You are on page 1of 15

Psychology of Religion and Spirituality © 2012 American Psychological Association

2012, Vol. 4, No. 4, 249 –263 1941-1022/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027940

Attitudes Toward Nontraditional Women Using IVF: The


Importance of Political Affiliation and Religious Characteristics
Alexandra E. Sigillo, Monica K. Miller, and Dana A. Weiser
University of Nevada, Reno

Assisted reproductive technology (ART) has advanced in recent years and it is likely
that the public’s perceptions of such technology have changed over time. The use of
ART by nontraditional women (e.g., single women, lesbians) has increased; thus, it is
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

important to assess the public’s attitudes toward in vitro fertilization (IVF), the most
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

common ART, and the women who use such technology. The purpose of the study is
to determine whether certain demographics, political and religious affiliations, and
religious characteristics relate to approval of IVF use by nontraditional women.
Undergraduate students were recruited from a public, medium-sized university in the
western United States; 267 participants responded to multiple questions on an online
survey. Using ordinary least square regression analyses, findings indicate that political
affiliation is one of the strongest predictors, and that certain religious characteristics are
more important in assessing attitudes than religious affiliation. Implications for assess-
ing attitudes toward nontraditional women using IVF and inclusion of religious affil-
iation and characteristics in future studies are discussed.

Keywords: in vitro fertilization (IVF), attitudes, family structure

Born in 1978, Louise Brown was the first U.S. births (Centers for Disease Control and
“test tube baby.” Her conception through in Prevention et al., 2009). As the number of
vitro fertilization (IVF), rather than sexual in- women who use IVF increases, it becomes
tercourse, provided hope to millions of infertile progressively more important to understand
women and couples unable to conceive a child. individuals’ attitudes toward IVF and the
IVF is the most common type of assisted repro- women who use it, especially nontraditional
ductive technology (ART), accounting for 99% women (e.g., single women, lesbians).
of all procedures (Centers for Disease Control Attitudes toward IVF and the women who
and Prevention, American Society for Repro- use it may be dependent on individuals’ views
ductive Medicine, & Society for Assisted Re- about the morality of the technology. Individu-
productive Technology, 2009). IVF involves als’ perceptions about the acceptability of bio-
removing eggs from a woman’s body and fer- technologies are closely related to their percep-
tilizing the eggs with sperm outside the womb. tions about the morality of such biotechnologies
The resulting embryos are then transferred
(Priest, 2000). For example, using nationwide
into the woman’s uterus, increasing the
adult samples, agricultural (e.g., genetically
chances for the woman to become pregnant.
In 2007, 142,435 ART procedures were per- modified foods) and genetic (e.g., genetic ther-
formed resulting in a little over 1% of total apy, animal cloning) biotechnologies have be-
come more acceptable over time (Priest, 2000;
Singer, Corning, & Lamias, 1998), suggesting
that these biotechnologies are perceived as more
This article was published Online First April 30, 2012. moral as well. Few studies have examined atti-
Alexandra E. Sigillo, Interdisciplinary PhD Program in tudes toward IVF and the types of women who
Social Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno; Monica K.
Miller, Department of Criminal Justice and Interdisciplinary use this reproductive technology. The purpose
PhD Program in Social Psychology, University of Nevada, of the current study was to explore how specific
Reno; Dana A. Weiser, Interdisciplinary PhD Program in variables (i.e., age, gender, race, political and
Social Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno.
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
religious affiliations, and religious characteris-
dressed to Alexandra E. Sigillo, 1410 Whipple Avenue, tics) relate to approval of IVF use by nontradi-
Redwood City, CA 94062. E-mail: sigilloa@gmail.com tional women, including single women, lesbi-
249
250 SIGILLO, MILLER, AND WEISER

ans, women with degenerative illnesses, elderly women using IVF compared to men. However,
women, transsexuals, and transgenders. in a sample of medical professionals, men were
more likely to approve of single women and
Individual Differences Related to Attitudes marginally more likely to approve of lesbians
having access to ART compared to women (de
Toward IVF
la Fuente Fonnest, Sondergaard, Fonnest, &
The review below presents findings from the Vedsted-Jacobsen, 2000). This suggests that the
most relevant studies concerning the relation- composition of the sample studied may relate to
ship between the variables of interest and atti- approval of IVF for various types of women. As
tudes toward IVF. Where gaps in the literature the current study involves a college sample, a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

exist, results from studies regarding other repro- pattern similar to Lasker and Murray’s findings
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ductive-related procedures are presented. Addi- was expected such that women are more sup-
tionally, findings about general attitudes toward portive than men regarding IVF use by nontra-
nontraditional women are presented, as infer- ditional women.
ences can then be made about IVF use by such
women. Race

Age Many African Americans, compared to Eu-


ropean Americans, are distrustful of physicians
No studies have investigated whether age re- and the health care system (Jacobs, Rolle, Fer-
lates to individuals’ attitudes toward IVF. Age rans, Whitaker, & Warnecke, 2006) and hesitant
differences have been examined with respect to to participate in medical research (Huang &
other reproductive-related procedures, such as Coker, 2010). Multiple explanations have been
embryonic stem cell research and single embryo provided in these studies (e.g., historical rela-
transfer. In a community sample, younger indi- tionships with medical institutions). As such, it
viduals were more disapproving of embryonic was expected that African Americans would
stem cell research compared to older individuals exhibit similar distrust regarding use of IVF.
(Sullivan, 1993). In a sample of women using Early research demonstrated that African Amer-
IVF, younger females were more likely to ac- icans are less likely to approve of IVF compared
cept and use single, rather than multiple, em- to Caucasians (Dunn et al., 1988); however,
bryo transfer compared to older females (de more recent research has yet to investigate
Lacey, Davies, Homan, Briggs, & Norman, possible racial differences. This study ex-
2007). Thus, it is expected that younger indi- pands on previous research by exploring
viduals are less approving of IVF for nontradi- whether ethnic minorities exhibit less ap-
tional women compared to older individuals. proval for nontraditional women using IVF
compared to Caucasians.
Gender
Political Affiliation
Studies assessing gender differences have
demonstrated mixed results. Early studies re- No research has examined the association
vealed that women were less likely to approve between political affiliation and approval of
of IVF compared to men (Turtle, Harrajchi, IVF. Republicans typically assert that embryos
Perry, & Tully, 1988) or that there were no are living beings with fundamental rights. As
gender differences (Dunn, Ryan, & O’Brien, such, they consider abortion to be murder as
1988). However, more recent studies have dem- embryonic lives are destroyed (Strickler &
onstrated that women are more likely to approve Danigelis, 2002). Similarly, Republicans may
of IVF compared to men (Lasker & Murray, be less likely to support IVF as embryos may be
2001). Researchers have identified gender dif- destroyed. Conversely, Democrats typically
ferences regarding attitudes toward nontradi- contend that women have a right to privacy and
tional women using fertility treatment. Lasker control of their own bodies (Strickler & Dani-
and Murray (2001) found that college women gelis, 2002). Thus, Democrats may be less con-
were more likely to approve of lesbians and cerned with the possibility of destroyed em-
marginally more likely to approve of single bryos and more likely to support women’s
ATTITUDES TOWARD IVF 251

reproductive choices, including IVF. Addition- Although not investigated in this study due to
ally, it is suspected that Republicans would be the sample composition, other religions have
particularly opposed to IVF use by nontradi- particular tenets regarding IVF. Jewish law dic-
tional women. Republicans tend to place higher tates that individuals have an obligation to pro-
value on the traditional nuclear family structure create and preserve the family (Kahn, 2006b;
(Arnold & Weisberg, 1996), and thus, may be Schenker, 2005). IVF use is acceptable in the
more disapproving of women seeking IVF out- Jewish community, but varies depending on
side a heterosexual marriage. Republicans are rabbis’ interpretations of the law (Kahn,
also typically less accepting of homosexuality 2006b). For example, most rabbis do not en-
(Burnett & Salka, 2009) and may be less likely dorse the use of donor sperm, but some may
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

to accept IVF use by lesbians. The current study permit it given certain situations (Kahn, 2006b).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

examined whether identifying as a Republican Similarly, Muslim tradition highly values mar-
or a Democrat relates to attitudes toward IVF riage and family. Thus, Muslim individuals be-
use by nontraditional women. lieve IVF is acceptable as long as the sperm and
egg come from a husband and wife (Larijani &
Religious Affiliation Zahedi, 2007). However, it appears that third-
party donation is becoming more acceptable in
Researchers have investigated the relation- certain Islamic countries (Inhorn & Birenbaum-
ship between religious affiliation and attitudes Carmeli, 2008).
toward IVF. The Catholic Church teaches that
life begins at conception and that embryos pos- Religious Characteristics
sess dignity similar to human beings; thus, the
Catholic Church condemns the use of IVF as it There is no guarantee, however, that individ-
views the destruction of embryos as loss of lives uals agree with all doctrines of a given religion
(Congregation for the Doctrine of the Faith, (see Miller, Maskaly, Peoples, & Sigillo, 2011).
2008). The Catholic Church also opposes the Thus, religious characteristics may better pre-
notion that conception occurs in a laboratory as dict approval of IVF than religious affiliation.
they believe that God, not scientists, controls Within any particular religious group, members
the beginning of life (Roberts, 2006; Schenker, may hold varying beliefs and it is important to
2005; Singer, 2009). Therefore, Catholics, com- identify whether specific religious characteris-
pared to other religious groups, are more likely tics such as fundamentalism, (Christian) ortho-
to disapprove of IVF (Evans & Hudson, 2007). doxy, devotionalism, evangelism, and intrinsic
Catholics may also specifically oppose IVF use and extrinsic religiosity influence individuals’
by lesbians since the Church considers homo- approval of IVF use by various women.
sexual acts immoral (Libreria Editrice Vaticana, Fundamentalism has been described as Bib-
1997). lical interpretism (Young, 1992), belief in ab-
Other Christian affiliations vary in their ap- solute truths (Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 2004),
proval of IVF. Conservative Protestant denom- and strict adherence to religious ideology (Jon-
inations (e.g., Southern Baptists) align with the athan, 2008). No studies have examined the
Catholic view and object to IVF because human relationship between fundamentalism and indi-
embryos are destroyed (Pauls & Hutchinson, viduals’ attitudes toward IVF. However, funda-
2002). Liberal Protestant denominations (e.g., mentalists who attended religious services
Methodists) accept IVF if restricted to hetero- frequently were more opposed to reproductive-
sexual married couples (Schenker, 2005). Con- related procedures (e.g., embryonic stem cell
servative Protestants are more opposed to IVF research, preimplantation diagnosis) compared
compared to liberal Protestants (Evans & Hud- to fundamentalists who attended religious ser-
son, 2007), particularly IVF access by single vices less frequently (Evans & Hudson, 2007).
women and lesbians (de la Fuente Fonnest et Because such procedures involve the possible
al., 2000). Thus, it was suspected that Protes- destruction of embryos, it was suspected that
tants would disapprove of IVF use by single fundamentalists would similarly disapprove of
women and lesbians despite greater Protestant IVF. Further, fundamentalists may be likely to
approval for IVF in the context of heterosex- disapprove of IVF for lesbians as fundamental-
ual marriages. ism has been associated with more negative
252 SIGILLO, MILLER, AND WEISER

attitudes toward gay men and lesbians (Alte- ism is related to attitudes toward nontraditional
meyer & Hunsberger, 1992; Jonathan, 2008; women using IVF.
Kirkpatrick, 1993; Laythe, Finkel, Bringle, & According to Allport and Ross’s (1967) clas-
Kirkpatrick, 2002). sic work, intrinsic religiosity refers to individ-
Orthodoxy has been described as beliefs that uals who “live” their religion and are committed
are consistent with traditional religious doc- to their faith. Intrinsic religiosity is positively
trines (Randolph-Seng, Nielsen, Bottoms, & related to fundamental measures (Genia, 1996;
Filipas, 2008) and the level of acceptance of Kirkpatrick, 1993), suggesting that similar to
Christian beliefs and doctrines (Jonathan, fundamentalists, individuals with high intrinsic
2008). In contrast to fundamentalism, ortho- religiosity would disapprove of IVF, especially
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

doxy predicted less negative attitudes toward for lesbians. However, research has demon-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

gay men and lesbians (Jonathan, 2008; Laythe strated that when fundamentalism is statistically
et al., 2002); however, another finding demon- controlled, intrinsic religiosity is unrelated to
strated no relation between orthodoxy and atti- attitudes toward gay men and lesbians (Kirkpat-
tudes toward gay men and lesbians (Kirkpat- rick, 1993; McFarland, 1989). Compared to in-
rick, 1993). Therefore, orthodoxy, as compared trinsic, extrinsic religiosity refers to individuals
to fundamentalism, may relate differently to who “use” their religion as means to achieve an
approval of nontraditional women using IVF, end (McFarland, 1989) and is categorized into
particularly for lesbians. personal and social extrinsic religiosity. Per-
Evangelism refers to the desire and attempt to sonal refers to using one’s religion to satisfy
convert others to one’s faith (Young, 1992). personal needs (i.e., gain comfort), whereas so-
Similar to fundamentalism, evangelists who at- cial refers to using one’s religion to satisfy
tended religious services frequently were more social needs (i.e., meet new people; Genia,
opposed to reproductive-related procedures 1996). Personal extrinsic religiosity has exhib-
ited a positive, albeit weak, correlation with
compared to evangelists who attended religious
fundamental measures (Genia, 1996; Kirkpat-
services less frequently (Evans & Hudson,
rick, 1993), but was unrelated to negative atti-
2007). Additionally, evangelical Christians
tudes toward gay men and lesbians, even when
were more opposed to embryonic stem cell re-
controlling for fundamentalism (McFarland,
search compared to individuals of no religious 1989). Social extrinsic religiosity has no asso-
faith (Bryant & Gudgin, 2008). Because these ciation with fundamental measures (Genia,
procedures involve possible embryo destruc- 1996; Kirkpatrick, 1993), and this measure was
tion, evangelists may be more likely to disap- unrelated to negative attitudes toward gay men
prove of IVF for similar reasons. Furthermore, and lesbians, even when fundamentalism was
evangelists may be less supportive of IVF use held constant (McFarland). Based on contradic-
by single mothers as evangelical Protestants, tions within research, this study examined
compared to Catholics, were more likely to be whether intrinsic, personal extrinsic, and social
concerned about the decreasing importance of extrinsic religiosity relate to approval of IVF
marriage and the family (Brooks, 2002). Fi- use by various women.
nally, evangelists may disapprove of IVF use by
lesbians because biblical scripture depicts ho- The Present Research
mosexual acts as immoral (Thumma, 1991) and
they trust the word of the Bible (Kahn, 2006a). Reproductive technology has advanced in re-
Devotionalism has been described as emo- cent years and it is likely that the public’s per-
tional religious involvement (Giorgi, 1992), ceptions of such technology have evolved over
religious salience (Young, 1992), religious im- time. Thus, it is important to gauge attitudes
portance (Putney & Middleton, 1961), and ad- toward IVF and the women who use such tech-
herence to private worship (Lupfer & Wald, nology. The purpose of the study is to determine
1985). No studies have investigated the rela- whether certain demographics, political and re-
tionship between devotionalism and attitudes ligious affiliations, and religious characteristics
toward IVF, other reproductive-related proce- relate to approval of IVF use by nontraditional
dures, or nontraditional women. The current women. Attitudes toward six nontraditional
study will finally explore whether devotional- women were assessed as IVF approval may be
ATTITUDES TOWARD IVF 253

contingent upon the type of woman using the can (6.4%), African American (5.5%), and
technology. Specifically, the researchers mea- Native American (1.3%). Religious affiliations
sured the degree of morality associated with included Catholicism (35.4%), Protestantism
nontraditional women using IVF and the level (7.9%), Atheism/Agnosticism (15.0%), and
of support for doctors refusing to provide IVF Other (41.7%) including Eastern Orthodox
for these women. Both concepts were assessed (0.8%), Judaism (0.8%), Buddhism (0.8%), Is-
as they may represent two related, yet separate, lam (0.4%), Mormon (0.4%), a nonlisted faith
constructs that individuals use to determine ap- (13.0%), and those of no particular faith
proval of IVF. (25.6%). Participants’ political affiliation in-
It is hypothesized that older individuals, cluded Democrat (40.1%), Republican (26.8%),
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Caucasians (compared to ethnic minorities), and Other (33.1%) including Independent


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Democrats (compared to Republicans), and (9.3%), another party (5.1%), and no specific
individuals higher in orthodoxy will perceive affiliation (18.7%).
nontraditional women’s use of IVF as moral
and support doctors who provide IVF treat- Materials
ment. Further, it is hypothesized that men,
Catholics and Protestants (compared to other Dependent variables. To assess percep-
religious affiliations), and individuals higher tions of morality, participants were asked “Is it
in fundamentalism and evangelism will con- moral for a woman to have in vitro fertilization
sider women’s use of IVF as less moral and if she . . .?” The sentence was completed with
support doctors who refuse to provide IVF either: “is a single mother,” “is a 60-year-old
treatment. No specific hypotheses are pro- woman,” “has early onset Alzheimer’s and will
posed for Atheists/Agnostics, devotionalism, likely die in 10 years,” “was born a man but had
and intrinsic, personal extrinsic, and social a sex change to a woman,” “was born a woman
extrinsic religiosity; as such, they will be but lives as a man,” or “is a lesbian.” For each
considered exploratory variables. woman, doctor refusal was assessed with the
question: “Should a doctor refuse to perform in
Method vitro fertilization for this woman?” Both ques-
tions were rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 1
Participants accessed a Web site and com- (no, absolutely not) to 5 (yes, absolutely).
pleted an online survey individually and at Higher morality ratings indicate perceptions
their own convenience. They were asked to that IVF is more moral and higher doctor refusal
rate whether using IVF is moral and whether ratings indicate greater support for doctors re-
doctors should refuse IVF for six types of fusing to provide IVF treatment.
women. Participants then completed seven re- Predictor variables. Participants self-
ligious scales (i.e., fundamentalism, ortho- reported their age, gender, race, and political
doxy, evangelism, devotionalism, and intrin- and religious affiliations.
sic, personal extrinsic, and social extrinsic Fundamentalism was assessed with Alte-
religiosity), answered demographic questions meyer and Hunsberger’s (2004) Revised 12-
(i.e., age, gender, race), and indicated their Item Fundamentalism Scale, an updated version
political and religious affiliations. of the 20-Item Religious Fundamentalism Scale
(see Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992). Twelve
Participants items were rated on a scale of 1 (strongly dis-
agree) to 5 (strongly agree) (e.g., “No single
Participants were 267 undergraduate students book of religious teachings contains all the in-
recruited at a public, medium-sized university trinsic, fundamental truths about life”). Each
in the western United States who received credit participant’s responses were averaged; higher
in their introductory-level classes including scores indicate higher levels of fundamentalism
psychology, sociology, and criminal justice. (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .89; M ⫽ 2.62).
The mean age was 20.89 years (Mdn ⫽ 20 Orthodoxy, evangelism, and devotionalism
years; range ⫽ 18 to 57 years) and 39.3% were were assessed using Putney and Middleton’s
male. Participants identified themselves as Cau- (1961) measure. Originally, the last two scales
casian (74.6%), Latino (12.3%), Asian Ameri- were labeled fanaticism and importance; how-
254 SIGILLO, MILLER, AND WEISER

ever, recent researchers have termed these as Table 1


evangelism and devotionalism, respectively Means, Medians, and Standard Deviations for
(e.g., Bornstein & Miller, 2009). Six items as- Morality and Doctor Refusal Ratings
sessed orthodoxy (e.g., “I believe there is a Scenario Mean Median SD
Divine plan and purpose for every living person Single mother morality 3.73 4.00 1.09
and thing”), six addressed evangelism (e.g., “I Single mother doctor refusal 2.24 2.00 1.03
have a duty to help those who are confused Elderly woman morality 2.46 2.00 1.11
about religion”), and six measured devotional- Elderly woman doctor refusal 3.55 4.00 1.10
ism (e.g., “I very often think about matters Alzheimer’s disease morality 2.24 2.00 1.12
relating to religion”). Items were rated from 1 Alzheimer’s disease doctor refusal 3.60 4.00 1.15
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Sex change to woman morality 2.90 3.00 1.21


(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Sex change to woman doctor refusal 3.10 3.00 1.20


each participant’s responses were averaged for Woman lives as man morality 2.89 3.00 1.21
each scale. Higher scores indicate higher levels Woman lives as man doctor refusal 3.01 3.00 1.22
of orthodoxy (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .86; Lesbian morality 3.71 4.00 1.24
M ⫽ 3.09), evangelism (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ Lesbian doctor refusal 2.36 2.00 1.24
.72; M ⫽ 2.65), and devotionalism (Cronbach’s
alpha ⫽ .87; M ⫽ 2.95).
Gorsuch and McPherson’s (1989) Extrinsic/
Intrinsic Revised Scale assessed intrinsic and woman with Alzheimer’s disease scenario had
extrinsic religiosity. The measure included eight the lowest mean score (M ⫽ 2.24). In compar-
intrinsic (e.g., “I enjoy reading about my reli- ison, the woman with Alzheimer’s disease sce-
gion”), three personal extrinsic (e.g., “I pray nario had the highest mean for doctoral refusal
mainly to gain relief and protection”), and three ratings (M ⫽ 3.60), whereas the single mother
social extrinsic (e.g., “I go to church because it scenario had the lowest mean score (M ⫽ 2.24).
helps me to make friends”) religiosity items. Therefore, participants on average rated the sin-
Items were rated on a scale of 1 (strongly dis- gle mother scenario as the most moral and were
agree) to 5 (strongly agree). Each participant’s the least supportive of doctoral refusal. In con-
responses were averaged for each subscale. trast, participants on average rated the woman
Higher scores indicate higher levels of intrinsic with Alzheimer’s disease scenario as the least
(Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .72; M ⫽ 2.81), personal moral and were the most supportive of doctor
extrinsic (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .84; M ⫽ 2.93), refusal.
and social extrinsic (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .89; Tables 2 and 3 display the univariate results
M ⫽ 2.20) religiosity. for all morality and doctor refusal ratings. Al-
though the results of the omnibus regression
Results models are presented in text, the univariate re-
sults are of primary interest to the current re-
A series of ordinary least square regression search as the purpose of the study is to deter-
analyses were conducted to examine which de- mine which variables are significantly related to
mographic, political and religious affiliation, perceptions of IVF. The regression model sig-
and religious characteristic variables signifi- nificantly predicted participants’ ratings of how
cantly predicted participants’ morality and doc- moral it is for a single mother to use IVF (R2 ⫽
tor refusal ratings for each scenario. The omni- .21; adjusted R2 ⫽ .13; F(14, 136) ⫽ 2.59, p ⫽
bus regression model contained age, gender, .002). Only fundamentalism was significantly
race, political and religious affiliations, funda- related to morality ratings, indicating that indi-
mentalism, orthodoxy, evangelism, devotional- viduals with higher scores rated this scenario as
ism, and intrinsic, personal extrinsic, and social less moral. Devotionalism marginally predicted
extrinsic religiosity. All variables were entered ratings as individuals with higher scores rated
in the model simultaneously and no multicol- the scenario as less moral. Additionally, the
linearity was observed. relationship between personal extrinsic religios-
See Table 1 for all mean, median, and stan- ity and morality approached significance as in-
dard deviation scores for each scenario. The dividuals with higher scores rated the scenario
single mother scenario had the highest mean for as more moral. The regression model also sig-
morality ratings (M ⫽ 3.73), whereas the nificantly predicted participants’ ratings of
ATTITUDES TOWARD IVF 255

Table 2
Multiple Regressions of Demographic, Political, and Religious Variables on Morality Ratings
Scenario Predictors B t ␤ p-value
Single mother Age ⫺.01 ⫺.28 ⫺.02 .779
Gender .17 .89 .08 .374
Race .08 .41 .04 .686
Political Affiliation .29 1.52 .14 .130
Catholicism ⫺.22 ⫺1.18 ⫺.10 .238
Protestantism ⫺.35 ⫺1.04 ⫺.09 .302
Atheist/Agnostic .06 .18 .02 .009ⴱ
⫺.51 ⫺2.65 ⫺.36
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

AH Fundamentalism .857
⫺.29 ⫺1.69 ⫺.25 .094†
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

PM Devotionalism
PM Orthodoxy .08 .43 .07 .666
PM Evangelism .14 .75 .10 .458
GM Intrinsic .12 .48 .08 .631
GM Extrinsic-Social ⫺.03 ⫺.22 ⫺.02 .825
GM Extrinsic-Personal .25 1.83 .24 .069†
Elderly woman Age .00 .10 .01 .923
Gender ⫺.01 ⫺.06 ⫺.01 .949
Race ⫺.11 ⫺.46 ⫺.04 .643
Political Affiliation .29 1.28 .12 .203
Catholicism .11 .50 .05 .619
Protestantism ⫺.29 ⫺.74 ⫺.07 .460
Atheist/Agnostic .11 .28 .03 .779
AH Fundamentalism .21 .95 .14 .346
PM Devotionalism .02 .12 .02 .906
PM Orthodoxy ⫺.38 ⫺1.80 ⫺.29 .074†
PM Evangelism ⫺.18 ⫺.85 ⫺.12 .399
GM Intrinsic .48 1.67 .28 .097†
GM Extrinsic-Social .17 1.21 .12 .228
GM Extrinsic-Personal ⫺.11 ⫺.70 ⫺.10 .492
Alzheimer’s disease Age .01 .49 .04 .628
Gender .16 .74 .07 .458
Race ⫺.46 ⫺1.96 ⫺.17 .052†
Political Affiliation .14 .62 .06 .535
Catholicism .28 1.32 .12 .189
Protestantism .10 .26 .02 .799
Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.23 ⫺.59 ⫺.07 .559
AH Fundamentalism .40 1.84 .26 .068†
PM Devotionalism .14 .71 .11 .479
PM Orthodoxy ⫺.42 ⫺2.05 ⫺.32 .042ⴱ
PM Evangelism ⫺.25 ⫺1.22 ⫺.16 .224
GM Intrinsic .08 .27 .04 .791
GM Extrinsic-Social .04 .31 .03 .754
GM Extrinsic-Personal ⫺.11 ⫺.69 ⫺.09 .492
Sex change to woman Age ⫺.03 ⫺1.06 ⫺.09 .293
Gender .23 .98 .09 .332
Race .32 1.21 .11 .228
Political Affiliation .41 1.69 .16 .093†
Catholicism ⫺.11 ⫺.47 ⫺.04 .638
Protestantism ⫺.13 ⫺.30 ⫺.03 .763
Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.41 ⫺.95 ⫺.11 .346
AH Fundamentalism ⫺.20 ⫺.83 ⫺.12 .410
PM Devotionalism .05 .22 .04 .824
PM Orthodoxy ⫺.59 ⫺2.62 ⫺.41 .010ⴱ
PM Evangelism .19 .84 .11 .401
GM Intrinsic .21 .67 .11 .502
(table continues)
256 SIGILLO, MILLER, AND WEISER

Table 2 (continued)
Scenario Predictors B t ␤ p-value
GM Extrinsic-Social .03 .19 .02 .852
GM Extrinsic-Personal .11 .62 .09 .537
Woman lives as man Age ⫺.02 ⫺.81 ⫺.07 .419
Gender .15 .65 .06 .520
Race .12 .47 .04 .638
Political Affiliation .35 1.44 .14 .151
Catholicism ⫺.29 ⫺1.22 ⫺.11 .223
Protestantism ⫺.41 ⫺.95 ⫺.09 .343
Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.68 ⫺1.56 ⫺.17 .120
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

AH Fundamentalism ⫺.18 ⫺.76 ⫺.11 .449


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

PM Devotionalism .05 .22 .03 .827


PM Orthodoxy ⫺.52 ⫺2.30 ⫺.36 .023ⴱ
PM Evangelism ⫺.03 ⫺.12 ⫺.02 .905
GM Intrinsic .23 .75 .13 .454
GM Extrinsic-Social ⫺.05 ⫺.36 ⫺.04 .718
GM Extrinsic-Personal .23 1.30 .18 .196
Lesbian Age ⫺.00 ⫺.07 ⫺.01 .946
Gender .36 1.66 .14 .099†
Race .31 1.31 .11 .193
Political Affiliation .32 1.44 .13 .152
Catholicism ⫺.27 ⫺1.23 ⫺.11 .223
Protestantism .02 .05 .01 .958
Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.66 ⫺1.64 ⫺.17 .104
AH Fundamentalism ⫺.57 ⫺2.57 ⫺.35 .011ⴱ
PM Devotionalism ⫺.27 ⫺1.37 ⫺.20 .174
PM Orthodoxy ⫺.02 ⫺.08 ⫺.01 .934
PM Evangelism .05 .22 .03 .829
GM Intrinsic .13 .45 .07 .657
GM Extrinsic-Social ⫺.00 ⫺.02 ⫺.00 .988
GM Extrinsic-Personal .22 1.35 .18 .179
Note. N ⫽ 150 –151.

p ⬍ .10. ⴱ p ⬍ .05.

whether a doctor should refuse IVF for a single icantly predict participants’ ratings of whether a
mother (R2 ⫽ .19; adjusted R2 ⫽ .11; F(14, doctor should refuse IVF for an elderly woman
135) ⫽ 2.25, p ⫽ .009). Only fundamentalism (R 2 ⫽ .17; adjusted R 2 ⫽ .09; F(14,
was significantly related to participants’ ratings 133) ⫽ 1.99, p ⫽ .023). Democrats were less
of doctor refusal as higher scores were related to likely to support doctor refusal compared to
more support for doctor refusal. There was a Republicans and Caucasians were less likely to
trend for Catholics, compared to other religions, support doctor refusal compared to ethnic mi-
to report greater support for doctor refusal, al- norities. Individuals higher in intrinsic religios-
though this did not quite reach significance. ity were also significantly less likely to support
The regression model did not significantly doctor refusal.
predict participants’ ratings of how moral it is The regression model marginally predicted
for an elderly woman to use IVF (R2 ⫽ .11; participants’ ratings of how moral it is for a
adjusted R2 ⫽ .02; F(14, 136) ⫽ 1.22, p ⫽ woman with early onset Alzheimer’s disease to
.271). There was a marginally significant rela- use IVF (R2 ⫽ .14; adjusted R2 ⫽ .05; F(14,
tionship between orthodoxy and morality rat- 136) ⫽ 1.58, p ⫽ .092). Orthodoxy was nega-
ings, as well as between intrinsic religiosity and tively related to morality ratings as individuals
morality ratings. These results indicate a trend higher in orthodoxy rated this scenario as sig-
in which individuals lower on orthodoxy and nificantly less moral. A trend emerged with
higher on intrinsic religiosity regarded this sce- Caucasians rating this scenario as less moral
nario as more moral. The overall model signif- compared to ethnic minorities as this relation-
ATTITUDES TOWARD IVF 257

Table 3
Multiple Regressions of Demographic, Political, and Religious Variables on Doctor Refusal Ratings
Scenario Predictors B t ␤ p-value
Single mother Age .01 .60 .05 .549
Gender ⫺.29 ⫺1.54 ⫺.13 .126
Race ⫺.06 ⫺.30 ⫺.03 .763
Political Affiliation ⫺.21 ⫺1.11 ⫺.10 .269
Catholicism .34 1.85 .16 .066†
Protestantism .21 .63 .06 .532
Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.29 ⫺.86 ⫺.09 .390
.046ⴱ
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

AH Fundamentalism .38 2.02 .28


⫺.07 ⫺.40 ⫺.06
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

PM Devotionalism .692
PM Orthodoxy ⫺.07 ⫺.42 ⫺.06 .678
PM Evangelism .10 .56 .07 .577
GM Intrinsic ⫺.03 ⫺.11 ⫺.02 .912
GM Extrinsic-Social .05 .44 .04 .659
GM Extrinsic-Personal ⫺.04 ⫺.31 ⫺.04 .761
Elderly woman Age .03 1.08 .09 .283
Gender .27 1.21 .11 .228
Race ⫺.63 ⫺2.59 ⫺.23 .011ⴱ
Political Affiliation ⫺.47 ⫺2.06 ⫺.19 .042ⴱ
Catholicism ⫺.06 ⫺.24 ⫺.02 .808
Protestantism .38 .96 .09 .341
Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.33 ⫺.79 ⫺.09 .429
AH Fundamentalism ⫺.04 ⫺.18 ⫺.03 .854
PM Devotionalism .07 .35 .05 .730
PM Orthodoxy .25 1.16 .18 .250
PM Evangelism .26 1.19 .16 .236
GM Intrinsic ⫺.66 ⫺2.27 ⫺.37 .025ⴱ
GM Extrinsic-Social ⫺.17 ⫺1.25 ⫺.12 .215
GM Extrinsic-Personal .15 .89 .12 .378
Alzheimer’s disease Age .04 1.30 .11 .195
Gender ⫺.10 ⫺.44 ⫺.04 .659
Race ⫺.38 ⫺1.54 ⫺.14 .125
Political Affiliation ⫺.61 ⫺2.63 ⫺.24 .010ⴱ
Catholicism .38 1.68 .15 .095†
Protestantism ⫺.22 ⫺.53 ⫺.05 .596
Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.21 ⫺.50 ⫺.05 .618
AH Fundamentalism ⫺.60 ⫺2.62 ⫺.37 .010ⴱ
PM Devotionalism .27 1.35 .21 .180
PM Orthodoxy .05 .23 .04 .821
PM Evangelism .43 1.95 .26 .053†
GM Intrinsic ⫺.44 ⫺1.49 ⫺.24 .139
GM Extrinsic-Social ⫺.13 ⫺.91 ⫺.09 .363
GM Extrinsic-Personal ⫺.04 ⫺.24 ⫺.03 .813
Sex change to woman Age .03 1.06 .09 .291
Gender ⫺.09 ⫺.38 ⫺.03 .703
Race ⫺.42 ⫺1.66 ⫺.14 .099†
Political Affiliation ⫺.45 ⫺1.92 ⫺.17 .057†
Catholicism .16 .69 .06 .490
Protestantism ⫺.44 ⫺1.07 ⫺.10 .288
Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.52 ⫺1.23 ⫺.13 .221
AH Fundamentalism .03 .14 .02 .888
PM Devotionalism .14 .67 .10 .503
PM Orthodoxy .30 1.35 .21 .178
PM Evangelism .09 .41 .05 .684
(table continues)
258 SIGILLO, MILLER, AND WEISER

Table 3 (continued)
Scenario Predictors B t ␤ p-value
GM Intrinsic .10 .32 .05 .753
GM Extrinsic-Social .20 1.39 .13 .166
GM Extrinsic-Personal ⫺.40 ⫺2.36 ⫺.31 .019ⴱ
Woman lives as man Age .05 1.54 .13 .125
Gender ⫺.02 ⫺.06 ⫺.01 .951
Race ⫺.32 ⫺1.18 ⫺.11 .241
Political Affiliation ⫺.47 ⫺1.88 ⫺.18 .063†
Catholicism .08 .34 .03 .732
Protestantism ⫺.24 ⫺.54 ⫺.05 .588
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.51 ⫺1.14 ⫺.13 .257


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

AH Fundamentalism ⫺.02 ⫺.07 ⫺.01 .941


PM Devotionalism .22 1.02 .16 .308
PM Orthodoxy .04 .15 .02 .879
PM Evangelism .32 1.35 .18 .180
GM Intrinsic ⫺.04 ⫺.14 ⫺.02 .890
GM Extrinsic-Social .01 .09 .01 .932
GM Extrinsic-Personal ⫺.26 ⫺1.46 ⫺.20 .147
Lesbian Age ⫺.01 ⫺.41 ⫺.03 .685
Gender .11 .50 .04 .621
Race ⫺.26 ⫺1.03 ⫺.09 .304
Political Affiliation ⫺.49 ⫺2.13 ⫺.19 .035ⴱ
Catholicism .28 1.22 .10 .224
Protestantism ⫺.01 ⫺.03 ⫺.00 .978
Atheist/Agnostic ⫺.12 ⫺.30 ⫺.03 .768
AH Fundamentalism .59 2.58 .35 .011ⴱ
PM Devotionalism .16 .78 .11 .436
PM Orthodoxy ⫺.52 ⫺2.43 ⫺.36 .016ⴱ
PM Evangelism .41 1.92 .24 .058†
GM Intrinsic .04 .14 .02 .891
GM Extrinsic-Social ⫺.21 ⫺1.53 ⫺.14 .129
GM Extrinsic-Personal ⫺.04 ⫺.22 ⫺.03 .825
Note. N ⫽ 148 –151.

p ⬍ .10. ⴱ p ⬍ .05.

ship approached significance. There was also a in this scenario, although this relationship was
marginally significant relationship between fun- not quite significant.
damentalism and morality ratings as individuals The regression model did not significantly
with higher fundamentalism scores rated this predict participants’ ratings of how moral it is
scenario as more moral. Ratings of whether a for a woman who was born a man and had a sex
doctor should refuse IVF for a woman with change to use IVF (R2 ⫽ .14; adjusted R2 ⫽ .05;
early onset Alzheimer’s disease were signifi- F(14, 135) ⫽ 1.54, p ⫽ .107). However, ortho-
cantly predicted by the model (R2 ⫽ .17; ad- doxy was significantly related to morality rat-
justed R2 ⫽ .08; F(14, 136) ⫽ 1.93, p ⫽ .028). ings with individuals higher in orthodoxy rating
Democrats were less likely to support doctoral the sex change scenario as less moral. A trend
refusal compared to Republicans. Fundamental- was also found with Democrats rating the sce-
ism was negatively associated with doctor re- nario as more moral compared to Republicans.
fusal as individuals higher on fundamentalism The overall model significantly predicted par-
were significantly less likely to support doctor ticipants’ ratings of whether a doctor should
refusal. Catholics, compared to other religions, refuse IVF for a woman who was born a man
tended to demonstrate more support for doctor and had a sex change (R2 ⫽ .20; adjusted R2 ⫽
refusal, although this only approached signifi- .11; F(14, 136) ⫽ 2.38, p ⫽ .005). Personal
cance. Finally, a positive relationship between extrinsic religiosity significantly predicted doc-
evangelism and doctor refusal ratings was found tor refusal ratings with higher scores indicating
ATTITUDES TOWARD IVF 259

less support for doctor refusal. Democrats and doctors refusing to perform IVF on such women
Caucasians were less likely to support doctor were differentially associated with specific reli-
refusal compared to Republicans and ethnic mi- gious characteristics, broad political affiliation,
norities respectively, although these relation- and some demographic variables. These find-
ships only approached significance. ings further differed based upon women’s par-
The regression model did not significantly ticular nontraditional lifestyle.
predict participants’ ratings of how moral it is
for a woman who lives as a man to use IVF Nontraditional Lifestyles and Attitudes
(R 2 ⫽ .14; adjusted R 2 ⫽ .05; F(14, Toward IVF
136) ⫽ 1.52, p ⫽ .111). However, those higher
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

on orthodoxy rated the scenario as significantly The type of nontraditional lifestyle was found
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

less moral. Additionally, the model did not sig- to be strongly associated with ratings of moral
nificantly predict whether a doctor should re- worthiness and strength of doctor refusal en-
fuse IVF for a woman who lives as a man (R2 ⫽ dorsement. Overall, single mothers and lesbians
.13; adjusted R2 ⫽ .04; F(14, 135) ⫽ 1.46, p ⫽ were perceived as more morally worthy of IVF
.135). Political affiliation was found to have a and less deserving of doctor refusals compared
marginal relationship with Democrats indicat- to women living as men and women who are
ing less support for doctor refusal compared to born as men and have a sex change. Attitudes
Republicans. toward single mothers and lesbians may be
The regression model significantly predicted more crystallized than for these transgendered
participants’ ratings of how moral it is for a women as participants may have been more
lesbian to use IVF (R2 ⫽ .23; adjusted R2 ⫽ .15; familiar with single mothers and lesbians. If
F(14, 136) ⫽ 2.83, p ⫽ .001). Fundamentalism participants’ perceptions of these women are
was significantly related to morality ratings as already formed, then their attitudes are more
individuals with higher scores rated this sce- easily accessible when making judgments con-
nario as less moral. Gender was marginally re- cerning them and their use of IVF. Higher doc-
lated to morality ratings as women indicated tor refusal ratings were found for elderly
that they considered this scenario as more moral women and women with Alzheimer’s disease.
compared to men. The regression model signif- These results may be due to pragmatic concerns
icantly predicted whether a doctor should refuse for future children’s well-being as participants
IVF for a lesbian (R2 ⫽ .26; adjusted R2 ⫽ .18; may have believed that these women would be
F(14, 135) ⫽ 3.30, p ⬍ .001). Democrats were physically unable to raise their child.
significantly less likely to support doctor refusal
compared to Republicans. Orthodoxy signifi- Effects of Religion, Politics, and
cantly predicted doctor refusal ratings as indi- Demographics on Attitudes Toward IVF
viduals with higher scores were less likely to
support doctor refusal. Fundamentalism was Religious, political, and demographic charac-
also positively related to support for doctor re- teristics of participants were highly predictive
fusal with individuals higher on fundamental- of attitudes toward the moral worthiness of IVF
ism indicating more support for doctoral re- and support for doctors refusing to perform the
fusal. There was also a positive relationship procedure.
between evangelism and support for doctoral Religious characteristics were the strongest
refusal, although this relationship only ap- predictors of the morality of IVF use by non-
proached significance. traditional women and support for doctors re-
fusing to provide IVF for such women. Funda-
Discussion mentalism was negatively related to IVF moral-
ity and positively related to doctor refusals of
The current study investigated the relation- IVF for single mothers and lesbians, but not for
ship between religious, political and demo- women suffering from Alzheimer’s disease.
graphic characteristics and attitudes toward IVF These findings parallel research from Kirkpat-
for women leading nontraditional lifestyles. rick (1993) regarding the positive association
Overall, attitudes regarding the morality of IVF between fundamentalism and prejudice toward
use by nontraditional women and support for lesbians. Orthodoxy was inversely associated
260 SIGILLO, MILLER, AND WEISER

with doctor refusals of IVF for lesbians, consis- 2001). With respect to race, Caucasians were
tent with previous research demonstrating the less likely to support doctors refusing IVF for
negative relationship between orthodoxy and elderly women and women who were born men
attitudes toward lesbians (Jonathan, 2008; Lay- and had a sex change, but unexpectedly per-
the et al., 2002). Orthodoxy was also inversely ceived women with Alzheimer’s using IVF as
related to the moral worthiness of IVF for some less moral compared to ethnic minorities. Reli-
of the other nontraditional women, findings that gious affiliation, of the three studied in our
were not anticipated. Evangelism was positively sample, Catholicism, Protestantism and Athe-
associated with doctors refusing IVF for women ism/Agnosticism, was unrelated to attitudes to-
with Alzheimer’s disease and lesbians; such ward nontraditional women using IVF.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

support for doctor refusal coincides with previ-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ous research depicting evangelists’ disapproval Implications


of reproductive-related procedures because of
possible embryo destruction (Evans & Hudson, Overall, views about the morality of IVF for
2007). Devotionalism was negatively related to nontraditional women differed in this sample
perceptions of IVF morality for single mothers. based on both the specific nontraditional life-
Individuals who scored higher in intrinsic reli- style and participants’ own religious character-
giosity considered elderly women using IVF as istics. Participants did not equally endorse IVF
more moral and less worthy of doctor refusal. for all types of women. This variability is likely
Individuals who endorsed higher levels of per- due to preexisting perceptions of nontraditional
sonal extrinsic religiosity perceived single women; perhaps participants had more crystal-
mothers using IVF as more morally worthy, and lized attitudes toward certain nontraditional
women who were born men and had a sex women, such as lesbians, but not toward others,
change as less deserving of doctor refusals for such as women who live as men. Participants
IVF. Social extrinsic religiosity did not produce are more supportive of IVF for single mothers
any effects for the scenarios. and lesbians which suggests that they are gen-
Broad political affiliation exhibited strong ef- erally more accepting of these types of women.
fects on attitudes toward the moral worthiness Over time, it seems reasonable that such support
of IVF and support for doctors refusing to pro- may be provided to transgendered individuals
vide treatment. Democrats rated the scenarios as as the public becomes more familiar and accept-
more moral and were less supportive of doctor ing of these lifestyles. Attitudes toward nontra-
refusals compared to Republicans. These find- ditional women may become more tolerant over
ings are consistent with the extant literature time; therefore, it is important to continually
demonstrating that Democrats are more liberal assess attitudes toward IVF, and particularly its
in their views concerning reproductive health use by nontraditional women.
issues (Strickler & Danigelis, 2002) and, similar In addition to differential support provided to
to abortion, may consider IVF use as a woman’s the variety of nontraditional women, some vari-
right. ables exhibited differences between the depen-
Demographics were also predictive of atti- dent measures for the same woman. This sug-
tudes toward IVF. Previous studies have sug- gests that morality and doctor refusal represent
gested that younger individuals are more two separate constructs. Morality may be
concerned with reproductive technologies; viewed as individuals’ private beliefs and ap-
however, results from this study did not dem- proval. Some may believe that everyone has a
onstrate this relationship between age and atti- right to procreate, while others may believe that
tudes toward IVF for nontraditional women, only certain types of women should have the
due most likely to the limited age range in our opportunity to use IVF. Conversely, doctor re-
sample of college students. Gender did not pro- fusal may be linked to beliefs about legal rights.
duce strong effects, with the only difference Some individuals may believe that doctors have
being that females, as compared with males, a duty to comply with their patients’ needs and
rated lesbians using IVF as more moral. This that refusals are infringements on patients’
finding somewhat coincides with other studies rights, while others may believe that physicians
which demonstrated that women are more likely have the right to refuse to provide treatment
to accept IVF in general (Lasker & Murray, (Daar, 2008). Thus, future research should in-
ATTITUDES TOWARD IVF 261

vestigate both measures as they represent two refusal. However, future studies should include
separate constructs that relate to support of IVF other possible constructs of IVF attitudes, and
use by nontraditional women. test these measures, in order to further what is
A final implication is that religious charac- known about individuals’ attitudes toward IVF
teristics, such as fundamentalism, orthodoxy, and the women who use such technology.
devotionalism, and intrinsic and extrinsic reli-
giosity, are better predictors of attitudes toward Conclusion
the morality of IVF for nontraditional women
than religious affiliation. Religious characteristics were related to atti-
tudes toward the morality of IVF use and sup-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Limitations and Future Directions port for doctor refusal to perform IVF on a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

variety of nontraditional women. Religious


As with any study, there were some limita- characteristics, in comparison to religious affil-
tions. Foremost, the current sample did not have iation, were better predictors of IVF attitudes.
much variability in religious affiliation which Furthermore, political affiliation was another
may have affected results. Furthermore, the strong predictor, suggesting that this variable is
sample was composed of undergraduate stu- important in shaping individuals’ attitudes to-
dents who may not strictly follow the tenets of ward IVF. Across religious and political char-
their religious affiliation, thus resulting in null acteristics, generally more support for IVF
findings. This seems likely as college students exists when the recipient exemplifies a more
demonstrate less ability to integrate religious familiar type of woman, such as a single mother
teachings into their moral and political beliefs or a lesbian. Finally, attitudes toward IVF and
(Sullins, 1999). Future studies should obtain a its use by various types of women should be
more representative sample of religious affilia- continually assessed to determine whether such
tions to determine whether this effect transcends attitudes become more acceptable over time.
the college student sample.
Another limitation is the narrow range of
scenarios that were included in the study. Par- References
ticipants only responded to six scenarios. A Allport, G. W., & Ross, J. M. (1967). Personal reli-
wider variety, such as a woman with a different gious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Person-
illness (e.g., cancer) or a control scenario, ality and Social Psychology, 5, 432– 443. doi:
would have provided a better understanding of 10.1037/h0021212
attitudes toward IVF use by nontraditional Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (1992). Author-
women and an opportunity to make compari- itarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and
sons between a traditional woman (i.e., a het- prejudice. International Journal for the Psychol-
erosexual married woman) and nontraditional ogy of Religion, 2, 113–133. doi:10.1207/
women. Future research should determine s15327582ijpr0202_5
Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (2004). A revised
whether individuals’ responses are dependent religious fundamentalism scale: The short and
on whether IVF is used by women for physical sweet of it. International Journal for the Psychol-
or genetic reasons (e.g., blocked fallopian ogy of Religion, 14, 47–54. doi:10.1207/
tubes) or for social reasons (e.g., a lifestyle s15327582ijpr1401_4
choice to remain single). Future studies should Arnold, L. W., & Weisberg, H. F. (1996). Parent-
also assess individuals’ attitudes toward policy hood, family values, and the 1992 presidential
issues associated with IVF use. For example, election. American Politics Quarterly, 24, 194 –
singles, gay men, and lesbians have limited IVF 220. doi:10.1177/1532673X9602400204
access in states with mandatory insurance laws Bornstein, B. H., & Miller, M. K. (2009). God in the
that cover only heterosexual married couples courtroom: Religion’s role at trial. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
(Daar, 2008; Waldman, 2006). Brooks, C. (2002). Religious influence and the poli-
A final limitation of the study is that the tics of family decline concern: Trends, sources,
scenarios and dependent measures were not pi- and U.S. political behavior. American Sociological
loted. Results from this study suggest that atti- Review, 67, 191–211. doi:10.2307/3088892
tudes toward IVF can be assessed through mul- Bryant, J., & Gudgin, M. (2008). Attitudes amongst
tiple constructs, such as morality and doctor young adults to use of embryonic stem cells in
262 SIGILLO, MILLER, AND WEISER

research and therapy: Comparison of evangelical Gorsuch, R. L., & McPherson, S. E. (1989). Intrinsic/
Christian students with non-Christian students. Extrinsic measurement: I/E-Revised and single-
Science & Christian Belief, 20, 91–105. item scales. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Burnett, R. C., & Salka, W. M. (2009). Determinants Religion, 28, 348 –354. doi:10.2307/1386745
of electoral support for anti-gay marriage consti- Huang, H.-H., & Coker, A. D. (2010). Examining
tutional amendments: An examination of 2006 issues affecting African American participation in
votes on ballot measures in the states. Journal of research studies. Journal of Black Studies, 40,
Homosexuality, 56, 1071–1082. doi:10.1080/ 619 – 636. doi:10.1177/0021934708317749
00918360903275476 Inhorn, M. C., & Birenbaum-Carmeli, D. (2008).
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Ameri- Assisted reproductive technologies and culture
can Society for Reproductive Medicine, & Society change. Annual Review of Anthropology, 37, 177–
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

for Assisted Reproductive Technology. (2009). 196. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.37.081407


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

2007 Assisted reproductive technology success .085230


rates: National summary and fertility clinic re- Jacobs, E. A., Rolle, I., Ferrans, C. E., Whitaker,
ports. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and E. E., & Warnecke, R. B. (2006). Understanding
Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and African Americans’ views of the trustworthiness
Prevention. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/ of physicians. Journal of General Internal Medi-
art/ART2007/PDF/COMPLETE_2007_ART.pdf cine, 21, 642– 647. doi:10.1111/j.1525-1497
Congregation for the Doctrine of the Faith (2008). In- .2006.00485.x
struction “Dignitas Personae” of certain bioethical Jonathan, E. (2008). The influence of religious fun-
questions. Retrieved from http://www.usccb.org/ damentalism, right-wing authoritarianism, and
comm/Dignitaspersonae/Dignitas_Personae.pdf Christian orthodoxy on explicit and implicit mea-
Daar, J. F. (2008). Accessing reproductive technolo- sures of attitudes toward homosexuals. Interna-
gies: Invisible barriers, indelible harms. Berkeley tional Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 18,
Journal of Gender, Law & Justice, 23, 18 – 82. 316 –329. doi:10.1080/10508610802229262
Kahn, M. M. (2006a). Conservative Christian teach-
de Lacey, S., Davies, M., Homan, G., Briggs, N., &
ers: Possible consequences for lesbian, gay and
Norman, R. J. (2007). Factors and perceptions that
bisexual youth. Intercultural Education, 17, 359 –
influence women’s decisions to have a single em-
371. doi:10.1080/14675980600971368
bryo transferred. Reproductive BioMedicine On-
Kahn, S. M. (2006b). Making technology familiar:
line, 15, 526 –531. doi:10.1016/S1472-6483(10)
Orthodox Jews and infertility support, advice, and
60384-7
inspiration. Culture, Medicine & Psychiatry, 30,
de la Fuente Fonnest, I., Sondergaard, F., Fonnest,
467– 480. doi:10.1007/s11013-006-9029-8
G., & Vedsted-Jacobsen, A. (2000). Attitudes Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1993). Fundamentalism, Christian
among health care professionals on the ethics of orthodoxy, and intrinsic religious orientation as
assisted reproductive technologies and legal abor- predictors of discriminatory attitudes. Journal for
tion. Acta Obstetricia & Gynecologica Scandi- the Scientific Study of Religion, 32, 256 –268. doi:
navica, 79, 49 –53. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0412 10.2307/1386664
.2000.079001049.x Larijani, B., & Zahedi, F. (2007). Ethical and reli-
Dunn, P. C., Ryan, I. J., & O’Brien, K. (1988). gious aspects of gamete and embryo donation and
College students’ acceptance of adoption and five legislation in Iran. Journal of Religion &
alternative fertilization techniques. Journal of Sex Health, 46, 399 – 408. doi:10.1007/s10943-007-
Research, 24, 282–287. doi:10.1080/ 9108-3
00224498809551425 Lasker, J. N., & Murray, D. E. (2001). Attitudes
Evans, J. H., & Hudson, K. (2007). Religion and toward technologies for conception: A 15-year
reproductive genetics: Beyond views of embryonic follow-up. Journal of Applied Social Psychol-
life? Journal for the Scientific Study of Reli- ogy, 31, 2165–2183. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816
gion, 46, 565–581. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5906 .2001.tb00169.x
.2007.00378.x Laythe, B., Finkel, D. G., Bringle, R. G., & Kirkpat-
Genia, V. (1996). I, E, quest, and fundamentalism as rick, L. A. (2002). Religious fundamentalism as a
predictors of psychological and spiritual well- predictor of prejudice: A two-component model.
being. Journal for the Scientific Study of Reli- Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41,
gion, 35, 56 – 64. doi:10.2307/1386395 623– 635. doi:10.1111/1468-5906.00142
Giorgi, L. (1992). Religious involvement in a secu- Libreria Editrice Vaticana. (1997). Catechism of the
larized society: An empirical confirmation of Mar- catholic church (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: USCC
tin’s general theory of secularization. The British Publishing.
Journal of Sociology, 43, 639 – 656. doi:10.2307/ Lupfer, M., & Wald, K. (1985). An exploration of
591342 adults’ religious orientations and their philoso-
ATTITUDES TOWARD IVF 263

phies of human nature. Journal for the Scientific Awareness and attitudes. Public Opinion Quar-
Study of Religion, 24, 293–304. doi:10.2307/ terly, 62, 633– 664. doi:10.1086/297864
1385818 Singer, P. (2009). Thirty years of ‘test-tube’ babies.
McFarland, S. G. (1989). Religious orientations and Free Inquiry, 29, 20 –21.
the targets of discrimination. Journal for the Sci- Strickler, J., & Danigelis, N. L. (2002). Changing
entific Study of Religion, 28, 324 –336. doi: frameworks in attitudes toward abortion. Sociolog-
10.2307/1386743 ical Forum, 17, 187–201. doi:10.1023/A:
Miller, M. K., Maskaly, J., Peoples, C. D., & Sigillo, 1016033012225
A. E. (2011). The impact of mock jurors’ religious Sullins, D. P. (1999). Catholic/Protestant trends on
characteristics on their verdicts. Manuscript sub- abortion: Convergence and polarity. Journal for
mitted for publication. the Scientific Study of Religion, 38, 354 –369. doi:
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Pauls, M., & Hutchinson, R. C. (2002). Bioethics for 10.2307/1387757


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

clinicians: 28. Protestant bioethics. Canadian Sullivan, L. (1993). In the path of Daedalus: Middle-
Medical Association Journal, 166, 339 –343. class Australians’ attitudes to embryo research.
Priest, S. H. (2000). US public opinion divided over British Journal of Sociology, 44, 271–302. doi:
biotechnology? Nature Biotechnology, 18, 939 – 10.2307/591220
942. doi:10.1038/79412 Thumma, S. (1991). Negotiating a religious identity:
Putney, S., & Middleton, R. (1961). Dimensions and The case of the gay evangelical. Sociology of Re-
correlates of religious ideologies. Social Forces, 39, ligion, 52, 333–347.
285–290. doi:10.2307/2573423 Turtle, A. M., Harrajchi, N., Perry, A., & Tully, L.
Randolph-Seng, B., Nielsen, M. E., Bottoms, B. L., (1988). University student attitudes to in vitro fer-
& Filipas, H. (2008). The relationship between tilization and its ramifications. Australian Journal
ethnicity, Christian orthodoxy, and mental health. of Sex, Marriage & Family, 9, 189 –201.
Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 11, 795– 805. Waldman, E. (2006). Cultural priorities revealed:
doi:10.1080/13674670802084788 The development and regulation of assisted repro-
Roberts, E. F. S. (2006). God’s laboratory: Religious ductive in the United States and Israel. Health
rationalities and modernity in Ecuadorian in vitro Matrix: Journal of Law Medicine, 16, 65–106.
fertilization. Culture, Medicine & Psychiatry, 30, Young, R. L. (1992). Religious orientation, race and
507–536. doi:10.1007/s11013-006-9037-8 support for the death penalty. Journal for the Sci-
Schenker, J. G. (2005). Assisted reproductive prac- entific Study of Religion, 31, 76 – 87. doi:10.2307/
tice: Religious perspectives. Reproductive Bio- 1386833
Medicine Online, 10, 310 –319. doi:10.1016/
S1472-6483(10)61789-0 Received January 25, 2010
Singer, E., Corning, A., & Lamias, M. (1998). Revision received March 14, 2011
Trends: Genetic testing, engineering, and therapy: Accepted February 8, 2012 䡲

You might also like