You are on page 1of 8

HEALTH COMMUNICATION

https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2021.1888438

The Effect of Social Media on Stress among Young Adults during COVID-19 Pandemic:
Taking into Account Fatalism and Social Media Exhaustion
Annabel Ngien and Shaohai Jiang
Department of Communications and New Media, National University of Singapore

ABSTRACT
Social media has been widely used as an important source of health information, particularly during public
health crises. However, findings regarding social media’s impact on young adults’ mental health are
mixed. There is a need to identify social mechanisms underlying the effect of social media on mental
health outcomes. Our study breaks new ground by proposing and testing a moderated mediation
pathway from social media use to stress in young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. We conducted
a two-wave online panel survey in the context of China. With a general basis of the Street’s three-stage
model, our results indicated that social media failed to directly affect young adults’ stress. Instead, fatalism
completely mediated this relationship. Also, one’s perceived social media exhaustion negatively moder­
ated this mediation pathway. The findings have significant theoretical and practical implications for the
use of social media to promote health, well-being, and quality of life among young people during public
health crises.

The COVID-19 pandemic is causing unprecedented disrup­ whether health informational seeking via online platforms
tions to many people’s lives. Particularly, young adults are (e.g., social media) is effective in improving young people’s
distressed due to isolation and social distancing, and higher well-being. The inconsistent results may be due to the lack
threats of potential years of life lost (Jimenez et al., 2020). of investigation of mechanisms underlying the effect of
A survey study conducted in France showed that two-thirds health-related social media on young adults’ emotional
of college students experienced stress during COVID-19 health. The focus on the direct effect ignores the complex
(Husky et al., 2020). Another study among university stu­ psychological consequences of social media use, which
dents in Greece also demonstrated the negative impact of might in turn have more direct influence on health out­
lockdown during COVID-19 on their mental health, such comes. Street (2003) highlighted the need to explore speci­
as increased anxiety and depression (Kaparounaki et al., fic mediators influencing the relationship between
2020). To cope with COVID-19 related stress, many interactive media use and health outcomes. In the current
young people turn to social media to seek health informa­ literature, scarce research has centered on young adults’
tion and peer interactions. Compared to the average indi­ health-related social media use and its effect during public
viduals, youths are 63% more likely to use social media as health emergencies (i.e., COVID-19). This is concerning,
a key source of information and advice (Nielsen, 2009). because public health crises can evoke greater threat apprai­
Despite the popularity of social media, prior research on sal, which could lead to greater stress in youths. Also, most
the influence of health-related social media use on younger previous studies are based in developed countries, with
generations’ well-being has illustrated mixed findings. For limited empirical evidence in developing countries such as
example, Asibey et al. (2017) indicated that university stu­ China, the context of this study. Only 9.8% of Chinese
dents’ health-related Internet use, including social media population have adequate health literacy as compared to
use for health information seeking, was positively asso­ 41% in developed European countries (World Health
ciated with healthy lifestyle change. However, other studies Organisation, 2016). The lower health literacy is also
demonstrated null or insignificant effects. For instance, Yoo observed in Chinese young people (Guo et al., 2020).
et al. (2016) found that college students’ exposure to anti- Thus, people in China may encounter more barriers in
smoking messages via social media failed to significantly obtaining and interpreting health information for decision-
influence their smoking attitudes and intentions. Even more making during public health crises. To fill these research
concerning, excessive social media exposure to health infor­ gaps, the current study focused on young adults’ health
mation during public health emergencies was found to information seeking via social media (e.g., Weibo,
increase depression and anxiety (Gao et al., 2020). These WeChat) during the COVID-19 pandemic in China, and
mixed findings are also reflected in a systematic review by investigated the underlying mechanism of how social media
Pretorius et al. (2019), stating that it still remains unclear use can help young adults to cope with stress.

CONTACT Shaohai Jiang cnmjs@nus.edu.sg Department of Communications and New Media, National University of Singapore, Blk AS6, #03-41, 11 Computing
Drive, 117416, Singapore
© 2021 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
2 A. NGIEN AND S. JIANG

Conceptual framework because of their low emotional capability to handle health


threats.
The conceptual framework is based on Street’s (2003) three-
Social media may play a key role in reducing fatalism. First,
stage model of health promotion using interactive media. The
seeking health information on social media can improve health
model highlights that media use seldom directly influences
knowledge. COVID-19 information is widely accessible via
health. Instead, in most situations, this relationship is indirect,
social media, empowering and activating users to take preven­
mediated by users’ interaction with the media platform, and its
tive actions (Gao et al., 2020). As one’s internal locus of control
subsequent intermediate outcomes.
increases during social media usage, fatalism will decrease.
Specifically, Stage 1 of the model explores the antecedents
Befort et al. (2013) found that using the Internet, including
(e.g., institutional, technological, and user-related) of media
social media, as a primary source of cancer information nega­
use. This merges into Stage 2, the user-media-message interac­
tively predicted the fatalistic belief that preventing cancer is
tion. In this process, an interplay of user (e.g., emotional state,
impossible. Second, compared to traditional media, social
desire for information), media (e.g., degree of interactivity) and
media allows people to search for information within their
message (e.g., readability, cultural appropriateness) character­
social networks, enhancing collective knowledge, social cohe­
istics occurs, resulting in certain intermediate outcomes (e.g.,
sion and rapport, which helps overcome fatalistic fears. The
knowledge, motivation, self-efficacy), which finally lead to
important role of social media use in lowering fatalism has
health outcomes (e.g., lifestyle change, healthy behavior uptake)
been documented in prior research. For instance, although
in Stage 3.
news exposure from mass media might increase fatalism,
Modeling a pathway from interactive media use to health
Internet use for health information can decrease fatalism (Lee
improvement, we (a) propose a pathway linking social media
et al., 2012). Mass media is unidirectional, while social media
use for COVID-19 information (media use) to reduced stress
features the bidirectional flow of information. Instead of inter­
(health outcome), (b) argue that fatalism (intermediate out­
preting information alone, individuals can use social media to
come) plays a mediating role, and (c) predict that this media­
ask questions, receive feedback, and collaborate with each
tion pathway will be moderated by individuals’ social media
other to cope with health challenges, resulting in less uncer­
exhaustion from social media use (user-media-message inter­
tainties and fatalistic beliefs (Ramanadhan et al., 2013). Thus,
action). This model is illustrated in Figure 1. The following
we posit:
sections discuss these key concepts of this study, and provide
evidence for the pathways proposed.
H1: Social media use is negatively related to fatalism among
young adults in China during COVID-19.
Social media use, fatalism and stress
Prior research has documented that fatalism is associated
Fatalism is the belief that one’s health is predetermined by fate, with negative health consequences. Fatalism evokes the per­
chance, luck or God and beyond the capacity of personal ceived inevitability of predestined outcomes, and thus
control (Powe & Finnie, 2003). As fatalistic individuals believe decreases one’s coping motivation. As fatalism narrows the
in the perceived inevitability of death, they are reluctant to range of resources to be utilized, it reduces capacities of tack­
adopt protective behaviors. Jimenez et al. (2020) stated that ling stressful situations, and increases the feeling of being over­
fatalism is a salient issue in the context of COVID-19, due to whelmed by emotional burdens (Pearlin et al., 1981). Also,
the lack of an effective treatment. They also found that youths fatalism is closely linked to cognitive avoidance. Fatalistic indi­
exhibit higher fatalistic COVID-19 beliefs than older adults, viduals conform to their expected fate, and avoid activities that

Social
media
exhaustion

-.09*

Social media
use Fatalism Stress
-.18*** .14***

NS

Wave 1 Wave 2

*p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001


NS=not significant
Figure 1. Conceptual framework. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. NS = not significant
HEALTH COMMUNICATION 3

challenge what they believe is inevitable (Peng et al., 2021). In one important contributing factor is social media exhaustion,
the COVID-19 pandemic, Jimenez et al. (2020) found that or the self-evaluated and subjective sense of tiredness from
fatalistic beliefs led to avoidance of recommended preventive social media use. Particularly among young adults, fear of
behaviors, intensifying COVID-19 related fear and stress. This missing out, or “pervasive apprehension that others might be
is especially so for young people who exhibit higher avoidance having rewarding experiences from which one is absent”
tendencies due to the lack of problem-solving skills and experi­ (Przybylski et al., 2013, p. 8) has been prevalent. When inter­
ences that facilitate effective emotion management (Nieto et al., acting with others on social media, they are expected to main­
2019). tain a good personal image, reply quickly to messages, and
While the effect of fatalism on stress has seldom been sustain growing online ties. Such online interaction requires
examined, previous studies drew the link between fatalism much time and energy, leading to social burnout. Particularly
and reduced well-being in youths. In an adolescents-based during COVID-19, users rely heavily on social media for work
study, Roberts et al. (2000) found that fatalism resulted in and leisure connections in the lockdown period, exposing
greater pessimism, lower self-esteem, and more passive coping. young adults to an influx of social demands.
Similarly, fatalistic youths engaged in higher suicide planning, In the present study, we posit that social media exhaustion
had more accepting attitudes toward suicide and were more might serve as a moderator that weakens the benefits of social
impulsive (Jamieson & Romer, 2008). Extending from the media. According to the three-stage model, the effectiveness of
literature, we predict: interactive media use on health promotion depends on the
user’s experience with the website and its online content.
H2: Fatalism is positively related to stress among young adults Social media exhaustion can therefore tap into the user-
in China during COVID-19. media-message interaction process, where users feel fatigue
when facing a plethora of information from social media.
In light of the above, one pathway linking social media use During COVID-19, people often have excessive social media
to stress will likely be indirect through the mediating variable use, and the sheer amount and complexity of information
of fatalism. Specifically, when individuals use social media for encountered from social media would result in social media
health information, they have better understanding of the exhaustion, which decreases individuals’ information proces­
health issue, lowering their fatalistic beliefs. The reduced fatal­ sing capabilities, and increases cognitive overload. Under such
ism can in turn decrease users’ stress, as they become more circumstances, users become unable to sufficiently absorb
confident in coping with the health challenge (Go & You, health information and utilize the medical knowledge to exe­
2018). Fatalism is particularly prevalent in the Confucian- cute preventive behaviors, weakening the influence of social
influenced China. As Confucianism suggests to leave to heaven media use on reducing fatalism. Further, social media exhaus­
to take care of things man cannot do, Chinese people are often tion prompts social withdrawal, an increasingly common ten­
fatalistic when facing misfortunes, which impedes coping dency amongst youths who isolate themselves from others to
motivations and efforts (Li et al., 2015). Supporting this indir­ re-charge (Li & Wong, 2015). This discourages individuals
ect relationship, Street (2003) emphasized the need to examine from seeking health information from online communities,
health-related mediators linking media use to health outcomes. reducing sources of advice that can promote proactive health
He also argued that these mediators should be situated within measures (Straughan & Seow, 1998). As such, we propose:
socio-cultural factors for more context-sensitive analyses.
While past research has not explored the mediation pathway H4a: Social media exhaustion will weaken the negative rela­
we proposed, several studies supported the indirect effect of tionship between social media use and fatalism among young
social media use on emotional health outcomes. For instance, adults in China during COVID-19.
participation in a WeChat HIV intervention program indir­
ectly reduced depression via the mediator of active coping Assuming social media exhaustion negatively moderates the
(Zhu et al., 2019). Wen et al. (2016) found that WeChat use association between social media use and fatalism, it is also
predicted subjective well-being through the mediator of user likely that social media exhaustion conditionally influences the
motivations. Thus, in line with the documented indirect effect strength of the indirect relationship between social media use
of social media use, we posit: and stress, demonstrating a pathway of moderated mediation
between the study variables.
H3: Fatalism will mediate the relationship between social
media use and stress among young adults in China during H4b: Social media exhaustion will moderate the negative and
COVID-19. indirect effect of social media use on stress, through the mediator
of fatalism among young adults in China during COVID-19.

The moderating role of social media exhaustion


As noted earlier, the extent to which social media use affects Methods
emotional well-being varies vastly. While scholars have pro­
Sampling
posed several reasons for the inefficacy of social media, such as
lack of trust in online content, low desire for information, and We conducted two waves of online surveys among young
limited technology skills (Eysenbach et al., 2004), we argue that adults (aging from 18 to 30) in China during the outbreak of
4 A. NGIEN AND S. JIANG

COVID-19 pandemic. The samples were recruited by an online Social media exhaustion was measured by three items
survey company (www.wjx.cn). This company had an online adapted from prior research (Maier et al., 2015). Respondents
panel of more than 2.6 million members in China in diversified were asked to identify to what degree they agreed with the
geographical locations and demographics. In February 2020, following three statements: During the COVID-19 pandemic,
633 people completed the Wave 1 survey. The same question­ in the past four weeks, (1) “Activities that require me to use
naire was sent to Wave 1 participants after three months in social media make me feel exhausted”; (2) “Using social media
May 2020. 423 people completed the Wave 2 survey, with the makes me feel tired”; and (3) “Using social media is a burden to
retention rate of 67%. me”. The responses were on a 5-point Likert scale where
1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. The higher
value demonstrated higher level of social media exhaustion
Measurement (Wave 1: Cronbach’s alpha = .82; Wave 2: Cronbach’s
alpha = .84).
All questionnaire items were first created in English and trans­
Demographic variables including age, gender (1 = male,
lated into Chinese. Back translation was conducted by two
2 = female), education (ranging from 1 = middle school or
graduate students who know both languages to make sure the
below to 5 = postgraduate), and personal monthly income in
Chinese and English items had the similar meaning. In Table 1,
Chinese Yuan (CNY) from 1 = CNY 3000 or below to
we presented descriptive statistics in both Wave 1 and Wave 2.
6 = above CNY 18000 were controlled.
Social media use was measured by two items that asked
respondents whether they have actively searched for COVID-
19 information on (1) social media (e.g., Weibo), and (2) social Statistical analysis
networking sites (e.g., WeChat). These two items were dichot­
All statistical analyses were performed using Stata 14. To test
omous (1 = Yes, 0 = No), and adapted from previous studies
the mediation effect, we conducted path analysis using struc­
(Liu & Jiang, 2021). Responses were summed up for data
tural equation modeling (SEM). In the covariance structure
analysis. Weibo and WeChat are two popular social media
analysis, maximum likelihood of estimation was used. In SEM,
platforms in China. Similar to other social media (e.g.,
the control variables served as exogenous variables while endo­
Facebook), Weibo and WeChat allow users to post updates in
genous variables included social media use, fatalism, and stress.
text, photo, or video to their friends or followers, with like,
Paths were drawn from exogenous variables to all endogenous
comment or share functions.
variables. Consistent with Figure 1, paths were also drawn from
Fatalism was measured by five items drawn from prior
social media use to fatalism and stress, and from fatalism to
research (Shen et al., 2009). Respondents were asked to indicate
stress. For social media use and fatalism variables, Wave 1 data
their agreement with five statements on a 5-point Likert scale
were used. For stress, Wave 2 data were used. Also, to reduce
(1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). Sample items were
confounding effects, following previous studies (Chae & Lee,
“My health is determined by fate”, and “My health is deter­
2019), stress at Wave 1 was controlled, and all the demographic
mined by luck”. We averaged the responses to create one scale.
variables were controlled as well. In the panel survey design,
The higher the value, the greater perceived fatalism (Wave 1:
Eveland and Morey (2011) contended that the length of time
Cronbach’s alpha = .83; Wave 2: Cronbach’s alpha = .87).
necessary for a cause to produce an effect was a central con­
Stress was measured by five items, drawn from the SF-36
cern. In our study, we considered that social media use for
scale (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992). Respondents were asked to
health information (Wave 1) could quickly produce an effect
report in the past four weeks, how frequently they have
on one’s fatalistic beliefs (Wave 1). However, the time to
encountered the following emotional states: (1) “Have you
produce an impact on mental health outcome, in this case,
been a nervous person”; (2) “Have you felt so down in the
stress (Wave 2) would be longer, as prior research demon­
dumps that nothing could cheer you up”; (3) “Have you felt
strated that changing one’s health behaviors or outcomes
calm and peaceful”; (4) “Have you felt downhearted and blue”;
could take weeks or even months (Slater, 2007). To examine
and (5) “Have you been a happy person”. Responses were on
the mediation effect more closely, running SEM using Stata 14
a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = very frequently to 5 = very
can generate the bootstrapped confidence interval (CI). At the
rarely. Items (1), (2), and (4) were reversely coded. The higher
.05 level with 5,000 re-samples, if the lower and upper CIs are
score represented greater stress (Wave 1: Cronbach’s
either both below or above zero, the mediation effect is
alpha = .80; Wave 2: Cronbach’s alpha = .81).
supported.
To test the moderation effect, we performed hierarchical
Table 1. Descriptive statistics. regression analysis. In the first step, fatalism was regressed on
Wave 1 (N = 633) Mean Wave 2 (N = 423) Mean demographic variables, social media use, and social media
Variables (SD) (SD) exhaustion. In the second step, an interaction term between
Age 24.69 (3.73) 25.14 (3.69) social media use and social media exhaustion (after being mean
Gender (being male) 40.1% 38.5%
Education 3.66 (.73) 3.71 (.72) centered) was entered into the model. This inclusion of inter­
Income 2.41 (1.31) 2.47 (1.31) action term provided test for moderation effect.
Social media use 1.64 (.60) 1.64 (.60) To further examine the moderated mediation effect or con­
Fatalism 1.82 (.79) 1.83 (.80)
Stress 2.30 (.78) 2.20 (.75) ditional indirect effect, we adopted a normal theory-based
Social media 2.56 (1.00) 2.57 (1.01) approach that has been widely used in previous studies
exhaustion (Preacher et al., 2007). Specifically, we looked at the
HEALTH COMMUNICATION 5

significance level of conditional indirect effects at three differ­ H4b explored the moderated mediation effect, specifically
ent values of the moderator, social media exhaustion, including the conditional indirect effect of social media use on stress,
high (1 standard deviation above the mean), medium (the through fatalism, at three levels of social media exhaustion.
mean), and low (1 standard deviation below the mean). Our results showed that no matter whether social media users
reported low, moderate, or high levels of social media exhaus­
tion, fatalism remained a significant mediator, demonstrating
Results a significant moderated mediation effect. However, the direc­
The initial SEM was saturated, failing to generate a chi- tion of the moderated mediation effect was different from our
square value. Of the paths from endogenous variables, we expectation. As shown in Table 3, as the level of social media
found that the path from social media use to stress was exhaustion increased, the negative and indirect effect of social
insignificant (β = −.01, p = .80, 95% CI [−.0895 to .0687]). media use on stress also increased. Therefore, H4b was not
To seek the most parsimonious explanation, previous supported. The indirect and negative effect of social media use
research suggested to trim non-significant paths (McCoach, on stress was stronger for those who reported higher levels of
2003). Therefore, we pruned the path from social media to social media exhaustion.
stress. The pruned SEM model had a satisfactory fit, χ2
(1) = 0.07, p = .80; RMSEA = .000 (90% confidence interval:
Discussion
.000–.082); CFI = 1.000; and SRMR = .001 (Hu & Bentler,
1999). Despite the prevalence of social media use among young
H1 proposed that social media use was negatively related to adults, research on the direct impacts of social media on their
fatalism. As shown in Table 2, social media use had a negative mental health has often produced conflicting results. The pre­
and significant relationship with fatalism (β = −.18, p < .001, sent study broke new ground by empirically testing
95% CI [−.2680 to −.0904]), suggesting that the greater social a moderated mediation pathway linking social media to stress
media use, the less perceived fatalism, supporting H1. among young adults during COVID-19 pandemic in China.
H2 posited that fatalism was positively associated with Our results showed that social media reduced fatalism,
stress. As illustrated in Table 2, this relationship was positive a finding consistent with past studies that supported the posi­
and significant (β = .14, p < .001, 95% CI [.0625 to .2230]), tive impact of social media on coping with health-related issues
meaning that as one’s perception of fatalism increased, his or (Farpour et al., 2017). Young adults often use social media to
her stress level would also increase, supporting H2. consume health information due to its easy access and inde­
H3 examined the mediating role of fatalism on the relation­ pendence from parental support (Fergie et al., 2016). This
ship between social media use and stress. The bootstrapping reduces worries and fears about health consequences that will
approach offered support for the mediation effect of fatalism happen next, diminishing feelings of fatalistic helplessness. In
(95% CI [−.0496 to −.0155]), supporting H3. Given the insig­ China, fatalism has long been prevalent, and it has a negative
nificant direct effect of social media on stress, the complete connotation, Ming, meaning that one’s health status is prede­
mediation was supported. termined by fate. Thus, many Chinese people feel incapable,
H4a hypothesized that social media exhaustion would hopeless and pessimistic in health crises (Cheng et al., 2013).
weaken the negative relationship between social media use Social media for health purposes can promote individuals’
and fatalism. As demonstrated in Table 3, the interaction informational sense-making, which helps de-mystify the public
term between social media use and social media exhaustion health issue and increase their locus-of-control. The uncer­
was negative and significant (β = −.09, p < .05, 95% CI [−.1538 tainty management theory supports this positive relationship,
to −.0018]). This results showed that social media exhaustion positing that individuals perceive greater control over the
strengthened the negative effect of social media use on fatalism, future when uncertainty about situations is managed through
which illustrated a different direction of moderation effect as information seeking (Brashers, 2001). Further, during pan­
predicted. Thus, H4a was not supported. When individuals had demics, social media overcomes social isolation limitations by
a higher level of social media exhaustion, the role of social enabling users to connect with one another online. As younger
media in reducing fatalistic feelings was stronger. generations heavily use social media and depend on peer

Table 2. Predictors of endogenous variables in SEM. Table 3. Regression results for moderated mediation effect.
Social Predictor β SE t p
media Block 1: Main Predictors Fatalism
Effect Age Gender Education Income use Fatalism Social media use (SM) −.15 .06 −3.28 .001
Social Direct −.05 .13** .10 .04 Social media exhaustion (SME) .26 .04 5.46 .000
media Block 2: Interaction Terms
use Social media use (SM) −.15 .06 −3.15 .002
Indirect NA NA NA NA Social media exhaustion (SME) .26 .04 5.56 .000
Fatalism Direct .04 −.02 .08 −.06 −.18*** SM*SME −.09 .04 −2.01 .045
Indirect .01 −.02 −.02 −.01 NA Conditional indirect effect
Stress Direct .01 −.06 .01 −.12* P .14*** Stress
Indirect .01 −.01 .01 −.01 −.03 *** NA M-1 SD −.038 .02 −2.36 .018
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. M −.044 .02 −2.22 .027
Coefficients are standardized. M + 1 SD −.051 .02 −2.09 .037
NA = not applicable; P = pruned path Note: Numbers in bold indicate significant effect.
6 A. NGIEN AND S. JIANG

interactions for support, health information obtained from disease, and ignore negative ones to maintain better emotional
young adults’ online social networks strengthens the belief well-being. This explanation is supported by the mood man­
that they are in charge of their health destinies. This result is agement theory which suggests that exhausted individuals use
especially pertinent to China, where its collectivist culture media to block dysphoric moods or substitute negative emo­
facilitates more interdependence within one’s social network, tions with positive ones (Zillmann, 1988). In addition, Sweller’s
and Chinese people have greater interpersonal trust of health (1988) cognitive load theory also states that people have
information or resource from in-group members, such as close a limited working memory load, and to overcome this limita­
friends (Van Hoorn, 2014). tion, during exhaustion, they would resort to selective infor­
Our paper also demonstrated a positive relationship mation exposure. Particularly, young adults have higher health
between fatalism and stress. This finding is in line with prior literacy and digital skills than their older counterparts. When
research that supported the association between fatalism and facing exhaustion from social media use, they can more easily
poor mental health (Wagner et al., 2020). As fatalism is linked locate useful information source and better distinguish
to the belief in the efficacy of divine or spiritual instead of between accurate and fake health news (Loos & Nijenhuis,
personal forces in shaping life outcomes (Straughan & Seow, 2020). Given their focus on selective reliable source rather
1998), it prompts perceived uncontrollability of the future than a wider range of alternative sources during social media
which increases stress. Roseman (1996) suggested that events fatigue, young adults might be more capable of dealing with
appraised as having low control potential (e.g., COVID-19) fatalistic beliefs and stress.
results in accommodative (i.e., resignation to aversive condi­ Several limitations should be noted. First, this study used
tions) feelings, such as depression or stress, instead of reactive convenience samples recruited from online surveys, which
(i.e., resistance to aversive conditions) ones like motivation to limit the generalizability of our findings. Future studies may
fight against stress. This is particularly true for young adults ideally use probability samples that cover a broader diversity of
who are less emotionally capable than older people to handle participants. Second, the present study only measured whether
COVID-19 related threats, due to the lack of maturity and or not participants used social media and social networking
social resources (Ellwood, 2020). sites to seek health information, while it failed to identify more
An important finding from this study is the insignificant nuanced information behaviors, such as the type and amount
direct effect of social media on mental health. This result is of information searched for. The operationalization in our
consistent with the core tenet of the three-stage model and the study may result in biased results, and a general statement
growing body of research supporting complete mediation that using social media for health information is beneficial
effects (Oh et al., 2020). The process of interactive media use would be uninformative. It is important to understand which
for health promotion is complicated and dynamic. Prior types of information and the level of social media usage are
research has suggested that health-related Internet use influ­ associated with better health-related outcomes. Therefore,
ences health indirectly through its effects on factors directly future research should include more diverse dimensions of
contributing to health improvement (Jiang & Street, 2016). In health information acquisition and explore if our model can
our study, social media affects stress through fatalism. When be well applied. Third, we only examined one mediation path­
users receive health information from social media, they way from social media to stress, via fatalism, while other user-
become empowered to believe their actions can produce media-message interaction process and intermediate outcomes
desired health outcomes, which reduces stress. Thus, a direct could be at play as well. For instance, during periods of dis­
statement that social media can be linked to good or poor connectedness (i.e., sudden shortfall in social support or
emotional well-being is uninformative. We need to identify resources), social media can increase problem-solving skills,
the social mechanisms underlying this relationship. which helps to lower stress (Hoek et al., 2012). Fourth, this
Another important finding pertains to the moderating role study only collected data in China, and thus its generalizability
of social media exhaustion. Although the moderation effect is in other countries could be limited. Given that COVID-19 is
significant, its direction diverges from our expectations. The a global health crisis, more comparative research is needed,
results showed that social media use had a stronger negative particularly in societies with different health challenges and
effect on fatalism as well as greater negative indirect effect on health care systems.
stress for people with higher levels of social media exhaustion. Despite the limitations, our study has important theoretical
This result is different from prior research which suggests that implications. First, we extended the three-stage model by con­
when people feel exhausted from receiving a plethora of infor­ sidering moderation effects. Our model offers a more compre­
mation on social media, they would be less motivated to lever­ hensive framework by highlighting the need to situate social
age on tips or encouragement for desired outcomes, inhibiting factors (e.g., social media exhaustion) as moderating the effect
the potential of social media to decrease fatalism (Bang & Reio, of media use on personal health-related factors (e.g., fatalism).
2017). To explain why social media exhaustion strengthens the Second, to the best of our knowledge, our moderated media­
benefits of social media in coping with fatalism and stress, tion pathway is the first to demonstrate the effect of young
selective exposure might be important to note. During the adults’ social media use on mental health during global health
COVID-19 pandemic, people often expose to a large amount emergencies. Future research can use this analytical model to
of negative content on social media (e.g., the increasing num­ study social media for youths’ health promotion during pan­
ber of death, miserable story). When they feel exhausted from demics. Third, we expanded the cultural applicability of the
social media interactions, they might choose to turn to more three-stage model. While the model was initially designed for
positive information, such as role models of handling this the Western general public, we demonstrated that it is
HEALTH COMMUNICATION 7

applicable to young adults despite media environment and BMJ, 328(7449), 1166–1170. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.328.7449.
socio-economic variances. For instance, this model is relevant 1166
to the intensified fatalistic culture in China. Farpour, H. R., Habibi, L., & Owji, S. H. (2017). Positive impact of social
media use on depression in cancer patients. Asian Pacific Journal of
This study also offers significant practical implications. Cancer Prevention, 18(11), 2985–2988. https://doi.org/10.22034/
First, our study demonstrated that social media for health APJCP.2017.18.11.2985
information can reduce stress. Thus, during public health Fergie, G., Hunt, K., & Hilton, S. (2016). Social media as a space for
emergencies, young adults should more often utilize social support: Young adults’ perspectives on producing and consuming user-
media for health purposes as an effective way to cope with generated content about diabetes and mental health. Social Science &
Medicine, 170, 46–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.10.006
emotional burdens. Second, the moderating role of social
Gao, J., Zheng, P., Jia, Y., Chen, H., Mao, Y., Chen, S., Wang, Y., Fu, H., &
media exhaustion documented in this study informs us that Dai, J. (2020). Mental health problems and social media exposure
facing the plethora of health information and the increasing during COVID-19 outbreak. PLoS ONE, 15(4), e0231924. https://doi.
information sharing during COVID-19, health communicators org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231924
should teach young people to focus on some preferred and Go, E., & You, K. H. (2018). Health-related online information seeking
reliable social media sources and make more meaningful con­ and behavioral outcomes: Fatalism and self-efficacy as mediators. Social
Behavior and Personality, 46(5), 871–879. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.
nections with online peers for productive discussion. Third, 6501
our study also found that fatalism reduced emotional well- Guo, S., Yu, X., Davis, E., Armstrong, R., Riggs, E., & Naccarella, L. (2020).
being. As such, health communicators need to make efforts to Adolescent health literacy in Beijing and Melbourne: A cross-cultural
lower people’s fatalistic beliefs. Some strategies might include comparison. International Journal of Environmental Research and
introducing a role model to enhance young adults’ motivation Public Health, 17(4), 1242. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17041242
Hoek, W., Schuurmans, J., Koot, H. M., & Cuijpers, P. (2012). Effects of
to cope with health problems, and teaching effective self- internet-based guided self-help problem-solving therapy for adoles­
management skills. It is important to note that more culturally cents with depression and anxiety: A randomized controlled trial.
appropriate training problems are needed, as fatalism is influ­ PLoS ONE, 7(8), e43485. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
enced by the unique culture rooted in China. 0043485
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives.
Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55.
Funding https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
Husky, M. M., Kovess-Masfety, V., & Swendsen, J. D. (2020). Stress and
The authors have no funding to report. anxiety among university students in France during COVID-19 man­
datory confinement. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 102, 152191. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152191
References Jamieson, P. E., & Romer, D. (2008). Unrealistic fatalism in U.S. youth
Asibey, B. O., Agyemang, S., & Dankwah, A. B. (2017). The internet use ages 14 to 22: Prevalence and characteristics. Journal of Adolescent
for health information seeking among Ghanaian University students: A Health, 42(2), 154–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.07.
cross-sectional study. International Journal of Telemedicine and 010
Applications, 2017, 1756473. https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/1756473 Jiang, S., & Street, R. L. (2016). Pathway linking internet health informa­
Bang, H., & Reio Jr, T. G. (2017). Examining the role of cynicism in the tion seeking to better health: A moderated mediation study. Health
relationships between burnout and employee behavior. Journal of Work Communication, 32(8), 1024–1031. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.
and Organizational Psychology, 33(3), 217–228. https://doi.org/10. 2016.1196514
1016/j.rpto.2017.07.002 Jimenez, T., Restar, A., Helm, P. J., Cross, R. I., Barath, D., & Arndt, J.
Befort, C. A., Nazir, N., Engelman, K., & Choi, W. (2013). Fatalistic cancer (2020). Fatalism in the context of COVID-19: Perceiving coronavirus
beliefs and information sources among rural and urban adults in the as a death sentence predicts reluctance to perform recommended pre­
USA. Journal of Cancer Education, 28(3), 521–526. https://doi.org/10. ventive behaviors. SSM - Population Health, 11, 100615. https://doi.org/
1007/s13187-013-0496-7 10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100615
Brashers, E. (2001). Communication and uncertainty management. Kaparounaki, C. K., Patsali, M. E., Mousa, D. V., Papadopoulou, E. V.,
Journal of Communication, 51(3), 477–497. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. Papadopoulou, K. K., & Fountoulakis, K. N. (2020). University stu­
1460-2466.2001.tb02892.x dents’ mental health amidst the COVID-19 quarantine in Greece.
Chae, J., & Lee, C. J. (2019). The psychological mechanism underlying Psychiatry Research, 290, 113111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.
communication effects on behavioral intention: Focusing on affect and 2020.113111
cognition in the cancer context. Communication Research, 46(5), Lee, C., Niederdeppe, J., & Freres, D. (2012). Socioeconomic disparities in
597–618. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650216644021 fatalistic beliefs about cancer prevention and the internet. Journal of
Cheng, H., Sit, J. H., Twinn, S. F., Cheng, K. F., & Thorne, S. (2013). Communication, 62(6), 972–990. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.
Coping with breast cancer survivorship in Chinese women: The role of 2012.01683.x
fatalism or fatalistic voluntarism. Cancer Nursing, 36(3), 236–244. Li, T. M., & Wong, P. W. (2015). Youth social withdrawal behavior
https://doi.org/10.1097/NCC.0b013e31826542b2 (hikikomori): A systematic review of qualitative and quantitative
Ellwood, B. (2020, May 26). Social support and sense of belonging may studies. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 49(7),
protect against the psychological impact of COVID-19. PsyPost. https:// 595–609. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867415581179
www.psypost.org/2020/05/social-support-and-sense-of-belonging- Li, Y., Chen, P. Y., Chen, F., & Wu, W. (2015). Roles of fatalism and
may-protect-against-the-psychological-impact-of-covid-19-56873 parental support in the relationship between bullying victimization and
Eveland, W. P., & Morey, A. C. (2011). Challenges and opportunities of bystander behaviors. School Psychology International, 36(3), 253–267.
panel designs. In E. P. Bucy & R. L. Holbert (Eds.), Sourcebook for https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034315569566
political communication research: Methods, measures, and analytical Liu, P. L., & Jiang, S. (2021). Patient-centered communication mediates
techniques (pp. 19–33). Routledge. the relationship between health information acquisition and patient
Eysenbach, G., Powell, J., Englesakis, M., Rizo, C., & Stern, A. (2004). trust in physicians: A five-year comparison in China. Health
Health related virtual communities and electronic support groups: Communication, 36(2), 207–216. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.
Systematic review of the effects of online peer to peer interactions. 2019.1673948
8 A. NGIEN AND S. JIANG

Loos, E., & Nijenhuis, J. (2020). Consuming fake news: A matter of age? Roseman, I. J. (1996). Appraisal determinants of emotions: Constructing
The perception of political fake news stories in Facebook ads. In Q. Gao a more accurate and comprehensive theory. Cognition & Emotion, 10
& J. Zhou (Eds.), International Conference on Human-Computer (3), 241–278. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999396380240
Interaction (pp. 69–88). Cham: Springer. Shen, L., Condit, C. M., & Wright, L. (2009). The psychometric property
Maier, C., Laumer, S., Eckhardt, A., & Weitzel, T. (2015). Giving too much and validation of a fatalism scale. Psychology & Health, 24(5), 597–613.
social support: Social overload on social networking sites. European https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440801902535
Journal of Information Systems, 24(5), 447–464. https://doi.org/10. Slater, M. D. (2007). Reinforcing spirals: The mutual influence of media
1057/ejis.2014.3 selectivity and media effects and their impact on individual behavior
McCoach, D. B. (2003). SEM isn’t just the Schoolwide enrichment model and social identity. Communication Theory, 17(3), 281–303. https://doi.
anymore: Structural equation modeling (SEM) in gifted education. org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2007.00296.x
Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 27(1), 36–61. https://doi.org/ Straughan, P. T., & Seow, A. (1998). Fatalism reconceptualized:
10.1177/016235320302700104 A concept to predict health screening behavior. Journal of
Nielsen. (2009). How teens use media. https://www.nielsen.com/wp- Gender, Culture, & Health, 3(2), 85–100. https://doi.org/10.1023/
content/uploads/sites/3/2019/04/How-Teens-Use-Media.pdf A:1023278230797
Nieto, M., Romero, D., Ros, L., Zabala, C., Martínez, M., Ricarte, J. J., Street, R. L. (2003). Mediated consumer–provider communication in
Serrano, J. P., & Latorre, J. M. (2019). Differences in coping strategies cancer care: The empowering potential of new technologies. Patient
between young and older adults: The role of executive functions. The Education and Counseling, 50(1), 99–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/
International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 90(1), 28–49. S0738-3991(03)00089-2
https://doi.org/10.1177/0091415018822040 Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on
Oh, S., Lee, S. Y., & Han, C. (2020). The effects of social media use on learning. Cognitive Science, 12(2), 257–285. https://doi.org/10.1207/
preventive behaviors during infectious disease outbreaks: The mediat­ s15516709cog1202_4
ing role of self-relevant emotions and public risk perception. Health Van Hoorn, A. (2014). Individualist–collectivist culture and trust radius.
Communication. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(2), 269–276. https://doi.org/
10410236.2020.1724639 10.1177/0022022114551053
Pearlin, L. E., Menaghan, E. G., Lieberman, M. A., & Mullan, J. T. (1981). Wagner, G., Glick, P., Khamma-sh, U., Shaheen, M., Brown, R.,
The stress process. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22(4), Goutam, P., Karam, R., Linnemayr, S., & Massad, S. (2020). Exposure
337–356. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136676 to violence and its relationship to mental health in Palestinian youth.
Peng, W., Carcioppolo, N., Occa, A., Ali, K., Yang, Q., & Yang, F. (2021). Eastern Mediterranean Health Journal, 26(2), 189–197. https://doi.org/
Feel worried, overloaded, or fatalistic? The determinants of cancer 10.26719/2020.26.2.189
uncertainty management preferences. Health Communication, 36(3), Ware, J. E., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-ltem short-form
347–360. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2019.1692489 health survey (SF-36). Medical Care, 30(6), 473–483. https://doi.org/10.
Powe, B. D., & Finnie, R. (2003). Cancer fatalism: The state of the science. 1097/00005650-199206000-00002
Cancer Nursing, 26(6), 454–465. https://doi.org/10.1097/00002820- Wen, Z., Geng, X., & Ye, Y. (2016). Does the use of WeChat lead to
200312000-00005 subjective well-being?: The effect of use intensity and motivations.
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moder­ Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 19(10), 587–592.
ated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0154
Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42(1), 185–227. https://doi.org/10. World Health Organisation. (2016). The role of governments in promoting
1080/00273170701341316 health literacy. World Health Organization. https://www.who.int/
Pretorius, C., Chambers, D., & Coyle, D. (2019). Young people’s online healthpromotion/conferences/9gchp/health-literacy-government-role
help-seeking and mental health difficulties: Systematic narrative review. /en/
Journal of Medical Internet Research, 21(11), e13873. https://doi.org/10. Yoo, W., Yang, J., & Cho, E. (2016). How social media influence college
2196/13873 students’ smoking attitudes and intentions. Computers in Human
Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Behavior, 64, 173–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.061
Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing Zhu, M., Cai, W., Li, L., Guo, Y., Monroe-Wise, A., Li, Y., Zeng, C.,
out. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1841–1848. https://doi.org/ Qiao, J., Xu, Z., Zhang, H., Zeng, Y., & Liu, C. (2019). Mediators of
10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014 intervention effects on depressive symptoms among people living with
Ramanadhan, S., Mendez, S. R., Rao, M., & Viswanath, K. (2013). Social HIV: Secondary analysis of a mobile health randomized controlled trial
media use by community-based organizations conducting health pro­ using latent growth curve modeling. JMIR mHealth and uHealth, 7(11),
motion: A content analysis. BMC Public Health, 13(1), 1129. https:// e15489. https://doi.org/10.2196/15489
doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-1129 Zillmann, D. (1988). Mood management: Using entertainment to full
Roberts, R. E., Roberts, C. R., & Chen, I. G. (2000). Fatalism and risk of advantage. In L. Donohew, H. E. Sypher, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.),
adolescent depression. Psychiatry, 63(3), 239–252. https://doi.org/10. Communication Social Cognition, and Affect (pp. 147–171). Lawrence
1080/00332747.2000.11024917 Erlbaum Associates.

You might also like