You are on page 1of 17

Child Development, September/October 2017, Volume 88, Number 5, Pages 1727–1742

Early Math Trajectories: Low-Income Children’s Mathematics Knowledge


From Ages 4 to 11
Bethany Rittle-Johnson Emily R. Fyfe
Vanderbilt University Indiana University

Kerry G. Hofer Dale C. Farran


Abt Associates Vanderbilt University

Early mathematics knowledge is a strong predictor of later academic achievement, but children from low-
income families enter school with weak mathematics knowledge. An early math trajectories model is pro-
posed and evaluated within a longitudinal study of 517 low-income American children from ages 4 to 11. This
model includes a broad range of math topics, as well as potential pathways from preschool to middle grades
mathematics achievement. In preschool, nonsymbolic quantity, counting, and patterning knowledge predicted
fifth-grade mathematics achievement. By the end of first grade, symbolic mapping, calculation, and patterning
knowledge were the important predictors. Furthermore, the first-grade predictors mediated the relation
between preschool math knowledge and fifth-grade mathematics achievement. Findings support the early
math trajectories model among low-income children.

Proficiency in mathematics is critical to academic, The goal of the current study is to elucidate
economic, and life success. Greater success in specific early math knowledge that is predictive of
mathematics is related to college completion, higher later mathematics achievement for children from
earnings, and better health decisions (Reyna, low-income backgrounds. We propose an early
Nelson, Han, & Dieckmann, 2009; Ritchie & Bates, math trajectories model of specific early math
2013). Mathematics knowledge begins to develop at knowledge that influences later mathematics
a young age, and this early knowledge matters. achievement, integrating a broader range of mathe-
General math knowledge before school entry pre- matics topics than has been considered in past
dicted mathematics and reading outcomes across studies. We evaluated the early math trajectories
primary and secondary schools (Duncan et al., model within a longitudinal study of over 500 low-
2007; Jordan, Kaplan, Ramineni, & Locuniak, 2009; income children from ages 4 to 11. The current
Watts, Duncan, Siegler, & Davis-Kean, 2014). Unfor- study helps advance theories of mathematics
tunately, children from low-income families enter development and identifies specific early math
school with weaker mathematics knowledge than knowledge that might merit particular attention for
children from more advantaged backgrounds, and low-income children.
this weak early math knowledge at school entry
helps explain their weak math knowledge later in
elementary school (Jordan et al., 2009).
Early Mathematics Development

The research supported by the Heising-Simons Foundation Proficiency in mathematics requires developing
(#2013-26) and by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Depart- knowledge of multiple topics and their interrela-
ment of Education, through Grants R305A140126 and tions (Common Core State Standards, 2010). Longi-
R305K050157 to Dale C. Farran. Emily R. Fyfe was supported by
a Graduate Research Fellowship from the National Science Foun- tudinal research in psychology has documented
dation. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do that early math knowledge predicts later math
not represent views of the funders. The authors thank Kayla Polk, knowledge but has primarily relied on global mea-
Carol Bilbrey, and Dana True for their assistance with data collec-
tion and coding as well as the staff, teachers, and children in the sures of math knowledge (Duncan et al., 2007;
Metro Nashville Public Schools for participating in this research.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Bethany Rittle-Johnson, Department of Psychology and Human © 2016 The Authors
Development, Vanderbilt University, 230 Appleton Place, Pea- Child Development © 2016 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
body #552, Nashville, TN 37203. Electronic mail may be sent to All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2017/8805-0028
bethany.rittle-johnson@vanderbilt.edu. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12662
1728 Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Hofer, and Farran

Watts et al., 2014). Other longitudinal research has Cooper, 1980) as well as large set sizes (Xu, Spelke,
documented the importance of a particular early & Goddard, 2005). Nonsymbolic quantity knowl-
math topic for mathematics achievement a few edge provides a foundation for mapping between
years later but without considering a broad range magnitudes and verbal and symbolic numbers (Gil-
of early math topics or following children into the more, McCarthy, & Spelke, 2010; Piazza, 2010; van
middle grades (reviewed below). Based on a syn- Marle, Chu, Li, & Geary, 2014), and an intuitive
thesis of past research, we propose an early math understanding of simple arithmetic (Barth, La Mont,
trajectories model that encompasses a set of six Lipton, & Spelke, 2005). Beginning in preschool,
early math topics that should be of particular individual differences in the speed and precision of
importance for supporting mathematics achieve- nonsymbolic quantity knowledge of both small and
ment in the middle grades (ages 10–14). large set sizes are related to mathematics knowledge
6 months to 2 years later (Chen & Li, 2014; Desoete
& Gregoire, 2006; LeFevre et al., 2010; Libertus,
Components of Early Mathematics Knowledge
Feigenson, & Halberda, 2013). The relation is stron-
Central to early math knowledge is numeracy gest before age 6 (Fazio, Bailey, Thompson, &
knowledge, children’s knowledge of the meaning of Siegler, 2014). Although there is some controversy
whole numbers and number relations (Jordan, over the strength of this relation with appropriate
Kaplan, Nabors Olah, & Locuniak, 2006; National controls (De Smedt, No€el, Gilmore, & Ansari, 2013),
Research Council, 2009). This includes nonsymbolic we expected nonsymbolic quantity knowledge in
quantity knowledge, which does not require knowl- preschool to predict later mathematics achievement
edge of verbal or symbolic number names. Numer- even with a range of control variables.
acy knowledge that involves linking to verbal or
symbolic number names is separated into three
Counting
related, but distinct, topics: counting (counting
objects and verbal counting, also called numbering), Counting, also termed number or numbering
symbolic mapping (also called numerical relations), knowledge, includes knowledge of the number–
and calculation (also called arithmetic operations), word sequence, the ability to make a one to one
consistent with confirmatory factor analyses by Pur- correspondence between objects and count words,
pura and Lonigan (2013). and the use of the largest count word to identify
Early mathematics knowledge extends beyond the cardinality of the set (Purpura & Lonigan,
numeracy knowledge, though there is less consensus 2013). Many children begin to learn to count
on which additional math topics are important. between the ages of 2 and 3, and individual differ-
Commonly highlighted topics are shape, patterning, ences in 4- and 5-year-old children’s counting pre-
and measurement knowledge (National Research dicts their mathematics achievement in elementary
Council, 2009). We considered patterning, as there is school (Aunola, Leskinen, Lerkkanen, & Nurmi,
longitudinal evidence for its importance in math, 2004; Muldoon, Towse, Simms, Perra, & Menzies,
and shape knowledge, given widespread beliefs about 2013). Thus, we expected counting in preschool to
its importance. We did not include measurement predict later mathematics achievement, though we
knowledge, given the dearth of assessments and expected its importance to dissipate by the end of
research on measurement knowledge before school first grade, given that counting receives less atten-
entry. We briefly review evidence on the develop- tion after kindergarten and the lack of longitudinal
ment of each of the six topics, including evidence for evidence for the predictive power of counting
its predictive relation to later mathematics achieve- knowledge past kindergarten.
ment. Each topic encompasses conceptual knowl-
edge of underlying principles as well as procedural
Symbolic Mapping Knowledge
knowledge for solving problems within the topic.
Symbolic mapping knowledge is the knowledge
of the mapping between symbolic numerals, their
Nonsymbolic Quantity Knowledge
number names, and their magnitudes, including
Nonsymbolic quantity knowledge is knowledge their relative magnitudes (Jordan et al., 2006). In
of the magnitude of sets without the need to use mathematics education, this is often termed numeri-
verbal or symbolic number names. This knowledge cal relations but with the inclusion of nonsymbolic
begins to develop in infancy, including the ability to quantity knowledge in this category (Purpura &
discriminate between small set sizes (Starkey & Lonigan, 2013). Symbolic mapping knowledge
Early Math Trajectories 1729

begins to develop by age 3 for small set sizes and patterns (i.e., linear patterns with a core unit, like the col-
gradually extends to larger set sizes and more diffi- ors blue–red, that repeats, such as blue–red–blue–red).
cult tasks (Benoit, Lehalle, & Jouen, 2004). Differ- Preschool children are often asked to copy or extend
ences in this knowledge in the primary grades are a pattern, and preschool and kindergarten children
consistently related to a range of mathematical out- are also able to make the same kind of pattern using
comes (Fazio et al., 2014; Fuchs, Geary, Fuchs, new colors or shapes and to identify the core unit
Compton, & Hamlett, 2014; Geary, 2011; Sasanguie, that repeats (Clements & Sarama, 2009; Rittle-John-
Van den Bussche, & Reynvoet, 2012), with some evi- son, Fyfe, Hofer, & Farran, 2015; Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe,
dence of a relation beginning at age 5 in a middle McLean, & McEldoon, 2013). Intervention research
socioeconomic status sample (Kolkman, Kroesber- indicates that patterning knowledge is causally
gen, & Leseman, 2013). Recent evidence and theory related to early mathematics achievement. A pre-
suggests that symbolic mapping knowledge is more school patterning intervention led to greater numer-
important than nonsymbolic number knowledge in acy knowledge at the end of kindergarten than
predicting achievement (De Smedt et al., 2013). In typical preschool instruction (Papic, Mulligan, &
particular, symbolic mapping knowledge is often Mitchelmore, 2011). A patterning intervention for
more highly correlated with concurrent mathematics struggling first-grade students led to comparable or
achievement than is nonsymbolic quantity knowl- better numeration, calculation, algebra, and measure-
edge (Fazio et al., 2014; Holloway & Ansari, 2009). ment knowledge at the end of the school year than a
Furthermore, symbolic mapping, but not nonsym- numeracy intervention (Kidd et al., 2013, 2014). Lon-
bolic quantity knowledge, at ages 5 and 6 predicted gitudinal evidence is sparse, though pattern items
mathematics achievement the following year when a were part of a cluster of diverse items from a kinder-
range of measures were included in the model (Kolk- garten assessment that best predicted eighth-grade
man et al., 2013; Sasanguie et al., 2012; Vanbinst, achievement, providing some indirect evidence for
Ghesquiere, & De Smedt, 2015). Thus, we predicted its long-term importance (Claessens & Engel, 2013).
that in early primary school, symbolic mapping but We predicted that patterning knowledge in
not nonsymbolic quantity knowledge would be pre- preschool and primary school would predict later
dictive of later achievement. mathematics achievement.

Calculation Shape
Calculation, or arithmetic operations, is the ability Knowledge of shapes and their properties is a
to calculate the combination or separation of sets component of early math standards and considered
(Purpura & Lonigan, 2013). Toddlers can calculate foundational to later geometric thinking (Common
when set sizes are very small and verbal number Core State Standards, 2010; National Research
names are not needed (Huttenlocher, Jordan, & Council, 2009). Preschool children can identify typi-
Levine, 1994), and preschool children can verbally cal shapes but struggle to classify atypical and
name the result of simple problems demonstrated nonvalid instances of shapes (Satlow & Newcombe,
with objects or embedded in stories (Purpura & 1998). In elementary school, children learn defini-
Lonigan, 2013). Early in primary school, children can tional properties of two- and three-dimensional
calculate without the support of objects with a range shapes and learn to compose geometric shapes out
of numbers (Jordan et al., 2006). Object-supported of other shapes (Clements & Sarama, 2009; Satlow
calculation at ages 5 and 6 is correlated with later & Newcombe, 1998). We have not identified evi-
mathematics achievement (LeFevre et al., 2010), and dence that shape knowledge is correlated with gen-
general calculation knowledge in first and second eral math measures or predictive of later
grades is predictive of mathematics achievement the mathematics knowledge. Logically, children’s
following year (Cowan et al., 2011) as well as in the knowledge of shape properties seems most likely to
middle grades (Geary, 2011). Thus, we predicted that predict later achievement. We explored whether
calculation knowledge by the early primary grades shape knowledge in preschool or primary school
would predict later mathematics achievement. would predict later mathematics achievement.

Patterning Early Math Trajectories Model


Patterning is finding a predictable sequence, and Based on existing theory and evidence, we pro-
the first patterns children interact with are repeating pose the Early Math Trajectories Model (see Figure 1).
1730 Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Hofer, and Farran

Preschool Early Primary Middle Grades


(Ages 3-5) (Ages 7-8) (Ages 10-14a)

Non-Symbolic Symbolic Math


Quantyb Mapping Achievement
(Numeraon,
Algebra,
Counngc Calculaon Geometry &
Quantave
Concepts)

Paerning d Paerning

Shape?e

Figure 1. Early math trajectories model, including current evidence.


Note. aCurrent evidence is for ages 10–11. bNonsymbolic quantity knowledge was a reliable predictor of middle-grade math achieve-
ment at the beginning and end of pre-k. At the end of pre-k, nonsymbolic quantity knowledge continued to predict achievement with
first-grade mediators in the model, suggesting there may be a direct effect of nonsymbolic quantity knowledge (as indicated by the
dashed line) as well as an indirect effect (through the first-grade predictors). cCounting was a reliable predictor of achievement at the
beginning of pre-k but not the end of pre-k. At the beginning of pre-k, counting knowledge continued to predict achievement with
first-grade mediators in the model, suggesting there may be a direct effect of early counting knowledge (as indicated by the dashed
line) as well as an indirect effect (through the first-grade predictors). dPatterning was not adequately assessed at the beginning of pre-k,
but it was a reliable predictor of achievement at the end of pre-k, mediated through first-grade predictors. eShape knowledge was not a
unique predictor.

It includes six early math topics and proposes a tra- calculations. Nonsymbolic quantity and counting
jectory from preschool (ages 4–5) to early primary knowledge could also support pattern knowledge.
grades (ages 7–8) to middle grades (ages 10–14, with Counting objects is useful to reproduce the exact
a focus on ages 10–11 in the current study). number of each item in the core pattern unit and to
Although each type of knowledge begins to develop iterate the core unit an appropriate number of
before ages 4 and 5, this model focuses on time times. Nonsymbolic quantity knowledge of equiva-
points when individual differences in knowledge of lent sets may also support copying a pattern.
a particular topic are sufficient to predict later math- In preschool, pattern knowledge develops
ematics knowledge, as indicated by prior research. rapidly. Improving patterning knowledge in pre-
In preschool, nonsymbolic quantity and counting school supports the development of symbolic map-
knowledge are fairly well developed and are ping and calculation knowledge (Papic et al., 2011).
widely believed to support the development of Working with patterns may provide early opportu-
symbolic mapping and calculation knowledge. For nities to deduce underlying rules and/or promote
example, children have many experiences seeing a spatial skills. Sophisticated numeracy knowledge
set of objects and having its numerosity labeled by requires deducing underlying rules from examples,
adults or determining its cardinality through count- such as the successor principle for symbol–quantity
ing. These experiences help children learn the map- mappings (e.g., the next number name means add-
ping between particular quantities and verbal and ing one). Repeating pattern knowledge also involves
alphanumeric number names (Benoit et al., 2004; spatial relations and spatial short-term memory
Krajewski & Schneider, 2009). Furthermore, chil- (Collins & Laski, 2015), and spatial skills support
dren often count in order to calculate (Geary, 2011). numeracy development (Mix & Cheng, 2012).
Nonsymbolic quantity knowledge also supports In turn, individual differences in symbolic map-
precise calculation knowledge (Park & Brannon, ping and calculation knowledge in the early pri-
2013), perhaps because mentally tracking the mary grades are strong predictors of later
approximate number of items when objects are mathematics achievement (e.g., Geary, 2011). This
combined or separated helps ground precise knowledge is necessary for working with middle
Early Math Trajectories 1731

grades mathematics topics, such as advanced num- We evaluated three primary hypotheses based
ber, algebra, and geometry. Patterning knowledge on the early math trajectories model. First, we
in the primary grades supports mathematics hypothesized that in preschool, nonsymbolic quan-
achievement across a range of topics at the end of tity, counting, and patterning knowledge would
the same school year (Kidd et al., 2013, 2014) and predict age 11 mathematics achievement. Second,
so may also predict later mathematics achievement. we hypothesized that at the end of first grade, sym-
Identifying, extending, and describing patterns are bolic mapping, calculation, patterning, and possibly
important in middle grades mathematics, such as shape knowledge would predict age 11 mathemat-
determining functional relations and generalizing ics achievement and that nonsymbolic quantity and
place value knowledge. Finally, learning properties counting knowledge would no longer be predictive
of shapes in the primary grades may support geom- with a range of other measures in the model. Third,
etry knowledge directly and may support broader we hypothesized that knowledge of symbolic map-
middle grades math achievement because it pro- ping, calculation, and patterning in first grade
vides opportunities to develop the mathematical would mediate the relation between preschool math
practice of generalizing principles from examples. knowledge and age 11 achievement. We explored
Overall, the early math trajectories model sug- the role of shape knowledge.
gests that nonsymbolic quantity, counting, and pat-
terning knowledge in preschool may support
symbolic mapping, calculation, and patterning Method
knowledge in the primary grades, which in turn
Participants
may support middle grades mathematics achieve-
ment. Shape knowledge may also predict later Participants were drawn from a longitudinal fol-
mathematics achievement. Although previous low-up study of 517 children originally recruited at
research provides support for individual compo- the beginning of the prekindergarten year in 2006
nents of the model, it has not considered such a for a 3-year longitudinal study. Children had been
range of early math topics nor charted their impact recruited from 57 pre-k classes at 20 public schools
over a long period of time. and 4 Head Start sites in a large urban city in Ten-
nessee, all of which served children who qualified
for free or reduced priced lunch (family income
Current Study
< 1.85 times the U.S. federal income poverty guide-
In the current study, we evaluated the early math line). Of the 771 children in the original sample, we
trajectories model within a longitudinal study of were able to locate and reconsent 519 children in
over 500 low-income American children from ages 4 2013 for a 4-year follow-up study in middle school,
to 11. In the early years, children were assessed at all within the same large, urban school district.
four time points: the beginning of the pre-k school Outcome data are from the first year of the follow-
year (the year before entry into formal schooling in up study, and two children were not available for
the United States when children are 4), the end of retesting in this year so were not included in the
pre-k, the end of kindergarten, and the end of first current analytic sample. The final reconsented sam-
grade. Their mathematics achievement was assessed ple was 56% female, 79% Black, 9% White, 8% His-
4 years later when most children were in fifth grade panic, and 4% other races; 9% English learners; and
(age 11) because we were particularly interested in all continued to qualify for free or reduced price
how early math knowledge predicted success learn- lunch. Forty percent had attended preschool at pub-
ing the more challenging and diverse mathematical lic schools and the other 60% had attended pre-
content of the middle grades. Research comparing school at Head Start centers. Based on maternal
low-income children to their more advantaged peers report when the children were in pre-k, 43% of
indicates that their mathematics development is families had an annual income under $10,000, 38%
delayed but does not follow a different trajectory had an income between $10,000 and $25,000, and
(Claessens & Engel, 2013; Jordan et al., 2006). the remaining 19% had an income over $25,000;
Because the early math trajectories model is based 25% of mothers had less than a high school
on research that included children from more advan- diploma, 33% had a high school diploma or GED,
taged backgrounds, we expected that children in the and 42% had some postsecondary education.
current study might develop specific mathematics At the beginning of pre-k, children had a mean
knowledge at a slower rate but follow the same tra- age of 4.44 years (SD = 0.32), and at the time of the
jectory as proposed by the model. most recent assessment, the average age was
1732 Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Hofer, and Farran

11.1 years (SD = 0.33). Most students were in fifth knowledge as well as geometry, patterning, and
grade, but 14% had been retained a grade and were measurement knowledge. Based on our review of
still in fourth grade. Students were distributed the literature, we broke the numeracy items into
across 76 schools. We will continue to assess these four subscales. Purpura and Lonigan (2013) catego-
students each year throughout middle school. In rized REMA items similarly, except they did not
pre-k, children were drawn from classrooms that have a separate category for nonsymbolic quantity
had been randomly assigned to implement a speci- items (see Table S1 in the Supporting Information
fic prekindergarten mathematics curriculum (Build- for a list of each REMA item number that corre-
ing Blocks, Sarama & Clements, 2004) or to sponds to each subscale, and Table S2 for the num-
continue with business as usual instruction (316 ber of items and Cronbach’s alpha by time point
[61%] were part of the treatment group in pre-k, for each subscale).
and 201 [39%] were part of the control group). Nonsymbolic quantity items involved working
There were differences in mathematics knowledge with quantities without the need for verbal number
based on instructional condition at the end of pre-k labels or symbols. Easy items included deciding
but not at the end of kindergarten or first grade in which set of objects had more (e.g., in pictures of
this sample (Hofer, Lipsey, Dong, & Farran, 2013). three grapes vs. four grapes). Harder items
included ordering different numbers of objects from
smallest to largest (with 6–12 objects). There was no
Outcome Measures
time limit imposed on these items, so children
Four standardized mathematics assessments were could count to help them complete the task,
individually administered. The first was the quanti- although counting was not encouraged. Thus, the
tative concepts subtest from the Woodcock Johnson nonsymbolic items did not directly assess the
Achievement Battery III, which assesses students’ approximate number system or subitizing but
knowledge of basic mathematical concepts, symbols, rather tapped a more general construct of quantity
and vocabulary, including numbers, shapes, and knowledge that did not require symbolic mapping
sequences. Standard scores were used. The other knowledge. Internal consistency of the measure was
three were taken from the KeyMath 3 Diagnostic adequate (a = .52–.70; xt = .62–.86).
Assessment (Connolly, 2007), a norm-referenced Counting items involved knowledge of the num-
measure of essential mathematical concepts. The nu- ber–word sequence in forward and backward
meration subtest measures students’ understanding order, counting sets of objects in ordered and hap-
of whole and rational numbers, covering topics such hazard arrays, identifying the cardinality of a set,
as identifying, comparing, and rounding one-, two-, and detecting violations of the one to one corre-
and three-digit numbers as well as fractions, deci- spondence principle. Easy items included counting
mal values, and percentages. The algebra subtest or giving 4–8 objects. More difficult items included
measures students’ understanding of prealgebraic counting 30 pennies or 7 packs of 10 batteries.
and algebraic concepts, including recognizing and Internal consistency was high (a = .78–.87).
describing patterns and functions; working with Symbolic mapping items involved mapping
operational properties, variables, and equations; and between verbal number names or symbolic numerals
representing mathematical relations. The geometry to quantities. Easier items required mapping number
subtest measures students’ spatial reasoning and names to objects (e.g., matching the numeral 2 to the
ability to analyze, describe, compare, and classify picture of 2 grapes) and harder items required com-
two- and three-dimensional shapes. Other measures paring numerical values (“Which is biggest, 7 or 9 or
of basic cognitive and numeracy skills were assessed 5?” and “Which is smaller, 27 or 32?”). Internal con-
as part of the larger project, but are beyond the sistency was good (a = .77–.88).
scope of the current article. Calculation items involved combining or separat-
ing sets. Easier items involved calculations when
groups of objects were presented along with num-
Predictor Measures ber names, but some objects were not visible (e.g.,
figuring out how many pennies if added 6 more
Early Math Knowledge
pennies to 3 pennies under a cover). Harder items
Mathematics knowledge in pre-k through first involved calculations when objects were not
grade was assessed using the Research-Based Early included, with increasingly larger numbers (e.g.,
Mathematics Assessment (REMA; Clements, Sar- adding 3 to 69 or figuring out what number is 4 less
ama, & Liu, 2008). It focuses on numeracy than 60). Calculation items were not given at the
Early Math Trajectories 1733

beginning of pre-k, and although some calculation prekindergarten year is related to basic math
items were given at the end of pre-k, performance knowledge at the end of prekindergarten and
was very low (M = 0.2 out of 10), so we did not use kindergarten (Blair & Razza, 2007; McClelland
the measure at this time point. Internal consistency et al., 2007). Teachers rated children’s self-regulated
on the calculation items at the end of kindergarten behavior using the Instrumental Competence Scale
and first grade was strong (a = .88 and .91). for Young Children-Short Form (Lange & Adler,
Patterning items involved working with repeating 1997). Teachers rated behavior as displayed in the
patterns made out of colored shapes or unifix classroom on a 4-point Likert scale, and we used
cubes. Easier items involved copying and extending the four items focused on self-regulated behavior,
an AB pattern (e.g., making the same red–blue pat- such as “finishes tasks and activities” and “has dif-
tern with their own set of red and blue shapes or ficulty planning and carrying out activities that
continuing the current pattern by adding at least have several steps (reverse).” Scores could range
two colored shapes to the existing items). Harder from 1 to 4, with 4 reflecting the greatest regulation
items involved identifying the part that repeats in and 1 the least (a = .85–.89).
an AAB pattern or making the same kind of pattern
with different objects (e.g., for a red–yellow–yellow
Work-Related Skills
pattern, making the same kind of pattern using
green and blue blocks). Only a few patterning items Attentive behavior, especially in the classroom,
were given at the beginning of pre-k, with very low should increase children’s opportunities to engage
performance on the items (M = 0.6 of 4), so we did in and learn from instruction, and teacher rating of
not include a patterning measure at the beginning attentive behavior is predictive of later mathematics
of pre-k. At the remaining time points, internal con- achievement (Duncan et al., 2007; Fuchs et al.,
sistency was mediocre to adequate (a = .56–.63; 2014). Teacher rating of attention is moderately cor-
xt = .71–.81). The lower reliability of the patterning related with direct measures of working memory,
measure indicates a higher rate of random error in and teacher ratings can be as strong or a stronger
the measure, so we interpret findings with this predictor of future mathematics achievement than
measure cautiously. direct measure of working memory (Fuchs et al.,
Shape items focused on knowledge of shapes. 2012, 2014). In part, teacher ratings are situated in
Easier items focused on identifying or making a the context in which much mathematics learning
square, triangle, or rectangle, including discriminat- occurs and may better capture fluctuations across
ing atypical and nonexamples of each shape. Med- the day and from day to day in working memory
ium difficulty items included identifying the capacity and attention than a single, direct assess-
number of sides in a particular shape and compos- ment. Attentive behavior was measured via teacher
ing new shapes (e.g., hexagon) out of other shapes. ratings of children’s work-related skills items on the
Hard items focused on identifying same size angles Cooper–Farran Behavioral Rating Scale (Cooper &
and describing properties of triangles and squares. Farran, 1991). These 16 items assess children’s
Internal consistency was high (a = .89–.96). attentiveness, ability to follow directions, and task
A few measurement items were given on the persistence in the classroom on a 7-point behavior-
assessment but could not be used in the analyses. It anchored scale (a = .95–.96).
was not possible to create a reliable measurement
subscale because few items were given, few chil-
Narrative Recall Skill
dren answered the items correctly, and alpha relia-
bility was consistently below .5. Language skill, intelligence, and working mem-
ory capacity are also related to mathematics
achievement (Duncan et al., 2007; Geary, 2011). For
General Cognitive and Academic Skills
example, language plays a central role in learning
Mathematics achievement is also supported by mathematics because some mathematics knowledge
general cognitive and academic skills. We included is represented linguistically, and mathematics is
four measures that assessed a range of skills. most often taught formally and informally using
language (National Research Council, 2009). Narra-
tive recall was used as a direct measure of the com-
Self-Regulated Behavior
bination of these skills. Children listen to a
Self-regulated behavior includes the ability to narrative and then retell the story and/or answer
plan and finish tasks, and self-regulation in the comprehension questions. Narrative recall requires
1734 Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Hofer, and Farran

vocabulary knowledge, language skill, and working data sets and then aggregated them to run analy-
memory capacity, and empirical evidence indicates ses.
that narrative recall is moderately to strongly corre- Students were nested in their school at fifth
lated with independent measure of vocabulary, ver- grade. We used multilevel models to account for
bal IQ, and working memory capacity (Florit, Roch, this nesting. Specifically, we used multilevel regres-
Altoe, & Levorato, 2009; Strasser & Rıo, 2013). The sion models with two levels: (a) the individual level
measure differed by time point. At the beginning and (b) the school level. We ran a separate model
and end of pre-k, narrative recall skill was mea- at each predictive time point.
sured using the Renfrew Bus Story–North American
Edition (Glasgow & Cowley, 1994). We used the
information score, which captures the accuracy and Results
completeness of children’s retelling. At the end of
Descriptive Statistics
kindergarten and first grade, the Story Recall sub-
test from the Woodcock Johnson III Tests of Reflective of the disadvantaged nature of the
Achievement was used. Story Recall assesses chil- sample, average age-equivalent scores on the Key-
dren’s narrative recall by requiring them to answer Math assessment when children were an average of
questions about stories the assessor reads aloud. W 11.1 years indicated that children were approxi-
scores were used. mately 2 years behind in mathematics (numeration:
M = 9.2 years, SD = 2.0; algebra: M = 9.2, SD = 1.9;
geometry: M = 8.6, SD = 2.0). Similarly, children’s
Reading Skill
standard scores on the quantitative concepts assess-
Reading skill is also predictive of mathematics ment indicated that they were about two thirds of a
achievement (Duncan et al., 2007; Watts et al., standard deviation below the national average.
2014). As a measure of reading skill, the Letter– Descriptive statistics at each early time point are
Word Identification subtest of the Woodcock John- presented in the Supporting Information. Tables S3
son III Tests of Achievement was used. It assesses through S6 show descriptive statistics and correla-
children’s letter and word identification ability. W tions among key predictor variables within a single
scores were used. time point and the outcome variables. Patterns of
correlations were similar across time and all correla-
tions were statistically significant at p < .05.
Procedure
The four math outcomes in fifth grade were
All direct measures of children’s knowledge were strongly to very strongly correlated with each other,
collected one on one in a quiet room at the chil- rs(515) = .53–.83, ps < .001. Furthermore, early math
dren’s school. For all direct measures (except the knowledge had highly consistent correlation pat-
Renfrew Bus Story), there was a stop criterion such terns across the four outcomes. Thus, specific early
that after children failed a specified number of math topics did not seem to influence one math
items (depending on the measure, as specified by outcome but not another. For conciseness, we com-
the implementation manual), the assessment was bined the four outcomes into one composite “math-
stopped. Except for the beginning of pre-k assess- ematics achievement” measure. Specifically, we
ment time point, all data were collected near the summed z scores across the four measures and then
end of the school year. standardized the composite variable for interpreta-
tion purposes.
At each early time point, knowledge of each
Data Analysis
math topic was moderately correlated with math
Some children were missing data at one or more outcomes in fifth grade (see final row in Tables S3–
of the early time points. For early math predictors, S6). For example, Table S3 shows that correlations
missing data were rare and ranged from 0% to 7%. between math skills at the beginning of pre-k and
For early nonmath predictors, the percent of miss- mathematics achievement in fifth grade ranged
ing cases for any given variable ranged from 0% to from .34 to .44, ps < .001. Table S6 shows that cor-
22%, with most missing data involving teacher rat- relations were even stronger for relating math skills
ings of self-regulation and work-related skills. We at the end of first grade to mathematics achieve-
used multiple imputation in SPSS to impute all ment in fifth grade, ranging from .42 to .66,
missing values. Every analytic variable was ps < .001. Indeed, for math topics measured in both
included in the imputation model. We imputed 30 pre-k and first grade, the correlations with fifth-
Early Math Trajectories 1735

grade math achievement were statistically stronger so that parameter estimates represented standard-
in first-grade than at the beginning of pre-k, ized regression coefficients. Table 1 presents regres-
ps < .05. Shape and patterning skills had slightly sion results for key variables from our fully
weaker correlations with later achievement than controlled models (see Table S7 in Supporting Infor-
early numeracy measures. However, all early math mation for more details). Each model predicted
predictors tended to have similar or stronger corre- fifth-grade mathematics achievement from skills at
lations with fifth-grade mathematics achievement an earlier time point (beginning of pre-k, end of
than nonmath predictors, including reading, narra- pre-k, end of kindergarten, and end of first grade).
tive recall, work-related skills, and self-regulation. Several early math topics had substantial associa-
Because many correlations among predictors tions with mathematics achievement in fifth grade.
were moderate to high, we tested for multicollinear- Some of these associations were consistent from
ity by estimating variance inflation factors (VIF) for pre-k to first grade, whereas others changed over
all independent variables. A VIF score of 10 is con- time. Early nonmath skills were often less strongly
ventionally the threshold for testing whether multi- associated with fifth-grade mathematics achieve-
collinearity is biasing the results. All VIF scores for ment, and with the exception of narrative recall
independent variables were < 5, with two excep- skill, these nonmath skills were inconsistently pre-
tions. At the end of kindergarten, attentive behavior dictive of fifth-grade mathematics achievement.
and self-regulation had VIF scores of 5.25 and 5.03, Results were consistent with our hypotheses
respectively. based on the early math trajectories model (see
Table 1). First, we hypothesized that in pre-k, non-
symbolic quantity, counting, and patterning knowl-
Predicting Fifth-Grade Mathematics Achievement
edge would predict fifth-grade mathematics
Our primary goal was to evaluate the early achievement. At the beginning of pre-k, nonsym-
math trajectories model by testing whether knowl- bolic quantity and counting knowledge were
edge of specific math topics at early time points unique predictors, with a 1 SD increase in either
predicted mathematics achievement in fifth grade one associated with about an eighth of a standard
after controlling for other math and nonmath skills deviation increase in fifth-grade mathematics
as well as family and student demographics. We achievement (bs = .13, SEs = .05 and .06, ps < .05).
were interested in which math topics mattered and At the end of pre-k, nonsymbolic quantity and pat-
when they mattered. In each nested regression terning knowledge were unique predictors, with a
model, all continuous variables were standardized 1 SD increase in either one associated with about a

Table 1
Regression Estimates Predicting Mathematics in Fifth Grade From Early Math and Nonmath Skills

Mage = 4.4 Mage = 5.0 Mage = 6.1 Mage = 7


Beginning of pre-k End of pre-k End of kindergarten End of first grade
Measure 1 2 3 4

Math predictors
Nonsymbolic quantity .13 (.05)** .19 (.05)*** .12 (.06)* .03 (.05)
Counting .13 (.06)* .02 (.06) .07 (.06) .01 (.04)
Symbolic mapping .06 (.05) .11 (.06) .09 (.06) .26 (.05)***
Shape .05 (.04) .03 (.05) .05 (.04) .03 (.04)
Patterning –– .18 (.05)*** .08 (.04)* .08 (.04)*
Calculation –– –– .22 (.04)*** .24 (.04)***
Nonmath predictors
Reading .04 (.05) .10 (.04)* .07 (.04) .11 (.04)**
Narrative recall .11 (.04)* .17 (.04)*** .10 (.03)** .09 (.03)**
Work-related skills .10 (.07) .08 (.07) .04 (.06) .18 (.05)**
Self-regulation .05 (.07) .01 (.06) .21 (.06)** .01 (.05)
Controls Inc. Inc. Inc. Inc.

Note. Standard errors are in parentheses. Inc. = all models presented include the full list of control variables: age in fifth grade, current
grade level (fourth or fifth), gender, ELL status in pre-k, ethnicity, pre-k school type (public or Head Start), and socioeconomic status
(respondent’s education level and income level). *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
1736 Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Hofer, and Farran

fifth of a standard deviation increase in later achievement. We performed two mediation analy-
achievement (bs = .19 and .18, SEs = .05 and .06, ses that focused on the predictors at the beginning
ps < .05). As expected, nonsymbolic quantity and and end of pre-k. Specifically, we tested whether
counting lost their predictive power by the end of the associations between significant predictors in
first grade. pre-k and fifth-grade mathematics achievement
Second, we hypothesized that at the end of first were mediated by significant math predictors in
grade, symbolic mapping, calculation, patterning, first grade. We used nested regression models to
and possibly shape knowledge would predict fifth- complete the stepwise mediation approach recom-
grade mathematics achievement. Indeed, at the end mended by Baron and Kenny (1986), as well as a
of first grade, symbolic mapping (b = .26, SE = .05, bootstrapping technique recommended by Preacher
p < .05) and calculation (b = .24, SE = .04, p < .05) and Hayes (2008) to obtain estimates for the indi-
were the strongest predictors of fifth-grade mathe- rect effects of the pre-k predictors.
matics achievement. Patterning was also a unique As shown in Tables 2a and 3a, the relevant pre-
predictor of mathematics achievement (b = .08, dictors in pre-k were associated with the hypothe-
SE = .04, p < .05), but shape knowledge was not a sized mediators in first grade. Specifically, at the
unique predictor of mathematics achievement in beginning of pre-k, counting, nonsymbolic quantity,
fifth grade. and narrative recall all significantly predicted sym-
We also explored the role of different math bolic mapping, patterning, and calculation in first
topics at the end of kindergarten. Similar to the end grade (bs = .10–.24). At the end of pre-k, pattern-
of pre-k, nonsymbolic quantity and patterning ing, nonsymbolic quantity, reading, and narrative
knowledge at the end of kindergarten were unique recall all significantly predicted symbolic mapping,
predictors of fifth-grade mathematics achievement patterning, and calculation in first grade, with one
(b’s = .08–.12, see Table 1) as well as calculation exception for reading (bs = .03–.32). As shown in
skills at the end of kindergarten (b = .22, SE = .04, Tables 2b and 3b, mediation results indicate that
p < .001). Shape knowledge was never a unique the direct associations between predictors in pre-k
predictor of fifth-grade mathematics achievement. and mathematics achievement in fifth-grade
However, exploratory analyses indicated that shape (bs = .13–.25) were reduced in strength and often
knowledge was a significant predictor when pat- not significant (bs = .01–.13) after including the
terning knowledge was excluded. If patterning first-grade mediators.
knowledge was excluded, then shape knowledge The bootstrapping technique produced similar
was a significant predictor at the end of pre-k conclusions as shown in Tables S14 and S15 in the
(b = .10, SE = .04, p < .05), at the end of kinder- Supporting Information. For the beginning of pre-k
garten (b = .08, SE = .03, p < .05), and at the end of predictors, knowledge of the first-grade math topics
first grade (b = .08, SE = .03, p < .05). mediated the effect of counting and nonsymbolic
We verified that the general predictive patterns quantity knowledge. For the end of pre-k predic-
were similar for children originally assigned to tors, knowledge of the first-grade math topics medi-
treatment and control conditions in pre-k. That is, ated the effect of nonsymbolic quantity knowledge
the pattern of results in all three samples (i.e., and patterning knowledge. Furthermore, in each
whole sample, treatment children only, and control model, knowledge of all three first-grade topics
children only) largely supports the Early Math Tra- were significant mediators, but symbolic mapping
jectories model (see Tables S8 through S11 in Sup- and calculation knowledge were significantly stron-
porting Information for descriptive statistics and ger mediators than patterning in first grade.
correlations by condition, and Tables S12 and S13
for the regression results when the models were
done separately by condition). There was one devi-
Discussion
ation from our hypotheses. Specifically, for control
children only, calculation skill was not a statistically The current study provides strong evidence in
significant predictor at the end of first grade. How- support of the early math trajectories model for
ever, it was a significant predictor at the end of low-income children in the United States. Individ-
kindergarten, and the coefficient at the end of first ual differences in nonsymbolic quantity, counting,
grade was not trivial (b = .12, SE = .07). and patterning knowledge in preschool were pre-
Third, we hypothesized that first-grade math dictive of fifth-grade mathematics achievement over
knowledge would mediate the relation between and above a variety of other math and cognitive
pre-k knowledge and fifth-grade mathematics skills. By the end of first grade, individual
Early Math Trajectories 1737

Table 2 Table 3
First-Grade Skills as Mediators of the Association Between Beginning First-Grade Skills as Mediators of the Association Between End of
of Pre-K Skills and Fifth-Grade Mathematics Achievement Pre-K Skills and Fifth-Grade Mathematics Achievement

Hypothesized mediators in first Hypothesized mediators in first


grade grade

Symbolic Symbolic
mapping Patterning Calculation magnitude Patterning Calculation

(a) Regression estimates for predicting first-grade mediators from (a) Regression estimates for predicting first-grade mediators from
the beginning of pre-k skills the end of pre-k skills
Nonsymbolic quantity .17*** .19*** .23*** Nonsymbolic quantity .20*** .18*** .20***
Counting .16** .18** .24*** Patterning .23*** .14** .32***
Narrative recall .10* .11* .10* Reading .18*** .03 .11**
Controls Inc. Inc. Inc. Narrative recall .09* .19*** .10**
Controls Inc. Inc. Inc.
Predicting fifth-grade
mathematics achievement Predicting fifth-grade
mathematics achievement
Without With
mediators mediators Without With
mediators mediators
(b) Regression estimates for predicting fifth-grade mathematics
achievement with and without hypothesized mediators in first (b) Regression estimates for predicting fifth-grade mathematics
grade achievement with and without hypothesized mediators in first
Beginning of pre-k predictors grade
Nonsymbolic quantity .16*** .01 End of pre-k predictors
Counting .23*** .09* Nonsymbolic .25*** .13***
Narrative recall .13** .06 Patterning .23*** .07
End of first-grade mediators Reading .15*** .06
Symbolic mapping — .36*** Narrative recall .19*** .11**
Patterning — .10** End of first-grade mediators
Calculation — .29*** Symbolic mapping — .30***
Controls Inc. Inc. Patterning — .08*
Calculation — .26***
Note. Inc. = all models presented include the full list of control Controls Inc. Inc.
variables, which include age in fifth grade, current grade level
(fourth or fifth), gender, ELL status in pre-k, ethnicity, pre-k Note. Inc. = all models presented include the full list of control
school type (public or Head Start), and socioeconomic status (re- variables, which include age in fifth grade, current grade level
spondent’s education level and income level). *p < .05. **p < .01. (fourth or fifth), gender, ELL status in pre-k, ethnicity, pre-k
***p < .001. school type (Head Start or public), and socioeconomic status (re-
spondent’s education level and income level). *p < .05. **p < .01.
***p < .001.
differences in symbolic mapping, calculation, and
patterning knowledge were important predictors of Schneider, 2009; LeFevre et al., 2010). The current
later mathematics achievement; shape knowledge study extends past research in four important
was not. Furthermore, first-grade knowledge medi- ways.
ated the relation between preschool math knowl- First, the early math trajectories model was
edge and fifth-grade mathematics achievement, based on past research with children from more
supporting our proposed trajectory. advantaged backgrounds or from a range of eco-
nomic backgrounds without consideration of this
factor. The current results suggest that children
Early Math Trajectories Model
from low-income backgrounds follow a similar tra-
The Early Math Trajectories model was based on jectory. Such findings are consistent with research
past longitudinal evidence that identified knowl- that shows children from low-income backgrounds
edge of particular math topics at particular grade follow a similar trajectory in numeracy or general
levels that predicted mathematics achievement 1 to mathematics development as their peers from more
3 years later (Jordan et al., 2009; Krajewski & advantaged backgrounds (Claessens & Engel, 2013;
1738 Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Hofer, and Farran

Jordan et al., 2006). The current research suggests (De Smedt et al., 2013). Past research has reported
that children from low-income families follow a that symbolic mapping, but not approximate non-
similar trajectory when considering a wider range symbolic quantity knowledge, at ages 5 and 6 pre-
of knowledge topics over a wider age range. Chil- dicted mathematics achievement the following year
dren from low-income backgrounds experience per- when other measures were included in the model
sistent difficulties in mathematics, and the current (Kolkman et al., 2013; Sasanguie et al., 2012;
study highlights specific early math knowledge that Vanbinst et al., 2015). This prior research was con-
might merit particular attention. ducted with Dutch-speaking children from middle-
Second, we evaluated the role of two non- to upper-middle-income families and focused on
numeracy math topics—patterning and shape—that approximate nonsymbolic knowledge of large sets.
are rarely considered in longitudinal research. We In the current study, we worked with a large sam-
found that early patterning but not early shape ple of low-income U.S. children, our measure of
knowledge was a unique predictor of later mathe- nonsymbolic quantity knowledge tapped precise
matics achievement, over and above other math knowledge of nonsymbolic set sizes between 2 and
and nonmath skills. This is the first evidence that 12, and math achievement was assessed many years
early knowledge of repeating patterns is predictive later. By age 7, we found a similar result that sym-
of mathematics achievement many years later. This bolic mapping knowledge, but not nonsymbolic
converges with recent experimental evidence that quantity knowledge, predicted later mathematics
improving children’s patterning skill in preschool achievement. However, the reverse was true at
or first grade improves their mathematics knowl- younger ages, in which our measure of nonsym-
edge (Kidd et al., 2013, 2014; Papic et al., 2011). bolic quantity knowledge, but not symbolic map-
These results suggest that patterning knowledge ping knowledge, was predictive of later
merits increased attention in theories of mathemati- mathematics achievement (perhaps reflecting a
cal development, although the conclusions remain delay in our low-income sample). Furthermore,
tentative given the low reliability of the patterning symbolic mapping knowledge at age 7 helped
measure in the current study. Working with pat- mediate the relation between nonsymbolic quantity
terns may provide early opportunities to deduce knowledge in preschool and middle grades math
underlying rules and/or promote spatial skills. achievement. This provides the most direct evi-
In contrast, shape knowledge was tentatively dence to date in support of the hypothesis that with
included in our model because it is a component of age, symbolic mapping knowledge replaces non-
early mathematics standards in the United States symbolic quantity knowledge in predicting future
and considered foundational to later geometric mathematics achievement. The current findings
thinking (Common Core State Standards, 2010; lend support from a new type of population, a new
National Research Council, 2009). Early shape measure, and a longer time scale. However, these
knowledge was moderately correlated with later results must be interpreted with caution given the
mathematics achievement, including geometry restricted sample, the modest predictive power of
knowledge. However, early shape knowledge was our measures, and the limitations in our nonsym-
never a unique predictor of mathematics achieve- bolic quantity knowledge measure (e.g., there was
ment after controlling for other math and nonmath no time limit, so children could have counted to
knowledge. Exploratory analyses indicated that complete the items, and the reliability estimates
shape knowledge was a significant predictor when were low at some early time points).
patterning knowledge was excluded from the mod- Fourth, the current findings suggest that non-
els. Given the paucity of research on the topic, symbolic quantity knowledge may support more
future research needs to consider alternative ways than symbolic mapping knowledge. Nonsymbolic
to measure shape knowledge, how shape and pat- quantity knowledge in preschool was also predic-
terning knowledge are related, and how the two tive of calculation and patterning knowledge. Prior
contribute to mathematics development. research with adults indicates that improving non-
Third, the current study provides new evidence symbolic quantity knowledge for large sets leads to
on the long-term longitudinal relations between improved precise calculation (Park & Brannon,
nonsymbolic quantity, symbolic mapping knowl- 2013). Better facility with nonsymbolic quantities
edge, and mathematics achievement. Researchers may help ground calculation skill and promote
have theorized that with age, symbolic mapping knowledge of calculation principles (e.g., the n + 1
knowledge replaces nonsymbolic quantity knowl- principle for arithmetic). The link to patterning
edge in predicting future mathematics achievement knowledge is more speculative. For example,
Early Math Trajectories 1739

nonsymbolic quantity knowledge of equivalent sets needs to be included in longitudinal research. The
may support copying repeating patterns. Overall, model should also be evaluated when additional
the current findings highlight the need to attend to measures of early cognitive skills are available, such
the range of math knowledge that early nonsym- as spatial or executive function skills. For example,
bolic quantity knowledge may support, as well as patterning tasks require spatial skills, such as notic-
the decreasing importance of nonsymbolic quantity ing spatial relations between objects, and spatial
knowledge as those other skills develop. Future skills are correlated with mathematics achievement
research is needed to evaluate whether our findings (Mix & Cheng, 2012). It is also possible that our pat-
generalize to different populations and measures of terning measure taps early spatial skills more so than
nonsymbolic quantity knowledge. our shape measure and thus that spatial skills rather
than pattern knowledge per se explains the current
results. In addition, inclusion of direct, specific mea-
Educational Implications
sures of executive function skills, such as inhibitory
Longitudinal evidence cannot indicate the causal control, and nonverbal reasoning ability may reduce
role of different early math topics in mathematics the associations between early math knowledge and
development. However, the current findings sug- later mathematics achievement. In addition, the
gest that some early math topics may merit addi- model should be evaluated with children from a
tional attention in preschool and the primary broader range of backgrounds and at later grade
grades than they currently receive, at least for levels. Children from more advantaged backgrounds
low-income children. Counting, calculation, and often develop mathematics knowledge at a faster
symbolic mapping knowledge already receive con- rate, and it is possible that their developmental tra-
siderable attention from teachers and parents jectories are not the same as for low-income children.
(Skwarchuk, Sowinski, & LeFevre, 2014). However, Furthermore, the unique influence of specific early
nonsymbolic quantity knowledge may merit more math topics on specific later math topics (e.g., shape
attention in preschool, and patterning knowledge knowledge predicting geometry but not numeration
may merit more attention across preschool and the knowledge) may emerge in later grades as mathe-
early primary grades. For example, recent U.S. con- matics instruction and knowledge becomes more
tent standards for school mathematics include specialized. Finally, future research needs to better
shape, but not patterning knowledge, and give lim- specify the theoretical underpinnings of the model,
ited mention of nonsymbolic quantity knowledge such as how and why one type of knowledge might
(Common Core State Standards, 2010). The current support another type of knowledge.
findings do not align with this choice of topics. In conclusion, findings supported the early math
Causal evidence is needed before making policy trajectories model. Nonsymbolic quantity, counting,
recommendations, although causal evidence for the and patterning knowledge in preschool predicted
value of patterning in the early grades is already in fifth-grade mathematics achievement. By the end of
place (Kidd et al., 2014; Papic et al., 2011). first grade, symbolic mapping, calculation, and pat-
terning knowledge became important predictors,
and first-grade skills mediated the relation between
Limitations and Future Directions
preschool skills and fifth-grade achievement. Both
The current study was the first evaluation of our theory and practice must attend more to trajectories
Early Math Trajectories model, and it focused on a from knowledge of specific early math topics to
sample of low-income American children. The pro- middle grades mathematics achievement.
posed knowledge components could be measured in
a variety of ways, so it will be important to test
whether the model holds up when topics are opera- References
tionalized in other ways (e.g., nonsymbolic quantity
Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Lerkkanen, M.-K., & Nurmi, J.-
knowledge measured via approximate number sys- E. (2004). Developmental dynamics of math perfor-
tem acuity), further subdivided (e.g., separating ver- mance from preschool to grade 2. Journal of Educational
bal counting and counting objects) or expanded (e.g., Psychology, 96, 699–713. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.699
to include numeral knowledge, Ryoo et al., 2015). Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-med-
This is particularly true for shape knowledge iator variable distinction in social psychology research:
because past research has not evaluated its predic- Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.
tive value. A more reliable and appropriate measure Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–
of patterning knowledge for younger children also 1182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
1740 Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Hofer, and Farran

Barth, H., La Mont, K., Lipton, J., & Spelke, E. S. (2005). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental
Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. Psychology, 43, 1428–1446. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Uni- 1428
ted States of America, 102, 14116–14121. doi:10.1073/ Fazio, L. K., Bailey, D. H., Thompson, C. A., & Siegler, R.
pnas.0505512102 S. (2014). Relations of different types of numerical
Benoit, L., Lehalle, H., & Jouen, F. (2004). Do young chil- magnitude representations to each other and to mathe-
dren acquire number words through subitizing or matics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
counting? Cognitive Development, 19, 291–307. chology, 123, 53–72. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.013
doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.03.005 Florit, E., Roch, M., Altoe, G., & Levorato, M. C. (2009).
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, Listening comprehension in preschoolers: The role of
executive function, and false belief understanding to memory. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27,
emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. 935–951. doi:10.1348/026151008x397189
Child Development, 78, 647–663. doi:10.1111/j.1467- Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Powell, S. R.,
8624.2007.01019.x Schumacher, R. F., Hamlett, C. L., & Vukovic, R. K.
Claessens, A., & Engel, M. (2013). How important is (2012). Contributions of domain-general cognitive
where you start? Early mathematics knowledge and resources and different forms of arithmetic develop-
later school success. Teachers College Record, 115, 1–29. ment to pre-algebraic knowledge. Developmental Psy-
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and teaching chology, 48, 1315–1326. doi:10.1037/a0027475
early math: The learning trajectories approach. New York, Fuchs, L. S., Geary, D. C., Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., &
NY: Routledge. Hamlett, C. L. (2014). Sources of individual differences
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J. H., & Liu, X. H. (2008). in emerging competence with numeration understand-
Development of a measure of early mathematics ing versus multidigit calculation skill. Journal of Educa-
achievement using the rasch model: The research-based tional Psychology, 106, 482–498. doi:10.1037/a0034444
early maths assessment. Educational Psychology, 28, 457– Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement
482. doi:10.1080/01443410701777272 growth in mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study.
Chen, Q., & Li, J. (2014). Association between individual Developmental Psychology, 47, 1539–1552. doi:10.1037/
differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math a0025510
performance: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 148, Gilmore, C. K., McCarthy, S. E., & Spelke, E. S. (2010).
163–172. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.016 Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics
Collins, M. A., & Laski, E. V. (2015). Preschoolers’ strate- achievement in the first year of formal schooling.
gies for solving visual pattern tasks. Early Childhood Cognition, 115, 394–406. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.
Research Quarterly, 32, 204–214. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq. 02.002
2015.04.004 Glasgow, C., & Cowley, J. (1994). Renfrew Bus Story Test—
Common Core State Standards. (2010). Washington, DC: North American edition. Centreville, DE: Centreville
National Governors Association Center for Best Prac- School.
tices & Council of Chief State School Officers. Hofer, K. G., Lipsey, M. W., Dong, N., & Farran, D.
Connolly, A. J. (2007). KeyMath—3 diagnostic assessment. (2013). Results of the early math project: Scale-up cross-site
San Antonio, TX: Pearson. results. Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University. Retrieved
Cooper, D. H., & Farran, D. (1991). The Cooper-Farran from my.vanderbilt.edu/mathfollowup/reports/techni-
behavioral rating scales. Brandon, VT: Clinical Psychol- calreports/
ogy. Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2009). Mapping numerical
Cowan, R., Donlan, C., Shepherd, D.-L., Cole-Fletcher, R., magnitudes onto symbols: The numerical distance
Saxton, M., & Hurry, J. (2011). Basic calculation profi- effect and individual differences in children’s mathe-
ciency and mathematics achievement in elementary matics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, chology, 103, 17–29. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001
786–803. doi:10.1037/a0024556 Huttenlocher, J., Jordan, N. C., & Levine, S. C. (1994). A
De Smedt, B., No€el, M., Gilmore, C. K., & Ansari, D. mental model for early arithmetic. Journal of Experimen-
(2013). The relationship between symbolic and nonsym- tal Psychology: General, 123, 284–296. doi:10.1037/0096-
bolic numerical magnitude processing and the typical 3445.123.3.284
and atypical development of mathematics: Evidence Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Nabors Olah, L., & Locuniak,
from brain and behavior. Trends in Neuroscience and M. N. (2006). Number sense growth in kindergarten: A
Education, 2, 48–55. doi:10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.001 longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathe-
Desoete, A., & Grégoire, J. (2006). Numerical competence matics difficulties. Child Development, 77, 153–175.
in young children and in children with mathematics doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00862.x
learning disabilities. Learning and Individual Differences, Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M.
16, 351–367. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2006.12.006 N. (2009). Early math matters: Kindergarten number
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, competence and later mathematics outcomes. Develop-
K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., & Japel, C. (2007). mental Psychology, 45, 850–867. doi:10.1037/a0014939
Early Math Trajectories 1741

Kidd, J. K., Carlson, A. G., Gadzichowski, K. M., Boyer, Psychological Science, 24, 2013–2019. doi:10.1177/09567
C. E., Gallington, D. A., & Pasnak, R. (2013). Effects of 97613482944
patterning instruction on the academic achievement of Piazza, M. (2010). Neurocognitive start-up tools for sym-
1st-grade children. Journal of Research in Childhood Edu- bolic number representations. Trends in Cognitive
cation, 27, 224–238. doi:10.1080/02568543.2013.766664 Sciences, 14, 542–551. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.008
Kidd, J. K., Pasnak, R., Gadzichowski, K. M., Gallington, Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and
D. A., McKnight, P., Boyer, C. E., & Carlson, A. (2014). resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indi-
Instructing first-grade children on patterning improves rect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior
reading and mathematics. Early Education & Develop- Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi:10.3758/brm.40.3.879
ment, 25, 134–151. doi:10.1080/10409289.2013.794448 Purpura, D. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2013). Informal numeracy
Kolkman, M. E., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. skills: The structure and relations among numbering,
(2013). Early numerical development and the role of relations, and arithmetic operations in preschool. Ameri-
non-symbolic and symbolic skills. Learning and Instruc- can Educational Research Journal, 50, 178–209. doi:10.
tion, 25, 95–103. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.12.001 3102/0002831212465332
Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Exploring the Reyna, V. F., Nelson, W. L., Han, P. K., & Dieckmann, N.
impact of phonological awareness, visual-spatial work- F. (2009). How numeracy influences risk comprehen-
ing memory, and preschool quantity-number compe- sion and medical decision making. Psychological Bul-
tencies on mathematics achievement in elementary letin, 135, 943–973. doi:10.1037/a0017327
school: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Jour- Ritchie, S. J., & Bates, T. C. (2013). Enduring links from
nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 516–531. childhood mathematics and reading achievement to
doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.009 adult socioeconomic status. Psychological Science, 24,
Lange, G., & Adler, F. (1997, April). Motivation and 1301–1308. doi:10.1177/0956797612466268
achievement in elementary children. Paper presented at Rittle-Johnson, B., Fyfe, E. R., Hofer, K., & Farran, D.
the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in (2015, March). It’s a pattern! The importance of early pat-
Child Development, Washington, DC. tern knowledge for middle grade mathematics achievement.
LeFevre, J. A., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S. L., Smith-Chant, B. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child
L., Bisanz, J., Kamawar, D., & Penner-Wilger, M. Development, Philadelphia, PA.
(2010). Pathways to mathematics: Longitudinal predic- Rittle-Johnson, B., Fyfe, E. R., McLean, L. E., & McEldoon,
tors of performance. Child Development, 81, 1753–1767. K. L. (2013). Emerging understanding of patterning in
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x 4-year-olds. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14,
Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2013). Is 376–396. doi:10.1080/15248372.2012.689897
approximate number precision a stable predictor of Ryoo, J. H., Molfese, V. J., Brown, E. T., Karp, K. S.,
math ability? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, Welch, G. W., & Bovaird, J. A. (2015). Examining factor
126–133. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2013.02.001 structures on the test of early mathematics ability-3: A
McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., longitudinal approach. Learning and Individual Differ-
Farris, C. L., Jewkes, A. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2007). ences, 41, 21–29. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2015.06.003
Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2004). Building Blocks for
literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental early childhood mathematics. Early Childhood Research
Psychology, 43, 947–959. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4. Quarterly, 19, 181–189. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.014
947 Sasanguie, D., Van den Bussche, E., & Reynvoet, B.
Mix, K. S., & Cheng, Y.-L. (2012). The relation between (2012). Predictors for mathematics achievement? Evi-
space and math. In J. B. Benson (Ed.), Advances in child dence from a longitudinal study. Mind Brain and Educa-
development and behavior (Vol. 42, pp. 197–243). New tion, 6, 119–128. doi:10.1111/j.1751-228X.2012.01147.x
York, NY: Elsevier. Satlow, E., & Newcombe, N. (1998). When is a triangle
Muldoon, K. P., Towse, J., Simms, V., Perra, O., & Men- not a triangle? Young children’s developing concepts of
zies, V. (2013). A longitudinal analysis of estimation, geometric shape. Cognitive Development, 13, 547–559.
counting skills, and mathematical ability across the first doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90006-5
school year. Developmental Psychology, 49, 250–257. Skwarchuk, S. L., Sowinski, C., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2014).
doi:10.1037/a0028240 Formal and informal home learning activities in rela-
National Research Council. (2009). Mathematics learning in tion to children’s early numeracy and literacy skills:
early childhood: Paths toward excellence and equity. Wash- The development of a home numeracy model. Journal
ington, DC: National Academies Press. of Experimental Child Psychology, 121, 63–84.
Papic, M. M., Mulligan, J. T., & Mitchelmore, M. C. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.006
(2011). Assessing the development of preschoolers’ Starkey, P., & Cooper, R. G. (1980). Perception of num-
mathematical patterning. Journal for Research in Mathe- bers by human infants. Science, 210, 1033–1035.
matics Education, 42, 237–268. doi:10.1126/science.7434014
Park, J., & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Training the approxi- Strasser, K., & Rıo, F. D. (2013). The role of comprehen-
mate number system improves math proficiency. sion monitoring, theory of mind, and vocabulary depth
1742 Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Hofer, and Farran

in predicting story comprehension and recall of kinder- Table S5. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
garten children. Reading Research Quarterly, 49, 169–187. for Key Predictors at the End of Pre-K and Key
doi:10.1002/rrq.68 Outcomes in Fifth Grade
van Marle, K., Chu, F. W., Li, Y., & Geary, D. C. (2014). Table S6. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
Acuity of the approximate number system and
for Key Predictors at the End of Kindergarten and
preschoolers’ quantitative development. Developmental
Key Outcomes in Fifth Grade
Science, 17, 492–505. doi:10.1111/desc.12143
Vanbinst, K., Ghesquiere, P., & De Smedt, B. (2015). Does Table S7. Regression Estimates Predicting Math
numerical processing uniquely predict first graders’ Achievement in Fifth Grade From Early Math and
future development of single-digit arithmetic? Learning Nonmath Skills
and Individual Differences, 37, 153–160. doi:10.1016/j.lin- Table S8. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
dif.2014.12.004 for Key Predictors at the Beginning of Pre-K and Key
Watts, T. W., Duncan, G. J., Siegler, R. S., & Davis-Kean, Outcomes in Fifth Grade for Treatment Condition
P. E. (2014). What’s past is prologue: Relations between Table S9. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
early mathematics knowledge and high school achieve- for Key Predictors at the Beginning of Pre-K and Key
ment. Educational Researcher, 43, 352–360. doi:10.3102/ Outcomes in Fifth Grade for Control Condition
0013189x14553660
Table S10. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
Xu, F., Spelke, E. S., & Goddard, S. (2005). Number sense
for Key Predictors at the End of First Grade and Key
in human infants. Developmental Science, 8, 88–101.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00395.x Outcomes in Fifth Grade for Treatment Condition
Table S11. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
for Key Predictors at the End of First Grade and Key
Supporting Information Outcomes in Fifth Grade for Control Condition
Table S12. Regression Estimates Predicting Math
Additional supporting information may be found in Achievement in Fifth Grade From Early Math and
the online version of this article at the publisher’s Nonmath Skills for Treatment Condition
website: Table S13. Regression Estimates Predicting Math
Table S1. Categorization of Research-Based Early Achievement in Fifth Grade From Early Math and
Mathematics Assessment (REMA) Items Into Each Nonmath Skills for Control Condition
of the Six Early Math Subscales Table S14. Bootstrap Results for Indirect Effects of
Table S2. Cronbach’s Alpha by Time Point for Predictors at the Beginning of Pre-K on Fifth-Grade
Each Early Math Subscale Math Achievement Through Proposed Mediators in
Table S3. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations First Grade
for Key Predictors at the Beginning of Pre-K and Table S15. Bootstrap Results for Indirect Effects of
Key Outcomes in Fifth Grade Predictors at the End of Pre-K on Fifth-Grade Math
Table S4. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Achievement Through Proposed Mediators in First
for Key Predictors at the End of First Grade and Grade
Key Outcomes in Fifth Grade
This document is a scanned copy of a printed document. No warranty is given about the
accuracy of the copy. Users should refer to the original published version of the material.

You might also like