You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/305512554

Measuring emotions during epistemic activities: the Epistemically-Related


Emotion Scales

Article  in  Cognition and Emotion · July 2016


DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1204989

CITATIONS READS

195 5,252

4 authors:

Reinhard Pekrun Elisabeth Vogl


Ludwig-Maximilian-University of Munich (LMU); Australian Catholic University (ACU) Ludwig-Maximilians-University of Munich
385 PUBLICATIONS   37,538 CITATIONS    44 PUBLICATIONS   945 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Krista R. Muis Gale M. Sinatra


McGill University University of Southern California
95 PUBLICATIONS   4,696 CITATIONS    139 PUBLICATIONS   8,936 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

DFG-Projekt "FEEL - Forschung zum Emotionalen Erleben im Lehr-Lern-Kontext" View project

ECoWeB: Assessing and Enhancing Emotional Competence for Well-Being in the Young: A principled, evidence-based, mobile-health approach to prevent mental
disorders and promote mental well-being View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Gale M. Sinatra on 25 July 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Cognition and Emotion

ISSN: 0269-9931 (Print) 1464-0600 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcem20

Measuring emotions during epistemic activities:


the Epistemically-Related Emotion Scales

Reinhard Pekrun, Elisabeth Vogl, Krista R. Muis & Gale M. Sinatra

To cite this article: Reinhard Pekrun, Elisabeth Vogl, Krista R. Muis & Gale M. Sinatra (2016):
Measuring emotions during epistemic activities: the Epistemically-Related Emotion Scales,
Cognition and Emotion, DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1204989

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1204989

View supplementary material

Published online: 22 Jul 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pcem20

Download by: [93.204.107.184] Date: 22 July 2016, At: 23:20


COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1204989

BRIEF ARTICLE

Measuring emotions during epistemic activities: the Epistemically-


Related Emotion Scales
Reinhard Pekruna,b†, Elisabeth Vogla†, Krista R. Muisc and Gale M. Sinatrad
a
Department of Psychology, University of Munich, Munich, Germany; bInstitute for Positive Psychology and Education, Australian
Catholic University, Sydney, Australia; cDepartment of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal,
Quebec, Canada; dRossier School of Education, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Measurement instruments assessing multiple emotions during epistemic activities are Received 23 July 2015
largely lacking. We describe the construction and validation of the Epistemically- Revised 12 June 2016
Accepted 15 June 2016
Downloaded by [93.204.107.184] at 23:20 22 July 2016

Related Emotion Scales, which measure surprise, curiosity, enjoyment, confusion,


anxiety, frustration, and boredom occurring during epistemic cognitive activities.
KEYWORDS
The instrument was tested in a multinational study of emotions during learning Epistemic emotion; surprise;
from conflicting texts (N = 438 university students from the United States, Canada, curiosity; confusion; value
and Germany). The findings document the reliability, internal validity, and external appraisal
validity of the instrument. A seven-factor model best fit the data, suggesting that
epistemically-related emotions should be conceptualised in terms of discrete
emotion categories, and the scales showed metric invariance across the North
American and German samples. Furthermore, emotion scores changed over time as
a function of conflicting task information and related significantly to perceived task
value and use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies.

Research has started to acknowledge the importance Further progress in research on epistemic emotions
of emotions for complex learning and cognitive per- requires measurement instruments to assess these
formance (see e.g. Pekrun & Linnenbrink-Garcia, emotions. Specifically, instruments are needed that
2014). In this nascent field of inquiry, researchers measure multiple emotions during epistemic activity
have focused on examining achievement emotions, and that are suited to track the dynamics of these
such as hope, pride, anxiety, or shame related to emotions over time. To date, measurement of these
success and failure. However, emotions that are trig- emotions has focused on assessing curiosity (Jirout &
gered by the cognitive characteristics of tasks can Klahr, 2012). In addition, researchers have used qualitat-
also be of fundamental importance for learning, ive self-report and single-item instruments assessing
such as surprise, curiosity, or confusion about contra- various emotions, such as emotion checklists (e.g.
dictory information. As proposed by epistemologists, D’Mello & Graesser, 2012). By contrast, systematic
these affective states represent epistemic emotions multi-item instruments suited to assess a broader
because they relate to the knowledge-generating range of these emotions are lacking. We aimed to
qualities of cognitive activities (Brun, Doğuoğlu, & redress this deficit by developing multi-item self-report
Kuenzle, 2008; Morton, 2010). Empirical findings scales measuring seven major emotions that occur
support the view that epistemic emotions can strongly during epistemic activity and are believed to be of
impact learning and performance (e.g. D’Mello, primary importance for learning and the generation of
Lehman, Pekrun, & Graesser, 2014; Kang et al., 2009). knowledge (Epistemically-Related Emotion Scales (EES)).

CONTACT Reinhard Pekrun pekrun@lmu.de


Supplemental data for this article can be accessed here doi:10.1080/02699931.2016.1204989.

The first two authors contributed equally to this work.
© 2016 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2 R. PEKRUN ET AL.

We first outline the concept of epistemic emotions state qualifies as an epistemic emotion if it comprises
that guided this research. Next, we describe the devel- these elements. Accordingly, surprise, enjoyment,
opment of the EES. We then report results from a mul- anxiety, frustration, and boredom related to knowl-
tinational investigation that used conflicting texts edge and knowing can be considered epistemic
about climate change to arouse epistemic emotions. emotions. This is also true for curiosity and confusion
Data from this study were employed to examine the which traditionally have not been classified as
reliability, internal validity, and external validity of emotions, but have been shown to involve affective
the instrument. feelings, physiological arousal, motivational impulses,
as well as specific patterns of facial expression (e.g.
Markey & Loewenstein, 2014; Reeve, 1993; Rozin &
Defining epistemic emotions
Cohen, 2003).
Epistemic emotions relate to the knowledge-generat- Epistemic emotions differ from other groups of
ing qualities of cognitive tasks and activities (Brun human emotions, such as social, moral, or achieve-
et al., 2008). For these emotions, knowledge and the ment emotions, in terms of their object focus (Brun
generation of knowledge are the objects of emotions. et al., 2008). As noted, knowledge and the generation
This definition of epistemic emotions by object focus of knowledge are the objects for epistemic emotions;
Downloaded by [93.204.107.184] at 23:20 22 July 2016

is equivalent to the definition of other types of episte- in social, moral, and achievement emotions, other
mic variables that share the same focus, such as epis- persons, moral norms, or success and failure, respect-
temic cognition, epistemic metacognition, and ively, are the objects. Some emotions are epistemic by
epistemic beliefs. Epistemic emotions differ from nature, such as curiosity and confusion, whereas
these other epistemic variables by their affective others can belong to various categories of emotion,
nature. depending on the object focus of attention. For
Epistemic emotions represent a major category of example, during cognitive activities, some emotions
human emotion serving evolutionary-based purposes can be experienced as epistemic emotions or as
of acquiring knowledge about the world and the self. achievement emotions (Pekrun & Perry, 2014). A stu-
A prototypical situation for the arousal of epistemic dent’s frustration at not deriving a correct solution
emotions is discrepant information and appraisals of to a mathematics problem would be considered an
cognitive incongruity that can trigger surprise and epistemic emotion if the focus is on the cognitive
curiosity (Kang et al., 2009), confusion, frustration, incongruity resulting from the unsolved problem.
and boredom when the incongruity cannot be However, if the focus is on personal failure and the
resolved (D’Mello & Graesser, 2012; D’Mello et al., inability to solve the problem, then the student’s frus-
2014), anxiety in the case of severe incongruity and tration would be considered an achievement emotion.
information that deeply disturbs existing beliefs As such, it is important to acknowledge that epistemic
(Hookway, 2008), or enjoyment and delight when emotions can share affective properties with other cat-
the problem is solved. Appraisals of the positive egories of emotion but differ from them in terms of
value of an epistemic activity should promote positive their object focus (Brun et al., 2008).
epistemic emotions (curiosity, enjoyment) and reduce
boredom. Epistemic emotions are closely related to
Development of the EES
the concepts of cognitive emotions (Scheffler, 1991)
and knowledge emotions (Silvia, 2010). We prefer to We aimed to develop a self-report instrument captur-
use the term epistemic emotion because it is well ing major emotions that occur frequently in epistemic
established in epistemology (Brun et al., 2008) and contexts and are functionally relevant for learning and
aligned with terms denoting other constructs related cognitive problem solving. Curiosity, joy, confusion,
to epistemic processes, such as epistemic beliefs. anxiety, frustration, and boredom have been found
To conceptualise epistemic emotions, it is critical to to be especially frequent and powerful in these con-
attend to the defining features of emotion. We texts (see e.g. D’Mello, 2013; Pekrun & Stephens,
propose to adopt a multicomponent approach (e.g. 2012). We added surprise which represents an
Scherer, 2009), which implies that emotions are immediate affective reaction to cognitive incongruity
systems of coordinated psychological processes and serves to promote subsequent epistemic cogni-
including affective, cognitive, physiological, motiva- tion. As such, we developed scales measuring surprise,
tional, and behavioural components. An affective curiosity, enjoyment, confusion, anxiety, frustration,
COGNITION AND EMOTION 3

and boredom. The scales represent all of the emotions factor analysis (CFA) were employed to test internal
targeted in D’Mello and Graesser’s (2012; D’Mello et al., validity in terms of the distinctiveness of the seven
2014) model of impasse-driven emotions and Pekrun emotions and the measurement equivalence of the
and Stephens’s (2012) classification of epistemic EES across the participant samples from different
emotions. countries.
Our goal was to create an instrument that tracks To evaluate external validity, we analysed the
changes in these emotions across episodes of cogni- impact of the conflicting texts, which were supposed
tive activity and renders psychometrically sound to increase surprise, curiosity, and confusion. In
scores. Accordingly, it was necessary to create multiple addition, we investigated relations between the
scales consisting of several items each. However, to measured emotions, the perceived value of the learn-
allow for an examination of these emotions as tem- ing task, and participants’ self-reported use of cogni-
porally fluctuating states, the instrument needs to be tive and metacognitive learning strategies. We
simple and quick to administer. expected perceived value of the task to relate posi-
To serve these purposes, the EES uses emotion tively to the intensity of the emotions except
adjectives as items, which allows for quick responses boredom, and we expected activating emotions (i.e.
and limits the influence of the items on respondents’ surprise, curiosity, enjoyment, confusion, anxiety, and
Downloaded by [93.204.107.184] at 23:20 22 July 2016

emotions. To keep scale length at a minimum, three frustration) to correlate positively and boredom to cor-
items were included per scale. As such, the instrument relate negatively with strategy use (see Muis et al.,
contains 7 three-item scales (21 items overall; Appen- 2015; Pekrun & Stephens, 2012).
dix). Selection of items was based on existing emotion
scales and information about frequently used emotion
words (see Supplemental Material). Items are Method
answered using a 5-point Likert scale that asks Participants
respondents to report how strongly they feel the
emotion (1 = not at all to 5 = very strong). The instru- The sample included N = 438 university students from
ment includes contextualised instructions (Appendix), the United States (n = 138, 116 females), Canada (n =
which can be adapted to address different settings. 152, 114 females), and Germany (n = 148, 129
Additionally, a short version of the EES is available females). Sample size was defined by the availability
that uses one item per emotion only (Appendix). of participants. At each of the three sites, we recruited
Items were selected for their semantic properties to as many participants as possible until the end of the
best represent the respective emotion, and all had predefined time period for the study, and the data
factor loadings >.69 (Supplemental Material; Table of all participants were included in the analysis.
S1). This version may be especially useful when it is Mean age was M = 21.76 years (SD = 4.28 years), with
necessary to keep administration time short. Similar M = 20.09, 21.81, and 23.27 (SD = 1.72, 3.87, and 5.67)
to experience sampling methodology, it is minimally for the United States, Canadian, and German
invasive and can be employed to measure respon- samples, respectively.
dents’ emotions while carrying out a task.
Procedure
Aims of the present research
The EES was administered as part of Muis et al.’s (2015)
The present study examined item and scale statistics, study on epistemic beliefs and epistemic processes
reliability, internal test validity, and external test val- when reading conflicting texts about climate
idity for the EES. Data from a multinational investi- change. Being a controversial topic, this issue is par-
gation were used (Muis et al., 2015). The study ticularly well suited to induce epistemic emotions.
examined epistemic emotions in the context of a learn- The findings of a think-aloud study using the same
ing task in which students had to read and interpret texts confirmed that this procedure elicited cognitive
conflicting texts. In the conceptual change literature, conflict, and that the vast majority of the emotions
use of conflicting information presented in texts is a that occurred during reading the texts were epistemic
standard procedure to trigger cognitive conflict (Muis (Muis et al., 2015).
& Duffy, 2013), which is expected to induce epistemic Participants first responded to a task value ques-
emotions. Correlational analysis and confirmatory tionnaire. Next, they were confronted with four texts,
4 R. PEKRUN ET AL.

which were adopted from Bråten and Strømsø (2009). EES. To estimate parameters, the robust maximum like-
The first pair of texts presented conflicting information lihood estimator (MLR) was used which is robust to non-
on the causes of climate change (man-made vs. normality of the observed variables. Emotions were
natural), whereas the second pair presented conflict- modelled as latent variables, and the scale items
ing information on the consequences of climate served as manifest indicators. First, CFA was used to
change (positive vs. negative). Prior to reading the examine the structural validity of the EES, including
texts, participants were alerted to the conflicting infor- competitive testing of different models representing
mation contained in the texts (see Supplemental the seven emotions (Figure 1). Second, multi-group
Material). After each of the first three texts, partici- CFA was employed to investigate measurement invar-
pants completed the short 7-item version of the EES iance in a series of sequentially more restrictive
to report the emotions they had experienced while models fixing parameters to be equal across participant
reading the texts. After the last text, participants com- groups from different countries. The fit of each model
pleted the full 21-item version of the EES to report the was compared to the fit of the previous model. Specifi-
emotions they had experienced while reading all four cally, we tested (a) a configural invariance model; (b) a
texts. Following this, they self-reported the frequency metric invariance model that restricted factor loadings
with which they used cognitive and metacognitive to be equal across groups; and (c) a scalar invariance
Downloaded by [93.204.107.184] at 23:20 22 July 2016

strategies to learn the material across the four texts. model that additionally restricted intercepts to be
equal (Byrne, 2008).
Measures
Emotions Results and discussion
The long and short versions of the EES were used to Item and scale statistics
assess students’ epistemically-related emotions (see
Appendix). Item and scale statistics document that there was suf-
ficient variance in scores on all items and scales,
Perceived task value and learning strategies despite positive skewness for some of the scales (see
Perceived value of the task was assessed with seven Supplemental Material, Tables S1 and S2). Further-
items adapted from Wigfield (1994; e.g. “In general, I more, item-total correlations (part-whole corrected)
find learning about climate change very interesting”). were above .53 for all items, and reliabilities were in
Four scales from the Motivated Strategies for Learn- the α = .76 to .88 range (Table 1). These findings indi-
ing Questionnaire (MSLQ; Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & cate that the EES scales show sufficient variation and
McKeachie, 1993) assessed participants’ use of that scale reliabilities range from good to excellent.1
strategies during learning, including rehearsal,
critical thinking, elaboration, and metacognitive self--
Internal validity
regulation (4, 5, 6, and 12 items, respectively). For
both measures, participants responded on a 1 (not Correlational analysis
at all true of me) to 7 (very true of me) scale, and Correlations between scores for the long EES scales
the scores were summed to form the task value and scores for the short EES scales (averaged across
and strategy indexes (αs = .86, .59, .83, .72, and .69, assessments) ranged from r = .65 to .83 for the same
respectively). emotions, suggesting convergent validity
(Supplemental Material, Table S5). Correlations
Scale versions for German participants between scales for different emotions indicate that
The original English-language versions of the EES, the the emotions measured by the EES are distinct from
Task Value Scale, and the MSLQ scales were translated each other, with most of the correlations ranging
into the German language by two German emotion from r = .10 to .40 (see Table 1 for the long scales
experts and one bilingual translator. and Supplemental Material, Table S3, for the short
scales). However, the pattern of correlations diverges
from the relations found for other types of emotions.
Rationale for CFA
Typically, positive emotions correlate positively, nega-
CFA using Mplus 7.3 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012) was tive emotions correlate positively, and correlations
employed to test the internal validity of the 21-item between positive and negative emotions tend to be
COGNITION AND EMOTION 5
Downloaded by [93.204.107.184] at 23:20 22 July 2016

Figure 1. Structure of CFA models for epistemic emotions. Notes: Upper part: Model 1 (one-factor model); middle part: Model 2 (two-factor
model); lower part: Model 3 (seven-factor model). Item labels are su = surprise; cu = curiosity; jo = enjoyment; co = confusion; ax = anxiety; fr
= frustration; and bo = boredom.

negative (e.g. achievement emotions; Pekrun & Ste- homogeneity and distinctiveness of the scales (long
phens, 2012). Herein, all emotions except boredom versions), we used CFA and compared three models
showed positive correlations, including surprise, posi- that varied in their degree of differentiation between
tively valenced emotions (curiosity, enjoyment), as emotions (Figure 1). Model 1 was a one-factor model
well as negatively valenced emotions (confusion, that contained one bipolar factor representing all
anxiety, frustration). As such, these correlations emotions. Model 2 was a two-factor model that differ-
suggest that emotions during epistemic activities are entiated between positive and negative epistemic
primarily linked along the arousal dimension of affect. The items for curiosity and enjoyment were
emotion rather than the valence dimension. In contrast used as indicators for positive affect, and the items
to deactivating boredom, epistemic surprise, curiosity, for confusion, anxiety, frustration, and boredom were
enjoyment, confusion, anxiety, and frustration can be used as indicators for negative affect. In two variants
considered activating emotions, and these emotions of this model, the items for surprise were used as indi-
correlated positively, regardless of their valence. cators of either positive affect (Model 2a) or negative
affect (Model 2b). Finally, Model 3 was a seven-factor
Confirmatory factor analysis model that differentiated between all seven emotions.
Preliminary support for the internal validity of the EES The one-factor model did not fit the data;
was provided by exploratory factor analysis (see Χ 2(210) = 2724.12, p < .01, comparative fit index
Supplemental Material). To further examine the (CFI) = .383, Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = .322, root
6 R. PEKRUN ET AL.

Table 1. Correlations for Epistemic Emotion Scales, task value, and learning strategies.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Manifest and latent correlations between the emotion scalesa
1. Surprise .84** .52*** .37*** .44*** .39*** .21*** −.26***
2. Curiosity .58*** .88** .30*** .16** .32*** .09 −.62**
3. Enjoyment .44*** .32*** .78** .19*** .13** .06 −.12***
4. Confusion .53*** .17** .24*** .78** .46*** .45*** .14**
5. Anxiety .43*** .30*** .15* .58*** .76** .60*** −.08
6. Frustration .26*** .09 .07 .56*** .77*** .77** .16**
7. Boredom −.32*** −.72*** −.13* .15* −.05 .19** .86**
Bivariate correlations with task value and learning strategies
Task value .08 .25*** .18*** .01 .05 .06 −.15**
Rehearsal .20*** .24*** .19*** .03 .04 −.06 −.24***
Critical thinking .11* .34*** .17*** .11* .24*** .17*** −.19***
Elaboration .17*** .33*** .21*** .09 .19*** .12*** −.23***
Metacognitive self-regulation .27*** .34*** .29*** .22*** .20*** .07 −.20***
Partial correlations with task value and learning strategiesb
Task value −.06 .11* −.04 −.03 .04
Rehearsal .12* .14** .03 −.02 −.05
Critical thinking −.09 .08 .06 .15** .15**
Downloaded by [93.204.107.184] at 23:20 22 July 2016

Elaboration .00 .12* .05 .10* .11*


Metacognitive self-regulation .11* .21*** .17*** .10* .04
a
Latent correlations in lower-left diagonal matrix; manifest correlations in upper right diagonal matrix; alpha coefficients in the diagonal.
b
Partial correlations controlling for curiosity and boredom. These correlations were calculated to meet a concern that the bivariate correlations of
surprise, enjoyment, confusion, anxiety, and frustration with task value and learning strategies are driven by curiosity and boredom. The cor-
relations show that most of these relations are preserved when controlling for curiosity and boredom although reduced in size, which suggests
that these emotions have relations with value and strategies that are independent from the influence of curiosity and boredom.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .165, manifest correlations, with positive correlations
and standardized root mean square residual between the activating emotions and a more differen-
(SRMR) = .176. Fit for the two variants of the two-fac- tiated pattern of relations for boredom. Importantly,
tor model was better but also not acceptable. Inter- although some of these correlations were relatively
estingly, Model 2a (surprise as part of positive high, they clearly indicate that all of the emotion con-
affect) showed a slightly better fit than Model 2b structs are separable, given that latent correlations are
(surprise as part of negative affect), with Χ 2(188) = corrected for unreliability and represent the highest
2086.80, p < .01, CFI = .515, TLI = .458, RMSEA = .152, possible estimates of these relations. As such, the
SRMR = .164, and Χ 2(188) = 2252.39, CFI = .473, TLI results of CFA show that the seven emotions included
= .411, RMSEA = .158, and TLI = .171, respectively. in the EES are distinct. These emotions would not be
This is in line with the correlations for surprise adequately represented by summary factors of posi-
which indicated that surprise is primarily related to tive and negative affect; rather, they need to be
curiosity and enjoyment (Table 1), and suggests assessed in differentiated ways.
that surprise is positively valenced in an epistemic
context. Measurement equivalence across countries
In contrast to the one-factor and two-factor Multi-group CFA with the long version of the EES was
models, the seven-factor model showed a good fit; used to examine measurement equivalence of the
Χ 2(168)= 418.38, CFI = .936, TLI = .920, RMSEA = .058, instrument across countries. The type of measurement
and SRMR = .056. The factor loadings for the model invariance needed to infer equivalence depends on
were above .60 for all items, and above .70 for 18 of the purpose of measurement (Byrne, 2008). Metric
the 21 items (Supplemental Material, Table S1). This and scalar invariance are required if the goal is to
finding confirms that the EES emotion factors can be compare score distributions (e.g. means), whereas
considered sufficiently homogenous. metric invariance is sufficient to establish that the
Furthermore, the analysis provides estimates for measured constructs can be conceptualised in the
the latent correlations between the emotions (Table same way across groups. Accordingly, we sought to
1). The pattern of correlations is the same as the establish metric invariance to ensure that the
COGNITION AND EMOTION 7

emotions are interpreted in the same way across the predicted, surprise and confusion significantly
three countries. increased from Text 1 to Text 2, and then signifi-
To compare model fit for the configural, metric, and cantly decreased from Text 2 to Text 3 (for details,
scalar invariance models, we followed recommen- see Supplemental Material, Table S7 and Figure S1;
dations by Chen (2007). To test metric invariance, a and Muis et al., 2015). Curiosity, enjoyment, and
change of ≥−.010 in CFI, supplemented by a change anxiety decreased from Text 1 to Text 2, and sub-
of ≥.015 in RMSEA or a change of ≥.030 in SRMR sequently increased from Text 2 to Text 3. Frustration
would indicate non-invariance. To test scalar invar- and boredom significantly decreased from Text 2 to
iance, a change of ≥−.010 in CFI, supplemented by a Text 3.
change of ≥.015 in RMSEA or a change of ≥.010 in These findings corroborate that reading conflict-
SRMR would indicate non-invariance. The configural ing materials promotes surprise and confusion. Unex-
invariance and metric invariance models showed a pectedly, curiosity was reduced during Text 2;
similar fit to the data (Supplemental Material, Table however, curiosity recovered during Text 3. Similarly,
S6). The differences between these models were Text 2 reduced participants’ enjoyment, which was
ΔCFI = −.003, ΔRMSEA = .001, and ΔSRMR = .019, indi- restored during Text 3; conversely, the negative
cating that metric invariance could be accepted. The emotions confusion, anxiety, frustration, and
Downloaded by [93.204.107.184] at 23:20 22 July 2016

differences between the scalar and metric invariance boredom decreased from Text 2 to Text 3. In sum,
models were ΔCFI = −.024, ΔRMSEA = .006, and the results confirm that emotions during epistemic
ΔSRMR = .001; as such, the loss in CFI suggests that activities show a temporal dynamic that is aligned
scalar invariance cannot be assumed, whereas the with the amount of text-induced cognitive incongru-
differences in RMSEA and SRMR would imply that ity, except for the decrease of curiosity from Text 1
scalar invariance can be accepted. to Text 2.
These findings confirm metric measurement
invariance of the EES across groups. For scalar invar- Relations with task value and self-reported
iance, the evidence is mixed; this suggests that com- learning strategies
parisons across groups that require scalar invariance, As expected, curiosity and enjoyment correlated
such as comparisons of mean scores, should be used positively and boredom negatively with the per-
cautiously. In the present research, we were inter- ceived value of the task (Table 1; Muis et al.,
ested in establishing structural measurement prop- 2015). Furthermore, surprise, curiosity, and enjoy-
erties of the EES as well links with external ment correlated positively with all four self-reported
variables, rather than comparing emotion scores learning strategies; anxiety correlated positively with
across countries. The finding that the EES showed all of the strategies except rehearsal; confusion cor-
metric invariance supports this approach and related positively with critical thinking and metacog-
suggests that the emotion constructs used in the nitive self-regulation; and frustration correlated
EES are conceptually equivalent across the three positively with critical thinking and elaboration.
countries. These findings are in line with our expectation
that activating emotions can promote strategy use.
They are also consistent with recent evidence
External validity
suggesting that not only pleasant epistemic
Impact of contradictory texts on emotions emotions such as enjoyment, but also unpleasant
We expected participants’ emotions assessed by the emotions such as confusion can promote learning
short scales to change across texts because the texts (D’Mello et al., 2014). By contrast, in line with evi-
contained conflicting information, with Text 1 and dence that boredom undermines any systematic
Text 2 directly contradicting each other. Based on effort at learning (e.g. Pekrun, Götz, Daniels, Stu-
the hypothesis that cognitive incongruity can pinsky, & Perry, 2010), boredom related negatively
trigger surprise, curiosity, and confusion, we to all four strategies. The relations were most pro-
expected these emotions to increase from Text 1 nounced for curiosity and boredom, respectively,
to Text 2. For Text 3, we expected these emotions thus highlighting the important role of these two
to return back to levels experienced during Text 1 emotions in epistemic contexts. In sum, the findings
given that no conflicting information was presented are consistent with our hypothesis that emotions
in Text 3 compared to the first two texts. As during epistemic activities are linked to value
8 R. PEKRUN ET AL.

appraisals and strategy use during learning, thus emotions. The present findings proffer the EES as
also attesting to the external validity of the EES. one promising method that can be used to explore
these emotions.
Limitations
Note
Due to the use of adjectives to measure emotion, it
cannot fully be ruled out that respondents use the 1. There were no gender differences in mean emotions
EES items to also report non-epistemic emotions scores (long version of the EES) except for enjoyment,
Ms = 4.92 and 5.45, SDs = 2.04 and 2.25, for female and
that can occur during epistemic activity, such as male students, respectively; d = −.025, t = −2.04, p = .042,
achievement or social emotions. In a think-aloud
study with 56 Canadian undergraduate students
which used the same materials and procedure as Disclosure statement
the current study, the nature of the emotions occur- No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
ring during reading the texts was examined (Muis
et al., 2015). The findings show that the vast
majority of the emotions (83%) were epistemic. Funding
Downloaded by [93.204.107.184] at 23:20 22 July 2016

Nonetheless, scores derived from the EES should This work was supported by the University of Munich [grant
be interpreted cautiously and with reference to number VII.1-H172.10], by the Social Sciences and Humanities
the specific instructions and epistemic context pro- Research Council of Canada [grant number SSHRC 410-2011-
0182], and by a Visiting Fellowship Award from the Center for
vided for answering the instrument. Also, the instru-
Advanced Study, University of Munich, awarded to Krista Muis.
ment measures major epistemically-related
emotions but does not cover all types of these
emotions. Future research could expand upon the References
present research by constructing additional scales Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2009). Effects of task instruction and
for emotions that occur less frequently in epistemic personal epistemology on the understanding of multiple
contexts and are not included in the EES (e.g. awe texts about climate change. Discourse Processes, 47, 1–31.
and admiration). doi:10.1080/01638530902959646
Brun, G., Doğuoğlu, U., & Kuenzle, D. (Eds.). (2008). Epistemology
and emotions. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.
Conclusion Byrne, B. M. (2008). Testing for multigroup equivalence of a
measuring instrument: A walk through the process.
The present research shows that the EES are reliable Psicothema, 20, 872–882.
and can be used to examine a range of major Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of
emotions that occur during epistemic activities. The measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A
Multidisciplinary Journal, 14, 464–504. doi:10.1080/
findings also contribute support for the internal 10705510701301834
and external validity of the measure. Future research D’Mello, S. K. (2013). A selective meta-analysis on the relative inci-
should further validate the EES by including more dence of discrete affective states during learning with tech-
male participants, samples from other age groups nology. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 1082–1099.
doi:10.1037/a00326749
and cultures not considered here, and multiple
D’Mello, S. K., & Graesser, A. C. (2012). Dynamics of affective states
types of cognitive tasks, such as problem-solving during complex learning. Learning and Instruction, 22, 145–
tasks or contradictory texts on less familiar topics 157. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.10.001
about which participants do not have existing D’Mello, S. K., Lehman, B., Pekrun, R., & Graesser, A. C. (2014).
opinions. As compared with alternative methods Confusion can be beneficial for learning. Learning and
Instruction, 29, 153–170. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.05.003
such as think-aloud protocols, physiological
Hookway, C. (2008). Epistemic immediacy, doubts and anxiety:
measures, or behavioural observation, the instrument On a role for affective states in epistemic evaluation. In G.
is easy to administer, cost-effective, and minimally Brun, U. Doğuoğlu, & D. Kuenzle (Eds.), Epistemology and
task-invasive in terms of keeping interruption of emotions (pp. 51–65). Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.
ongoing activities to a minimum. As noted at the Jirout, J., & Klahr, D. (2012). Children’s scientific curiosity: In search of
outset, epistemic emotions are critically important an operational definition of an elusive concept. Developmental
Review, 32, 125–160. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2012.04.002
for cognitive learning, problem solving, and the gen- Kang, M. J., Hsu, M., Krajbich, I. M., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M.,
eration of knowledge, which suggests that research- Wang, J. T. Y., & Camerer, C. F. (2009). The wick in the candle of
ers across disciplines should attend to these learning: Epistemic curiosity activates reward circuitry and
COGNITION AND EMOTION 9

enhances memory. Psychological Science, 20, 963–973. doi:10. Scheffler, I. (Ed.). (1991). In praise of the cognitive emotions and
1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02402.x other essays in the philosophy of education. New York, NY:
Markey, A., & Loewenstein, G. (2014). Curiosity. In R. Pekrun & L. Routledge.
Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International handbook of emotions Scherer, K. R. (2009). The dynamic architecture of emotion:
in education (pp. 228–245). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis. Evidence for the component process model. Cognition and
Morton, A. (2010). Epistemic emotions. In P. Goldie (Ed.), The Emotion, 23, 1307–1351. doi:10.1080/02699930902928969
Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion (pp. 385–399). Silvia, P. J. (2010). Confusion and interest: The role of knowledge
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. emotions in aesthetic experience. Psychology of Aesthetics,
Muis, K. R., & Duffy, M. C. (2013). Epistemic climate and epistemic Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 75–80. doi:10.1037/a0017081
change: Instruction designed to change students’ epistemic Wigfield, A. (1994). Expectancy-value theory of achievement
beliefs and learning strategies and improve achievement. motivation: A developmental perspective. Educational
Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 213–225. doi:10. Psychology Review, 6, 49–78. doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1015
1037/a0029690
Muis, K. R., Pekrun, R., Azevedo, R., Sinatra, G., Trevors, G., Meier,
E., & Heddy, B. C. (2015). The curious case of climate change:
Epistemic emotions mediate relations between epistemic
Appendix
beliefs, learning strategies and learning outcomes. Learning Epistemically-Related Emotion Scales
and Instruction, 39, 168–183. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.
06.003 Instruction
Downloaded by [93.204.107.184] at 23:20 22 July 2016

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus user’s guide: Seventh We are interested in the emotions you experienced when
edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. reading the texts about climate change. For each emotion,
Pekrun, R., Götz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupinsky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. please indicate the strength of that emotion by circling the
(2010). Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control- number that best describes the intensity of your emotional
value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected response during learning of the texts.
emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 531–549.
doi:10.1037/a0019243 Not at all Very little Moderate Strong Very strong
Pekrun, R., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (Eds.). (2014). International Curious 1 2 3 4 5
handbook of emotions in education. New York, NY: Taylor & Bored 1 2 3 4 5
Francis. Confused 1 2 3 4 5
Pekrun, R., & Perry, R. P. (2014). Control-value theory of achieve- Surprised 1 2 3 4 5
Interested 1 2 3 4 5
ment emotions. In R. Pekrun & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.),
Anxious 1 2 3 4 5
International handbook of emotions in education (pp. 120– Frustrated 1 2 3 4 5
141). New York: Taylor & Francis. Inquisitive 1 2 3 4 5
Pekrun, R., & Stephens, E. J. (2012). Academic emotions. In K. R. Dull 1 2 3 4 5
Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychol- Amazed 1 2 3 4 5
ogy handbook (Vol. 2, pp. 3–31). Washington, DC: American Worried 1 2 3 4 5
Psychological Association. Happy 1 2 3 4 5
Pintrich, P., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993). Muddled 1 2 3 4 5
Reliability and predictive validity of the Motivated Strategies Irritated 1 2 3 4 5
Monotonous 1 2 3 4 5
for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and
Excited 1 2 3 4 5
Psychological Measurement, 53, 801–813. doi:10.1177/ Astonished 1 2 3 4 5
0013164493053003024 Dissatisfied 1 2 3 4 5
Reeve, J. (1993). The face of interest. Motivation and Emotion, 17, Nervous 1 2 3 4 5
353–375. Joyful 1 2 3 4 5
Rozin, P., & Cohen, A. B. (2003). High frequency of facial Puzzled 1 2 3 4 5
expressions corresponding to confusion, concentration, and Not at all Very little Moderate Strong Very strong
worry in an analysis of naturally occurring facial expressions Note: Items for the seven-item short version of the EES are surprised,
of Americans. Emotion, 3, 68–75. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.68 curious, excited, confused, anxious, frustrated, and bored.

View publication stats

You might also like