You are on page 1of 11

Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Early Childhood Research Quarterly

Individual differences in basic arithmetic skills in children with and


without developmental language disorder: Role of home numeracy
experiences
Tijs Kleemans ∗ , Eliane Segers, Ludo Verhoeven
Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Montessorilaan 3, P.O. Box 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The present study investigated the role of kindergarten home numeracy experiences in predicting indi-
Received 12 May 2017 vidual differences in second-grade basic arithmetic (i.e., small problem sizes and large problem sizes)
Received in revised form among children with developmental language disorders (DLD) and their peers with normal language
23 November 2017
achievement (NLA), after controlling for kindergarten cognitive and linguistic capacities and first-grade
Accepted 17 January 2018
Available online 9 February 2018
basic arithmetic. Forty children with DLD and 103 children with NLA were tested on cognitive, linguistic,
and basic arithmetic skills, and their parents filled in a questionnaire on home numeracy activities and
numeracy expectations. The results showed that children with DLD scored below their peers with NLA on
Keywords:
Basic arithmetic skills basic arithmetic skills, with larger group differences on arithmetic with large problem sizes than small
Home numeracy experiences problem sizes. Furthermore, for both groups of children, home numeracy experiences were positively
Parent–child activities related to arithmetic with large, but not small problem sizes, suggesting that the role of home numeracy
Parents’ expectations in basic arithmetic might be restricted to those arithmetic skills that children have not fully mastered
Developmental language disorder yet.
© 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction and this has been found to impact their basic arithmetic skills in
first grade (Kleemans, Segers, & Verhoeven, 2013). In second grade,
Experiences at home impact children’s academic abilities. This arithmetic starts to rely more on phonological working memory
goes for both home literacy experiences (Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014) as well as language-based fact retrieval, and the impact of lin-
as well as home numeracy experiences (Skwarchuk, Sowinski, & guistic skills on arithmetic in this phase is well established (e.g.,
LeFevre, 2014). Children with learning problems may have a dual Durkin et al., 2013; LeFevre, Fast et al., 2010). It remains unclear,
risk; besides having a learning problem, their home environment however, to what extent home numeracy experiences continue to
(in terms of informal learning experiences) is often below that play a role in predicting individual differences in basic arithmetic
of children without learning problems (Martin, Volkmar, & Lewis, skills in second grade. In the present study, we investigated this in
2007). With regard to the development of numeracy, children with a longitudinal design, in which children with DLD and NLA were
developmental language disorder (DLD) form an interesting group. compared.
These children show significant delays within the domains of recep-
tive and/or expressive language, as well as phonological working 1.1. Child factors of basic arithmetic skills
memory skills (Bishop, Snowling, Thompson, & Greenhalgh, 2016),
which hinders them to form adequate representations of count- Basic arithmetic skills comprise the addition and subtraction
ing words and number facts (Fazio, 1994, 1996). As a consequence, of single digits (i.e., 1–9) in first and second grades, and are pre-
they also lag behind in early numeracy and basic arithmetic skills ceded by the acquisition of early numeracy skills in kindergarten
(e.g., Durkin, Mok, & Conti-Ramsden, 2013). Furthermore, the home (Desoete & Grégoire, 2006; Jordan, Kaplan, Locuniak, & Ramineni,
numeracy experiences of children with DLD have been found to 2007). Evidence from neurocognitive research suggests that a dis-
be poorer than that of normal language achieving (NLA) children, tinction can be made between arithmetic with small problem sizes
(i.e., arithmetic with sums and minuends below 10) and arithmetic
with large problem sizes (i.e., arithmetic with sums and minuends
∗ Corresponding author. above 10 and less than 20, including carryover operations) (e.g.,
E-mail address: m.kleemans@bsi.ru.nl (T. Kleemans). Stanescu-Cosson et al., 2000). The solution process of arithmetic

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.01.005
0885-2006/© 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72 63

with small problem sizes relies on having access to arithmetic facts et al., 2012); a better naming speed might reduce the problems in
that are stored in long-term memory (De Smedt, Taylor, Archibald, basic arithmetic skills.
& Ansari, 2010). For arithmetic with large problem sizes, phono-
logical working memory (i.e., phonological loop) is required as 1.2. Home factors of basic arithmetic skills
well, as such problems exceed the 10, and a solution procedure
needs to be carried out before the correct answer can be given (e.g., Not only child factors predict learning outcomes in the early
7 + 5 = 7 + 3 = 10 + 2 = 12). This solution procedure relies on verbal years of primary education, but individual differences in the home
strategies that may vary from using verbal counting (e.g., seven environment are also related to academic success (Melhuish et al.,
plus three is “seven,”,’ “eight,” “nine,” “ten”) to temporarily main- 2008). It is well established that home literacy experiences are asso-
taining arithmetic facts during the solution process (e.g., seven plus ciated with children’s literacy skills (see e.g., Sénéchal & LeFevre,
three is ten), which are both assumed to rely on phonological codes 2014; Sénéchal & Young, 2008). In a similar way, home numer-
(Dehaene, Piazza, Pinel, & Cohen, 2003) and are supported by the acy experiences relate to children’s numeracy skills (Blevins-Knabe,
phonological loop (Lee & Kang, 2002). 2016; Huntsinger, Jose, & Luo, 2016; Skwarchuk et al., 2014). Home
From the literature on individual differences in basic arithmetic numeracy experiences can be seen as a multi-componential, but
skills in children with DLD and NLA, a theoretical framework can interrelated construct that facilitate numeracy learning. The two
be derived that includes cognitive as well as linguistic child factors main components of home numeracy experiences are parent–child
that influence the development of these skills (see e.g., Kleemans, numeracy activities and parents’ numeracy expectations.
Segers, & Verhoeven, 2016; LeFevre, Fast et al., 2010). To begin With respect to parent–child numeracy activities, LeFevre,
with, both general intelligence and working memory as cogni- Clarke, and Stringer (2002) found the frequency of parent–child
tive factors have been found to predict the development of basic numeracy activities to be positively related with the counting
arithmetic skills. The role of general intelligence in basic arith- skills of French- and English-speaking Canadian children. Compa-
metic skills has been identified in both children with DLD (Cowan rable results have been found in other cultures (e.g., Turkish- and
et al., 2005) and children with NLA (De Smedt et al., 2009; Stock, Chinese-speaking children) as well (Cankaya & LeFevre, 2016): the
Desoete, & Roeyers, 2009). For working memory, three related higher the frequency of parent–child numeracy activities, the better
systems can be identified (Baddeley, 1996): one is a primary sys- the numeracy skills of the children. Furthermore, Skwarchuk (2009)
tem (i.e., central executive functioning) that is responsible for the showed that parents’ involvement in activities with basic and com-
control and regulation of cognitive processes, and two slave sys- plex numeracy goals was positively related to children’s early
tems that are involved in processing verbal (i.e., phonological loop) numeracy skills. Longitudinal relationships between parent–child
and visual–spatial information (i.e., visual–spatial sketchpad). In numeracy activities and later basic arithmetic have also been
a meta-analysis by Friso-van den Bos, Kroesbergen, Van der Ven, reported. To begin with, in a sample of 609 German kinder-
and Van Luit (2013), it was found that the central executive (as gartners, Niklas and Schneider (2013) showed that the reported
measured by a backward digit span) was a strong predictor of math- frequency of parental engagement in numeracy activities at home
ematical achievement throughout primary school, whereas the role was positively associated with basic arithmetic skills one year
of the slave systems was related to children’s strategy use. To be later. Furthermore, Huntsinger et al. (2000) reported the frequency
more precise, the visual–spatial sketchpad was found to be asso- of parent–child numeracy activities to be positively related to
ciated with finger counting (Rasmussen & Bisanz, 2005), whereas later basic arithmetic. LeFevre et al. (2009) found not only simi-
the phonological loop was found to be related to more mature lar results, but also controlled for cognitive (i.e., spatial memory)
strategies that relied on verbal counting or fact retrieval (De Smedt, and linguistic (i.e., vocabulary) child factors. Although most studies
Holloway, & Ansari, 2011; Hecht, Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, found positive relationships between the frequency of parent–child
2001; Siegler, 1996). numeracy activities and basic arithmetic skills, negative relation-
Next to cognitive factors, linguistic factors such as phonologi- ships have been reported in both cross-sectional (Blevins-Knabe
cal awareness and grammatical ability also predict the acquisition & Musun-Miller, 1996) and longitudinal studies (Ciping, Silisinkas,
of basic arithmetic. Phonological awareness was uniquely related Wei, & Georgiou, 2015) as well. These negative relationships were
to small additions and small subtractions in both children with explained by the fact that when children are starting to lag behind at
NLA (Hecht et al., 2001; Simmons & Singleton, 2008) and children school, parents may increase the frequency of numeracy activities
with DLD (Kleemans, Segers, & Verhoeven, 2012), as the quality of in the home (Ciping et al., 2015).
verbal codes (e.g., counting words) relies on phonological repre- With respect to parents’ numeracy expectations, it was found
sentations that are stored in long-term memory (De Smedt et al., that higher expectations on the part of the parent resulted in more
2010). Furthermore, grammatical ability and basic arithmetic skills numeracy-related practices, which was related to better numeracy
are based on the same underlying rules and develop on the princi- achievement on the part of the child (LeFevre, Polyzoi, Skwarchuk,
ple of recursion (e.g., Hauser, Chomsky, & Fitch, 2002). For example, Fast, & Sowinksi, 2010). A more recent line of research combined
the order of words in a sentence determines its meaning (“The boy both components of home numeracy experiences to determine
chased the dog” vs. “The dog chased the boy”), which is also the the unique role of each factor in predicting numerical abilities.
case in the order of the numbers and operations in an arithmetical Cross-sectional evidence has been provided by Kleemans, Peeters
problem (“15 − 8 = ” vs. “8 − 15 = ”). Grammatical ability has been et al. (2012), who found both aspects to be positively related with
found to predict single-digit addition and subtraction problems the early numeracy skills of 89 typically developing kindergart-
(i.e., small and large problem sizes) in children with DLD (Cowan ners, after controlling for cognitive and linguistic child factors.
et al., 2005; Durkin et al., 2013) and children with NLA (Kleemans In a longitudinal study by Skwarchuk et al. (2014), formal home
et al., 2014). Finally, naming speed as an additional child factor numeracy practices (e.g., the parent practicing simple sums) as well
should be taken into consideration as well, because the solution as parents’ expectations were positively associated with symbolic
process of arithmetic in second grade requires fast retrieval of lin- number knowledge in first grade. In addition, in children with DLD,
guistically encoded arithmetic facts (Koponen, Mononen, Rasanen, the role of home numeracy experiences in first-grade arithmetic
& Ahonen, 2006). Interestingly, naming speed differentially pre- was evidenced: both parent–child numeracy activities and parents’
dicted the numerical skills of children with DLD in kindergarten as numeracy expectations were positively related to arithmetic with
well as first grade (Kleemans, Segers, & Verhoeven, 2011; Kleemans small problem sizes in first grade, after controlling for cognitive and
linguistic child factors. Those relations were stronger for parents of
64 T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72

children with DLD, as compared to parents of children with NLA 1. How do children with DLD and NLA differ in their basic arith-
(Kleemans et al., 2013). metic skills?
2. To what extent can the basic arithmetic skills of children with
DLD and NLA in second grade be predicted from their home
1.3. The present study numeracy experiences, as measured in kindergarten, when cog-
nitive and linguistic child factors are being controlled for?
The aim of the present study was to examine the role of
home numeracy experiences in predicting individual differences in
With respect to the first research question, we expected chil-
second-grade basic arithmetic in children with DLD and their peers
dren with DLD to have lower basic arithmetic skills than children
with NLA. The present study extended previous research in two
with NLA, with larger group differences on arithmetic with large
ways. First, it explored more in depth how children with DLD differ
problem sizes. With respect to the second research question, we
with their peers with NLA in basic arithmetic skills in second grade.
expected that home numeracy experiences were positively related
Previous research on individual differences in basic arithmetic skills
to the basic arithmetic skills (i.e., small problem sizes and large
children did not take into account the distinction between arith-
problem sizes) of both children with DLD and NLA, after controlling
metic with small problem sizes and arithmetic with large problem
for their cognitive and linguistic skills.
sizes. Given that second-grade arithmetic with large problem sizes
not only depends on language-based fact retrieval, but also on
phonological working memory (De Smedt et al., 2009), it can be 2. Method
expected that, when compared to children with NLA, children with
DLD have even more problems in solving arithmetic with large 2.1. Participants
problem sizes. Second, the present study was a longitudinal study
on the role of home numeracy experiences, as measured in kinder- Participants of the present study were part of a three-year lon-
garten, in second grade basic arithmetic, after taking into account gitudinal study on the role of cognitive and linguistic precursors
cognitive and linguistic child factors. From the research conducted in numerical development (see also Kleemans et al., 2013). The
so far, it has become clear that both aspects of home numeracy relevant characteristics of both groups are displayed in Table 1.
experiences are related to early numeracy skills in kindergarten Between Time 1 (i.e., second year of kindergarten) and Time 3,
as well as basic arithmetic skills in first grade. However, the rel- two years later (i.e., second grade), twenty-nine children dropped
ative contribution of each of the child and home factors beyond out of the study due to having moved or staying an extra year in
the start of formal arithmetic education in first grade is still unex- kindergarten or first grade. In addition, no differences were found
plored. Moreover, when contrasted to typical populations, less is on the predictor and outcome measures between the group of
known about the home numeracy experiences of atypical popula- children who dropped out after kindergarten or first grade and
tions, which is important with an eye on early identification and the group of children who participated in second grade. Further-
prevention of math problems in these groups (Kaufmann, 2008). more, all children spoke Dutch as their native language. In addition,
The question rises whether home numeracy experiences are also both groups had a nonverbal intelligence within normal range
related to second-grade basic arithmetic, and we investigated this (M = 5.00, SD = 2.00). Finally, both groups did not differ in nonver-
in the present study as a follow-up to Kleemans et al. (2013). To bal intelligence (kindergarten: p = .274; second grade: p = .139), age
determine the unique contribution of each of these factors, we also (kindergarten: p = .135; second grade: p = .089), and socioeconomic
took into account the autoregressive effect of prior arithmetic skills status (kindergarten: p = .507; second grade: p = .105).
in first grade.
The present study was carried out in the Netherlands. In Dutch
schools, children do not receive formal reading and arithmetic edu- 2.2. Materials
cation until first grade. Halfway through kindergarten, it is expected
that children gain phonological awareness (e.g., rhyming, blending) 2.2.1. Child factors (Time 1: kindergarten)
and have knowledge of some letters of the alphabet. Moreover, 2.2.1.1. General intelligence. The Raven’s Coloured Progressive
they should be able to arrange, count, and organize objects (e.g., Matrices (Raven, 1956) was used to assess the capacity of general
apples), and are expected to solve elementary arithmetic problems intelligence. In this task, thirty-six visual patterns were presented,
(e.g., one apple plus one apple makes two apples). Halfway through with increasing difficulty. In each puzzle, a piece of information
first grade, children are expected to solve arithmetic problems with was missing, and the child had to choose the correct piece out of six
small problem sizes, and halfway through second grade, children alternatives. All correct answers were counted and converted into
should not only instantly retrieve the solution of small problem standard scores, based on Dutch norms (Van Bon, 1986). This task
sizes (i.e., arithmetic problems with sums and minuends below 10), had good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = .90; Van Bon, 1986). Fur-
but are also assumed to successfully solve large problem sizes (i.e., thermore, this test had sufficient criterion and construct validity
arithmetic problems with sums and minuends above 10 and less according to the Dutch Committee on Diagnostic Assessment Mat-
than 20 that contain carryover operations) (Van der Stap, 2012). ters [Commissie Testaangelegenheden Nederland; Cotan] (Cotan,
In the Netherlands, children who are diagnosed with DLD are 2017).
referred to schools for special education in which they receive an
adapted curriculum in language as well as math in relatively small 2.2.1.2. Working memory: central executive functioning. Central
classrooms. The diagnosis of DLD is given by a multidisciplinary executive functioning was assessed by means of the Number Recall
team consisting of clinical linguists, educational psychologists, subtest from the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, sec-
speech therapists, and a physician. To receive this diagnosis, chil- ond edition (KABC-II) (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004). In this task, the
dren should have severe problems in receptive or productive experimenter pronounces sequences of digits that the child is asked
language domains (>1.5 SD on a minimum of two subtests out of to repeat in backward order. The series of digits increased from two
a standardized series of tests). Furthermore, in the present study, to nine digits. Testing was terminated after three consecutive mis-
children with hearing impairment (>30 dB), intellectual disabilities, takes. The number of correctly recalled sequences was counted.
physical disabilities, or autism spectrum disorders were excluded. Cronbach’s alpha was .85, reflecting good reliability (Kaufman,
The following research questions were addressed: Lichtenberger, Fletcher-Janzen, & Kaufman, 2005). In addition, the
T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72 65

Table 1
Descriptive characteristics of the children with DLD and children with NLA.

Time 1 (kindergarten) Time 3 (second grade)

DLD (n = 61) NLA (n = 111) DLD (n = 40) NLA (n = 103)

Variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)


Age (months) 73.95 (5.578) 72.70 (4.420) 98.23 (5.385) 96.71 (4.421)
Socioeconomic statusa 2.43 (.562) 2.37 (.485) 2.53 (.544) 2.39 (.480)
Nonverbal intelligence 4.42 (2.391) 4.88 (2.773) 4.26 (2.421) 4.99 (2.715)

Note. DLD = developmental language disorder; NLA = normal language achievement.


a
Parents were asked for highest completed education, varying from 1 (only postsecondary education, i.e., high school), 2 (intermediate education, i.e., education oriented
towards vocational training), 3 (higher education, i.e., university of applied sciences), to 4 (research university).

criterion and construct validity were evaluated as sufficient (Cotan, more, this test was found to have sufficient criterion validity and
2017). good construct validity (Cotan, 2017).

2.2.1.6. Grammatical ability. Three grammar tasks from the Taal-


2.2.1.3. Working memory: phonological loop. The capacity of the
toets Alle Kinderen [Language Test for All Children] (Verhoeven &
phonological loop was measured using the subtest Woorden en
Vermeer, 1993) were used to assess grammatical ability. Each item
Zinnen Nazeggen [Repeating Words and Sentences] from the ESM
started with the presentation of three pictures. Then, the child
toets [Test for Children With SLI] (Verhoeven, 2005). The test was
heard a sentence that matched one of the three pictures and had to
administered in two parts. In the first part, 12 sequences of words
select the picture that corresponded with the auditory-presented
were presented to the child, with the number of words increasing
sentence. On each task, a different skill of grammatical ability was
as testing progresses. An interval of one second was left between
assessed. First, the child had to complete 42 items that all contained
each word. The child had to reproduce the sequence of word in
different kinds of function words (e.g., “Which glass is full?”). Sec-
the correct order. In the second part, the child had to recall 12
ond, 32 sentences structures (e.g., “She pushes him”) were presented,
sentences, with increasing difficulty. In both parts, testing was ter-
and finally, the child heard 32 sentences that all reflect different
minated after four consecutive failures. The number of correctly
kinds of nuances (e.g., “The glass is not full yet”). The number of cor-
recalled sequences or sentences was counted. Cronbach’s alpha was
rect answers was the score on each task. Cronbach’s alpha was at
.88, indicating good reliability (Verhoeven, 2005). Furthermore, this
least .82, indicating good reliability (Verhoeven & Vermeer, 2006).
test was found to have good criterion validity and sufficient con-
In addition, both criterion validity and construct validity were eval-
struct validity (Cotan, 2017).
uated as good (Cotan, 2017).

2.2.1.4. Working memory: visual-spatial sketchpad. The subtest 2.2.1.7. Naming speed. Naming speed was measured using the sub-
Geheugenspan [Memory Span] from the Revisie Amsterdamse Kinder test Rapid naming: Plaatjes [Rapid Naming: Pictures] from the
Intelligentie Test [Revision Amsterdam Child Intelligence Test] ESM toets [Test for Children With Specific Language Impairment]
(RAKIT) (Bleichrodt, Drenth, Zaal, & Resing, 1987) was used to (Verhoeven, 2005). The task consisted of five columns comprising
assess the visual–spatial sketchpad. A sequence of abstract fig- 120 high-frequent randomly presented items: schoen [shoe], eend
ures was shown to the child, with the number of abstract figures [duck], bril [glasses], huis [house], and kam [comb]. After a prac-
increasing as testing progresses. The task of the child was to cor- tice trial of ten items, the child was to name as many pictures as
rectly reproduce this sequence, by using little blocks on which these possible within a minute. The score on this task was the number
abstract figures were depicted. When the child failed on three con- of correctly named pictures. Cronbach’s alpha was .95, indicating
secutive sequences, testing was terminated. Correctly reproduced excellent reliability (Verhoeven, 2005). Furthermore, this test was
sequences were then counted. Reliability of this task was good found to have good criterion validity and sufficient construct valid-
(Cronbach’s alpha in the present sample = .81). In addition, criterion ity (Cotan, 2017).
and construct validity were found to be good (Cotan, 2017).
2.2.2. Home factors (Time 1: kindergarten)
2.2.1.5. Phonological awareness. Phonological awareness was mea- Parents of the participants filled in a questionnaire on home
sured using the Screenings instrument Beginnende Geletterdheid numeracy practices. This questionnaire was based on previous
[Diagnostic Instrument for Emergent Literacy] (Vloedgraven, questionnaires on home numeracy practices (cf. Kleemans et al.,
Keuning, & Verhoeven, 2009). This test consisted of four tasks. 2013; LeFevre et al., 2009). To verify that the questionnaire
During the first three tasks (i.e., rhyming, blending, and synthe- indeed covered the hypothesized home numeracy constructs (i.e.,
sis), children were shown three response alternatives (both visually parent–child numeracy activities and parents’ numeracy expecta-
and auditory), followed by the auditory presentation of the target tions), a principal component analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation
word. The instructions for the subsequent part of each task differed. was used. The number of factors was based on theoretical inter-
For rhyming, the child was asked which picture (e.g., “bee,” “ball,” pretability as well as acceptable eigenvalues (>1; Allen & Yen, 1979).
and “monkey”) rhymed with the target word (e.g., “What rhymes An item was assigned to a particular factor, when its factor load-
with wall?”). For blending, the child had to select the picture start- ing was over .35. A two-factor structure emerged from the results,
ing with the same initial sound (e.g., “c for cat”). For synthesis, the each factor accounting for at least 26.72% of the variability. The first
child had to indicate which of the three pictures matched the orally factor covered parent–child numeracy activities; the second factor
presented letter-sound sequence (e.g., “c-a-t”). Finally, for recep- consisted of parents’ numeracy expectations. Comparable results
tive letter knowledge, four lowercase graphemes were presented on were found for principal axis factoring (PAF) and maximum likeli-
a computer screen. The child heard a phoneme (e.g., “l for lips”) hood (ML) extraction methods. The results are shown in Table 2.
and had to click on the corresponding grapheme. On each task, the
number of correct answers was counted. Cronbach’s alpha in the 2.2.2.1. Parent–child numeracy activities. Parents had to rate six
current sample was >.90, indicating excellent reliability. Further- items on the extent to which they participate in numeracy activities
66 T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72

Table 2
Results of the principal axis factoring analysis with promax rotation on aspects of home numeracy experiences (n = 143).

Variable Eigenvalue Percent explained variance ˛ Factor loading

Parent–child numeracy activities 5.355 41.194 .93


Making arithmetic puzzles .847
Talking about money when shopping .797
Doing counting activities .878
Playing counting games, using child computer or arithmetic software .859
Practicing numerical conceptual knowledge .861
Rehearsing counting rhymes .850
Parents’ numeracy expectations 3.474 26.724 .90
Expectation of child’s ability to count till 20 (forward and backward) .597
Expectation of child’s ability to count without hands .768
Expectations of child’s ability to count in groups of 2, 5 or 10 .679
Expectation of child’s performance on addition till 10 .834
Expectation of child’s performance on addition till 20 .888
Expectation of child’s performance on subtraction till 10 .836
Expectation of child’s performance on subtraction till 20 .875

with their child. Parents were asked: “How often did you and your sizes = .84; Cronbach’s alpha for large problem sizes = .83). Finally,
child engage in the following activities?” For each item, a 5-point preliminary testing of each of these tasks in a pilot study with a
scale (1–5) was used. The higher the score, the more frequent the similar age group showed sufficient construct validity with a stan-
parent participated in numeracy-related activities. The complete dardized arithmetic achievement test (r > .80).
list can be found in Appendix A. Cronbach’s alpha in the current
sample was .93, indicating excellent reliability. 2.3. Procedure

2.2.2.2. Parents’ numeracy expectations. In this part, parents had to The home numeracy questionnaire was sent to the parents after
fill out seven items on their expectations regarding numeracy with the schools agreed to participate. The questionnaire was adminis-
answering options on a four-point scale. Parents were asked: “To tered halfway through the second year of kindergarten. The parent,
what extent do you expect your child will master the following who was most involved in the numeracy-related activities with the
numeracy skills at the end of second grade?” Higher scores impli- child, was asked to fill in the questionnaires. Completed question-
cated higher expectations on numeracy achievement. The list of naires were returned by post.
items is included in Appendix A. Reliability analysis showed that Eight graduate student testers in educational psychology went
these items had good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha in the current to the schools after receiving training by an educational psycholo-
sample was .90). gist. Each of the testers already had experience for at least two years
in testing children between the ages of 5–9 years. The training con-
2.2.3. Basic arithmetic skills: small and large problem sizes sisted of a three-hour session in which the educational psychologist
The tasks to measure basic arithmetic skills were based on pre- (i.e., the first author) explained the standardized testing protocol
vious research on arithmetic with small and large problem sizes that each of the testers was expected to follow during testing. Then,
(Cowan et al., 2005). In first grade (Time 2), a battery of 40 addi- each of the testers practiced the standardized testing protocol with
tion and 40 subtraction problems with sums and minuends below a kindergartner (Time 1), first grader (Time 2), or second grader
10 was presented to the child (i.e., small problem sizes). In sec- (Time 3). Before the testers went to the schools, another meeting
ond grade (Time 3), the same battery of addition and subtraction with the educational psychologist was held to answer the remain-
items below 10 was administered, followed by another battery, of ing questions.
large problem sizes, consisting of 30 addition and 30 subtractions Each child was assigned to one tester, and all children were
problems with sums and minuends above 10 and less than 20, all tested in a quiet room in their own school. The test battery con-
comprising carryover operations. Each child started with the task sisted of four randomly presented blocks, half an hour each. Within
that contained the addition problems followed by the task with the a block, the same order of tests was used. The testing period com-
subtraction problems. On each task, the child had to correctly solve prised a period of two months, halfway through the second year
as many as arithmetical problems within a time limit of two min- of kindergarten (Time 1), halfway through first grade (Time 2), and
utes. All correct answers were counted. The complete list of items halfway through second grade (Time 3).
can be found in Appendix B.
E-prime software (Schneider, Eschmann, & Zuccolotto, 2002) 3. Results
was used to administer the various tasks. On each task, the same
procedure was used to present the items. First, an asterisk (i.e., To answer the first research question, on differences between
‘*’) was shown to draw the child’s attention to the middle of the the group of children with DLD and the group of children with NLA
screen, followed by the item (i.e., the arithmetical problem in Arabic in second grade, we first computed t-tests for independent sam-
notation) in horizontal format. The task of the child was to pro- ples with Holm–Bonferroni correction (Holm, 1979) on all child
vide the correct answer by using a numeric Universal Serial Bus and home factors. The results indicated that children with DLD
(USB) keyboard, with the Arabic digits (i.e., 0–9) depicted on it. scored below the group of children with NLA on all variables, with
The items were presented in quasi-random order (i.e., an Arabic medium to high effect sizes (d), varying between .377 and 2.688. As
digit presented in either the problem or solution did not occur in expected, children with DLD did not differ from children with NLA
a previous or following item) to prevent priming effect (Jackson & on general intelligence. The results are presented in Table 3.
Coney, 2007). Furthermore, the subtraction items were derived as Next, we conducted a multivariate analysis of variance with
an inverse of the addition items (e.g., 2 + 4 = 6 and 6 − 4 = 2). Each Problem type (small problem sizes, large problem sizes) and
task had a good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha >.83). This also goes Operation (addition, subtraction) as within-subjects factors and
for both sets of problem sizes (Cronbach’s alpha for small problem Group (DLD, NLA) as between-subjects factor. Next to main
T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72 67

Table 3
Descriptive statistics for the NLA group (n = 103) and DLD group (n = 40); results of t-tests for independent samples; and Cohen’s d.

Variable NLA DLD t d

M SD M SD

General intelligence 4.99 2.715 4.26 2.421 1.486 .249


Central executive functioning 3.96 1.468 1.78 1.527 7.903*** 1.331
Phonological loop 13.70 5.339 6.75 4.349 7.336*** 1.234
Visual–spatial sketchpad 5.62 1.890 3.13 1.305 8.984*** 1.775

Phonological awareness
Rhyming 9.22 1.137 5.98 2.082 9.343*** 2.688
Blending 7.94 2.367 4.75 2.085 7.472*** 1.259
Synthesis 8.44 1.949 5.23 2.247 8.469*** 1.426
Letter knowledge 21.86 5.557 11.85 5.432 9.732*** 1.639

Grammatical ability
Function words 35.83 2.753 29.33 3.951 9.545*** 2.589
Sentences structures 27.99 2.599 21.23 4.560 8.842*** 2.522
Nuances 26.92 2.976 21.63 3.656 8.945*** 1.501
Naming speed 39.92 12.596 34.25 11.591 2.466* .415

Parent–child numeracy activities


Making arithmetic puzzles 2.52 1.228 1.98 .660 3.439** .611
Talking about money when shopping 2.78 .885 2.38 .705 2.839** .603
Doing counting activities 2.50 1.065 1.85 .802 3.977*** .821
Playing counting games, using child computer or arithmetic software 2.53 1.101 1.85 .864 3.922*** .827
Practicing numerical conceptual knowledge 2.59 1.124 2.20 .939 2.117* .461
Rehearsing counting rhymes 2.26 1.111 1.70 1.043 2.762*** .465

Parents’ numeracy expectations


Expectation of child’s ability to count till 20 (forward and backward) 3.85 .364 3.25 .670 5.318*** 1.532
Expectation of child’s ability to count without hands 3.14 .852 2.78 .800 2.311* .389
Expectations of child’s ability to count in groups of 2,5 or 10 3.51 .752 3.20 .758 2.240* .377
Expectation of child’s performance on addition till 10 3.78 .418 3.23 .947 3.522*** 1.059
Expectation of child’s performance on addition till 20 3.38 .756 2.93 1.023 2.906** .489
Expectation of child’s performance on subtraction till 10 3.43 .824 2.93 .859 3.234*** .545
Expectation of child’s performance on subtraction till 20 3.12 .878 2.48 .933 3.854*** .649

Small problem sizes (first grade)


Addition till 10 26.36 10.037 15.50 8.470 3.884*** .654
Subtraction till 10 17.36 7.969 11.68 7.549 6.053*** 1.020

Small problem sizes (second grade)


Addition till 10 31.50 6.191 26.40 6.222 4.420*** .744
Subtraction till 10 25.99 8.022 20.23 4.588 5.373*** .976

Large problem sizes (second grade)


Addition till 20 22.72 7.689 14.10 7.182 6.125*** 1.032
Subtraction till 20 18.54 7.674 9.18 6.823 6.751*** 1.137

Note. Compared to our previous study (Kleemans et al., 2013), some children dropped out due to having moved or staying an extra year in first grade. Therefore, in the present
study, t-tests were computed again.
*
p < .05
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

effects of Operation (F(1, 141) = 225.858, p < .001, 2p = .616), tions between child and home factors and later basic arithmetic
Group (F(1, 141) = 40.734, p < .001, 2p = .224) and Problem type skills that were not affected by prior arithmetic skills. Further-
(F(1, 141) = 404.138, p < .001, 2p = .741), the results showed a more, to explore the relative contribution of the various cognitive
and linguistic child factors, these predictors were added in Step 2,
Group × Problem type interaction (F(1, 141) = 13.065, p < .001, 2p =
including a dummy group variable (NLA = 0, DLD = 1). In Step 3, both
.085), indicating that the differences between children with DLD
parent–child numeracy activities and parents’ numeracy expecta-
and their peers with NLA were larger for large problem sizes
tions were added to determine the unique contribution of each of
(B = 17.987, t = 6.946, p <. 001, 2p = .255) than for small problem
the home factors. Finally, to investigate whether there were addi-
sizes (B = 10.870, t = 4.654, p < . 001, 2p = .133).
tional differences between the groups in predicting basic arithmetic
To answer the second research question, we first computed
skills, all corresponding interaction terms between the child and
correlations between the child factors, home factors, and basic
home factors and group were added in Step 4. Adjusted R2 statistics
arithmetic skills. As can be seen in Table 4, correlations with the
were used to evaluate relative contribution (in terms of explained
dependent variables (i.e., small problem sizes and large problem
variance) of each step to the previous one as well as to correct for
sizes) were all significant.
the growing number of predictors in the model (Voeten & Van den
We then performed hierarchical regression analyses on both
Bercken, 2003).
outcome measures (i.e., small problem sizes and large problem
After controlling for prior arithmetic skills (i.e., arithmetic with
sizes). Prior to the regression analyses, we centered both child
small problem sizes in first grade) in Step 1, the results in Step 2
and home factors to prevent multicollinearity (Jewell, 2004). In
showed that general intelligence, phonological loop, phonological
Step 1, we first added the autoregressive effect of prior arithmetic
awareness, and grammatical ability as measured in kindergarten
skills, that is, arithmetic with small problem sizes in first grade.
positively predicted both basic arithmetic skills in second grade:
By controlling for this effect, we were able to investigate the rela-
68 T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72

Table 4
Correlations among the child factors, home factors, and basic arithmetic skills (n = 143).

Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12.

1. General intelligence –
2. Central executive functioning .217** –
3: Phonological loop .218** .527*** –
4. Visual–spatial sketchpad .100 .418*** .387*** –
5. Phonological awareness .133 .536*** .595*** .413*** –
6. Grammatical ability .245** .569*** .589*** .451*** .702*** –
7. Naming speed .076 .179* .179* .210* .215* .245** –
8. Parent–child numeracy activities .147 .324*** .173 .243** .211* .241** .164 –
9. Parents’ numeracy expectations .187* .333*** .337*** .201* .320*** .355*** .238** .208* –
10. Small problem sizes (first grade) .109 .227** .248** .188* .322*** .402*** .002 .229** .195* –
11. Small problem sizes (second grade) .333*** .437*** .526*** .307*** .516*** .528*** .206* .225** .305*** .366***
12. Large problem sizes (second grade) .317*** .497*** .580*** .426*** .587*** .607*** .168* .415*** .506*** .373*** .735*** –
*
p < .05
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

Table 5
Child and home factors of basic arithmetic skills in children with DLD (n = 40) and children with NLA (n = 103); and Cohen’s f2.

Predictor Small problem sizes (second grade) Large problem sizes (second grade)

R 2
f 2
ˇ R2 f2 ␤
*** ***
Step 1 .134 .577 .139 .595
Small problem sizes (first grade) .366*** .373***
*** ***
Step 2 .343 1.045 .393 1.322
General intelligence .174** .153*
Central executive functioning .102 .088
Phonological loop .219* .217**
Visual–spatial sketchpad .079 .153*
Phonological awareness .281** .244*
Grammatical ability .243* .220*
Naming speed .073 −.003
Group .368** .171

Step 3 .003 .006 .098*** .311


Parent–child numeracy activities .030 .216***
Parents’ numeracy expectations .049 .258***

Step 4 .049 .128 .048 .304


Group × Small problem sizes (first grade) −.087 −.026
Group × General intelligence −.057 −.068
Group × Central executive functioning −.078 .060
Group × Phonological loop .054 −.101
Group × Visual–spatial sketchpad −.048 −.173
Group × Phonological awareness −.126 −.058
Group × Grammatical ability −.135 −.164
Group × Naming speed .194* .193*
Group × Parent–child numeracy activities −.021 .195*
Group × Parents’ numeracy expectations .092 −.074

Total Radj
2
.446*** .624***

Note. Only significant interaction effects are displayed.


*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

higher scores on general intelligence, working memory, phonolog- nificant. The results thus suggest identical relationships between
ical awareness, and grammatical ability resulted in higher scores children with DLD and children with NLA in predicting basic arith-
on arithmetic with small problem sizes as well as large problem metic skills. All significant steps in the hierarchical regression
sizes. In addition, unique effects of the visual–spatial sketchpad in analysis (in terms of Cohen’s f2 ) had sufficient power (>.80) at the
kindergarten were found for arithmetic with large problems in sec- .05 level (two-tailed). The results are presented in Table 5.
ond grade: higher scores on the visual–spatial sketchpad resulted in
higher scores on arithmetic with large, but not small problem sizes.
4. Discussion
Furthermore, it was found in Step 3 that both parent–child numer-
acy activities and parents’ numeracy expectations in kindergarten
The present study investigated the longitudinal effects of the
positively predicted arithmetic with large problem sizes: higher
aspects of home numeracy experiences (i.e., parent–child numer-
scores on parent–child numeracy activities and parents’ numeracy
acy activities and parents’ numeracy expectations) on the basic
expectations were associated with higher scores on arithmetic with
arithmetic skills of children with DLD and their peers with NLA
large problem sizes, but not small problem sizes. Finally, in Step 4,
in second grade. First, differences between children with DLD and
the addition of explained variance in predicting arithmetic with
children with NLA on basic arithmetic skills were investigated. We
small problem sizes as well as large problem sizes was not sig-
found second-grade children with DLD to score below their peers
T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72 69

on arithmetic with both small and large problem sizes, with a larger whole did not add a significant percentage of explained variance,
difference for the large problem sizes. Given that arithmetic with it might be noted, that the individual interaction term between
small problems relies on having access to arithmetic facts that are parent–child numeracy activities and Group in predicting arith-
stored in long-term memory (De Smedt et al., 2010), the impaired metic with large problem sizes did reach significance (ˇ = .195;
linguistic skills of children with DLD may hinder them in acquir- p = .021). If allowed to further analyze this effect, it would have
ing those accurate arithmetic representations. The ability to solve led to the same conclusions as described in previous studies: chil-
arithmetic problems with large problem sizes not only depends on dren with DLD and lower parent–child numeracy activities (<−1 SD
applying the right solution procedure, but also on having instant below the mean score of the NLA group) performed worse on arith-
access to these arithmetic facts. metic with large problem sizes, when compared to children with
With respect to the longitudinal effects of aspects of home DLD and higher parent–child numeracy activities (>−1 SD below
numeracy experiences, it was first found that general intelligence, the mean score of the NLA group). This was also the case for the
the phonological loop, phonological awareness, and grammatical Naming speed × Group interaction (small problem sizes: ˇ = .194;
ability predicted the basic arithmetic skills (i.e., small and large p = .035 and large problem sizes: ˇ = .193; p = .012).
problem sizes) of children with DLD and their peers with NLA. These The results of the present study have some limitations. First,
results extended earlier research (cf. Durkin et al., 2013; Kleemans the present study did not have a representative distribution of
et al., 2014) in showing that both cognitive and linguistic factors SES. Parents of both children with DLD and NLA had relatively
account for the acquisition of not only first-grade basic arithmetic high educational levels (i.e., over 70 percent were middle or highly
skills, but also the more advanced second-grade basic arithmetic educated), which makes it difficult to generalize these findings to
skills. families of children with a lower SES. Previous research showed
Furthermore, the results indicated that for both groups of chil- that children that come from lower SES families face numerous
dren, parent–child numeracy activities and parents’ numeracy stressors, such as financial constraints and lower education. This
expectations were positively related to second-grade arithmetic may limit parents’ ability to support their children’s academic
achievement with large problem sizes. As an extension of previ- development and influence the type of number-related activities
ous research (e.g., Kleemans et al., 2013; Skwarchuk et al., 2014), parents report engaging in with their children at home (Ramani
the present study was the first to provide an in-depth view on & Siegler, 2014). Indeed, it has been found that differences in the
the unique role of each of the child and home factors on basic home environment were more strongly related to the numeracy
arithmetic skills beyond the start of formal arithmetic educa- skills of children with a lower SES (Melhuish et al., 2008; Siegler &
tion in first grade. It is interesting to note that home numeracy Ramani, 2009), and that especially these children may benefit a lot
experiences did not relate to second-grade arithmetic with small from playing informal numeracy activities to reduce their delays
problem sizes. This might be due to the fact that home numer- in numerical estimation skills, one of the most important prereq-
acy experiences are only related to those skills that are within the uisites for mathematics (see Siegler & Ramani, 2008). The stronger
zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 1978) of the child and relationships with the home numeracy environment may thus be
that are a subject of the educational curriculum at that time (cf. more localized on the lower ability range. We, therefore, cannot
Skwarchuk et al., 2014). According to the final terms of the Dutch rule out the possibility that home numeracy experiences are also
national curriculum (Van der Stap, 2012), the second graders in related to arithmetic with small problems in second grade when
the present study were expected to instantly retrieve the solu- the full range of SES was included. Future research could focus on
tion of arithmetic problems with small problem sizes. Furthermore, this.
they should successfully solve arithmetic with large problem sizes, Second, another limitation was that we did not assess home lit-
which is primarily dependent on the ability to shift from a finger eracy experiences. Manolitsis, Georgiou, and Tziraki (2013) found
counting strategy (i.e., visual–spatial sketchpad) to a more ver- that parent–child numeracy activities were associated with early
bal strategy (i.e., phonological loop) (Siegler, 1996). These verbal math acquisition, but when they replaced home numeracy expe-
strategies (i.e., verbal counting and temporarily maintaining arith- riences with home literacy experiences in a path analysis, parent
metic facts during the solution process), are considered to be of literacy teaching was also positively related to early math acqui-
crucial importance in order to develop strong problem–answer sition. However, Segers, Kleemans, and Verhoeven (2015) only
associations in long-term memory (De Smedt et al., 2009). As can found unique positive effects of home numeracy experiences after
be seen in Table 5, the visual–spatial sketchpad as a cognitive fac- controlling for home literacy in a sample of 60 kindergartners. Pre-
tor was still significant in predicting arithmetic with large problem vious research indicated that parents of children with DLD report
sizes, supporting the idea that the children were still in the pro- engaging in fewer literacy activities and also have lower literacy
cess of automatizing arithmetic with large problem sizes. The role expectations than parents of children with NLA (Skibbe, Justice,
of home numeracy experiences in basic arithmetic might thus be Zucker, & McGinty, 2008). We, therefore, cannot rule out the pos-
restricted to those arithmetic skills that children have not fully sibility that children with DLD are less interested in numeracy
mastered yet. It may, therefore, be expected that the effects of related activities because of their impaired linguistic skills and, as a
the aspects of home numeracy experiences may further reduce in consequence, parents are less motivated to engage in linguistically-
strength when children have acquired basic arithmetic skills, and oriented home practices. This could be a topic for future research.
other mathematical skills (e.g., geometry and algebra) are becom- Finally, because we used self-reports to assess home numeracy
ing increasingly important (Kleemans et al., 2016). experiences in children with DLD, we cannot rule out the possi-
In contrast to what was found in the present sample one year bility that the group differences on home numeracy experiences
earlier (Kleemans et al., 2013), the results of the present study may be caused because of language problems on the part of the
did not indicate that parent–child numeracy activities were more parent. Indeed, DLD has been found to be highly heritable (Bishop
strongly related to the basic arithmetic skills of children with DLD & Snowling, 2004). It would, therefore, be recommended in future
than their peers with NLA. Moreover, it was also found in our pre- research to also control for the language level of the parent when
vious research that naming speed differentially predicted the basic testing group differences on home numeracy experiences. A related
arithmetic skills of children with DLD. The absence of both effects issue that needs to be considered is the fact that the use of ques-
in the current study might be due to the relative small sample tionnaires in general may be biased because of social desirability
size of children with DLD and the corresponding loss of statisti- (Richman, Kiesler, Weisband, & Drasgow, 1999). Other ways of
cal power. Indeed, although the results indicated that Step 4 as a assessing home numeracy skills may overcome such limitations.
70 T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72

For example, Bjorklund, Hubertz, and Reubens (2004) found the 3. Expectations of child’s ability to count in groups of 2, 5 or 10
quality of parent–child interactions (by means of structured obser- 4. Expectation of child’s ability on addition till 10
vations) to be related to children’s numeracy skills as well. It is 5. Expectation of child’s ability on addition till 20
still unclear, however, to what extent such a measure as opposed 6. Expectation of child’s ability on subtraction till 10
to the use of questionnaires may be a better, more valid way to 7. Expectation of child’s ability on subtraction till 20
assess home numeracy experiences. Therefore, validation studies
to determine the most optimal way to assess home numeracy are Note. A scale ranging from 1 to 4 was included for each item.
called for (Skwarchuk et al., 2014).
To conclude, the results point out that home numeracy factors
relate to second-grade arithmetic with large, but not small prob- Appendix B.
lem sizes. This finding has implications for both the home and
school environment. First, parents should be made aware that they See Tables B1 and B2.
can make a difference when it comes to stimulating their child’s
arithmetic skills. To be more specific, parents could provide chil- Table B1
dren with those numeracy-related activities that are just above Small problem sizes.
their current competence level (Wells, 1999). Second, it can be rec- Addition Subtraction
ommended to introduce numeracy activities at home before the
3+6=9 9−6=3
start of formal arithmetic education. Consequently, professional 1+1=2 2−1=1
communication between teachers and parents is crucial. This impli- 5+4=9 9−4=5
cates that teachers should inform parents about their crucial role 2+1=3 3−1=2
in stimulating their child’s math skills along with providing them a 6+2=8 8−2=6
4+3=7 7−3=4
realistic image of their child’s current math skills. Finally, when it
1+5=6 6−5=1
comes to intervention on reducing delays in basic arithmetic skills, 3 + 7 = 10 10 − 7 = 3
it is important to take into account both child and home factors that 4+5=9 9−5=4
children bring into the classroom. 1+2=3 3−2=1
4+4=8 8−4=4
7 + 3 = 10 10 − 3 = 7
Appendix A. 4+2=6 6−2=4
5+3=8 8−3=5
Parent–Child Numeracy Activities 2+7=9 9−7=2
3+1=4 4−1=3
5+2=7 7−2=5
Question 4 + 6 = 10 10 − 6 = 4
How often did you and your child engage in the following activ- 7+2=9 9−2=7
ities? Circle 1 if the activity did not occur, circle 2 if it occurred on 3+5=8 8−5=3
1 + 9 = 10 10 − 9 = 1
a monthly base, circle 3 if it occurred on a weekly base, circle 4 if
2+4=6 6−4=2
it occurred on a daily base, and circle 5 if it occurred a few times a 1+8=9 9−8=1
day. 6 + 4 = 10 10 − 4 = 6
3+2=5 5−2=3
2+6=8 8−6=2
1. Making arithmetic puzzles (e.g., connect-the-dot activities)
1+3=4 4−3=1
2. Doing counting activities (e.g., playing with child cash register; 2 + 8 = 10 10 − 8 = 2
playing with number wall; playing with die) 1+4=5 5−4=1
3. Talking about money when shopping 3+3=6 6−3=3
4. Playing counting games, using child computer or arithmetic 2+5=7 7−5=2
6+3=9 9−3=6
software (e.g., playing with ‘My first computer’ (to practice arith-
8 + 2 = 10 10 − 2 = 8
metic and counting skills), playing Disney preschool) 3+4=7 7−4=3
5. Practicing numerical conceptual knowledge (e.g., ordering 5 + 5 = 10 10 − 5 = 5
objects by size, shape, colour; arranging objects by size, height; 1+6=7 7−6=1
2+3=5 5−3=2
mass, number; what is more/less)
9 + 1 = 10 10 − 1 = 9
6. Rehearsing counting rhymes 2+2=4 4−2=2
1+7=8 8−7=1
Note. A scale ranging from 1 to 5 was included for each item.

Parents’ Numeracy Expectations


Table B2
Question Large problem sizes.
To what extent do you expect your child to master the following
Addition Subtraction
numeracy skills at the end of kindergarten? Circle 1 if you expect
your child not to master a particular skill at all, circle 2 when you 8 + 9 = 17 17 − 9 = 8
5 + 6 = 11 11 − 6 = 5
expect your child to master the particular skill a little, circle 3 if
8 + 4 = 12 12 − 4 = 8
you expect your child to sufficiently master the particular skill, 9 + 7 = 16 16 − 7 = 9
and circle 4 when you expect your child to completely master the 8 + 3 = 11 11 − 3 = 8
particular skill. 5 + 9 = 14 14 − 9 = 5
6 + 7 = 13 13 − 7 = 6
9 + 2 = 11 11 − 2 = 9
1. Expectation of child’s ability to count till 20 (forward and back- 8 + 7 = 15 15 − 7 = 8
ward) 4 + 9 = 13 13 − 9 = 4
2. Expectation of child’s ability to count without hands 7 + 5 = 12 12 − 5 = 7
T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72 71

Table B2 (Continued) Developmental Science, 13, 508–520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.


2009.00897.x
Addition Subtraction
De Smedt, Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2011). Effects of problem size and
8 + 6 = 14 14 − 6 = 8 arithmetic operation on brain activation during calculation in children with
9 + 3 = 12 12 − 3 = 9 varying levels of arithmetical fluency. Neuroimage, 57(3), 771–781. http://dx.
6 + 5 = 11 11 − 5 = 6 doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.037
Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for
4 + 8 = 12 12 − 8 = 4
number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 487–506. http://dx.
7 + 6 = 13 13 − 6 = 7
doi.org/10.1080/02643290244000239
9 + 5 = 14 14 − 5 = 9 Desoete, A., & Grégoire, J. (2006). Numerical competence in young children and in
3 + 8 = 11 11 − 8 = 3 children with mathematical learning disabilities. Learning and Individual
6 + 9 = 15 15 − 9 = 6 Differences, 16, 351–367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2006.12.006
4 + 7 = 11 11 − 7 = 4 Durkin, K., Mok, P. L., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2013). Severity of specific language
5 + 8 = 13 13 − 8 = 5 impairment predicts delayed development in number skills. Frontiers in
2 + 9 = 11 11 − 9 = 2 Psychology, 4, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00581
7 + 8 = 15 15 − 8 = 7 Fazio, B. B. (1994). The counting abilities of children with specific language
9 + 4 = 13 13 − 4 = 9 impairment: A comparison of oral and gestural tasks. Journal of Speech,
5 + 7 = 12 12 − 7 = 5 Language, and Hearing Research, 37(2), 358–368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.
6 + 8 = 14 14 − 8 = 6 3702.358
3 + 9 = 12 12 − 9 = 3 Fazio, B. B. (1996). Mathematical abilities of children with specific language
impairment: A 2-year follow-up. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
7 + 4 = 11 11 − 4 = 7
Research, 39(4), 839–849. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3904.839
8 + 5 = 13 13 − 5 = 8
Friso-van den Bos, I., Van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. H.
7 + 9 = 16 16 − 9 = 7
(2013). Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A
meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 10, 29–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1016/j.edurev.2013.05.003
Appendix C. Supplementary data Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is
it, who has it and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1126/science.298.5598.1569
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, Hecht, S. A., Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2001). The relations
in the online version, at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.01. between phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences
005. in mathematical computation skills: A longitudinal study from second to fifth
grades. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 192–227. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1006/jecp.2000.2586
References Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure.
Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70.
Allen, M. J., & Yen, W. M. (1979). Introduction to measurement theory. Monterey, CA: Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., Larson, S. L., Balsink Krieg, D., & Shaligram, C. (2000).
Brooks/Cole. Mathematics, vocabulary, and reading development in Chinese American and
Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of European American children over the primary school years. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 49, 5–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Educational Psychology, 92(4), 745–760. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-0663.
027249896392784 92.4.745
Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., & Luo, Z. (2016). Parental facilitation of early
language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130(6), mathematics and reading skills and knowledge through encouragement of
858–886. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.858 home-based activities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 37, 1–15. http://dx.
Bishop, D. V., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., & Greenhalgh, T. (2016). CATALISE: doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.02.005
A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study. Identifying Jackson, N., & Coney, J. (2007). Simple arithmetic processing. Surface form effects
language impairments in children. Public Library of Science, 11(7) http://dx.doi. in a priming task. Acta Psychologica, 125(1), 1–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158753 actpsy.2006.05.003
Bjorklund, D. F., Hubertz, M. J., & Reubens, A. C. (2004). Young children’s arithmetic Jewell, P. (2004). Statistics for epidemiology. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.
strategies in social context: How parents contribute to children’s strategy Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Locuniak, M. N., & Ramineni, C. (2007). Predicting
development while playing games. International Journal of Behavioral first-grade math achievement from developmental number sense trajectories.
Development, 28(4), 347–357. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 22, 36–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.
Bleichrodt, N., Drenth, P. J. D., Zaal, J. N., & Resing, W. C. M. (1987). RAKIT. 1540-5826.2007.00229.x
Handleiding bij de revisie Amsterdamse kinder intelligentie test [Manual for the Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman assessment battery for children
revised Amsterdam child intelligence test]. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Zeitlinger. Kaufman, A. S., Lichtenberger, E. O., Fletcher-Janzen, E., & Kaufman, N. L. (2005).
Blevins-Knabe, B., & Musun-Miller, L. (1996). Number use at home by children and Essentials of KABC-II assessment. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons.
their parents and its relationship to early mathematical performance. Early Kaufmann, L. (2008). Dyscalculia: Neuroscience and education. Educational
Development and Parenting, 5, 35–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099- Research, 50(2), 163–175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00131880802082658
0917(199603)5:1<35:AID-EDP113 Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2011). Precursors to numeracy in
Blevins-Knabe, B. (2016). Early mathematical development: How the home kindergartners with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental
environment matters. In B. Blevins-Knabe, & A. Berghout Austin (Eds.), Early Disabilities, 32(6), 2901–2908. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.013
childhood mathematics skill development in the home environment (pp. 7–28). Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Naming speed as a clinical marker
Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. in predicting basic calculation skills in children with specific language
Cankaya, O., & LeFevre, J. A. (2016). The home numeracy environment: What do impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(3), 882–886.
cross-cultural comparisons tell us about how to scaffold young children’s Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2013). Relations between home
mathematical skills? In B. Blevins-Knabe, & A. Berghout Austin (Eds.), Early numeracy experiences and basic calculation skills in children with and without
childhood mathematics skill development in the home environment (pp. 87–104). specific language impairment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(2),
Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. 415–423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.10.004
Ciping, D., Silinskas, G., Wei, W., & Georgiou, G. K. (2015). Cross-lagged Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2014). Cognitive and linguistic predictors
relationships between home learning environment and academic achievement of basic arithmetic skills: Evidence from first and second language learners.
in Chinese. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 33, 12–20. http://dx.doi.org/10. International Journal of Development Disability and Education, 61(3), 306–316.
1016/j.ecresq.2015.05.001 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1034912X2014.934017
Cotan. (2017). Cotan Documentatie [Cotan Documentation]. Retrieved from Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). Towards a theoretical framework
https://www.cotandocumentatie.nl. on individual differences in numerical abilities: Role of home numeracy
Cowan, R., Donlan, C., Newton, E. J., & Lloyd, D. (2005). Number skills and experiences. In B. Blevins-Knabe, & A. Berghout Austin (Eds.), Early childhood
knowledge in children with specific language impairment. Journal of mathematics skill development in the home environment (pp. 87–104). Cham,
Educational Psychology, 97, 732–744. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
4.732 Kleemans, T., Peeters, M., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Child and home
De Smedt, B., Janssen, R., Bouwens, K., Verschaffel, L., Boets, B., & Ghesquière, P. predictors of early numeracy skills in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research
(2009). Working memory and individual differences in mathematics Quarterly, 27, 471–477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.12.004
achievement: A longitudinal study from first grade to second grade. Journal of Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Naming speed as a clinical marker
Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 86–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp. in predicting basic calculation skills in children with specific language
2009.01.004 impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(3), 882–886. http://dx.
De Smedt, B., Taylor, J., Archibald, L., & Ansari, D. (2010). How is phonological doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.12.007
processing related to individual differences in children’s arithmetic?
72 T. Kleemans et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 43 (2018) 62–72

Koponen, T., Mononen, R., Räsänen, P., & Ahonen, T. (2006). Basic numeracy in Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2008). Playing linear numerical board games
children with specific language impairment: Heterogeneity and connections to promotes low-income children’s numerical development. Developmental
language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 58–73. http:// Science, 11(5), 655–661. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00714.x
dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/005) Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board games—but not
LeFevre, J., Clarke, T., & Stringer, A. P. (2002). Influences of language and parental circular ones—improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding.
involvement on the development of counting skills: Comparisons of French- Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 545. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
and English-speaking Canadian children. Early Child Development and Care, 172, a0014239
283–300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03004430212127 Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children’s thinking.
LeFevre, J., Skwarchuk, S., Smith-Chant, B. L., Fast, L., Kamawar, D., & Bisanz, J. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(2009). Home numeracy experiences and children’s math performance in the Simmons, F., & Singleton, C. (2008). Do weak phonological representations impact
early school years. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 41, 55–66. http://dx. on arithmetic development? A review of research into arithmetic and dyslexia.
doi.org/10.1037/a0014532 Dyslexia, 14(2), 77–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.341
LeFevre, J., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S. L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Bisanz, J., Kamawar, D., et al. Skibbe, L. E., Justice, L. M., Zucker, T. A., & McGinty, A. S. (2008). Relations among
(2010). Pathways to mathematics: Longitudinal predictors of performance. maternal literacy beliefs, home literacy practices, and the early literacy skills of
Child Development, 81(6), 1753–1767. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624. preschoolers with specific language impairment. Early Education and
2010.01508.x Development, 19, 68–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409280701839015
LeFevre, J., Polyzoi, E., Skwarchuk, S., Fast, L., & Sowinski, C. (2010). Do home Skwarchuk, S.-L., Sowinkski, C., & LeFevre, J. (2014). Formal and informal home
numeracy and literacy practices of Greek and Canadian parents predict the learning activities in relation to children’s early numeracy and literacy skills:
numeracy skills of kindergarten children? International Journal of Early Years The development of a home numeracy model. Journal of Experimental Child
Education, 18(1), 55–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09669761003693926 Psychology, 121, 63–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.006
Lee, K. M., & Kang, S. Y. (2002). Arithmetic operation and working memory: Skwarchuk, S.-L. (2009). How do parents support preschoolers’ numeracy
Differential suppression in dual tasks. Cognition, 83(3), 63–68. http://dx.doi. experiences at home? Early Childhood Education Journal, 37(3), 189–197. http://
org/10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00010-0 dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10643-009-0340-1
Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G. K., & Tziraki, N. (2013). Examining the effects of home Stanescu-Cosson, R., Pinel, P., Van De Moortele, P. F., Le Bihan, D., Cohen, L., &
literacy and numeracy environment on early reading and math acquisition. Dehaene, S. (2000). Understanding dissociations in dyscalculia: A brain
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 692–703. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. imaging study of the impact of number size on the cerebral networks for exact
ecresq.2013.05.004 and approximate calculation. Brain, 123, 2240–2255. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Martin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Lewis, M. (2007). Lewis’s child and adolescent psychiatry. 1093/brain/123.11.2240
A comprehensive textbook. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins. Stock, P., Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2009). Predicting arithmetic abilities: The role
Melhuish, E. C., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., Taggart, B., Phan, M., of preparatory arithmetic markers and intelligence. Journal of
et al. (2008). Preschool influences on mathematics achievement. Science, 321, Psychoeducational Assessment, 27(3), 237–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
1161–1162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1158808 0734282908330587
Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2013). Casting the die before the die is cast: The Van Bon, W. H. J. (1986). Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices. Nederlandse normen
importance of the home numeracy environment for preschool children. en enige andere uitkomsten van onderzoek [Raven’s Coloured Progressive
European Journal of Psychology of Education, 29(3), 327–345. http://dx.doi.org/ Matrices. Dutch Norms and some other results of research]. Lisse, the
10.1007/s10212-013-0201-6 Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2014). How informal learning activities can promote Van der Stap, M. (2012). Van kerndoel tot leerlijn. Concretisering van de kerndoelen
children’s numerical knowledge. In R. C. Kadosh, & A. Dowker (Eds.), Oxford voor het speciaal onderwijs [Elaboration of the primary objectives in special
handbooks online http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199642342.013.012 education]. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Uitgeverij SWP.
Rasmussen, C., & Bisanz, J. (2005). Representation and working memory in early Verhoeven, L., & Vermeer, A. (1993). Taaltoets alle kinderen. handleiding [Language
arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91, 137–157. http://dx.doi. test for all children. Manual]. Arnhem, the Netherlands: Cito.
org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.01.004 Verhoeven, L., & Vermeer, A. (2006). Taaltoets alle kinderen. Verantwoording
Raven, J. C. (1956). Guide to using the coloured progressive matrices. London, United [Language test for all children. Psychometric analysis]. Arnhem, the Netherlands:
Kingdom: H.K. Lewis & Co. Cito.
Richman, W. L., Kiesler, S., Weisband, S., & Drasgow, F. (1999). A meta-analytic Verhoeven, L. (2005). ESM-Toets [Test for children with specific language
study of social desirability distortion in computer-administered impairment]. Arnhem, the Netherlands: Cito.
questionnaires, traditional questionnaires, and interviews. Journal of Applied Vloedgraven, J., Keuning, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2009). Screeningsinstrument
Psychology, 84(5), 754–775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.754 beginnende geletterdheid [Diagnostic instrument for emergent literacy]. Arnhem,
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2014). Continuity and change in the home literacy the Netherlands: Cito.
environment as predictors of growth in vocabulary and reading. Child Voeten, M. J. M., & Van den Bercken, J. H. L. (2003). Lineaire regressieanalyse [Linear
Development, 85, 1535–1551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12222 regression analysis]. Groningen, the Netherlands: Stenfert-Kroese.
Sénéchal, M., & Young, L. (2008). The effect of family literacy interventions on Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological
children’s acquisition of reading from kindergarten to grade 3: A meta-analytic processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
review. Review of Educational Research, 78, 880–907. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/ Wells, G. (1999). Dialogic inquiry: Towards a socio-cultural practice and theory of
0034654308320319 education. Cambridge University Press.
Schneider, W., Eschmann, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide.
Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
Segers, E., Kleemans, T., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). Role of parent literacy and
numeracy expectations and activities in predicting early numeracy skills.
Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 17(2–3), 219–236. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1080/10986065.2015.1016819

You might also like