You are on page 1of 23

Language Learning Great and Small:

Environmental Support Structures


and Learning Opportunities in a
Sociocognitive Approach to Second
Language Acquisition/Teaching
DWIGHT ATKINSON,1 ETON CHURCHILL,2 TAKAKO NISHINO,3 and HANAKO OKADA4
1
University of Arizona, Department of English, 1423 E. University Blvd, Rm 445, PO Box 210067, Tucson, AZ
85721–0067 Email: dwightatki@email.arizona.edu
2
Kanagawa University, Department of Cross-Cultural Studies, 3–27–1 Rokkakubashi, Kanagawa-ku, Yokohama,
Kanagawa, 221–8686, Japan Email: itonc001@kanagawa-u.ac.jp
3
Kanda University of International Studies, 1–4–1 Wakaba, Mihama-ku, Chiba-shi, Chiba, 261–0014, Japan
Email: nishino-t@kanda.kuis.jp
4
Sophia University, Faculty of Liberal Arts, 7–1 Kioi-cho, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo, 102–8554, Japan
Email: h-okada-c5s@sophia.ac.jp

Sociocognitive theory views learning, including second language acquisition, as the progressive alignment
of individuals vis-à-vis their ecosocial environments. In this article we irst update sociocognitive theory
in light of recent evolutionary/ecological research on learning/teaching: (a) Humans are evolutionarily
adapted to adapt to myriad environments, placing a premium on adaptive learning, (b) human adapta-
tion is effected substantially through niche construction—engineering environments to make them more
adaptive, and then transmitting the results culturally, placing a premium on adaptive teaching, (c) both
human learning and teaching are innate/instinctive, and co-evolved, and (d) there are many kinds of
‘teachers’ in the world. Second, we briely review 3 approaches to second language acquisition/teaching
(SLA/T) vis-à-vis sociocognitive theory: van Lier’s ecological-semiotic approach, Schumann’s interac-
tional instinct, and conversation analysis. Third, we apply our theoretical perspective exploratorily to
videotaped data of a Japanese learner/user of English as a lingua franca who is baking pastries with a
Finnish friend. Our analysis includes 5 widely studied ‘units of participation’: activity types, routines,
co-constructed tellings, repetition, and assessments. Analysis suggests that these constitute powerful en-
vironmental support structures yielding rich learning opportunities for SLA/T in moment-to-moment
interaction. Fourth and inally, we discuss our results vis-à-vis our theoretical approach. We conclude
by suggesting how our expanded view of teaching/learning might broaden SLA/T’s ‘pedagogical
imagination.’
Keywords: acquisition/learning/development; sociocognitive approaches; SLA; interaction; embodiment

A SOCIOCOGNITIVE APPROACH TO SECOND in human activity, including learning, so they


language acquisition/teaching (SLA/T) holds must be studied integratively. Bateson (1972) ex-
that the social, the cognitive, the embodied, pressed the core idea: “If you want to ( …) un-
and the material are fundamentally integrated derstand anything in human behavior, you are
always dealing with total circuits, completed cir-
cuits ( …). ‘Mind’ [is] immanent in the large bi-
The Modern Language Journal, 102, 3, (2018) ological system—the ecosystem” (pp. 465–466).
DOI: 10.1111/modl.12496 The rationale for this view is compelling: The hu-
0026-7902/18/471–493 $1.50/0 man nervous system, prominently including its
C National Federation of Modern Language Teachers
!
large, complex brain, links its body lexibly to
Associations
472 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
its dynamic environment, enabling that body to (Lave & Wenger, 1991), Wittgensteinian and
continuously adapt, or align, in order to survive. phenomenological philosophy (Shotter, 1996),
It is through such alignment that we also learn to behavioral/emotional synchrony and accommo-
survive better—that is, learn simpliciter.1 dation (Bargh, 2017), integrational linguistics
This view has been developed vis-à-vis SLA/T (Harris, 1998), and the ecological and evolu-
in a series of publications (e.g., Atkinson, 2002, tionary nature of teaching and learning (see
2010, 2011b, 2012, 2014, 2017; Atkinson et al., Atkinson, 2017, and the next section for review).
2007; Churchill et al., 2010). It can be con- Although “broadly compatible [with other] al-
trasted for introductory purposes with SLA/T ternative [SLA] ontologies” (Douglas Fir Group,
approaches emphasizing the ‘outside-in’ na- 2016, p. 21), it does not reduce to them. This
ture of learning. Thus, cognitive interactionism is indicated next, where we focus on current
(e.g., Long, 2015) views learning as the environ- developments in sociocognitive theory, and in
mental extraction and processing of linguistic subsequent sections, where we turn speciically
input/intake by a cognitive computer, leading to SLA/T. The latter include an empirical sup-
to development of a mental second language port section, which represents the bulk of the
(L2) competence, while neo-Vygotskian socio- article.
cultural theory features internalization—“the
processes through which interpersonal and THEORETICAL DEVELOPMENT: LEARNING,
person–environment interaction forms and TEACHING, EVOLUTION
transforms one’s internal mental functions”
(Lantolf & Thorne, 2006, p. 19). The outside-in A distinctive trait of our species is that we are
nature of these approaches equates learning evolutionarily adapted to adapt. That is, rather than
with ownership: “Internalization ( …) has to do evolving to it a speciic ecological niche, we have
with making something one’s own” (Lantolf & evolved to adapt to myriad niches. As Harari
Thorne, 2006, p. 162). (2015) put it, “Most mammals emerge from the
In contrast, a sociocognitive approach empha- womb like glazed earthenware from a kiln—any
sizes the ‘inside-out’ nature of learning—or, more attempt at remoulding will only scratch or break
accurately, the progressive alignment (i.e., ‘lining them. Humans emerge ( …) like molten glass
up,’ integration, attunement) of the learner with ( …). They can be spun, stretched, and shaped
the ecosocial world. The adaptive nature of learn- with ( …) surprising ( …) freedom” (p. 10).
ing is thus foregrounded. Turning the SLA ap- Organisms not pre-it to speciic environments
proaches just mentioned on their head, instead of must learn actively/adaptively from day one sim-
extracting input from the environment, we might say ply in order to survive. Learning is thus “as much
that learners are themselves input into the environ- a part of ( …) human nature as eating and sleep-
ment (Gee, 1995). Or, instead of acquiring a lan- ing. It is life-sustaining and inevitable” (Wenger,
guage, learners are themselves acquired by a language 1998, p. 1). Much more than other species, we are
(Sankoff & Laberge, 1974). The resulting pic- natural-born, life-long learners.
ture is well-captured in Ingold’s (2000) descrip- We are also natural-born, life-long teachers, and
tion of apprenticeship among indigenous Arctic adaptive teaching and learning co-evolved. This
hunters: distinctive claim is based on three theoretical per-
spectives in the human sciences:
The novice hunter learns by accompanying more ex- 1. Cultural niche construction: Various species
perienced hands in the woods. As he goes about, he (e.g., spiders) adapt by actively reconstruct-
is instructed in what to look out for, and his atten- ing their environments to make them more
tion is drawn to subtle clues that he might otherwise
adaptive. Such niche construction (Odling–Smee,
fail to notice: in other words, he is led to develop a so-
Laland, & Feldman, 1996) is innate. As an
phisticated perceptual awareness of the properties of
his surroundings and of the possibilities they afford evolutionary response to not being innately
for action ( …) What is involved ( …) is not a trans- preadapted to a speciic environment, how-
mission of representations ( …) but an education of ever, humans have taken niche construction
attention. (p. 37) (a) to a different level—we reconstruct our
environments far more pervasively than other
A sociocognitive approach to SLA/T is sup- species; and (b) in a different direction—we
ported by interdisciplinary research in situated/ transmit our speciic adaptations culturally as
distributed/extended cognition (see Atkinson, well as genetically.2 Cultural niche construction
2010, 2012, for reviews), multimodal/embodied (Laland & O’Brien, 2011) is thus the ability
interaction (Streeck, 2015), situated learning to transmit complex adaptive skills/technologies
Dwight Atkinson et al. 473
nongenetically, such that they are both preserved serious “teaching and learning are all around us”
and improved within/across generations. This is (Holmes et al., 2017, p. 268) and come in many
done substantially through teaching. shapes and sizes. Second, teaching/learning are
2. Natural pedagogy: Psychologists frequently lexible and adaptive vis-à-vis the particular en-
note how quickly and effortlessly children learn. vironments wherein they occur—there is no sin-
Csibra and Gergely (2011) traced these breathtak- gle, superior type. Third, things teach as well as
ing abilities to proto-human innovation of stone people, and these things can be human-designed,
tool technologies, dating back 1.5–3 million years. like road grids and computers, or designed by
As these technologies developed, they became too nature, like hurricanes and game-trails (Gee,
complex to learn through social (observation and n.d.). Fourth and inally, conventional notions
imitation) and/or asocial (trial-and-error) learn- of teaching should be broadened radically. In
ing alone. Sociocognitive repertoires thus evolved Gee’s words, “Teaching now becomes anything
in our ancestors whereby otherwise unlearnable that causes a human experience to be designed
survival skills could be transmitted culturally. or structured in a way that organizes and facili-
Teaching, like learning, is therefore coded in our tates learning” (n.d., p. 13). Or in our own words,
genes—in fact, they co-evolved. This is evidenced “Teaching ( …) is shaping situational contexts in
in Csibra and Gergely’s research on pre-linguistic ways that promote durable environmental adapta-
infants learning via face recognition, gaze di- tion” (Atkinson, 2017, p. 530), where the shaping
rection, referential pointing, and child-directed agent is left unspeciied.
speech, all of which caregivers utilize in teaching.
In sum, human teaching is evolved/innate and
co-evolved with learning. Recent research (e.g., RELATED THEORETICAL APPROACHES TO
Scalese Sugiyama, 2017) shows how natural ped- SLA/T
agogy continues across the lifespan. Likely exam-
SLA studies historically viewed language learn-
ples in SLA include ‘foreigner talk’ and conversa-
ing as psycholinguistic input processing. As SLA’s
tional repair.
sheer complexity became apparent, however,
3. Distributed teaching and learning systems: Study-
a “Theory Proliferation Era” (Atkinson, 2011a,
ing learning via video gaming, Holmes, Tran, and
p. xi) ensued. The guiding principle of this
Gee (2017) proposed:
era is that any theoretical approach assisting
the concept of “distributed teaching and learning
fuller/better understanding of SLA has value. It
system” [DTAL] as a means of directing attention is therefore imperative that different approaches
more closely to the elements of [a] larger ecosys- (a) develop themselves intensively, and (b) de-
tem that are organized around ( …) teaching and lineate their relationships with other approaches.
learning ( …). We wish to focus particularly on un- Our main aim in this article is the former, but a
derstanding teaching as ( …) manifested and dis- brief attempt at the latter motivates this section.3
tributed across a wide range of spaces, resources,
practices, and people. Furthermore, we are inter-
ested in understanding the relationships among Ecological–Semiotic Approach
( …) these. (p. 254)
van Lier (1996) critiqued mainstream SLA stud-
In this view, some DTALs are “intentionally ies for treating language learning as cognitive–
designed to support teaching and learning” computational input processing. As a partial re-
(p. 255), but others are not: Video games, sewing, placement, he proposed engagement: Learners are
car repair, and cooking exemplify the latter. In actively alive/attentive/responsive to the quality
fact, the complex ecological nature of DTALs sug- of their linguistic experience, not just its quan-
gests multiple teaching/learning types and re- tity. Thus, Nakahama, Tyler, and van Lier (2001)
sources, often operating simultaneously. Thus, found that “mutual[ly] engag[ing] and interac-
a multi-player videogame DTAL might comprise tional[ly] symmetr[ical]” (p. 383) classroom con-
other players, friends, user groups, blogs, on- versation provides richer learning opportunities
line instructional materials, and the design of than input-provision/processing tasks. van Lier
the game itself, each providing different forms of (2004) proposed a second alternative to input
teaching/learning at different levels of conscious- processing: Affordances are “what the environment
ness/intentionality. offers the animal, what it provides or furnishes”
The DTAL concept has major implications for (p. 91, quoting Gibson, 1979) for survival. Affor-
teaching/learning. First, it suggests that, beyond dances are relational and dynamic: They are the
what formal education conventionally provides, adaptive support structures environments grant
474 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
to species evolutionarily designed/positioned to analysis”—treating evolutionary adaptation at
utilize them. the species level—while natural pedagogy is a
The idea of affordances overlaps signiicantly “phenotypic analysis”—treating adaptation at the
with our own concept of alignment. van Lier’s behavioral level. The sociocognitive approach
approach may differ from our own, however, seeks to do both by investigating how species-
in that (a) it incorporates sociocultural theory level evolutionary adaptations support real-time
(SCT)—an ‘outside-in’ SLA/T approach, which adaptive behavior eventuating in learning.
further views learning/teaching as primarily con-
scious and intentional, and (b), although van Lier Conversation Analysis
(2004) saw affordances as always active in learn-
ing environments, he distinguished them from Conversation analysis (CA) also puts interac-
more “mediated,” “conscious,” “aware,” and “re- tion at the heart of SLA, and C. Goodwin’s dis-
lexive” (pp. 99–102) processes. We agree that tinctive version of multimodal CA has inspired
learning takes place at all levels of intentional- our work (e.g., Churchill et al., 2010). But the
ity and awareness—such lexibility is central to sociocognitive framework is an exogenous theory of
humans’ amazing adaptive skills—but we focus SLA, thus immediately running afoul of ‘purist’
more on the rich contextual “mindbodyworld” CA approaches (Markee & Kunitz, 2015). Nor do
(Atkinson, 2014) matrix, in which and by which we adhere to CA principles/requirements such as
all kinds of learning occur. After all, the more con- ‘next-turn proof procedure,’ ‘membership knowl-
scious, deliberate aspects of SLA have been well edge,’ ‘emicity,’ or the ‘doing/being’ performa-
explored in the ield, including by van Lier him- tive interpretation of social action. Additionally,
self. However, with Gadamer (1989), we believe CA practitioners hold widely varying views on
that “every linguistic experience of the world is cognition, including anti-cognitive orientations
[fundamentally] experience of the world, not ex- (Potter & Edwards, 2013), and may still prioritize
perience of language” (p. 546). the verbal channel/talk (Levinson, 2013).
What we share with CA is a focus on inely
Interactional Instinct tuned moment-to-moment interaction. Turn-
coordination, rhythmic synchrony, projectability,
Schumann (2013) proposed that all language and the tight interlocking of linguistic, facial,
acquisition results from interaction, but certain gestural, embodied, and material modalities
enabling conditions are guaranteed only for point to a partly ethological, highly evolved,
irst language acquisition (L1A): (a) neurophys- and ancient suite of interactional abilities, to
iological factors promoting afiliation/bonding, which language itself is likely a late addition
(b) neurophysiological factors promoting linguis- (Levinson, 2006). Interaction, in other words,
tic pattern identiication and memory, and (c) is as much about “metamessages” (Bateson, 1972)
abundant language exposure/practice. Highly as messages, and they function integratively. In
successful adult SLA, in this view, involves this view, interactional competence (Hall, 2017)
either atypical carryover of neurophysiologi- is both a universal precondition for SLA (i.e., the
cal factors or exceptional instrumental motiva- universal interaction instinct is a primary basis of
tion, in either case combined with extensive learning) and its outcome (i.e., the ability to use
practice. the L2 better). Likewise, although sequentiality
We share Schumann’s belief in the centrality (Schegloff, 2007) is vital in social action, we
of interaction in language acquisition, and neuro- also emphasize simultaneity/synchrony, antic-
physiology doubtless varies with age. Unlike Schu- ipation, discontinuous action, and the virtual
mann, however, we view human interaction in- structuring of activity types and participation
stincts as fundamentally robust and continuous structure/routines, as described in the next
across the lifespan: Pro-sociality and learning are section.
major evolved human survival strategies, and hu- Conversation analyst Stivers (2008) deined
mans can survive for many years. Indeed, if age alignment as the signaling of comprehension in
effects were so determinative, few high-level post- interaction, while afiliation is the signaling of
puberty L2 learner/users would exist. evaluative stance. In an interesting study of cross-
Roehrig (2013) investigated the relation- time L2 development, Dings (2014) seemed to
ship between the Interactional Instinct (II) follow Stivers by deining alignment in terms of
perspective and natural pedagogy, the latter in- ‘understanding’ and ‘information,’ but also in-
corporated earlier into our own framework. She cluded assessments, which Stivers categorized as
concluded that II constitutes a “genotypic afiliation. For us, afiliation inheres in all human
Dwight Atkinson et al. 475
interaction and is thus a fundamental aspect of help us look at SLA/T differently, as suggested
alignment (see notes 1 and 7). later.

EMPIRICAL SUPPORT
Research Approach
In this section, we explore empirically what
Our research approach is discourse-analytic,
we have developed theoretically. In brief, we
context-sensitive, microanalytic, and multimodal
seek to understand how the ecosocial environ-
(Erickson, 1992; Goodwin, 2000; Kendon, 1990;
ment actively supports SLA—how it ‘teaches’
Streeck, 2015). It studies environmental struc-
by enabling participation. A sociocognitive ap-
turing and adaptive action across the multi-
proach views SLA as adaptive integration of
ple modalities wherein learning/teaching oc-
person and environment via alignment, so it
curs in everyday interaction.4 Many of these
is crucial to know both what the environment
modal actions/structures (e.g., facial expressions,
affords and how it does so. Yet to speak of ‘per-
gestures) are ‘actual’: They appear moment-to-
son and environment’ suggests a discontinuity,
moment in interaction. Others, however (e.g.,
whereas we seek to highlight integrative relation-
routines), are ‘virtual’: They can be studied only
ality and participation: organism-plus-environment
by extracting larger patterns from moment-to-
(Bateson, 1972), or mindbodyworld (Atkinson,
moment interaction.
2014).
This relationality is highly complex, and Participants. The focal participant in this study
good language/semiosis to describe it is lacking was Rie, an undergraduate education major at
(Canagarajah, 2018; Streeck, 2015). But some a Japanese university.5 Rie spent her senior year
of its spirit may be found in phenomenolog- abroad in Finland, the irst 5 months studying
ical concepts like ‘compresence,’ ‘intercor- furniture design at a rural technical college, the
poreality,’ and ‘dwelling.’ In Streeck’s (2015) last 5 interning at an urban primary school and
interpretation, “The human being ( …) is ‘al- retirement community. Rie reported learning lit-
ways already’ immersed in the world through tle Finnish in Finland; instead, she was immersed
‘mindless’ (representation-less) absorbed cop- in a world of English—a tight-knit English-as-a-
ing or ‘dwelling.’ We ‘dwell in’ or ‘inhabit’ lingua-franca (ELF) community of international
the places where we, the tools ( …) we use, and Finnish students.
and the speaking routines ( …) we use all are” Although Rie had studied English as a foreign
(p. 423). These people, places, tools, and speak- language (EFL) for 8 years in Japan, her previ-
ing routines also afford learning and teaching ous experience in English-speaking environments
in the course of adaptive (and in this sense only was limited to 3 weeks at a U.S. English camp,
‘mindless’—i.e., part of our natural moment- two short trips to the United States, and a 10-
to-moment environmental alignment) “going lesson TOEFL preparation course with a ‘native
on” (Wittgenstein, 1958, in Shotter, 1996). They speaker.’ Rie reported producing little English
likewise provide a rich contextual matrix for on these occasions, although a pre-Finland En-
more mindful acts of teaching and learning— glish interview (see later in this article and the
without them such acts could not occur. In Clark’s online supporting information) and her TOEFL
(2016) formulation, we exist in “a complex struc- teacher’s account indicated a basic level of com-
tured world apt for engagement and action” prehension.
(p. xv). Rie’s English was basic when she arrived in Fin-
Our main focus in this section is therefore on land, but it is important to note that she was al-
environmental support structures for SLA and how ready a skilled cook/baker. Her family business is
they support alignment. By environmental sup- a food business, and in their free time Rie, her
port structures we mean whatever exists in the mother, and two sisters are serious amateur chefs.
environment that can be said to ‘teach,’ where Rie therefore already had an “activity-based iden-
teaching, as stated previously, is “anything that tity” (Gee, 2017) as a cook/baker when she ar-
causes a human experience to be designed or rived in Finland.
structured in a way that organizes and facili- The other participant in this study was Jaakko, a
tates learning” (Gee, n.d., p. 13). Teaching and Finnish student of carving/wood design at the col-
learning are fundamentally connected in this lege, a member of the tight-knit ELF circle, and
view—we take from the environment what it ac- Rie’s close friend. We know little about Jaakko’s
tively affords, and build our relations with it on previous experience studying and using English,
that basis. Such a reconceptualized teaching can but his English was considerably more functional
476 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
and sophisticated than Rie’s. In the U.S. Inten- (p. 368) participants can make if an activity is
sive English Program (IEP) context, Jaakko would to fall within a particular type. Examples include
likely have been placed (if at all) in an advanced attending church/mosque/temple, playing bas-
speaking class, compared to Rie’s pre-sojourn ketball, classroom teaching, and baking. Activ-
high-beginner placement. ity types “reduce entropy (increase predictabil-
ity) at all scales in a cultural cognitive ecosystem”
Data Collection. We gave Rie a video camera
(Hutchins, 2014, p. 38), or, in the original concep-
before her trip and asked her to record social
tualization of scaffolding (Wood, Bruner, & Ross,
activities she engaged in abroad. Five videos re-
1976), greatly reduce “degrees of freedom” in a
sulted, one of which is analyzed here: a 93-minute
problem-solving task. Activity types are a sine qua
video of Rie and Jaakko baking cinnamon rolls, an
non of successful interaction because, by severely
activity they mentioned (in the video) perform-
structuring the environment, they answer the fun-
ing four times previously. The videotaping took
damental social question, “What is it that’s going
place in March 2010 in Rie’s apartment, about
on here?” (Goffman, 1974/1986, p. 46).
8 months after her arrival in Finland. We tran-
Gee (2017) described “activity-based
scribed the irst 30 minutes of the video, employ-
identities"—freely chosen identities that are
ing a modiied version of Jefferson’s transcrip-
based on social practices “done in similar (but
tion system (Ochs, Schegloff, & Thompson, 1996,
not identical) ways repeatedly and ( …) regulated
pp. 461–465),6 supplemented with moment-by-
( …) by groups and institutions” (p. 73). He
moment description of gaze, gestures, facial ex-
emphasized that activity-based identities “are not
pressions, head and body movements, object use,
IN a person. They are reciprocal relationship[s]
and other environmental phenomena. We also in-
between a person and a social group and its
terviewed Rie before and after her Finland trip to
( …) [customary] activities” (p. 74). As already
ascertain longitudinal language development, as
mentioned, our participant Rie had a pre-existing
reported briely in the following and at length in
activity-based identity as a cook/baker.
the online supporting information.
2. Participation structures and routines: Par-
Data Analysis. The analytical categories em- ticipation structures (Erickson, 2004) are
ployed here have been widely used as analytical interactional–linguistic frames or schemas guid-
units of participation (Duranti, 1996) in research ing interaction within activity types. They do so
on social interaction and learning. They were cho- largely by assigning roles and responsibilities
sen as follows: We irst engaged with the data in- for producing multi-party linguistic formulas,
tensively while transcribing it, both individually or routines (Peters & Boggs, 1986). For Peters
and collectively. We then examined the transcript and Boggs, such “socializing situations occur
line by line (with frequent reference to the video) fairly predictably as to time, place, participants,
for patterns of alignment found in our past work, and desired outcomes,” so they “greatly facilitate
combined with our knowledge of analytical cate- language learning” (p. 80). Examples include
gories used in past interaction/learning research. classroom initiation–response–feedback (IRF)
The resulting categories thus (a) igured in our sequences, restaurant ordering schemas, and
past research, (b) were prominent in our aca- service encounter buyer–seller routines. Ohta
demic experience/education, and (c) appeared (1999) showed how teacher-controlled IRF rou-
to elucidate how alignment was environmentally tines were gradually integrated and expanded
supported in our data. We should note immedi- in student–student interaction, suggesting a
ately that these categories are not mutually ex- staged process of socialization in classroom
clusive: They represent different but overlapping SLA.
perspectives on a single focal phenomenon— 3. Co-construction of tellings: Jacoby and Ochs
alignment. We therefore sometimes look at (1995) deined co-construction as “the joint
the same data through different conceptual creation of a form, interpretation, stance, ac-
lenses. tion, activity, identity, institution, skill, ideology,
The ive analytical categories are as follows: emotion, or other culturally meaningful reality”
1. Activity types: Levinson (1979) deined ac- (p. 172), noting its vital role in “accomplish[ing]
tivity type as “any culturally recognized activ- ( …) mental and social behavior throughout ( …)
ity, ( …) whether or not ( …) talk takes place life” (pp. 172–173). They also suggested that
in it ( …) [Such activities] are goal-directed, co-construction pervades L1A and SLA, with for-
socially constituted, bounded events with con- eigner talk exemplifying the latter. Here we ex-
straints on participants, setting, ( …) but above amine how Rie and Jaakko co-constructed a dra-
all on the kinds of allowable contributions” matized account based on a cartoon appearing
Dwight Atkinson et al. 477
on an egg carton and a short accompanying text. 1992, p. 154).7 A short example from our data
Such co-tellings frequently combine high involve- follows (see the Appendix for transcription
ment, playfulness, imagination, and rhythmic conventions):
alignment, thus providing rich learning opportu- Example 1:
nities for SLA via engagement (van Lier, 1996).
In more traditional terms, Rie’s grammatical and Jaakko: ((Sniffing just-opened cardamom
discourse competence were actively and richly package he is holding)) This taste
scaffolded by Jaakko in a Zone of Proximal (.5) smells good
Development, allowing her to ‘outperform her Rie: o
(It) smells goodo . ((Holds out
competence.’ That is, Rie could not have com- spoon for cardamom))
municated as competently or participated as fully
without a willing co-participant wielding a larger
repertoire of linguistic-participatory tools, within Goodwin and Goodwin (1992) noted that as-
a rich ecosocial matrix enabling high-interest mu- sessments provide a structural “place for height-
tual engagement. ened mutual orientation and action” (p. 154),
4. Repetition: In recent research on human thus promoting attention, participation, affect,
evolution, unique abilities for exact imitation and engagement, which in turn facilitate learning
contributed signally to our unique evolutionary (van Lier, 1996). The assessment sequence just
trajectory. Such imitation enables high-idelity exempliied also includes (as often) repetition,
transmission of cultural knowledge/skills within as well as a “second”—or follow-up—assessment
and across generations, which are subsequently (Pomerantz, 1984).
built on/improved, yielding a “cultural ratchet”
effect found in no other species (Lewis & Results
Laland, 2012). At the same time, various
We analyzed our data qualitatively, as already
kinds of imitation occur widely across pri-
described. Additionally, we analyzed interview
mate species, appearing to serve key afiliative
data collected before and after Rie’s sojourn quan-
and cooperative functions (Price, Wood, &
titatively to ascertain cross-time language devel-
Whiten, 2016).
opment. These data are presented in the online
Moore (2011) reviewed studies showing lex-
supporting information; they show large increases
ible and adaptive use of repetition in lan-
in Rie’s linguistic luency and complexity over
guage learning/teaching/socialization, including
10 months, while accuracy remained stable. The
SLA/T. Larsen–Freeman (2012) reviewed the
qualitative analysis that follows aims to present
role of repetition in SLA/T speciically, ind-
different perspectives on how Rie’s cross-time
ing that it (a) enables conversion of knowl-
development was environmentally supported and
edge from short-term memory into long-term
occurred in moment-to-moment interaction.
memory, and of linguistic items in long-term
memory into chunks; (b) facilitates automaticity, Activity Type. The activity type studied here is
enabling luency and thus freeing up precious baking together. Collaborative baking is a form of
cognitive resources; and (c) is never “mere triadic interaction, wherein individuals focus shared
repetition”—instead, “in every recurrence there attention and action on co-active environmen-
is a compromise of iteration and innovation” tal affordances in completing a task (van Lier,
(Brown, 2005, p. 27). Here we deine repeti- 2000).8 As seen in Picture 1 (taken after the activ-
tion primarily as immediate repetition (often ity started), Rie and Jaakko sit side-by-side facing
with variation) of own or interlocutor talk, al- the video camera, with a cooking pot serving as
though repetition obviously pervades language mixing bowl centered on the table before them.
use. Arrayed around the pot left to right are: a coffee
5. Assessments: Evaluation inheres in all animal cup (serving as measuring cup); several transpar-
interaction: Knowing whether to approach/avoid ent bags of dry ingredients; a large wooden spoon
potential interactors—and how to align with (for stirring); a latware spoon (serving as mea-
them if approached—is fundamentally adaptive; suring spoon—both spoons are partly obscured
we are thus highly tuned to evaluation-enabling in the picture); a sake bottle (serving as rolling
multimodal behavior (Kinsbourne & Jordan, pin); a latware knife; a bag of lour; butter; eggs;
2009). Assessments represent a modest part and a kitchen towel. The lack of specialized mea-
of this larger phenomenon: “evaluat[ions] in suring tools is notable—Rie had recently moved
some fashion [of] persons and events being into a furnished apartment with minimal cooking
described in ( …) talk” (Goodwin & Goodwin, equipment.
478 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
PICTURE 1 (P1): Activity Setting

Activities are typically initiated cross-modally. thumb. At 54 seconds Jaakko enters the pic-
As the video begins, Rie is positioning the video ture (gesturing left hand irst) from right, as
camera so it takes in the scene. After about Excerpt 1 begins.
40 seconds, she steps back fully into the pic- For most of this excerpt, Rie and Jaakko ap-
ture and starts pulling at a band-aid on her right pear to be in different activity-worlds. Thus, in

EXCERPT 1:
Activity Initiation (Bold print marks activity-initiating elements)
Dwight Atkinson et al. 479
lines 1–2, Jaakko gestures toward the chair to so that by the end of the excerpt Rie and Jaakko
Rie’s left and requests, “Please sit” (Picture 2). occupy the same ‘activity world.’ This total activ-
Still focused intently on her band-aid, Rie queries ity environment plays a constitutive and dynamic
and expands Jaakko’s utterance—“Sit down?” (3– role throughout the baking activity—and it is a
4)—in an “absent-minded” voice. Rie then sits pedagogical role per our deinition of teaching,
down as Jaakko answers, “Yeah” (5), but immedi- since it organizes and enables social action-for-
ately looks off-screen toward the video controller, learning. In Excerpts 2–4, we focus directly on lin-
stands, and begins moving off-screen, uttering, guistic structuring of the environment via partic-
“It started?” (10; Picture 3). Jaakko simultane- ipation structures/routines; as a result, the total
ously sits down and starts to rub his right hand environment, which continues to shape the inter-
across the table, commenting that it needs clean- action, is discussed more selectively.
ing (11–12). Still off-camera, Rie responds with
Participation Structures/Routines. Here, we irst
a quiet “Yeah” (13), 2 seconds elapse, and then
establish the nature of the cooperative baking
Jaakko utters “So” (15)—a discourse marker com-
routine—the primary participation structure ap-
monly signaling a new activity or participation
pearing in the interaction—in Excerpts 2 and 3
structure (Schiffrin, 1987). Rie then comes on-
and Table 1. We then show how it explicitly sup-
screen briely before disappearing again as she
ported SLA in Excerpt 4.10
echoes Jaakko’s “so” quietly, pauses, and contin-
In Excerpt 2, as Jaakko starts to open the lour
ues in measured speech, “Did I start it? No?”
bag (1), Rie attempts to initiate the routine by
Then, following a 3-second pause, she utters a
stating “irst” while picking up the butter (3;
loud and emphatic “Yes!” followed by “ok” (18–
Picture 6); Jaakko simultaneously utters “Let’s
19). As she sits down again, Rie glances briely at
see” (5). Rie then retracts her bid (“Not not this
Jaakko, touches the pot with her left hand, and
irst”—6) and in line 8 Jaakko initiates the routine
looks directly into the camera, smiling (19–21;
with “First thing.” He then repositions the lour
Picture 4). Jaakko then restates his “So,” turns to
bag (Picture 7) in order to read the recipe printed
Rie, and requests an explicit deinition of the ac-
(in Finnish) on its side and continues, “We have
tivity/situation (Goffman, 1964) in a loud, cheer-
the milk?” (10), while tapping his right index in-
ful voice: “What are we doing?” ending in a broad
ger on the pot/mixing bowl (Picture 8), which in-
smile (23–25; Picture 5). Rie laughingly replies,
deed already contains the milk, since they had to
“Making cinnamon rolls,” while gazing at and
warm it earlier. Jaakko next reads the amount of
then leaning toward Jaakko (26–27).
milk required—“Five deciliters” (14)—and moves
Although Rie and Jaakko’s actions mostly di-
to the next ingredient: “Now we put there one
verge in Excerpt 1,9 by the end of the excerpt
egg” (17). As Jaakko produces these initial moves
they are together physically (side-by-side before
in the routine, Rie actively participates by (a) co-
the mixing bowl), affectively (smiling and laugh-
gazing at the recipe,11 (b) performing the role
ing), cognitively (gaze-sharing—a visible sign of
of primary doer versus Jaakko’s role as primary
shared cognition), and linguistically (Jaakko re-
recipe reader/voicer (these roles are somewhat
quests and Rie provides the name of the activ-
ideal initially, since the irst ingredient—warmed
ity being initiated). The social actors are thus
milk—has already been prepared) by tipping the
now environmentally co-organized as an essential
pot/mixing bowl to check the milk (15; Picture 9)
step toward accomplishing a complex cooperative
and later reaching for the egg carton (18), and
activity.
(c) conirming each turn in Jaakko’s recipe read-
There is no magic moment of learning—no
ing with continuers (13, 15, 16, & 18) and
‘aha’ moment—in Excerpt 1. Yet, if activity types
repetition (18).
are without-which-nots for social action, they must
In Excerpt 3, which occurs almost 2 minutes
also be without-which-nots for learning/teaching.
after Excerpt 2 (for intervening material see
Put differently, the environment is undergo-
Excerpt 5), the baking routine continues, but
ing progressive structuring and alignment as
now includes negotiating the measurement of
Excerpt 1 proceeds. By getting seated, conirm-
ingredients.
ing the video camera is running, wiping the ta-
Here, as in Excerpt 2, Jaakko plays the primary
ble (perfunctorily), organizing affect and atten-
role of recipe reader, while Rie adds ingredients
tion as shared affect and attention, and, inally
to the mixing bowl. This is unproblematic in the
(and doubtless partly for the audience’s sake [see
case of the egg (1–2), which is a discrete unit, but
note 9] but still a means of organizing action),
not for the next ingredient: sugar. As already men-
explicitly marking activity initiation linguistically,
tioned, Rie and Jaakko lack a measuring cup and
the ecosocial environment becomes co-organized,
480 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
EXCERPT 2:
Baking Routine Initiation (Bold print marks routine elements in Excerpts 2–4)

spoons, so correct measurement of ingredients— more?” Picture 12), after which she recommences
as to some degree in all cooperative cooking—is pouring (19, 22; Picture 13). Jaakko’s eyes start to
up for negotiation. wander right beyond Rie as he conirms at line
Thus, after Jaakko reads “Sugar? Two deciliters” 24 and then intones “Hm:” (26).12 Rie then con-
(5), Rie asks, “Is that two” while holding up the cludes the sugar-adding step in the routine by say-
coffee cup they are using as a cup measure (8; ing, “And that’s it. That’s enough” (27, 29)—two
Picture 10). After a 2-second pause Jaakko ex- expressions they commonly use to bring recipe
presses doubt (“I I I hope that it is”—10), and steps to completion.
in line 11 Rie says “a little bit” as she starts to Although we have focused particularly on
pour sugar into the cup. Rie’s meaning is not linguistic structuring of the sociocognitive en-
clear here, but both parties use this expression vironment in Excerpts 2 and 3, it should be
repeatedly while measuring ingredients (often in noted that (a) the material structuring of the
the collocation “a little bit more?”) to request environment, as delineated earlier vis-à-vis activity
sociocognitive co-participation in the measuring type, continues to play a constitutive role in the
activity. Next, following another 2-second pause, action—without the written recipe on the lour
Jaakko again expresses doubt (“Is two deciliters re- bag, for instance, or the cup, sugar, and mixing
ally that much?”—14) while Rie continues to pour bowl, the whole operation would be signiicantly
sugar, responding initially to Jaakko’s query with less structured and thus less possible, (b) the
“I don’t know its its,” and then screaming playfully linguistic environment is itself material and mul-
when the sugar comes out in an unregulated way timodal, involving, inter alia, intonation, volume,
(15–17; Picture 11). Jaakko then ventures a guess rhythm, words, clauses, discourse markers, rep-
that the cup holds about 2.5 deciliters (18). Rie etition, physical production of speech, and the
irst questions Jaakko’s guess (“Really?”), but then tight discursive structuring of the routine itself,
inquires whether to add more sugar (“A little bit and (c) all the foregoing phenomena, while
Dwight Atkinson et al. 481
EXCERPT 3:
Baking Routine Negotiation

adding complexity, also add simplicity—they by-step procedural organization (i.e., “irst” an-
constitute stable conigurations of affordances ticipates further discrete steps). “Enough” like-
that enable action to go on, affording better wise plays an important organizational role in this
adaptation/alignment where necessary. Once routine, belying its frequency, as a bid to con-
again, there is no ‘aha’ learning in these data, clude a speciic step in the recipe. Other ele-
yet structuring the environment is a signiicant ments also occur only at certain points in the
ongoing accomplishment that both increases the routine, such as bare statements of ingredient
likelihood of ‘mindless coping’—itself a form of and amount, which all occur before the dough
learning (compare ‘automaticity’ in cognitivist is formed (i.e., as new ingredients are still being
SLA approaches)—and constitutes a dynamic added).
contextual matrix for more mindful learning The schematic structure revealed in Table 1
(including ‘aha’ moments, see Excerpt 4). As clearly illustrates the large decrease in “degrees of
such, it constitutes a dynamic natural pedagogical freedom” Wood et al. (1976) described as a major
environment. function of scaffolding. The language of baking
Table 1 presents the main linguistic elements was thus in no sense randomly formed or gener-
in the cooperative baking routine, listed in or- ated in our data; rather, it fell within well-deined
der of occurrence in the routine. These elements limits, thereby providing a discourse-level support
were determined primarily by counting colloca- structure for learning.
tion frequencies in the irst 30 minutes of video- Excerpt 4 shows directly how the cooperative
tape/1,061 lines of transcript. But frequency is baking routine occasioned a public ‘aha’ mo-
not the sole indicator of routinization: “First,” ment in SLA. It occurred about 2 minutes after
for instance, occurs just three times in these Excerpt 3, as Rie and Jaakko continued measuring
data, all at the routine’s beginning, yet it has two and adding ingredients to the mixing bowl/pot.
important functions: (a) initiating the routine, In Excerpt 4, lines 1–4, Jaakko and Rie com-
and (b) advance organizing the routine’s step- plete the next step in the recipe—measuring
482 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
TABLE 1
Linguistic Elements of Cooperative Baking Routine (Based on irst 30 minutes of video/1,061 lines of
transcript)

Element Examples Frequency

“First” “First”; “First thing” 3


1st person plu. pers. “We have the milk?” “Now we 23
PN + verb put there one egg?”; “Let’s put a
little more sugar”
Order-marking discourse markers “And”; “now”; “then” 26
(except “irst”, & “enough”)
Bare statement of ingredient/amount “And sugar? Two deciliters”; 8
“Five deciliters”; “One egg”
“That” + BE form + nominal or adjective “Is that two?”; “That’s two and a half” “And 23
that’s it”; “That’s suficient”
1st/2nd person sing. pers. “Can I put in there”; “And you put”; “You 16
PN + verb (often incl. modal) should put there”
“A little (bit)” “Yeah, a little bit”; “A little bit more”; Let’s 26
put a little more sugar”
“Enough” “Enough? Or?”: “Enough” 6

Note. plu. = plural; pers. = personal; PN = pronoun; sing. = singular

and adding cardamom (Picture 14). Then, does not appear elsewhere in the data), this
in lines 7–8, Jaakko reviews the ingredients is a clear instance of “microgenesis” (Lantolf,
added thus far and tacks on the next ingredient: 2000), as the learner publicly displays the notic-
“and then the salt.” Five transcript lines are ex- ing of a new linguistic object. Two points can
cluded at this point; when the excerpt resumes, be made. First, this learning opportunity actually
they are starting to measure the salt (9–12; provides far more than mere noticing of a new
Picture 15). In line 13 Jaakko indicates that the lexical item: Rie is learning a word that enables
proper amount of salt has now been added: “Yeah, a speciic speech act that marks a boundary in a
suficient amount,” with “suficient amount” play- speciic routine/participation structure within a
ing the same functional role as “enough” (see speciic activity type—a sophisticated tool for so-
Table 1)—announcing the completion of the cial action in its rich, naturally occurring so-
current recipe step. But the item “suficient” ciocognitive environment. Second, without the
appears to be unknown to Rie: She repeats “suf- rich contextual matrix within which this ‘aha’
icient amount” under her breath, pauses for moment occurred, it could not have occurred.
.8 seconds, then repeats “suficient?” in a louder That is, only through the dense structuring of
voice with rising intonation as she shifts her gaze the participation structure/routine did “sufi-
to Jaakko while blinking several times (14; Picture cient” appear in the irst place—as a synonym for
16). Jaakko responds by shifting his gaze to Rie “enough,” which itself plays a deinite, major role
and using the linguistic tool they commonly use in the routine, as just noted. The sociocognitive
for this function—“Enough” (16; Picture 17)— organization of the environment thus enabled
thereby anchoring the new item directly in the the learning/pedagogical action van Lier (1996)
routine. Rie then repeats “suficient” under her called “contingency”: “Elements of predictabil-
breath while continuing to gaze at Jaakko (17). ity (known-ness, the familiar)” afford access to
A 2-second pause follows, and Jaakko concludes elements of “unpredictability (newness, the unfa-
this side sequence by plugging the unknown miliar) (...) [through] joint interpretive work (...)
item directly into a major routine element—the which simultaneously connects the new to what is
“that” + BE form + nominal/adjective (see known, and sets up expectations for what is to hap-
Table 1): “That’s suficient” (19). A 2.5-second pen next” (pp. 170, 172).
pause follows as Rie moves on to the next step in Churchill (2007) described in detail how an-
the recipe by opening a yeast packet and uttering, other sociocognitive lexical tool was introduced
“Yeast? Not yet?” (21; Picture 18). and integrated into his linguistic repertoire: He
Although we do not know whether Rie ever tracked his own evolving understanding over a
used or even encountered “suficient” again (it 2-month period of a Japanese medical term that
Dwight Atkinson et al. 483
EXCERPT 4:
Visible Learning in Baking Routine

played an important role in helping him resolve extended discourse. Excerpt 5 is a highly co-
a medical crisis. In Churchill’s case, a bedside constructed account based on a cartoon of a
computer, medical chart, nurse, spouse, friends, chicken playing tennis on an egg carton and a
the Chinese/Japanese kanji system, a dictionary, short companion text. The excerpt begins where
and his journal were the natural teachers; in the Excerpt 2 ended (i.e., line 1 repeats the last line
present case it was Jaakko, the material environ- of Excerpt 2).
ment, the highly structured nature of the rou- The set of semantic propositions co-
tine, the virtual system of English grammar (e.g., constructed in Excerpt 5 is substantial and
the paradigmatic construction system of “That” + complex—here is a reconstruction: I bought this
BE form + adjective), intonation, gaze, attention, egg package/carton for our friend Hagi because
and other sociocognitive affordances which were it’s very cute—it’s a nice color and the chicken
the teachers, as they all converged to enable this pictured on it is playing tennis. This is because
learning opportunity. the chickens producing these eggs are not kept in
big chicken houses with lots of cages but instead
Co-Construction of Accounts/Tellings. While the
can walk around, playing in the dirt and pecking
cooperative baking routine was the main partic-
freely. This is not especially ecological but it’s
ipation structure supporting Rie and Jaakko’s
better for the chickens.
interaction, they also engaged in teasing, telling
Clearly, some of the lexicogrammar in this re-
stories, and, as described here, co-constructing
construction is not in the transcript, yet it is
dramatized accounts based on elements in the
equally clear that Jaakko’s assistance takes Rie sub-
ecosocial environment. Such high-involvement,
stantially beyond what she can do alone, thereby
highly scaffolded activities are likely fertile
providing highly effective scaffolding. Also no-
ground for participation-based language learn-
table is the complex multimodality of the co-
ing/teaching, that is, in learning how to produce
construction: It involves not just two people
484 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
EXCERPT 5:
Co-Construction of Dramatized Accounts

using language, but a picture; a written text (in point at the egg carton picture (her ingers are
Finnish); gaze; gesture; other kinds of body move- largely obscured by the cooking pot). Jaakko then
ment (e.g., Jaakko imitating a pecking chicken); puts his right index inger on the Finnish text
and objects like egg cartons, lour bags, tables, accompanying the picture and begins translating
and chairs—all in complex, symbiotic, coordi- (12–13).
nated interaction. The alignment is therefore As Jaakko translates he dramatically enacts
both deep and highly public, providing a rich en- the text with hand gestures (15–16 & 18–19;
vironment for learning how to go on by going on. Pictures 21, 22, and 25) and head movements
More speciically, after reaching for the egg car- (33–34; Picture 26). He also marks his place
ton in line 1 (Picture 19), Rie initiates what looks in the text—and publicly displays that he is
like a narrative “pre-telling” (Schegloff, 2007) by reading/translating—by repeatedly putting his
stating that she bought the egg carton because right index inger on it (12, 20, and 32–33;
a mutual friend “want this package she said” Pictures 23 & 24). Rie, for her part, contributes
(2–3). As she briely shifts her gaze to Jaakko to the co-construction via continuers (17, 28),
(Picture 20), Rie adds what is probably an expla- expansions/speciications (“walk around?” 23),
nation for their friend’s desire—“In this package summaries (“free to walk and free to,” 35–36),
is very cute” (4–5)—but does not link it linguis- causal inferences (e.g., “That’s why (...) she play-
tically to her immediately preceding utterance. ing tennis,” 36–37), and gestures (Picture 24).
Jaakko then takes Rie’s possible story-telling pref- She also adds a concluding assessment (“But
ace in a different direction, querying the mean- good for chicken,” 46), which may partly respond
ing of “cute”—“Nice color?” (7)—which Rie both to Jaakko’s “So” (42) as “a marker of potential
conirms and expands on (“Nice color a- sh- sh- [participation structure] transition” (Schiffrin,
that bird playing tennis,” 8–9), as she appears to 1987, p. 227). Goodwin and Goodwin (1992)
Dwight Atkinson et al. 485
EXCERPT 5:
(continued)

noted that assessments often conclude particular ple, mutual gaze is established three times in
talk sequences, and this appears to be the case Excerpt 5, whereas it occurs only once in
here: Following Jaakko’s “So,” Rie offers an ini- Excerpts 2–4—when Rie noticed the word
tial assessment of sorts (see note 7; assessments “suficient.” One likely explanation is that the
are described in more detail later)—“It is eco- account/telling activity is less grounded in
logical not ecological?” (43–44)—which Jaakko the sociomaterial environment than the coop-
then conirms with “Not ecological but” (45), to erative baking routine, therefore requiring a
which Rie adds a second assessment: “But good higher level of shared attention for its accom-
for chicken” (46). In lines 48–49 Jaakko inally plishment, which in turn requires more gaze-
brings the telling to a close with a second, “up- seeking/monitoring/embodied conirmation.
graded” assessment (Pomerantz, 1984), “Yeah bet- In any case, the account/telling activity provides
ter for chicken,” as he shifts his gaze back to numerous learning opportunities in a high-
the recipe on the lour bag, which he had al- involvement setting; we conclude this subsection
ready repositioned in lines 39–40, perhaps as by noting four such opportunities: (a) In line 7,
an embodied ‘pre-closing’ to the account/telling Jaakko’s “nice color” provides Rie with a linguis-
activity. tic base to build on (“Nice color a- sh- sh- that
In fact, Rie and Jaakko’s discursive dance is bird playing tennis,” 8–9), (b) in line 21, Jaakko
substantially more symbiotic than this rather produces a fairly vague/generalized description
dry, sequential description conveys. For exam- (“where they [i.e., the chickens] can do stuff”),
486 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
EXCERPT 6:
Repetition (Partly incorporating elements of Excerpt 2; bold print and lines mark repetitions)

which affords Rie the opportunity to specify: “W- relecting the sociocognitive complexity of begin-
walk around?” (23), (c) in line 45, in response to ning a new speech action/participation structure
Rie’s “It is ecological. Not ecological?” (43–44), involving a partial reorganization of the environ-
Jaakko provides a discourse-grammar frame for ment. Thus, in Excerpt 6 (partly incorporating el-
Rie to ill in: “Not ecological but” (45), which ements of Excerpt 2), we get rich and complex
Rie then completes with “But good for chicken” repetition (nonverbal description is minimized to
(46), and (d) after Rie produces “But good for highlight linguistic patterning).
chicken,” Jaakko provides a paradigmatic sub- Despite Rie’s lack of success in initiating the
stitution by reframing Rie’s utterance: “Yeah, b- routine (lines 8–11 in Excerpt 6), it could be ar-
better for chicken” (48–49). We cannot claim that gued that she generates a substantial learning op-
any of these instances lead to ‘aha’ moments or portunity: Her attempt prompts Jaakko to provide
cognitive uptake of new language structures, a successful model (12) in a timely, relevant fash-
but if language learning happens moment-by- ion. In any case, repetition is heavily used here
moment in the ine structure of interaction, then by both parties to sociocognitively bootstrap their
these are likely the kinds of instances that matter. way into a complex speech activity, and thereby to
Certainly, the highly co-constructed nature of the go on.
co-telling offers various learning opportunities, A second example of how repetition may pro-
and therefore natural pedagogical-type teaching. vide speciic learning opportunities occurs at the
start of Excerpt 4, reproduced below as Excerpt 7,
Repetition. Repetition pervades Rie and
as Rie and Jaakko negotiate the correct amount of
Jaakko’s interaction—thus, the cooperative bak-
cardamom.
ing routine analyzed earlier is fundamentally
structured by repetition. We treat repetition
EXCERPT 7:
more selectively here, however, as immediate
Repetition Presenting Learning Opportunities
repetition (often with variation) of own or inter-
(Partly repeating Excerpt 4; bold print marks
locutor talk, with our examples coming largely
repetition)
from the excerpts already provided.
Thus, in Excerpt 2 (lines 3–6) Rie bids to ini-
tiate the cooperative baking routine by using the 1 J: That’s enough? A little bit
discourse marker “irst.” She does not succeed— more?
in fact, she undercuts her bid almost immediately 2 R: A little bit more? (.5) Enough?
(.4) ↑Yeah
by negativizing it (“Not not this irst”) as she picks
3 J: Yea:h that’s it.
up the butter.13 Rie’s bid nonetheless effectively 4 (2.0)
moves the action forward, as Jaakko then success- 5 J: That’s (.) cardamom (.5) sugar
fully initiates the routine with a partial repeat, 6 egg and then the salt .
“First thing,” in line 8. In fact, the repetition at this [5 lines deleted]
point is dense and highly co-constructed, possibly 7 J: And that’s on:e teaspoon.
Dwight Atkinson et al. 487

In line 1 Jaakko queries “That’s enough? A ples were already mentioned in describing the co-
little bit more?,” which Rie virtually recycles, constructed account/telling in Excerpt 5 (lines 4–
with variation: “A little bit more? Enough? Yeah” 10 and 43–49, although only the latter was labeled
(2). Jaakko responds with “Yeah that’s it” (3) an assessment there).
and then basically summarizes the ingredients In Excerpt 5, lines 4–5 Rie offers a possible
they have added so far while tacking on a new explanation for why their friend Hagi wanted
one: “That’s cardamom sugar egg and then the the egg carton Rie is holding, but it is also
salt” (5–6). In doing so Jaakko twice reuses an assessment: “In this package is very cute.”
the same “that” + BE form + nominal/adjective Jaakko then queries what “cute” means by of-
pattern he used in line 1, thereby modeling fering a candidate second/follow-up assessment:
varying uses of this pattern in almost a substitu- “Nice color?” (7). The inal rising intonation is
tion drill-like way; he adds an additional instance important here because (a) assessment activity
in line 7. is often “fundamentally afiliative” (Heritage &
A third and inal example of repetition oc- Raymond, 2005), and (b) “the [original] speaker
curs in Excerpt 8 (which repeats material from has primary rights to evaluate the matter assessed”
Excerpt 5), where Jaakko queries what Rie (p. 16). As described previously, Rie does not re-
means by “very cute” (as found in Excerpt 5, spond to Jaakko’s candidate assessment simply
line 5): by saying “yes” or “no,” but rather by repeating
and then building on it: “Nice color a- sh- sh-
EXCERPT 8:
that bird playing tennis” (8–9), thus both embrac-
Repetition plus Expansion (Repeats material
ing and further developing the afiliative valence
from Excerpt 5)
of the assessment sequence. As in Ohta’s (1999)
classroom-based research, assessments appear to
1 J: Nice color? be fundamental building blocks of affective align-
2 R: Nice color a- (.4) sh- sh- that bird ment/shared emotion here: By taking up and
3 playing tennis . ((gazes at J)) building on Jaakko’s candidate assessment, mu-
tual positive affect is afforded. At the same time,
Here, Rie reproduces Jaakko’s utterance ex-
Jaakko’s assessment provides Rie with material to
actly but then expands on it substantially, thereby
build a syntactically more complex/multiclausal
illustrating (a) the cross-cognitive uptake and
account, and therefore to perform as a compe-
use of publicly displayed social action to build
tent participant. Or, to put it more sociocogni-
new/additional social action (Goodwin, 2013;
tively, syntax involves far more than simply arrang-
Moore, 2011), and (b) a possible example of the
ing/ordering (“tax[is]”/tactics) linguistic units
emergence of Rie’s ability to use the tool of clausal
together (“syn”) here—it facilitates people ar-
coordination to go on. One of the main ind-
ranging themselves together through language in
ings of our quantitative analysis (see online sup-
order to perform ecosocial action (Ochs et al.,
porting information) was that clausal coordina-
1996).
tion was absent in Rie’s pre-Finland interview data
A second example of assessments occurs near
but prominent post-Finland. Thus, we interpret
the end of Excerpt 9 (repeating material in
the “a-” in line 2 as an incomplete realization of
Excerpt 5), where Rie and Jaakko are discussing
“and,” the incompleteness of which may be due
the picture on the egg carton and its companion
to the increased (socio)cognitive effort needed
text.
to determine the correct subject (“sh[e]- sh[e]-”
versus “that bird”) of the ensuing clause (see EXCERPT 9:
Excerpt 5, lines 36–37 for an analogous instance Assessments Closing Action Sequence (Repeats
of processing dificulty). If this interpretation is material from Except 5)
correct, what is notable here is that the clausal
coordination is interactively constructed: Jaakko pro-
vides the original material (“Nice color”) to be co- 1 R: It is ecological ((shifts gaze to J;
ordinated, which Rie then adds “a[nd] (...) that J briefly returns gaze))
bird playing tennis.” In Goodwin’s (2013) words, 2 not ecological?=
3 J: =U:h not ecological but
“human beings inhabit each other’s actions”
4 R: ((Cracks egg on table)) But good (.)
(p. 8). for chicken.
Assessments. Evaluation is pervasive in our 5 [
data, as in all interactive discourse, but explicit 6 J: Yeah be-(.) better for chicken. (.5)
7 ((Reading recipe))
assessments occur only occasionally. Two exam-
488 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
As mentioned previously, assessments fre- “go on” (Shotter, 1996) in a richly structured
quently function to close action sequences. sociocognitive world “apt for engagement and
Jaakko probably begins to exit the ac- action” (Clark, 2016, p. xv) is thus a vital con-
count/telling at Excerpt 5, lines 39–40, where dition for—and important facet of—adaptive
he repositions the lour bag. Then, in line 42, learning/teaching. Engagement via alignment
he utters “So”—an activity transition-marking with mindbodyword is the main aspect of learn-
discourse marker (Schiffrin, 1987). But Rie ing/teaching we hope to have highlighted here.
continues with what we interpreted earlier as Such engagement can be mindful and inten-
a noncanonical assessment (see note 7): “It is tional, or not, or anywhere in between. That
ecological. Not ecological?” (lines 1 and 2 in is, if the means of human teaching and learn-
Excerpt 9), to which Jaakko responds, “U:h not ing have evolved over many millennia, they
ecological but.” Rie then takes up Jaakko’s “but” need not be consciously activated (i.e., inten-
and adds “but good for chicken,” which Jaakko tionally attended to) to function. Rather, they
overlaps with, “Yeah be- better for chicken.” As are part of our “‘mindless’ (...) absorbed cop-
Ohta (1999) argued based on her classroom ing” (Streeck, 2015, p. 423)—our continuous
data, afiliative “aligning expressions” occur moment-to-moment alignment in the existential
frequently in assessments, perhaps as a way mode phenomenologists call the ‘natural atti-
of showing interactors that they are on the tude,’ in which we ‘go on’ unthinkingly while
same affective page. Simultaneously, interclausal thinking of other things. This is not to deny the
syntax is afforded here through this rich co- enormous lexibility of adaptive attention in hu-
construction/alignment: Rie uses Jaakko’s “but” mans, or the many gradations between mindless
as a jumping-off point in producing “but good and mindful activity, but it is to assert the reality of
for chicken.” learning-by-going on. What else could ‘practice ef-
fects’ and ‘incidental learning,’ for example, be?
DISCUSSION Turning now to issues of SLA methodology
and epistemology, Boxer and Zhu (2017), among
In analyzing our data, we have tried to show how others, assert that scholars have been unable to
the rich mindbodyworld matrix of Rie and Jaakko’s demonstrate SLA in moment-to-moment interac-
baking activity/interaction supports it at every tion. This is true, if learning is deined as pri-
turn. Likewise, we have tried to indicate how this vate cognitive development. Long and Doughty
matrix conditions/affords teaching and learning. (2003) exemplify this position, criticizing schol-
This may be most obvious in the microgenetic ars who “persist in seeing external learner behav-
‘aha’ moment found in Excerpt 4, but for us this is ior, even group behavior, not mental states, as the
just the tip of the iceberg. Context and interaction proper domain of inquiry” (p. 86). Problems with
cannot be pried apart—the igure emerges only this position include (a) crypto-behaviorism—if
in relation to the ground: Social action, including development is private and internal then it is de-
learning and teaching, exists within larger struc- tectable only through measuring behavioral re-
tures of semiotic modalities, objects, and evolved sponses (Costall, 2004), (b) neglect of learning
skills that afford it—without which it cannot occur processes—learning artifacts are the main object
(Canagarajah, 2018; Goodwin, 2003). of study, and (c) disregard for the deep embed-
Although interaction/language is emergent dedness of social action in complex mindbodyworld
and agentively produced in this world, it is emer- ecologies. In mainstream SLA research, learning
gent and agentive largely on the basis of ecosocial so deined often leads to studies of (pre- and post)
stabilities, because “objects in the environment test-taking behavior.
[also] have agency to shape human actors” Based (at least tacitly) on such understandings
(Canagarajah, 2018, p. 272). Something similar of learning, various researchers have attempted
holds for learning and teaching—themselves to ind evidence of learning in interactional data
basic forms of interaction—in sociocognitive longitudinally (e.g., Dings, 2014; Ohta, 1999).
theory: As a natural function of interaction, These are worthy efforts, but they still treat learn-
learning opportunities (van Lier’s “newness, ing as change between discrete points—the points
unfamiliarity”) are contingent on the structured are now contextualized and focal, but the learn-
and structuring tools and matrices (van Lier’s ing process itself is still largely invisible. “Learning
“oldness, familiarity”) of social interaction. These in light” (Atkinson, 2010) is not yet on the table.
include socially deined activity types, routines, An alternative approach highlights moments
co-construction, repetition, and assessment ac- of demonstrable uptake of linguistic items, as
tion packages (Goodwin, 2003). Being able to we do here in Excerpt 4 and our earlier work.
Dwight Atkinson et al. 489
Studies of interactional repair have gone farthest differently, if teaching is whatever enables us to
in this direction—uptake of those opportunities learn, as argued by Gee (n.d.) and Atkinson
also typically involves focusing of attention, an im- (2017), it is widely distributed—not limited to hu-
portant avenue for learning and a major topic in man or human-designed teachers at all.
mainstream SLA studies. But implicitly deining Whether one accepts this argument or not,
SLA as the totality of uptake moments seems it seems obvious that human teaching extends
as insuficient as viewing it as the totality of ac- well beyond its traditional conceptualization: a
quired forms (Gregg, 1984). In addition, such formally designated ‘teacher’ teaching formally
‘aha’ moments are almost by deinition excep- designated ‘students’ in a formally designated,
tional, since they require focal awareness—a lim- specialized ‘classroom’ space, following formally
ited and expensive (socio)cognitive process. How approved methods and approaches to facilitate
can we conceptualize SLA in other—hopefully the acquisition of learning objects, thereby ful-
complementary—ways? illing learning objectives (Atkinson, 2017). This
Our theoretical starting point is that, instead of characterization is in no way intended to diminish
being primarily an invisible and exceptional cog- classroom teaching—it may be the most complex,
nitive process, learning is part of everyday life: powerful, adaptive macro-support structure for
What happens when people align with their ecoso- teaching in the world today.
cial environments while accomplishing daily ac- Yet such conceptualizations of classroom teach-
tivities. This is not a simple argument, and our ing leave out all kinds of natural pedagogy—
research is highly exploratory, yet something sim- embodied and embedded adaptive aligning
ilar is widely accepted in learning theory: Human behaviors which are pedagogically oriented
learning is a natural process, and although age (inasmuch as humans are pedagogically oriented
may affect it, it is life-long. It takes but one small by nature), but unlikely to be intentional or
additional step to place learning in an adaptive, part of a plan. Smotrova (2017), taking an SCT
ecosocial framework, as part of natural, ongoing perspective, found that the university English-
worldly alignment. Alignment is basic to human as-a-second-language (ESL) teacher she studied
existence, so treating learning as part of it does used “pedagogical gestures” in highly intentional
not seem radical at all. ways—as artiicial mediational means to accom-
A second step in our argument is that teaching plish her pedagogical objectives. From a sociocog-
is also natural. This claim is complex, but can be nitive perspective, however, this teacher—as every
presented as a logical sequence: (a) Humans have teacher—was doing far more than deploying a
evolved into nonpareil environmental adapters, well-planned pedagogical technique: Close ex-
(b) such radical adapters must structure their amination of the data/analysis shows that she was
environments radically and complexly to make using multiple embodied tools for alignment
them adaptive, (c) these complex structurings far (smiling, rhythmic head tilts, rhythmic body
outstrip what people can learn individually—that movement, and interactional synchrony) that
is, without teaching/cultural transmission, and were very unlikely to have been intentionally
therefore (d) humans have evolved a ‘pedagog- planned. Rather—or additionally—the teacher
ical instinct’—teaching has become an essential was adaptively aligning and ‘mindlessly’ inter-
part of our adaptive toolkit. Thus, we learn in our acting with her students in the “unio mystico or
particular ways through unique evolved sensitivi- socialized trance” (Goffman, 1957, p. 47) of
ties to pedagogical action, just as, conversely, our highly evolved, highly responsive, and at least
teaching naturally teaches to those sensitivities. partly spontaneous interaction. We believe that
This is the theory of natural pedagogy, wherein this element of teaching—its basis in evolved
learning and teaching co-evolved. and ancient human abilities to align with our
A third step in our argument is that, given environment—has been overlooked and could
our evolved sensitivities to pedagogical action, productively augment how we think about teach-
we both (a) design human environments to ing in SLA/T studies (Atkinson, 2017; Gee, n.d.;
teach/support learning, and (b) ind teaching Holmes et al., 2017; van Lier, 2004). In this sense,
widely in our world. Point (a) is easily demon- it can spark our pedagogical imagination by
strated: Literacy, for instance, is a form of envi- helping us look at classrooms, teachers, teaching,
ronmental design that enables various types of and learning in new and different ways.
learning—it is no exaggeration to say that books Our main focus in the empirical part of this ar-
teach. Point (b) is more dificult: It holds that we ticle has been the following: Rather than focusing
are so pedagogically oriented that we ind teach- exclusively on the intimate details of multimodal
ers opportunistically across the environment. Put interaction, we have tried to indicate how its
490 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
embedding in a structured sociomaterial ecology equally interested in everything that makes such action
allows it to emerge. Put differently, the cooper- possible and supports it in an ongoing way. This includes
ative baking activity did not just emerge; rather, all kinds of embodied and embedded ecosocial pro-
like all social activities, it was aided, abetted, cesses, affordances, and action: scaffolding; behavioral
(including linguistic) synchrony and accommodation;
organized, and enabled by complex environmen-
affect, emotion, and empathy; neural resonance (e.g.,
tal (including linguistic) structuring. This is not to
mirror neuron activity); activity systems, participation
suggest a binary distinction between interaction frameworks, and deinitions of the situation; instincts
and context (Canagarajah, 2018)—as stated ear- for high-idelity imitation/repetition; instincts and af-
lier they are fundamentally integrated in our view. fordances for teaching and learning; ecological pattern
Rather, it is to expand the focus on visible, public recognition/response; embodied anticipation; guided
interactivity to include what makes that interac- improvisation; tool use; intelligent use of space; iden-
tivity possible—the ecosocial structuring of the tity; and of course turn-taking, sequential organization,
environment. and repair.
To summarize and conclude, human interac- Recent research on embodied cognition indicates
integrated perception—emotion—cognition—motor
tion involving learning/teaching is emergent, but
action—tool—material environment cognitive ecolo-
its very emergence is enabled by dense environ-
gies. Phenomenological and Wittgensteinian philos-
mental structuring: It is only by severely limiting ophy prominently feature “felt senses,” almost by
the domain of possible actions—the degrees of deinition below the level of consciousness. Non-
freedom—that we can have social action at all. Western/nondualist ontologies include concepts
And that social action, while not environmentally (which we know only in Japanese but believe to be
determined per se, must accord and align with universal) like omoiyari and ishin-denshin, which might
the sociomaterial world. Ultimately, this means re- be described as fundamentally relational rather than
thinking second language learning/teaching as individual (see also Canagarajah, 2018).
2 Certain other primates do this—in a limited way.
part of—not apart from—the rest of the world.
That is, they still inhabit the same basic physical envi-
ronments and live much the same as they have for mil-
lions of years, indicating the relatively minor role cul-
tural niche construction has played for them.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The human case, for its part, is quite complex: Be-
cause we are evolutionarily adapted to adapt, most of
We dedicate this article to the memory of our beloved the speciic skills and abilities needed to adapt to par-
colleague, Takako Nishino. To be with Takako was to ticular environments cannot be genetically coded. The
experience a life lived full—full of wonder, delight, distinctive evolutionary solution to this problem is to
strength, intelligence, industry, humor, and, above all give humans the skills to design their environments in
else, an ininite capacity to love. Takako, you are with high-dimensional ways and then to preserve and im-
us each day in your liveliness and caring. In that sense prove those designs by transmitting them within/across
nothing has changed. generations.
We would also like to acknowledge the kind support 3 This section is included at the request of reviewers.

of April Ginther, Jinju Nishino, and Christine Tardy. Our coverage is necessarily quite selective/partial.
4 Note that audio–video recording presents only a

thin slice of the rich reality of multimodal, face-to-face


interaction for analysis. Physical proximity, smell, and
NOTES touch, for instance, but also sight and sound beyond
what video camera/microphone, with their highly lim-
1 ited focus and directionality, can record, are missing
Atkinson et al. (2007) deined alignment as “the
from our data.
complex means by which human beings effect co- 5 Our information about participants and context
ordinated interaction, and maintain that interaction
comes mainly from informal interactions between Rie
in dynamically adaptive ways” (p. 169). Alignment
and the third author, including regular Skype conversa-
therefore encompasses (a) the ongoing interactive
tions while Rie was abroad, and a site visit made by the
processes themselves, (b) the outcome of such pro-
irst author. Informed consent was obtained from Rie
cesses, and (c) the conditions and abilities enabling
and Jaakko to use information obtained in this way, as
such processes. It includes interaction with non-human
well as to use the video data.
phenomena, and fundamentally incorporates affect 6 Ochs et al. (1996) is a telling if partial indicator of
(see note 7).
the broad range of uses Jefferson’s transcription system
Our understanding of the notion of interaction—
has been put to in analyzing interaction over the past
always a work in progress (e.g., Atkinson, 2014)—seems
30 years (O’Connell & Kowal, 2009).
to diverge substantially from those who focus mainly 7 More precisely, assessments are usually deined as
on its massive sequentiality (Schegloff, 2007). While se-
“evaluative act[s], typically performed by an utterance
quential action is obviously central to interaction, we are
Dwight Atkinson et al. 491
that contains a negative or positive predication of a ref- to second language acquisition (pp. 143–166). New
erent or (...) state of affairs expressed by the subject York: Routledge.
or the object of the sentence” (Sorjonen & Hakulinen, Atkinson, D. (2012). Adaptive intelligence and second
2009, p. 281)—that is, explicit evaluative lexis. Goodwin language acquisition. Applied Linguistics Review, 2,
and Goodwin (1992), however, open the door to a much 211–232.
wider range of assessment tools/behaviors when they Atkinson, D. (2014). Language learning in mindbody-
treat intonation as a central resource for assessment. world: A sociocognitive approach to second lan-
This accords with our own and others’ (e.g., Kinsbourne guage acquisition. Language Teaching, 47, 467–
& Jordan, 2009) view that affective evaluation pervades 483.
all social interaction. Atkinson, D. (2017). Homo pedagogicus: The evolution-
8 It thus differs somewhat from canonical face-to-face ary nature of second language teaching. Language
conversation. Teaching, 50, 527–543.
9 The presence of the video camera obviously exer- Atkinson, D., Churchill, E., Nishino, T., & Okada, H.
cises great inluence at this point, as it is turned on, ad- (2007). Alignment and interaction in a sociocog-
justed, habituated to, and the baking activity is initiated. nitive approach to second language acquisition.
Research methodologists Heath, Hindmarch, and Luff Modern Language Journal, 91, 169–188.
(2010) noted that ixed video cameras are often “made Bargh, J. (2017). Before you know it. New York: Touch-
at home” (p. 49) after a few minutes of videotaping, as stone.
interactors get to work on their social projects, but they Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. Chicago:
also recommended monitoring signs of attention and University of Chicago Press.
action vis-à-vis the video camera throughout the activity Boxer, D., & Zhu, W. (2017). Discourse and second
to understand its possible inluence. We did so, inding language learning. In S. Wortham, D. Kim, & S.
that as soon as Rie and Jaakko became seriously engaged May (Eds.), Discourse and education (pp. 297–309).
in baking, they rarely looked at or seemed to perform Berlin: Springer.
for the camera. Still, being observed for research pur- Brown, J. (2005). Process and the authentic life: Toward a
poses clearly changes the ecology: This is the Observer’s psychology of value. Heusenstamm, Germany: Ontos
Paradox, which holds for all nonsurreptitious social Verlag.
observation. Canagarajah, S. (2018). Materializing ‘competence’:
10 It should be mentioned that almost 4 minutes Perspectives from international STEM scholars.
elapsed between Excerpt 1 and Excerpt 2 as Rie and Modern Language Journal, 102, 268–291.
Jaakko recalled a humorous incident, discussed whether Churchill, E. (2007). A dynamic systems account of
they had suficiently warmed the milk, and went off- learning a word: From ecology to form relations.
screen to wash their hands. Applied Linguistics, 29, 339–358.
11 Although Rie cannot read Finnish, she appears to Churchill, E., Nishino, T., Okada, H., & Atkinson, D.
attend to numerical amounts in the written recipe. (2010). Symbiotic gesture and the sociocognitive
12 As Jaakko explains in the immediate subsequent in- visibility of grammar. Modern Language Journal, 94,
teraction (not included here), he is gazing at the off- 234–253.
screen milk carton, considering using it as an effective Clark, A. (2016). Suring uncertainty: Prediction, action,
measuring tool. They decide against this as they are al- and the embodied mind. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni-
ready well along in the recipe. versity Press.
13 Rie does this multiple times in our data—she
Costall, A. (2004). From Darwin and Watson (and cogni-
states a proposition and immediately negativizes it (e.g., tivism) and back again: The principle of animal–
Excerpt 4, lines 43–44). We do not know why she does environment mutuality. Behavior and Philosophy,
so. 32, 179–195.
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as
evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions
REFERENCES of the Royal Society B, 366, 1149–1157.
Dings, A. (2014). Interactional competence and the de-
Atkinson, D. (2002). Toward a sociocognitive approach velopment of alignment activity. Modern Language
to second language acquisition. Modern Language Journal, 98, 742–756.
Journal, 86, 525–545. Douglas Fir Group. (2016). A transdisciplinary frame-
Atkinson, D. (2010). Extended, embodied cognition work for SLA in a multilingual world. Mod-
and second language acquisition. Applied Linguis- ern Language Journal, 100 (Supplement 2016),
tics, 31, 599–622. 19–47.
Atkinson, D. (2011a). Preface. In D. Atkinson (Ed.), Duranti, A. (1996). Linguistic anthropology. Cambridge:
Alternative approaches to second language acquisition Cambridge University Press.
(pp. xi–xii). New York: Routledge. Erickson, F. (1992). Ethnographic microanalysis of in-
Atkinson, D. (2011b). A sociocognitive approach to sec- teraction. In M. Lecompte, W. Millroy, & J. Preissle
ond language acquisition: How mind, body, and (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research in educa-
world work together in learning additional lan- tion (pp. 201–225). San Diego, CA: Academic
guages. In D. Atkinson (Ed.), Alternative approaches Press.
492 The Modern Language Journal 102 (2018)
Erickson, F. (2004). Talk and social theory. Cambridge, Kendon, A. (1990). Conducting interaction. Cambridge:
UK: Polity Press. Cambridge University Press.
Gadamer, H.–J. (1989). Truth and method. London: Kinsbourne, M., & Jordan, J. (2009). Embodied antici-
Bloomsbury. pation: A neurodevelopmental interpretation. Dis-
Gee, J. P. (n.d.). Integrated teaching science: An emerging course Processes, 46, 103–126.
ield? Unpublished manuscript. Laland, K., & O’Brien, M. (2011). Cultural niche con-
Gee, J. P. (1995). First language acquisition as a guide struction: An introduction. Biological Theory, 6,
for theories of learning and pedagogy. Linguistics 191–202.
& Education, 6, 331–354. Lantolf, J. (2000). Introducing sociocultural theory. In
Gee, J. P. (2017). Introducing discourse analysis. New York: J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second lan-
Routledge/Taylor & Francis. guage learning (pp. 1–26). Oxford: Oxford Univer-
Goffman, E. (1957). Alienation from interaction. Hu- sity Press.
man Relations, 10, 47–60. Lantolf, J., & Thorne, S. (2006). Sociocultural theory and
Goffman, E. (1964). The neglected situation. American the genesis of second language development. Oxford:
Anthropologist, 66, 133–136. Oxford University Press.
Goffman, E. (1974/1986). Frame analysis. Boston, MA: Larsen–Freeman, D. (2012). On the roles of repetition
Northeastern University Press. in language teaching and learning. Applied Linguis-
Goodwin, C. (2000). Action and embodiment within sit- tics Review, 3, 195–210.
uated human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Le-
1489–1522. gitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cam-
Goodwin, C. (2003). The body in action. In J. Coupland bridge University Press.
& R. Gwin (Eds.), Discourse, the body, and identity Levinson, S. (1979). Activity types and language. Linguis-
(pp. 19–42). New York: Palgrave Macmillan. tics, 17, 365–399.
Goodwin, C. (2013). The co-operative, transformative Levinson, S. (2006). On the human “interaction en-
organization of human action and knowledge. gine.” In N. Enield & S. Levinson (Eds.),
Journal of Pragmatics, 46, 8–23. Roots of human sociality (pp. 39–69). London:
Goodwin, C., & Goodwin, M. (1992). Assessments and Berg.
the construction of context. In A. Duranti & C. Levinson, S. (2013). Action formation and ascription.
Goodwin (Eds.), Rethinking context: Language as an In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), Handbook of conver-
interactive phenomenon (pp. 147–190). Cambridge: sation analysis (pp. 103–130). Malden, MA: Wiley–
Cambridge University Press. Blackwell.
Gregg, K. (1984). Krashen’s monitor and Occam’s Ra- Lewis, H., & Laland, K. (2012). Transmission idelity
zor. Applied Linguistics, 5, 79–100. is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367,
Hall, J. (2017, January). Interactional competence: Look-
2171–2180.
ing back, moving forward. Keynote presenta-
Long, M. (2015). Second language acquisition and task-
tion at the ICOP 2 Conference, Neufchatel,
based teaching. Malden, MA: Wiley–Blackwell.
Switzerland.
Long, M., & Doughty, C. (2003). SLA and cognitive sci-
Harari, Y. (2015). Sapiens: A brief history of humankind.
ence. In C. Doughty & M. Long (Eds.), Handbook of
New York: Harper.
second language acquisition (pp. 866–870). Malden,
Harris, R. (1998). Introduction to integrational linguistics.
MA: Wiley–Blackwell.
New York: Pergamon.
Markee, N., & Kunitz, S. (2015). CA-for-SLA studies of
Heath, C., Hindmarch, J., & Luff, P. (2010). Video in qual- classroom interaction: Quo vadis? In N. Markee
itative research. London: SAGE. (Ed.), Handbook of classroom discourse and interaction
Heritage, J., & Raymond, G. (2005). The terms of agree- (pp. 425–440). Malden, MA: Wiley–Blackwell.
ment: Indexing epistemic authority and subordi- Moore, L. (2011). Language socialization and repeti-
nation in talk-in-interaction. Social Psychology Quar- tion. In A. Duranti, E. Ochs, & B. Schieffelin
terly, 68, 15–38. (Eds.), Handbook of language socialization (pp. 209–
Holmes, J., Tran, K., & Gee, E. (2017). Distributed teach- 226). London: Blackwell.
ing and learning systems in the wild. In M. Young Nakahama, Y., Tyler, A., & van Lier, L. (2001). Negoti-
& S. Slota (Eds.), Exploding the castle: Rethinking how ation of meaning in conversational and informa-
video games & game mechanics can shape the future of tion gap activities: A comparative discourse analy-
education (pp. 253–269). Charlotte, NC: Informa- sis. TESOL Quarterly, 35, 377–405.
tion Age Publishing. Ochs, E., Schegloff, E., & Thompson, S. (Eds.). (1996).
Hutchins, E. (2014). The cultural ecosystem of human Grammar and interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge
cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 27, 34–49. University Press.
Ingold, T. (2000). The perception of the environment. Lon- O’Connell, D., & Kowal, S. (2009). Transcription sys-
don: Routledge. tems for spoken discourse. In S. D’hondt, J.–O.
Jacoby, S., & Ochs, E. (1995). Co-construction: An intro- Östman, & J. Verschueren (Eds.), Pragmatics of in-
duction. Research in Language & Social Interaction, teraction (pp. 240–254). Philadelphia/Amsterdam:
28, 171–183. John Benjamins.
Dwight Atkinson et al. 493
Odling–Smee, J., Laland, K., & Feldman, M. (1996). Stivers, T. (2008). Stance, alignment, and afiliation dur-
Niche construction. American Naturalist, 147, 641– ing storytelling. Research in Language & Social Inter-
648. action, 41, 31–57.
Ohta, A. (1999). Interactional routines and the social- Streeck, J. (2015). Embodiment in human communi-
ization of interactional style in adult learners of cation. Annual Review of Anthropology, 44, 419–
Japanese. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 1493–1512. 438.
Peters, A., & Boggs, S. (1986). Interactional routines as van Lier, L. (1996). Interaction in the language curricu-
cultural inluences upon language acquisition. In lum: Awareness, autonomy, and authenticity. London:
B. B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language so- Longman.
cialization across cultures (pp. 80–96). Cambridge: van Lier, L. (2000). From input to affordance: Social-
Cambridge University Press. interactive learning from an ecological perspec-
Pomerantz, A. (1984). Agreeing and disagreeing with tive. In J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and sec-
assessments: Some features of preferred and dis- ond language learning (pp. 245–259). Cambridge:
preferred turn shapes. In M. Atkinson & J. Her- Cambridge University Press.
itage (Eds.), Structures of social action (pp. 152– van Lier, L. (2004). The ecology and semiotics of language
164). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. learning: A sociocultural perspective. Boston: Kluwer
Potter, J., & Edwards, D. (2013). Conversation analysis Academic.
and psychology. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of practice. Cambridge:
Handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 701–725). Cambridge University Press.
Malden, MA: Wiley–Blackwell. Wood, D., Bruner, J., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tu-
Price, E., Wood, L., & Whiten, A. (2016). Adap- toring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psych-
tive cultural transmission biases in children and ology and Psychiatry, 17, 86–100.
nonhuman primates. Infant Behavior and Develop-
ment, 48, 45–53. APPENDIX: TRANSCRIPTION CONVENTIONS
Roehrig, J. (2013). Theories of the interactional instinct
and the pedagogical stance. In A. Joaquin & J. . Final/falling intonation followed by pause
Schumann (Eds.), Exploring the interactional instinct ? Rising intonation followed by pause
(pp. 108–132). Oxford: Oxford University Press. : Phoneme lengthening
Sankoff, G., & Laberge, S. (1974). On the acquisition (()) "Stage directions,” i.e., nonlinguistic event
of native speakers by a language. In D. DeKamp & descriptions
I. Hancock (Eds.), Pidgins and creoles: Current trends () Transcriber doubt (parentheses can be
and prospects (pp. 73–82). Washington DC: George- illed or unilled)
town University Press. (.6) Pauses timed by 10ths of a second
Scalese Sugiyama, M. (2017). Oral storytelling as ev- (.) Short untimed pauses
idence of pedagogy in forager societies. Fron- = ‘Latching,’ i.e., the second speaker’s turn
tiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/ begins without pause after the irst
fpsyg.2017.00471 speaker’s turn
Schegloff, E. (2007). Sequence organization in interaction. [ Overlapping of one speaker’s turn by
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. another’s
Schiffrin, D. (1987). Discourse markers. Cambridge: Cam- >No< Inward angle brackets enclose talk which
bridge University Press. is faster than surrounding talk
Schumann, J. (2013). A uniied perspective of irst and <No> Outward angle brackets enclose talk
second language acquisition. In A. Joaquin & J. which is slower than surrounding talk
Schumann (Eds.), Exploring the interactional instinct !No! Degree signs enclose talk which is quieter
(pp. 1–14). Oxford: Oxford University Press. than surrounding talk
Shotter, J. (1996). “Now I can go on:” Wittgenstein and No Underlining marks various kinds of ‘voice
our embodied embeddedness in the “hurly-burly” quality,’ such as emphasis and stress
of life. Human Studies, 19, 385–407. CAPS Notably high volume
Smotrova, T. (2017). Making pronunciation visible: Ges- ↑ Marked rise in intonation
ture in teaching pronunciation. TESOL Quarterly,
51, 59–89.
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Sorjonen, M.-L., & Hakulinen, A. (2009). Alternative re-
sponses to assessments. In J. Sidnell (Ed.), Con- Additional supporting information may be found
versation analysis: Comparative perspectives (pp. 281– online in the Supporting Information section at
303). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
the end of the article.

You might also like