic user motivation: uses and gratification well be applicable to fake news sharing. Thus, we hypoth-
theory (UGT) perspective esised that:
H2. Online environment factor will be positively related
It has been proven that the UGT has been often used to exam- to fake news sharing behaviour.
ine social media gratifications, suggesting technologies are
employed by individuals to satisfy their social and psycho- Fake news awareness as the moderating factor
logical wishes (Katz et al., 1974). In summary, the theory
reasons what individuals do with the media rather than what Evidence suggests that much of the dissemination of fake
the media does to individuals (Katz et al., 1973). In recent news is due to human behaviour, since people who come
years the UGT has been widely adopted to realise the fun- across erroneous material on social media may negligently
damental motivations behind engaging with several techno- propagate it (Rodrigo et al., 2022). Therefore, it has been
logical platforms in different context and domain (Sampat & found that users are important players that could help in cur-
Raj, 2022; Osei-Frimpong et al., 2022). It has also been used tailing fake news, if their knowledge and ability to realise
to examine fake news, news sharing and knowledge sharing the presence of fake news is intensified (Aoun Barakat et al.,
behaviour (Apuke & Omar, 2021a; Thompson et al., 2019). 2021). It has also been revealed that social media users who
Drawing from the UGT and past studies, the user factor in lack critical thinking are vulnerable to fake news (Penny-
this study comprised of status seeking, altruism, socializa- cook & Rand, 2019). These studies generally agree that the
tion, and information sharing. Thus, we hypothesised that: ability to differentiate real news from false news is related
H1. User factor will be positively related to fake news to critical reasoning that makes people aware of fake news
sharing behaviour. and its grave implications in society.
In this study, we propose that adequate fake news aware-
The extrinsic online environmental factor: social ness could help in curtailing or dissuading people from shar-
capital theory perspective ing fake news. In this study, we define fake news awareness
as the practical understanding of what fake news is and the
According to social capital theory propounded by Cole- skills to detect it due to increasing amount of media literacy.
man (1988), people get rewarded from connecting with Past research also found that adequate skills and knowledge
others and such rewards has been extended to sharing on on fake news is essential to identify fake news when they
social media (Zhang & Jung, 2022). In summary, the theory come across it (Tandoc et al., 2018). Prior studies have also
asserts that knowledge and resources are exchanged lead- argued that media literacy intervention can empower social
ing to the creation of value via social relationship (Tóth media users to combat fake news and critical thinking is
et al., 2022). Therefore, social capital is entrenched in the crucial for them to debunk false information (Maksl et al.,
association between people and their linkage with their 2017). It was found that deductive reasoning will help people
societies (Wang et al., 2022). The social capital has three to differentiate between factual and deceptive content (Lee,
dimensions that include relational, cognitive, and structural 2018) and those with higher levels of fake news awareness
aspects (Nahapiet & Ghoshal, 1998). The structural dimen- and knowledge have higher tendency to verify information
sion pays attention to the general pattern of relationship that before sharing as well as before consuming and believing
occurs between people (Nahapiet & Ghoshal, 1998). For (Pundir et al., 2021). Nevertheless, there are opposite views
example, people with specific network ties could get access regarding the effectiveness of fake news awareness as an
to information in time compared to others. The relational intervention for fake news spread. For example, Papapicco
dimension entails trust, norms of reciprocity, identification et al. (2022) found that individuals share false informa-
and obligation (Nahapiet & Ghoshal, 1998).. The cognitive tion on social media even though they are aware of the act
dimension comprises shared language, code and narratives of sharing fake news. Information overload has also been
(Chang & Chuang, 2011). However, recent study has added pointed as a factor that makes people vulnerable unable to
homophily to this aspect (Apuke & Omar, 2021c). distinguish between truthful and actual news, suggesting

13
Current Psychology

Fig. 1  A higher-order structural model of fake news sharing

the role of fake news awareness might not be that effective constructs that affect fake news sharing while fake news
(Zhang et al., 2022). awareness is positioned as the moderator that mitigate the
There are mixed findings on the effects of fake news effects.
awareness. Drawing from these streams of arguments, we
position fake news awareness as moderating factor rather
than direct predictor to determine whether user factor and Methods
online environment have stronger effect on fake news shar-
ing for those with low fake news awareness. In short, we Research design and samples
intend to test and substantiate if awareness regarding fake
news will curtail the intrinsic and extrinsic influence that This study used a participatory method of crowdsourcing
prompt fake news sharing. Thus, we hypothesised that: survey response to collect information and opinion from a
H3. Fake news awareness will moderate the relationship large group of people (Keating et al., 2013) who are nor-
between user factor and fake news sharing in such a way mally sourced via the Internet. This method has several
that the relationship will be stronger for those with low fake advantages. Crowdsourced data collection is a more effi-
news awareness. cient and less expensive than traditional data collection
H4. Fake news awareness will moderate the relationship method that allows researchers to gather diverse data in
between online environment factor fake news sharing in real time (Keating et al., 2013). Using this method, we col-
such a way that the relationship will be stronger for those lected 600 responses over a period of two months. We set
with low fake news awareness. two inclusion criteria; respondents’ age must be above 18
Figure 1 demonstrates the structural model linking user years old, and they must be active social media users. Of
factor and online environmental factor as higher order the 600 responses, only 451 were considered valid while

13
Current Psychology

Table 1  Profile of respondents Measures


Characteristics Frequency Percentage (%)
Gender
All measures were adapted from prior studies except for
Male 290 64.3
fake news awareness which was newly developed in this
Female 161 35.7
study. We adapted the constructs of intrinsic and extrinsic
Age
18–25 226 50.1 factors based on theoretical backings and evidence from
16–35 150 33.3 past studies. Also, they have been statistically validated to
36–45 32 7.1 belong to the clusters of intrinsic and extrinsic motivations
46–55 24 5.3 as shown in this study. The constructs were measured using
56 and above 19 4.2 a 5-point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to
Education strongly agree (5). It should be noted that all constructs have
SPM and below 96 21.3 been defined in the introduction of this research. We adapted
Diploma 133 29.5 altruism from the study of Apuke and Omar (2021a), while
Bachelor’s degree 153 33.9 status seeking, information sharing, and socialization were
Postgraduate 43 9.5
adapted from Thompson et al. (2019). These studies sug-
Others 26 5.8
gest that the identified constructs have been shown to be
Frequently used SNS
salient in explaining user factor. Trust in network was
WhatsApp 237 52.5
Facebook 96 21.3
adapted from Apuke and Omar (2021c), norm of reciprocity
Twitter 30 6.7 was adapted from Liu et al. (2016), while tie strength and
Instagram 67 14.9 homophily were adapted from the study of Ma et al. (2014).
Others 21 4.7 These studies assert that trust in network, norm of reciproc-
Time ity, tie strength and homophily are reflective of online envi-
1–3 h 179 39.7 ronment factor that prompt sharing behaviour. Fake news
4–6 h 181 40.1 sharing items were adapted from the studies of Laato et al.
7–9 h 44 9.8 (2020) and Chadwick and Vaccari (2019). As for fake news
10 h and above 47 10.4 awareness measure, we deduced indicators of knowledge
Race and skills for detecting fake news from past research (Tor-
Malay 272 60.3
res et al., 2018). The indicators were later validated by an
Chinese 147 32.6
experts’ panel, then pre-tested on some social media users.
Indian 32 7.1
We adhered to the procedure of Moore and Benbasat (1991)
 N = 451
in developing the items for fake news awareness measure
which was further supported by statistical analyses of con-
150 cases were found to have straight lining issue. Straight vergent validity and discriminant validity carried out in this
lining pattern occurs when the respondents give the identi- study (see results section).
cal or almost identical feedbacks for most questions (Hair
Jr, et al., 2017). In this study, we used Microsoft Excel to Common method bias
examine the straight lining problem and all 150 cases were
excluded for further analysis. Since we obtained our entire responses from the same
As crowdsourced data collection method is associated source it is necessary to test for common method bias. To
with sampling bias, we used statistics of Malaysian popula- identify any potential bias, the Harman’s single factor test
tion distribution to guide our selection of sample. Table 1 was used, and a factor only accounted for 23.3% of the
shows the characteristics of the study respondents. Accord- largest variance explained, suggesting no issue of common
ing to the Department of Statistics Malaysia. (2021), the method bias since it is less than 50% (Ramayah et al., 2018).
breakdown comprises 52% males and 48% females with Following the recommendations of Kock and Lynn (2012),
69.7% between the ages of 15 and 64. Malaysia is a multi- common method bias was further tested with the full col-
racial country with three major races of Malay (69.8%), linearity test when a dummy variable was regressed against
Chinese (22.4%) and Indian (6.8%) respectively. Our study all the variables in the model and the results showed that all
sample comprised of 64.3% males and 35.7% females, with values were below the threshold of 3.3, suggesting that our
95% between the ages of 18 and 55 years old. Furthermore, study does not suffer from the issue of common method bias
race distribution in this study was almost identical with the (see Table 2).
distribution of Malaysian population with Malay 60.3%,
Chinese 32.6% and Indian 7.1%.

13
Current Psychology

Table 2  Full collinearity assessment Assessing the higher order constructs (formative
Variable Random Dummy variable constructs)
Altruism 1.753
Homophily 1.957
The reflective constructs and formative constructs do not
Information sharing 2.036
use the same method of assessment. In the formative con-
Norm of reciprocity 1.926
Socialization 2.455 structs, the convergent validity, collinearity issues and the
Status seeking 1.744 relevance and significance of the outer weights that formed
Tie strength 1.645 the formative constructs are assessed (Ramayah et al.,
Trust in network 1.448 2018). In the case where the outer weight is not significant,
researchers are encouraged to use the outer loading as far as
Data analysis it is above 0.50 (Ramayah et al., 2018). Firstly, we ran the
redundancy analysis using a single global item and realise
To validate our proposed model, we used the two-stage that we had no issues of convergent validity as the values
approach by testing the measurement model first followed of the path coefficient demonstrate a sufficient convergent
by the structural model (Hair et al., 2017) which is common validity were user factor (0.762) and online environment
in any study using Partial Least Square (PLS) analysis. We factor (0.845) that were above the recommended value of
chose PLS as it is suitable for a complex modelling com- 0.70 (Ramayah et al., 2018), (See Table  5). Furthermore,
prising of both reflective and formative constructs (Hair et multicollinearity was not an issue since all the outer VIF
al. Jr, 2017; Hair et al., 2019). In this study, we used the were below the cut-off point of 3.3 (Hair et al., 2019), (See
repeated indicator approach to examine the reflective-for- Table 6). Additionally, the outer weights showed a signifi-
mative HCM of user factor and online environment factor. cant value for the four dimensions of user factor as well as
This approach is suitable for the reflective formative type II online environment factors (p < 0.001), (See Table 7). This
modelling and also most appropriate when the lower order implies that the indicator weights were significant and rel-
constructs have similar number of indicators and the higher evant. Thus, their contributionsto their respective construct
order constructs are exogenous variables (Duarte & Amaro, were evident.
2018). The lower order reflective constructs which com-
prised of altruism, socialization, status seeking, information The structural model assessment
sharing, trust in network, homophily, norm of reciprocity
and tie strength were first assessed for reliability and valid- We followed the recommendation of Hair Jr et al. (2017) in
ity. Afterwards, the higher order formative constructs of assessing the structural model. Firstly, we tested structural
user factors and online environment factors was analysed model’s VIF since the computation of the path coefficient
considering the weights of all lower order components, that connects the constructs were based on regression analy-
multicollinearity and redundancy analysis. Subsequent sub- ses. This process helps researchers to ascertain if collinear-
headings provide a detailed explanation. ity issues does not taint the regression results. The predictors
VIF values did not exceed the threshold of 3.3, suggesting
Assessing the reflective lower order constructs no collinearity issues (See Table 8) (Hair Jr, et al., 2017).
Secondly, we assessed the relevance and significance of the
To evaluate the lower order reflective constructs, we structural model (beta, β), T-value, effect size (f2), predic-
checked the internal consistency, convergent and discrimi- tive relevance (Q2), and the coefficient of determination
nant validity (Ramayah et al., 2018). Internal consistency (R2). The bootstrapping method with a resampling of 5000
was measured using Cronbach’s alpha, composite reliability on a one tail test option was used to estimate the significance
(CR) and rho_A. Convergent validity was tested using fac- of the path coefficient.
tor loadings and average variance extracted (AVE). The dis-
criminant validity was gauged using Heterotrait-Monotrait
Ratio of Correlations (HTMT). In this view, we realised that Results
all the factor loadings except IS 3, IS5, FNA 1, 2, 11, and
12 were above the threshold value of 0.7. As such, loadings As shown in Table 8, user factor positively predicted fake
below 0.7 were deleted (See Table  3). Furthermore, AVE, news sharing (β = 0.228, p < 0.001), lending support to H1.
CR, CA and rho_A were above the recommended threshold Additionally, just as predicted, the online environmen-
of 0.5, 0.7, and 0.7, respectively. We realised no issue of tal factor also significantly predicted fake news sharing
discriminant validity since the HTMT values were all below behaviour (β = 0.408, p < 0.001). This supported H2 (See
0.85 (See Table 4). Fig.  2). With regards to effect size, the threshold of 002,

13
Current Psychology

Table 3  Convergent validity (Lower order reflective constructs)


Second-order construct Items Outer Loading CA rho_A CR AVE M SD
Altruism ALT1 0.742 0.832 0.835 0.881 0.598 4.135 0.722
ALT2 0.794 4.161 0.710
ALT3 0.796 4.053 0.784
ALT4 0.775 3.980 0.812
ALT5 0.759 3.847 0.869
Information Sharing IS1 0.859 0.766 0.767 0.865 0.682 4.086 0.750
IS2 0.840 4.204 0.704
IS3 (Deleted) 0.406 3.512 1.002
IS4 0.777 3.957 0.783
IS5 (Deleted) 0508 3.195 1.065
Socialization SOC 1 0.775 0.835 0.836 0.883 0.602 3.955 0.798
SOC 2 0.773 3.718 0.955
SOC 3 0.811 3.758 0.872
SOC 4 0.765 3.898 0.812
SOC 5 0.755 3.762 0.928
Status seeking SS1 0.741 0.872 0.876 0.907 0.663 3.547 1.038
SS2 0.845 3.281 1.106
SS3 0.855 3.095 1.116
SS4 0.776 3.090 1.104
SS5 0.848 3.259 1.081
Trust in Network TRN1 0.812 0.887 0.888 0.922 0.746 3.159 0.984
TRN2 0.749 3.141 0.992
TRN3 0.804 3.237 0.944
TRN4 0.872 3.310 0.955
Homophily HOMP1 0.812 0.881 0.881 0.918 0.737 3.549 0.906
HOMP2 0.883 3.454 0.963
HOMP3 0.868 3.396 1.008
HOMP4 0.869 3.430 0.973
Norm of reciprocity NORM 1 0.802 0.806 0.805 0.873 0.634 3.323 1.007
NORM 2 0.835 3.334 1.058
NORM 3 0.834 3.314 1.021
NORM 4 0.850 3.545 0.913
Tie strength TS1 0.839 0.751 0.751 0.858 0.668 3.638 0.869
TS2 0.763 3.476 0.952
TS3 0.765 3.722 0.867
Fake news sharing FNS1 0.747 0.934 0.937 0.946 0.686 3.217 1.085
FNS2 0.818 3.115 1.064
FNS3 0.859 2.988 1.114
FNS4 0.859 2.953 1.133
FNS5 0.861 3.006 1.140
FNS6 0.801 3.020 1.112
FNS7 0.822 2.827 1.193
FNS8 0.853 2.765 1.171
Fake news awareness FNA 1 (Deleted) 0.582 0.782 0.872 0.865 0.667 3.756 0.981
FNA 2 (Deleted) 0.235 3.756 0.981
FNA 3 0.783 3.756 0.981
FNA 4 0.821 3.756 0.981
FNA 5 0.795 3.756 0.981
FNA 6 0.812 3.756 0.981
FNA 7 0.798 3.756 0.981
FNA 8 0.777 3.756 0.981
FNA 10 0.882 3.756 0.981
FNA 11 (Deleted) 0.421 3.756 0.981
FNA 12 (Deleted) 0.422 3.756 0.981

13
Current Psychology

Table 4  Discriminant validity Heterotrait–Monotrait (HTMT)


1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Altruism
2. Fake news awareness 0.384
3. Fake news sharing 0.291 0.536
4. Homophily 0.440 0.402 0.496
5. Information sharing 0.778 0.319 0.170 0.462
6. Norm of reciprocity 0.278 0.465 0.548 0.675 0.282
7. Socialization 0.667 0.488 0.441 0.579 0.771 0.545
8. Status seeking 0.397 0.600 0.625 0.486 0.276 0.549 0.632
9. Tie strength 0.355 0.325 0.308 0.662 0.472 0.672 0.504 0.240
10. Trust in network 0.246 0.472 0.476 0.487 0.155 0.565 0.431 0.428 0.414

Table 5  Redundancy analysis (Convergent validity of formative con- effect size ranged from small to moderate. After realizing
structs) the effect size, we checked the cross-validated redundancy
Construct Global item Beta value measure (Q2) to evaluate the model’s predictive relevance
User factor USF 0.762 (Hair et al., 2017). This outcome showed the Q2 was above
Online environment factor ONF 0.845
zero (Q2 = 0.214 > 0), suggesting that our model has a good
predictive relevance. Overall, we realised that our model
Table 6  Collinearity test (formative constructs)
explains 42% of the variance in people’s fake news sharing
Higher order Lower order Outer VIF
behaviour (See Fig. 2). This variance is termed substantial
User factors Altruism 1.223
based on the rule of Cohen (1988) were 0.26, 0.13 and 0.02
Information Sharing 1.456
Socialization 2.224
is seen as substantial, moderate and weak level of predictive
Status seeking 1.567 accuracy. Additionally, Falk and Miller (1992) recommend
Online environment factors Trust in Network 1.689 that R2 values should be equal or greater than 0.10 in order
Homophily 1.345 for the variance explained of a given construct to be deemed
Norm of reciprocity 1.878 adequate.
Tie strength 1.112 The moderation hypotheses (H3 and H4) proposed that
the relationship between user factor and online environment
0.015 and 0.35 have been suggested as small, moderate factor with fake news sharing will be weaker when fake
and large effect (Cohen, 1988). The results suggest that the news awareness becomes higher. Results of the interaction

Table 7  The relevance and significance of the indicator’s weight


Higher order constructs Lower order constructs (formative indicators) Outer Weight value Std. Error t-value P-value 95% BCa CI
LB UUB
User factors Altruism 0.336 0.015 22.019 0.0000 0.312 0.363
Information Sharing 0.218 0.011 19.428 0.0000 0.201 0.238
Socialization 0.378 0.014 26.922 0.0000 0.357 0.403
Status seeking 0.333 0.023 14.311 0.0000 0.357 0.403
Online environment factors Trust in Network 0.336 0.016 21.221 0.0000 0.312 0.364
Homophily 0.379 0.014 27.587 0.0000 0.358 0.404
Norm of reciprocity 0.330 0.013 25.701 0.0000 0.311 0.354
Tie strength 0.228 0.011 20.215 0.0000 0.21 0.247

Table 8  Results of path coefficient, Q2 and f2


Relationship Beta Stan- T-value 95% BCa CI Q2 f2 VIF Decision
dard
error
LB UB
User Factor -> Fake News Sharing 0.228 0.048 4.750*** 0.149 0.305 0.214 0.069 1.478 Accepted
Online environment factor -> Fake News Sharing 0.408 0.045 9.031*** 0.327 0.475 - 0.154 1.467 Accepted
User factors *fake news awareness -> Fake news sharing -0.187 0.039 2.921** -0.141 -0.088 - 0.168 - Accepted
Online environment factors*fake news awareness -> Fake -0.285 0.039 4.816*** -0.241 -0.098 - 0.321 - Accepted
news sharing
* Significant at p < 0.05, ** at p < 0.01, and *** at p < 0.001

13
Current Psychology

Fig. 2  The structural model

Fig. 3  The moderating effect of fake news awareness on the relation-


Fig. 4  The moderating effect of fake news awareness on the relation-
ship between user factor and fake news sharing
ship between online factor and fake news sharing

term representing user factor and fake news awareness Discussions


was negatively significant (β= -0.187, p < 0.01). This sup-
ports H3. The second interaction terms of online environ- Our study contributes to literature as it suggests that both
ment factor*fake news awareness, with fake news sharing intrinsic and extrinsic factors have significant effects on
behaviour serving as endogenous variable, was negatively fake news sharing. Social media users are motivated to
significant (β= -0.285, p < 0.001) as well. This supports H4. share fake news because of their inner motives, as well as
Figures 3 and 4 further clarifies these results. the influence of the network members itself, viz. the online
environment. In this, we extend beyond studies that argue

13
Current Psychology

that fake news sharing should either be mainly understood role of fake news awareness in reducing fake news sharing.
via user motivation (Apuke & Omar, 2021a), or online envi- Hence, our emphasis is on educating the people to increase
ronmental factors (Duffy et al., 2019; Goh et al., 2019). fake news awareness which in turn reduce fake news shar-
Past studies tend to examine the multifaceted concept of ing on social media.
user motivation and online environment according to each
dimension that makes up the concept. To examine user Theoretical contributions
motivation, for example, past research examined multiple
dimensions of motivation such as information-seeking, self- Our study found that user motivation has significant influ-
representation, altruism, entertainment individually as a sin- ence on fake news sharing and hence the assumptions of
gle factor. Unlike past studies, we combined the dimensions UGT are still relevant to explaining the reasons for fake
to become a key factor representing intrinsic and extrinsic news spread in social media. Past studies have heavily
motivation. In doing so, we treated the key variables as the relied on UGT as they found consistent support for the
higher-order-constructs and established their measurement influence of user motivation on fake news sharing (Apuke
using PLS analysis. We found support for the effects of both & Omar, 2020, 2021a). In addition to this internal factor,
user motivation (intrinsic factor) and online environmental our study also found support for external factor as online
(extrinsic factor) on fake news sharing. environment effect on fake news sharing was found signifi-
When the results were compared, we found that online cant. Our findings are consistent with the assertions made
environmental factor has stronger influence than user moti- by past research, using SCT perspective, that trust in net-
vation in influencing fake news sharing. This means social work, homophily, norm of reciprocity and tie strength were
interaction in the online setting has motivated social media the predictors to fake news sharing (Duffy et al., 2019; Goh
users to share fake news and this factor contributed the most et al., 2019). Hence, our study found support for both UGT
in this study. Meanwhile, users’ motivation effect on fake and SCT. In addition, we treated fake news awareness as a
news sharing was low but significant. This discovery sug- moderating factor in order to uncover whether it can act as
gests that pointing to users factor alone as the predictors to an intervention to curtail the spread of fake news. The inclu-
fake news sharing is insufficient because the temptations to sion of fake news awareness extends the model for fake
share fake news are greater for the medium than the user. news sharing, and hence contribute to our understanding of
Our study found that the influence of fake news aware- the fake news predictors and the preventive measure.
ness is significant to mitigate the effects of user motivation
and online environment on fake news sharing. It was found Practical implications
that fake news sharing was lower for those who have high
fake news awareness while it was higher for those who have Based on these findings, we cannot emphasize enough the
low awareness. This implies that fake news awareness can importance of digital and information literacy, and this
significantly reduce fake news sharing on social media. must be introduced at early ages. A recent report suggests
Hence, fake news awareness can be seen as an effective that 44% of those between 8 and 11 years old and 87% of
measure to curtail the spread of fake news. those between 12 and 15 years old use social media sites
Educating the public is key to this prevention strategy since the majority of them own tablet and smartphones
(Ren et al., 2023). Research has shown support for the the by then (Chaffey, 2021). Hence, we suggest that digital
importance of digital and information literacy in educating and information literacy should be introduced in the early
the public (Nistor & Zadobrischi, 2022). Digital literacy is years of primary school curriculum. We realized that
a significant strategy to aid people to distinguish between digital literacy programmes at Malaysian schools mainly
accurate and false news (Soetekouw & Angelopoulos, 2022; focus on technology competency, especially on how to
Guess et al., 2020) and hence help curtail the spread of fake use computer technology and internet applications and
news (Lee, 2018). Some scholars use the term ‘information less attention is given on the competency to evaluate
literacy’ to signify the ability to obtain, understand, evalu- online information and content. We witness today that
ate, and use information (Tifferet, 2021). Studies found that digitalization of all sectors – including the education
information literacy increased the probability of people system – has become an important agenda for Malaysia
identifying false news (Tandoc & Kim, 2022; Jones-Jang and many other countries. As this development is ram-
et al., 2021) as the ability to detect fake news depends on pant at the global setting, our findings could be extended
their critical reasoning (Pennycook & Rand, 2019). This is to global context which shows similar upward trend for
regarded as a more powerful way of reducing fake news digital transformation. It is of great importance for our
spread than correcting misleading news (Lee, 2018). Our young generations, who are the global citizens, to be dig-
study supports these assertions. We found the significant itally literate - both at technical and content levels - to

13
Current Psychology

protect themselves from falling into the problematic fake Declarations


news sharing behaviour, and eventually to help curtail the
Conflict of interest  None declared.
spread when they move into adulthood.

Limitations References
This study has some limitations. Firstly, the findings were Al-Emran, M., Shaalan, K., & Hassanien, A. E. (2020). Effects of
derived from a one-off survey which has limitations when Facebook Personal News sharing on Building Social Capital in
it comes to drawing causal conclusions. Future research Jordanian Universities. In M. Al-Emran, K. Shaalan, & A. E.
Hassanien (Eds.), Recent advances in Intelligent Systems and
is needed to study fake news sharing across different time
Smart Applications (pp. 653–670). Cham: Springer. https://doi.
series to understand better how it is affected by increasing org/10.1007/978-3-030-47411-9
awareness of fake news over time. Secondly, Malaysia has Altay, S., Hacquin, A. S., & Mercier, H. (2022). Why do so few people
28.4  million Internet users as of 2021 (Statista Research share fake news? It hurts their reputation. New Media & Society,
24(6), 1303–1324.
Department, 2022) and hence a sample size of 451 used in
Aoun Barakat, K., Dabbous, A., & Tarhini, A. (2021). An empirical
this study is relatively small. Although it meets the require- approach to understanding users’ fake news identification on social
ment of Krejcie and Morgan formula calculated at 95% of media. Online Information Review, Vol. ahead(ahead-of-print),
CL and 5% of CI, future research may consider to increase No. ahead-of-print. https://doi.org/10.1108/OIR-08-2020-0333
Apuke, O. D., & Omar, B. (2020). Modelling the antecedent factors
the sample size which could somewhat reflect a large-scale
that affect online fake news sharing on COVID-19: the moderat-
study. Thirdly, we have to admit the limitation of our crowd- ing role of fake news knowledge. Health Education Research,
sourced data collection method that could lead to sampling 35(5), 490–503. https://doi.org/10.1093/her/cyaa030
bias which in turn lead to unrepresentativeness of sample. Apuke, O. D., & Omar, B. (2021a). Fake news and COVID-19: mod-
elling the predictors of fake news sharing among social media
Nevertheless, our sample distribution is almost similar to
users. Telematics and Informatics, 56, 101475. https://doi.
the distribution of Malaysian population which could mini- org/10.1016/j.tele.2020.101475
mize the problem of generalizability to a certain extent. Apuke, O. D., & Omar, B. (2021b). Social media affordances and
Fourthly, this study focuses on the Malaysian context. information abundance: enabling fake news sharing during the
COVID-19 health crisis. Health Informatics Journal, 27(3),
Although fake news sharing can be assumed as a universal
1–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/14604582211021470
phenomenon, future research can validate the findings by Apuke, O. D., & Omar, B. (2021c). What drives news sharing behav-
employing a cross-countries or cross-cultural study to bet- iour among social media users? A relational communication
ter understand this global threat. Lastly, this study asserted model from the social capital perspective. International Sociol-
ogy, 36(3), 339–361. https://doi.org/10.1177/0268580920961323
that fake news awareness could act as an effective measure
Balakrishnan, V., Ng, K. S., & Rahim, H. A. (2021). To share or not
to curb fake news spread. Hence, future research is needed to share – the underlying motives of sharing fake news amidst
to study fake news sharing across different time series to the COVID-19 pandemic in Malaysia. Technology in Society, 66,
understand better the effect of an increasing awareness of 101676. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.techsoc.2021.101676
Castioni, P., Andrighetto, G., Gallotti, R., Polizzi, E., & De Domenico,
fake news over time.
M. (2022). The voice of few, the opinions of many: evidence
of social biases in Twitter COVID-19 fake news sharing. Royal
Society Open Science, 9(10), 220716.
Conclusion Carson, A., & Fallon, L. (2021). Fighting fake news: a study of
online misinformation regulation in the Asia Pacific LaTrobe
University https://opal.latrobe.edu.au/articles/report/Fighting_
Our study provides insights into possible solutions to Fake_News_A_Study_of_Online_Misinformation_Regulation_
tackle the growing threat of fake news. By implica- in_the_Asia_Pacific/14038340
tion, both intrinsic and extrinsic factors predicted fake Chadwick, A., & Vaccari, C. (2019). News Sharing on UK Social
Media. http://tiny.cc/hyn3lz
news sharing, and the latter has greater influence than
Chaffey, D. (2021). Our compilation of the latest social media statis-
the former. It is also concluded that fake news awareness tics of consumer adoption and usage of social networking plat-
reduces fake news sharing on social media. While this forms. https://www.smartinsights.com/social-media-marketing/
study supports the view that fake news proliferation is social-media-strategy/new-global-social-media-research/
Chang, H. H., & Chuang, S. (2011). Information & Management
due to human behaviour, it also suggests that people are
Social capital and individual motivations on knowledge sharing:
the key players to stop the spread of fake new by having participant involvement as a moderator. Information & Manage-
adequate knowledge and skills in detecting fake news on ment, 48(1), 9–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.im.2010.11.001
social media. Chen, X., Sin, S. C. J., Theng, Y. L., & Lee, C. S. (2015). Why stu-
dents share misinformation on Social Media: motivation, gender,
and study-level differences. Journal of Academic Librarianship,
Acknowledgements  This study acknowledges the support received
41(5), 583–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acalib.2015.07.003
from Universiti Sains Malaysia, Research University (RU) Grant
(1001/PCOMM/8016112).

13
Current Psychology

Cheng, J. W., Mitomo, H., Kamplean, A., & Seo, Y. (2021). Lesser Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1973). Uses and Gratifica-
evil? Public opinion on regulating fake news in Japan, South tions Research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 37(4), 509. https://doi.
Korea, and Thailand–A three-country comparison.Telecom- org/10.1086/268109
munications Policy, 45(9),102185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1974). The uses and grati-
telpol.2021.102185 fications approach to mass communication. Beverly Hills, Calif:
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci- Sage Publications.
ences (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Keating, M., Rhodes, B., & Richards, A. (2013). Crowdsourcing: a
Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. flexible method for innovation, data collection, and analysis in
American Journal of Sociology, 94(1988), S95–S120. social science research. In C.A. Hill, E. Dean & J. Murphy (Eds.),
Department of Statistics Malaysia (2021). Current population esti- Social media, sociality, and survey research, (pp.  179–201).
mates, Malaysia, 2021. https://www.dosm.gov.my/v1/index. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118751534.ch8
php?r=column/cthemeByCat&cat=155&bul_id=ZjJOSnpJR21s Kim, A., & Dennis, A. R. (2019). Says who? The effects of presenta-
QWVUcUp6ODRudm5JZz09&menu_id=L0pheU43NWJwRW tion format and source rating on fake news in social media. MIS
VSZklWdzQ4TlhUUT09 Quarterly: Management Information Systems, 43(3), 1025–1039.
Dias, G. P., & Silva, M. (2022). Revealing performance factors for https://doi.org/10.25300/MISQ/2019/15188
supply chain sustainability a systematic literature review from Kock, N., & Lynn, G. S. (2012). Lateral collinearity and misleading
a social capital perspective. Brazilian Journal of Operations & results in variance-based SEM: an illustration and recommenda-
Production Management, 19(1), 1–18. tions. Journal of the Association for Information Systems, 13(7),
Duarte, P., & Amaro, S. (2018). Methods for modelling reflective-for- 546–580.
mative second order constructs in PLS: an application to online Laato, S., Islam, A. K. M. N., Islam, M. N., & Whelan, E. (2020). What
travel shopping. Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Technology, drives unverified information sharing and cyberchondria during
9(3), 295–313. https://doi.org/10.1108/JHTT-09-2017-0092 the COVID-19 pandemic? European Journal of Information Sys-
Duffy, A., Tandoc, E., & Ling, R. (2019). Too good to be true, too good tems, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/0960085X.2020.1770632
not to share: the social utility of fake news. Information Com- Lee, N. M. (2018). Fake news, phishing, and fraud: a call for research
munication and Society, 0(0), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/136 on digital media literacy education beyond the classroom. Com-
9118X.2019.1623904 munication Education, 67(4), 460–466. https://doi.org/10.1080/0
Falk, R. F., & Miller, N. B. (1992). A primer for soft modeling. Uni- 3634523.2018.1503313
versity of Akron Press. Liu, L., Cheung, C. M. K., & Lee, M. K. O. (2016). An empirical
Goh, D., Ling, R., Huang, L., & Liew, D. (2019). News sharing as investigation of information sharing behavior on social com-
reciprocal exchanges in social cohesion maintenance. Informa- merce sites. International Journal of Information Management,
tion Communication and Society, 22(8), 1128–1144. https://doi. 36(5), 686–699. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2016.03.013
org/10.1080/1369118X.2017.1406973 Ma, L., Lee, C. S., & Goh, D. H. L. (2014). Understanding news shar-
Guess, A. M., Lerner, M., Lyons, B., Montgomery, J. M., Nyhan, B., ing in social media: an explanation from the diffusion of innova-
Reifler, J., & Sircar, N. (2020). A digital media literacy interven- tions theory. Online Information Review, 38(5), 598–615. https://
tion increases discernment between mainstream and false news in doi.org/10.1108/OIR-10-2013-0239
the United States and India. Proceedings of the National Acad- Maksl, A., Craft, S., Ashley, S., & Miller, D. (2017). The Usefulness of
emy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(27), 15536– a News Media Literacy Measure in Evaluating a News Literacy
15545. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1920498117 Curriculum. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077695816651970
Hair, J. F. Jr., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C., & Sarstedt, M. (2017). A Moore, G. C., & Benbasat, I. (1991). Development of an instrument
primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling to measure the perceptions of adopting an Information Technol-
(PLS-SEM) (2nd ed.). USA: Sage Publications. ogy Innovation. Information Systems Research, 2(3), 192–222.
Hair, J. F., Ringle, C. M., Gudergan, S. P., Fischer, A., Nitzl, C., & https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.2.3.192
Menictas, C. (2019). Partial least squares structural equation Nahapiet, J., & Ghoshal, S. (1998). Social Capital, Intellectual Capital,
modeling-based discrete choice modeling: an illustration in mod- and the organizational advantage. The Academy of Management
eling retailer choice. Business Research, 12(1), 115–142. https:// Review, 23(2), 242. https://doi.org/10.2307/259373
doi.org/10.1007/s40685-018-0072-4 Neo, R. (2022). When would a state crack down on fake news?
Insider Intelligence (2022). What social media networks are used Explaining variation in the governance of fake news in Asia-
in Asia Pacific? https://www.insiderintelligence.com/charts/ Pacific.Political Studies Review, 20(3),390–409. https://doi.
social-media-networks-in-asia-pacific/#:~:text=Most%20of%20 org/10.1177/147892992110139
the%20world's%20social,called%20the%20Asia-Pacific%20 Nistor, A., & Zadobrischi, E. (2022). The influence of fake news on
region Social Media: Analysis and Verification of web content during the
Islam, A. K. M. N., Laato, S., Talukder, S., & Sutinen, E. (2020). COVID-19 pandemic by Advanced Machine Learning Methods
Misinformation sharing and social media fatigue during COVID- and Natural Language Processing. Sustainability, 14(17), 10466.
19: an affordance and cognitive load perspective. Technological Osei-Frimpong, K., Otoo, B. A. A., McLean, G., Islam, N., & Soga, L.
Forecasting and Social Change, 159(July), 120201. https://doi. R. (2022). What keeps me engaging? A study of consumers’ con-
org/10.1016/j.techfore.2020.120201 tinuous social media brand engagement practices. Information
Jones-Jang, S. M., Mortensen, T., & Liu, J. (2021). Does media lit- Technology & People. (ahead-of-print).
eracy help identification of fake news? Information literacy helps, Papapicco, C., Lamanna, I., & D’Errico, F. (2022). Adolescents’ vul-
but other Literacies don’t. American Behavioral Scientist, 65(2), nerability to fake news and to racial hoaxes: a qualitative analysis
371–388. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764219869406 on italian sample. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction, 6(3),
Jost, P. J., Pünder, J., & Schulze-Lohoff, I. (2020). Fake news - 20.
does perception matter more than the truth? Journal of Behav- Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2019). Lazy, not biased : Susceptibil-
ioral and Experimental Economics, 85(3), 101513. https://doi. ity to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning
org/10.1016/j.socec.2020.101513 than by motivated reasoning. Cognition, 188(June 2018), 39–50.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.011

13
Current Psychology

Pundir, V., Devi, E. B., & Nath, V. (2021). Arresting fake news sharing Thompson, N., Wang, X., & Daya, P. (2019). Determinants of News
on social media: a theory of planned behavior approach. Manage- sharing Behavior on Social Media. Journal of Computer Informa-
ment Research Review, ahead-of-p(ahead-of-print). https://doi. tion Systems, 00(00), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/08874417.201
org/10.1108/MRR-05-2020-0286 9.1566803
Ramayah, T., Cheah, J., Chuah, F., Ting, H., & Memon, M. A. (2018). Tifferet, S. (2021). Verifying online information: development and val-
Partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) idation of a self-report scale. Technology in Society, 67, 101788.
using smartPLS 3.0. In In An updated guide and practical guide https://doi.org/10.1016/j.techsoc.2021.101788
to statistical analysis. Pearson. Torres, R. R., Gerhart, N., & Negahban, A. (2018). Epistemology
Ren, Z. B., Dimant, E., & Schweitzer, M. (2023). Beyond belief: how in the era of fake news: an exploration of information verifica-
social engagement motives influence the spread of conspiracy the- tion behaviors among social networking site users. Data Base
ories. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 104, 104421. for Advances in Information Systems, 49(3), 78–97. https://doi.
Rodrigo, P., Arakpogun, E. O., Vu, M. C., Olan, F., & Djafarova, E. org/10.1145/3242734.3242740
(2022). Can you be Mindful? The Effectiveness of Mindfulness- Tóth, Z., Nemkova, E., Hizsák, G., & Naudé, P. (2022). Social capital
Driven Interventions in Enhancing the Digital Resilience to Fake creation on professional sharing economy platforms: the prob-
News on COVID-19. Information Systems Frontiers, 1–21. lems of rating dependency and the non-transferability of social
Roozenbeek, J., & van der Linden, S. (2019). The fake news game: capital. Journal of Business Research, 144, 450–460.
actively inoculating against the risk of misinformation. Journal Tsang, S. J. (2022). Biased, not lazy: assessing the effect of COVID-
of Risk Research, 22(5), 570–580. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669 19 misinformation tactics on perceptions of inaccuracy and fake-
877.2018.1443491 ness. Online Media and Global Communication, 1(3), 469–496.
Sampat, B., & Raj, S. (2022). Fake or real news? Understanding Wang, N., Wang, L., Ma, Z., & Wang, S. (2022). From knowledge
the gratifications and personality traits of individuals sharing seeking to knowledge contribution: a social capital perspective on
fake news on social media platforms. Aslib Journal of Infor- knowledge sharing behaviors in online Q&A communities. Tech-
mation Management, 74(5), 840–876. https://doi.org/10.1108/ nological Forecasting and Social Change, 182, 121864.
AJIM-08-2021-0232 Wang, Y., Mckee, M., Torbica, A., & Stuckler, D. (2019). Social Sci-
Soetekouw, L., & Angelopoulos, S. (2022). Digital Resilience Through ence & Medicine systematic literature review on the spread of
Training Protocols: Learning To Identify Fake News On Social Health-related misinformation on Social Media. Social Science
Media. Information Systems Frontiers, 1–17. & Medicine, 240(September), 112552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Statista Research Department (2022). Internet user penetration Malay- socscimed.2019.112552
sia 2010–2025. https://www.statista.com/statistics/975058/ Zhang, C., Cao, T., & Ali, A. (2022). Investigating the Role of Per-
internet-penetration-rate-in-malaysia/ ceived Information Overload on COVID-19 Fear: A Moderation
Talwar, S., Dhir, A., Kaur, P., Zafar, N., & Alrasheedy, M. (2019). Why Role of Fake News Related to COVID-19. Frontiers in psychol-
do people share fake news? Associations between the dark side ogy, 13.
of social media use and fake news sharing behavior. Journal of Zhang, L., & Jung, E. H. (2022). How does WeChat’s active engage-
Retailing and Consumer Services, 51(September), 72–82. https:// ment with health information contribute to psychological well-
doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2019.05.026 being through social capital? Universal Access in the Information
Talwar, S., Dhir, A., Singh, D., Virk, G. S., & Salo, J. (2020). Sharing of Society, 21(3), 657–673.
fake news on social media: application of the honeycomb frame- Zhao, J., Zhu, C., Peng, Z., Xu, X., & Liu, Y. (2018). User willing-
work and the third-person effect hypothesis. Journal of Retailing ness toward knowledge sharing in social networks. Sustainability
and Consumer Services, 57, 102197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. (Switzerland), 10(12), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.3390/su10124680
jretconser.2020.102197
Tandoc, E. C. Jr., & Kim, H. K. (2022). Avoiding real news, believing Publisher’s Note  Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to juris-
in fake news? Investigating pathways from information overload dictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
to misbelief. Journalism, 14648849221090744.
Tandoc, E. C., Lee, J., Chew, M., Tan, F. X., & Goh, Z. H. (2021). Fall- Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds
ing for fake news: the role of political bias and cognitive ability. exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the
Asian Journal of Communication, 31(4),237–253. https://doi.org/ author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted
10.1080/01292986.2021.1941149 manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of
Tandoc, E. C., Ling, R., Westlund, O., Duffy, A., Goh, D., & Wei, Z., L such publishing agreement and applicable law.
(2018). Audiences’ acts of authentication in the age of fake news:
a conceptual framework. New Media and Society, 20(8), 2745–
2763. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444817731756

13

You might also like