You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/358108057

Multisource Network and Latent Variable Models of Sluggish Cognitive


Tempo, ADHD-Inattentive, and Depressive Symptoms with Spanish Children:
Equivalent Findings and Recommendations

Article  in  Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology · January 2022


DOI: 10.1007/s10802-021-00890-1

CITATIONS READS

4 87

5 authors, including:

G. Leonard Burns Jonathan Preszler


Washington State University Washington State University
182 PUBLICATIONS   6,089 CITATIONS    28 PUBLICATIONS   233 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Mateu Servera Stephen P. Becker


University of the Balearic Islands Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center
163 PUBLICATIONS   2,203 CITATIONS    262 PUBLICATIONS   8,853 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Bifactor Models View project

Child and Adolescent Behavior Inventory--Psychometric Properties and Normative Data View project

All content following this page was uploaded by G. Leonard Burns on 06 July 2023.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-021-00890-1

Multisource Network and Latent Variable Models of Sluggish Cognitive


Tempo, ADHD‑Inattentive, and Depressive Symptoms with Spanish
Children: Equivalent Findings and Recommendations
G. Leonard Burns1   · Jonathan Preszler1 · Amina Ahnach1 · Mateu Servera3 · Stephen P. Becker2

Accepted: 15 December 2021


© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2021

Abstract
Multisource network and latent variable models were used to examine the construct validity of sluggish cognitive tempo
(SCT) symptoms relative to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-inattentive (ADHD-IN) and depressive symptoms. The
five objectives were to determine the (1) distinctiveness of SCT, ADHD-IN, and depressive symptom communities, (2)
similarity of the three symptom communities across mother, father, and teacher ratings, (3) individual symptoms with the
strongest influence on other symptoms, (4) individual symptoms with the strongest relations to academic and social impair-
ment, and (5) similarity between network and latent variable model results. Mothers, fathers, and teachers rated SCT,
ADHD-IN, and depressive symptoms for 2,142 Spanish children (49.49% girls, ages 8–13 years, third to sixth grade).
Walktrap community analysis resulted in SCT, ADHD-IN, and depressive symptom communities with three SCT symptom
communities within the overall SCT symptom community (daydreams, mental confusion, and hypoactive communities).
The symptom networks were also similar across mothers, fathers, and teachers, especially mothers and fathers. Finally, for
all three sources, the same two SCT and two ADHD-IN symptoms showed unique relations with academic impairment and
the same depressive symptom showed unique relations with social impairment. A latent variable model yielded equivalent
results. Both models thus supported the validity of SCT symptoms relative to ADHD-IN and depressive symptoms. Com-
plexities are noted in the selection of network and latent variable models to study child and adolescent psychopathology with
recommendations for their selection.

Keywords  Sluggish cognitive tempo · ADHD · Depression · Impairment · Children · Network analysis · Network model ·
Latent variable model

Latent variable and network models are the two most common the LVM, the latent variable is not considered the cause of
frameworks for the study of psychopathology (Bringmann symptom covariation. The latent variable instead represents
& Eronen, 2018). Latent variable models first focus on the the common variance in the symptom set with its explana-
identification of syndromes (sets of highly related symptoms). tion sought through the identification of its unique and dif-
One version of a latent variable model (LVM) then focuses ferent correlates, especially relative to other syndromes. The
on the identification of the correlates of the latent variable or symptoms, however, remain the focus for assessment and
syndrome (risk factors, associated features, prognosis, and interventions (i.e., the symptoms in the syndrome as a sample
treatment response). Correlates of the latent variable can be from a much larger class of behaviors). Much of our current
internal and external to individuals. Within this version of understanding of psychopathology stems from this version
of a LVM model.1
* G. Leonard Burns
1
glburns@wsu.edu  Bringmann and Eronen (2018) describe different versions of a
latent variable model (i.e., there is no single latent variable model).
1 The version described in the first paragraph is the one most often
Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164‑4820, USA
2 used for research on psychopathology. In addition, although the iden-
Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, University tification of internal and external correlates of the common variance
of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, USA in the syndrome constitutes a type of explanation, the identification of
3
University of the Balearic Islands, Palma, Spain causal processes is much more complex.

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

Network models (NM) suggest that psychopathology data-generating model for symptom covariation was a factor
research should focus on the relations between individual model, network and latent variable models were expected to
symptoms rather than focus on the latent variable (e.g., yield equivalent findings. Although the same findings do not
McNally, 2016). More specifically, this model suggests that prove the data-generating model was a factor model, equiva-
interactions among individual symptoms (as well as pos- lent findings would be consistent with this assumption and
sible correlates of individual symptoms) across time result have implications for the most useful way to conceptualize
in causally connected symptoms with the mental disorder the covariation among the SCT, ADHD-IN, and depressive
emerging from such interactions. This model has provided symptoms.
useful analyses of a wide range of mental disorders (e.g., We first briefly summarize the findings from the applica-
Bansal et al., 2020; Eadeh et al., 2021; Fried et al., 2017; tion of a LVM to SCT symptoms and then review the one
Goh et al., 2021; Martel et al. in press; Mullarkey et al., study that used a NM with SCT symptoms. These findings
2019). are then used to provide the objectives and hypotheses for
Although latent variable and network models are often the study.
presented as competing models, it is increasingly recognized
that both models yield statistically equivalent results under Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Symptoms and a Latent
certain conditions (Bringmann & Eronen, 2018). More spe- Variable Model
cifically, if the data-generating model (i.e., the model that
generates the pattern of covariances among the symptoms) is SCT involves three highly-related sets of symptoms—
a factor model as opposed to a small-world network model, daydreams (e.g., lost in a fog, stares blankly into space), men-
then latent variable and network models yield statistically tal confusion (e.g., loses train of thought, difficulty expressing
equivalent results (Christensen & Golino, 2021; Christensen thoughts) and hypoactivity (e.g., low level of activity, easily
et al., 2020; van Bork et al., 2019). Equivalent results mean tired or fatigued). A LVM was used to identify the SCT symp-
that factor loadings will be equivalent to node strength (net- toms (symptoms with substantial loading on an SCT factor
work loadings). Equivalent results also mean that the num- and much higher loadings on SCT than an ADHD-IN factor;
ber of factors from a LVM will be the same as the number Barkley, 2013; Becker et al., 2020; Burns & Becker, 2021;
of symptom clusters from a NM. Sáez et al., 2019; see Becker et al., 2016, for a meta-analytic
If the NM and the LVM yield divergent results (e.g., three review). These studies yielded 13 and 15 item SCT scales
symptom clusters versus one factor), then there are many (Barkley, 2013; Burns & Becker, 2021), the two most widely
possible reasons for the divergence (e.g., collider variables used measures of SCT (Becker, 2021).
in networks, the data generating mechanism is a small world Table  1 shows the 15 item SCT scale. Studies with
network rather than a factor mechanism, violations of the mother, father, and teacher ratings of South Korean, Spanish,
assumptions of the factor model, see Christensen & Golion, Turkish, and United States children and adolescents support
2021; van Bork et al., 2019). A careful inspection of the data the reliability and validlity of scores from this scale (Başay
and model fit may help with the determination of the model et al., 2021; Becker et al., 2020; Burns & Becker, 2021;
most consistent data generating mechanism. In addition, the Burns, Preszler et al. 2021a; Burns, Servera et al., 2021;
development of procedures to identify the mechanism is an Jung et al., 2020; Sáez et al., 2019; Servera et al., 2018).
active research area (Christensen & Golion, 2021; van Bork SCT scores, for example, demonstrated different and unique
et al., 2019). relations with other symptom and impairment dimensions
relative to ADHD-IN scores. The framework for these stud-
ies was a LVM, with this model providing nearly all of our
Purpose of Current Study current understanding of SCT. We now turn to the under-
standing of SCT from a NM perspective.
This study first applied a network and then a latent vari-
able model to mother, father, and teacher ratings of slug- Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Symptoms and a Network
gish cognitive tempo (SCT), attention-deficit/hyperactivity Model
disorder (ADHD) inattentive (IN), and depressive symp-
toms with Spanish children.2 Given our assumption that the Only one study has applied a NM to SCT symptoms (Goh
et al., 2020). With parent ratings of United States children
and adolescents, Goh et al. (2020) found SCT, inattentive,
2
  The Spanish data has been used in a series of studies to investigate
the reliability and construct validity of scores from the scales of the
Child and Adolescent Behavior Inventory (Burns et  al., 2020, 2021, Footnote 2 (continued)
2021; Sáez et  al., 2019; Servera et  al., 2018). These earlier studies lier studies provide the framework for the comparison of the network
used the latent variable model. Aspects of the findings from these ear- and latent variable models in the current study.

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

Table 1  Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Items symptom community from the SCT symptoms, an attempt
to replicate aspects of Goh et al. (2020).3 Third, measures
Daydreams Subfactor
of academic and social impairment were included to deter-
2. Lost in a fog (fog)
mine if individual SCT, ADHD-IN, and depressive symp-
3. Stares blankly into space (stares)
toms have unique relations with impairment. These meas-
5. Daydreams (daydreams)
ures of impairment did not involve a tautological issue with
8. Gets lost in own thought (lost in thoughts)
the symptom ratings. Fourth, while Goh et al. (2020) used
12. Spaces or zones out (spaces out)
a single source for the symptom ratings, the current study
Mental Confusion Subfactor
used three different sources (mothers, fathers, and teachers),
6. Loses train of thought (train of thought)
thus making it possible to determine the similarity of the
10. Forgets what was going to say (forgets)
networks across informants. Fifth, the participants for the
11. Easily confused (confused)
current study were from Spain, thus allowing an attempt
13. Gets mixed up (mixed up)
to replicate aspects of Goh et al. (2020) with participants
14. Thinking is slow (slow thinking)
from another country. Sixth, although Goh et al. (2020) did
15. Difficulty expressing thoughts (tongue-tied)
not apply a LVM to their symptoms, the current study also
Hypoactive Behaviors Subfactor
applied a latent variable model to the symptom ratings. If
1. Behavior is slow (sluggish)
network and latent variable models yield equivalent results
4. Drowsy or sleepy (yawns) during the day (drowsy)
in this study, then such would question the usefulness of the
7. Low level of activity (underactive)
application of a NM to these symptoms. We now note the
9. Easily tired or fatigued (easily tired)
objectives and hypotheses for the study.
Originally an SCT item that studies have shown to more strongly
relate to ADHD-inattention factor than the SCT factor:
16. Lacks motivation to complete tasks (apathetic) Objectives and Hypotheses

Earlier studies identified the three SCT subfactors (Becker et  al., If the data-generating model for the covariation among SCT,
2020; Burns & Becker, 2021; Sáez et al., 2019). The drowsy or sleepy
(yawns) during the day sometimes has a slightly higher loading on
ADHD-IN, and depressive symptoms is a factor model, then
the daydreams subfactor than the hypoactive subfactor. The loses the application of network and latent variable models to
train of thought item also sometimes loads slightly higher on the day- these symptoms will yield equivalent results (Bringmann
dreams than mental confusion subfactor. The italicized phrase in the & Eronen, 2018; Christensen & Golino, 2021; Christensen
parenthses is the label for the symptom in Figs. 1, 2, 3
et al., 2020; von Bork et al., 2019). Equivalent results would
raise questions about the usefulness of applying a NM to
these symptoms. More specifically, is it useful to apply a
hyperactive, impulsive, and impairment item symptom NM if a factor model is the data-generating model? This
communities. SCT symptoms thus represented a distinct is an important matter to investigate given the increasing
symptom community from the other symptom communi- number of studies that apply a NM to child and adolescent
ties. The study also found that eight of 36 symptoms (one psychopathology and then interpret the results as if the
inattentive, one hyperactive, three impulsive, one SCT, and data-generating model was a small-world network model.
two mixed inattentive/SCT items) demonstrated unique rela- A systematic consideration of this question has the poten-
tions with different types of impairment. The authors noted, tial to improve the use of these models to study child and
however, an issue of wording overlap between symptom and adolescent psychopathology. Accordingly, we examined the
impairment items, thus creating a tautological question for covariation among SCT, ADHD-IN, and depressive symp-
the interpretation of symptom-impairment relations (Goh toms with the assumption that a factor model is the data-
et al., 2020). There is also a question if the application of generating model rather than a small-world network model.
a LVM to these symptoms would have yielded equivalent Objectives and hypotheses were based on this assumption.
results (i.e., if the data-generation model was a factor model, The first objective was to determine the symptom commu-
then a network and latent variable model would have yielded nities with a NM. The prediction was that SCT, ADHD-IN,
equivalent results). and depressive symptoms would represent separate symptom
This study builds on Goh and colleagues’ (2020) NM in
several ways. First, symptoms of depression were included
to determine if SCT and depressive symptoms represent 3
  Given our focus on the SCT, ADHD-IN, and depression symptoms,
distinct symptom communities. This comparison is impor- the complexity of this NM, especially across the three sources, and
tant given questions that SCT may partly reflect depression the clear separation of the ADHD-hyperactive/impulsive, and inatten-
tive symptoms in earlier NM studies (Goh et al., 2020; Martel et al.
(Garner et al., 2013). Second, ADHD-IN symptoms were in press; Preszler & Burns, 2019; Preszler et al., 2020), this study did
included to determine if these symptoms form a distinct not include the hyperactive/impulsive symptoms.

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

communities. The SCT symptom community was, however, factors (Becker et al., 2020; Burns & Becker, 2021; Sáez
expected to contain three subcommunities—daydreams, et al., 2019), these analyses did not evaluate the unique
mental confusion, and hypoactive behaviors. Table 1 shows effects of individual symptoms. However, given findings
the SCT symptoms expected in the three SCT symptom from a NM (Preszler & Burns, 2019), ADHD-IN symptoms
communities. The basis for the prediction of three SCT close attention to details, difficulty sustaining attention, and
communities was the prior identification of three SCT fac- reluctance to engage in tasks that require sustained mental
tors (Becker et al., 2020; Burns & Becker, 2021; Sáez et al., effort were expected to be uniquely associated with aca-
2019). SCT symptoms in each SCT symptom community demic impairment. It was an exploratory question which
were expected to match the symptoms on the three SCT individual SCT symptoms would have unique associations
factors.4 with academic impairment. Individual depressive symptoms,
The second objective was to evaluate the similarity however, were not expected to have unique associations with
of symptom networks across mother, father, and teacher academic impairment with any unique associations being
ratings. Similarity was expected, especially for mothers with social impairment. It was unknown if any individual
with fathers, given an earlier study with this sample found ADHD-IN and SCT symptoms would have unique associa-
invariance of like-item loadings and thresholds for SCT, tions with social impairment with the depressive symptoms
ADHD-IN, and depression symptoms across mothers and in the network.
fathers (Burns, Preszler et al., 2021; Burns, Servera et al., The fifth objective involved the application of a LVM to
2021). the SCT, ADHD-IN, and depressive symptom ratings. If a
The third objective was to determine the central symp- NM identified daydreams, mental confusion, hypoactive,
toms. Central symptoms are considered to maintain the ADHD-IN, and depressive symptom communities, then a
structure of the network because of their strong associa- factor analysis was expected to yield five factors correspond-
tions with other symptoms (such symptoms have a higher ing to the five symptom communities. Academic and social
likelihood of co-occurring with other symptoms in the net- impairment factors were also regressed on SCT, ADHD-IN,
work). ADHD-IN symptoms reluctant to engage in tasks and depression factors to allow a comparison with individual
that require concentration and effort, difficulty sustaining symptom-impairment relations from a NM. The regression
attention, and easily distracted were expected to be cen- results were expected to yield similar conclusions to the
tral ADHD-IN symptoms (Martel et al. in press; Preszler results with individual symptoms (e.g., the ADHD-IN fac-
& Burns, 2019; Preszler et al., 2020). It was an exploratory tor and individual ADHD-IN symptoms would have unique
question if any SCT or depressive symptoms would emerge assocations with academic impairment).
as central symptoms.
There are complexities with interpretation of central
symptoms (Bringmann et al., 2019). For example, if the Methods
data-generating model is a factor model, then a measure of
symptom centrality, expected influence (sum of a symptom’s Participants and Procedures
absolute edge weights), is equivalent to the sum of factor
loadings (Christensen & Golino, 2021). In addition, if a The participants were mothers, fathers, and teachers
subset of symptoms within a syndrome have highly simi- of 2,142 children (50.51% boys) in third through sixth
lar content (wording), then such symptoms can emerge as grades (ages 8–13 years; M = 10.30, SD = 1.21) from 32
central symptoms and symptoms with strong factor load- randomly selected schools on the Balearic Islands, Spain.
ings (Christensen et al., 2020). Symptom centrality and high A total of 3,855 families were contacted by the research-
factor loadings can thus be a reflection of redundancy in ers. Mothers and fathers provided ratings on 1,648 and
content/wording of such symptoms. We return to these com- 1,358 children, respectively (1,777 unique children were
plexities in the Discussion. rated by parents). A total of 196 teachers provided ratings
The fourth objective determined if any individual symp- on 1,773 children with each teacher rating an average of
toms had unique relations with academic and social impair- 10.93 (SD = 6.05) children. Approximately 85% of the
ment. Although SCT and ADHD-IN factors demonstrated children lived with both parents and approximately 13%
unique associations with academic and social impairment with a single parent (approximately 2% missing). Parents
indicated that 5.13% of the children had an ADHD diag-
nosis and 4.86% learning problems. Additional details on
the characteristics of the parents (education and family
4
  The item lacks motivation to complete tasks, originally considered income) and children can be found elsewhere (Sáez et al.,
an SCT item, was expected to be part of the ADHD-IN symptom
community due to its stronger relation with the ADHD-IN than SCT 2019). Mothers, fathers, and teachers provided written
factor (Sáez et al., 2019). informed consent.

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

Table 2  ADHD-Inattentive,
Depressive, Social Impairment, ADHD-Inattention Items
and Academic Impairment Fails to give close attention to details or makes careless mistakes (close attention)
Items Has difficulty keeping attention focused during tasks (sustain attention)
Does not seem to listen when spoken to directly (listen)
Does not follow through on instructions and fails to finish tasks (follow through)
Shows poor organization skills (organizational skills)
Avoids, dislikes or is reluctant to engage in tasks that require sustained mental effort (concentration)
Loses things (loses things)
Easily distracted by irrelevant (minor or little) things (easily distracted)
Forgetful in daily activities (forgetful)
Depressive Items
Seems sad, unhappy, or depressed (sad)
Seems to feel worthless (feels worthless)
Seems lonely (lonely)
Seems not to enjoy activities that he or she previously thought were fun (anhedonia)
Seems to feel hopeless about things (hopeless)
Seems not to have enough energy to complete tasks or participate in activities that he or she used to have
the energy to do (lacks energy)
Academic Impairment Items
Completion of homework assignments
Reading skills
Arithmetic skills
Writing skills
Overall academic skills
Social Impairment Items for Parent Scale
Interactions with father, guardian, or partner
Interactions with mother, guardian, or partner
Interactions with adults other than parents (grandparents, babysitters, adult family friends)
Interactions with brothers and sisters
Interactions with peers
Social Impairment Items for Teacher Scale
Interactions with teachers
Interactions with peers

The italicized phrase in the parenthses is the label for the symptom in Figs. 1, 2, 3

Measure interactions] for grade level). Social and academic impair-


ment items were reverse keyed, so higher scores represent
Child and Adolescent Behavior Inventory (CABI) impairment. Tables 1 and 2 show the wording of the items.
Earler studies support the reliability and validity these
Mothers, fathers, and teachers completed the CABI (Sáez scale scores (Becker et al., 2020; Burns, Preszler et al., 2021;
et al., 2019). SCT, ADHD-IN, depression, academic impair- Burns, Servera et al., 2021). Reliability (alpha) values for
ment, and social impairment scales were used in the study. mothers (fathers, teachers) for SCT, ADHD-IN, depres-
Symptoms were rated with 6-point anchors (0 = almost never sion, social impairment, and academic impairment scores
[never or about once per month], 1 = seldom [about once were 0.93 (0.92, 0.97), 0.95 (0.95, 0.97), 0.87 (0.84, 0.92),
per week], 2 = sometimes [several times per week], 3 = often 0.90 (0.91, 0.87), and 0.94 (0.95, 0.97), respectively. Inter-
[about once per day], 4 = very often [several times per day], rater factor correlations for SCT, ADHD-IN, depression,
and 5 = almost always [many times per day]). Social and social impairment, and academic impairment for moth-
academic impairment items were rated with 7-point anchors ers with fathers were 0.81 (SE = 0.02), 0.83 (SE = 0.01),
(0 = severe difficulty, 1 = moderate difficulty, 2 = slight dif- 0.73 (SE = 0.03), 0.70 (SE = 02), and 0.87 (SE = 0.01)
ficulty, 3 = average performance [average interactions] respectively, mothers with teachers 0.43 (SE = 0.02),
for grade level, 4 = slightly above average, 5 = moderately 0.55 (SE = 0.02), 0.43 (SE = 0.03), 0.18 (SE = 0.03), and
above average, and 6 = excellent performance [excellent 0.72 (SE = 0.02) respectively, and fathers with teachers

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

0.42 (SE = 0.03), 0.54 (SE = 0.03), 0.37 (SE = 0.04), 0.14 Centrality


(SE = 0.03), and 0.69 (SE = 0.02), respectively. Low social
impairment corrrelations for parents with teachers was The Expected Influence (EI) centrality measure was used
probably due to different items on parent and teacher social to identify symptoms that were relatively central to the
impairment scales along with setting influences. network (Bringmann et al., 2019). The EI measure is the
sum of the absolute value of edge weights associated with a
Missing Data symptom. A symptom high in EI has a higher likelihood of
co-occurring with other symptoms in the network (McNally,
For mother and father ratings, only three cases left any 2016). Bootstrapped stability analysis was conducted using
symptom ratings blank. There were no missing ratings the R package bootnet (Epskamp et al., 2018) to test the
for teachers. The calculation of the polychoric correlation stability of the EI values.
matrix among the symptom ratings within each source used
a pairwise procedure to maximize the information for each Impairment
correlation for network and latent variable model analyses.
The networks were again estimated to determine the unique
Analytic Strategy for Network Model relations of individual symptoms with social and academic
impairment measures.These networks were estimated with
Network Estimation the same procedures as the primary networks.

Given the symptom ratings were treated as categorical indi- Analytic Strategy for Latent Variable Model
cators, networks were estimated using the R mixed graphi-
cal model package (mgm; Haslbeck & Waldorp, 2015). The The Mplus statistical software (version 8.3) with the items
EBIC fit index penalty was used to regularize the network. treated as categorical indicators (WLSMV estimator) was
This procedure yields a network that shrinks small bivariate used for the LVM analyses (i.e., exploratory confirmatory
partial coefficients to 0 under the assumption that empirical factor analysis and exploratory structural regression analysis,
data has ambient noise correlations. The goal of regulariza- see Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009). The Mplus type = com-
tion is to estimate a network that better approximates the plex option was used to take into account children nested
underlying generative network and thus generalizes well to within teachers.
the population (Epskamp & Fried, 2018). This procedure
was applied to each source separately.
Results
Communities of Symptoms
Network Model Results
The walktrap algorithm was used to identify symptom com-
munities (Golino & Epskamp, 2017). One hundred iterations Community Analyses
of each algorithm were completed to ensure reliability of the
findings. The R package igraph (version 1.4.2.1; Csardi & Figures 1, 2, 3 show the symptom networks for mother,
Nepusz, 2006) was used for the community analyses. father, and teacher ratings, respectively. ADHD-IN and
depressive symptoms were in their respective communities
Network Comparisons Across Sources with the 15 SCT symptoms represented by three communi-
ties. One community reflected mental confusion (loses train
Three procedures were used to evaluate the similarity of of thought, forgets what was going to say, gets mixed up,
symptom networks across sources. First, edge weights were easily confused, thinking is slow, and difficulty expressing
correlated across each pair of sources to quantify their simi- thoughts). Daydreams was the second community (lost in
larity. Second, the same edge weights were subtracted across a fog, stares blankly into space, spaces or zones out, gets
sources to determine edge differences. Given there are cur- lost in own thoughts, and daydreams) with the third com-
rently no significant tests for such differences from the mgm munity representing hypoactive behaviors (drowsy or sleepy
method, these comparisons are descriptive. Third, total abso- [yawns] during the day, behavior is slow, easily tired or
lute value of all edge weights in each network (called “global fatigued, and low level of activity (underactive). It was espe-
strength”) were summed to evaluate differences of global cially noteworthy that the hypoactive SCT symptom commu-
strength across sources. nity was distinct from the depressive symptom community.

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

A. Mothers MGM

Loses things

Close Forgetful
attention

Sluggish
Easily
Organization
distracted Underactive
skills

Sustain
attention

Easily Drowsy
Tired

Follow
through

Concentration

Listen
Fog
Stares

Apathetic

Spaces out
Lost
in
Thoughts
Daydreams

Sad

Lacks energy

Train
of
Lonely Thought
Feels Worthless
Confused

Hopeless Mixed up
Anhedonia

Forgets

Slow Thinking

Tongue−Tied

Fig. 1  Mixed graphical model network graph of mother ratings. The Orange = ADHD—Inattention symptoms + symptom Apathetic
nodes represent symptoms of SCT, ADHD-IN and Depression and (lacks motivation to complete tasks symptom, outlined in a thick
the edges represent the statistical association between the symp- black border); green = depression symptoms. Light green, pink, and
toms after statistically controlling for the other symptoms. Thicker blue = SCT symptom communities. For ease of interpretation, edges
edges represent stronger associations, with green edges representing lower than .05 are not displayed in the network. For context, the larg-
positive associations. A Fruchterman-Reingold layout was speci- est edge weight is Forgets – Train of Thought (.39). Tables  1 and 2
fied for mothers. The colored groups represent the walktrap results. show the complete wording of the symptoms

Network Comparisons Across Sources thus differences were small. In contrast, edge weight dif-
ferences between parents and teachers had over twenty dif-
Although none of the networks shared the same strongest ferences above 0.10 (i.e., -0.22 was the largest for mother
edge weight, the three networks shared many similarities. with teacher comparisons and -0.29 the largest for father
For example, the networks correlated 0.94 for mothers with with teacher comparisons). The negative sign means the
fathers, 0.86 for mothers with teachers, and 0.84 for fathers edge weights were stronger for teacher ratings. The global
with teachers. These analyses were repeated after removing strength results also indicated that teacher ratings had more
all edges that equaled 0 to quantify the agreement without highly connected symptoms than mother and father rating.
the high number of shared 0 s inflating the correlations (i.e., Global strength values were 13.59, 12.69, and 11.81 for
0.89 for mothers with fathers, 0.75 for mothers with teach- teachers, mothers, and fathers, respectively.5
ers, and 0.72 for fathers with teachers).
5
Comparison of edge weight differences between mother   Higher partial correlations (and thus global strength) for teachers
and higher loadings for teachers were probably because each teacher
and father symptom networks revealed only three pairs with rated multiple children relative to mothers and fathers. Supplemental
an absolute value greater than 0.10 (range = 0.12 to 0.13), Tables S1 to S3 show the symptom-factor loadings.

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

B. Fathers MGM

Loses things

Close Forgetful
attention

Sluggish
Easily
Organization
distracted Underactive
skills

Sustain
attention

Easily Drowsy
Tired

Follow
through

Concentration

Listen
Fog
Stares

Apathetic

Spaces out
Lost
in
Thoughts
Daydreams

Sad

Lacks energy

Train
of
Lonely Thought
Feels Worthless
Confused

Hopeless Mixed up
Anhedonia

Forgets

Slow Thinking

Tongue−Tied

Fig. 2  Mixed graphical model network graph of father ratings. The Inattention symptoms + symptom Apathetic (lacks motivation to com-
nodes represent symptoms of SCT, ADHD-IN and Depression and plete tasks symptom, outlined in a thick black border); green = depres-
the edges represent the statistical association between the symptoms sion symptoms. Light green, pink, and blue = SCT symptom com-
after statistically controlling for the other symptoms. Thicker edges munities. For ease of interpretation, edges lower than .05 are not
represent stronger associations, with green edges representing posi- displayed in the network. For context, the largest edge weight is Easily
tive associations and red edges. This network is on the same layout as Tired – Underactive (.31). Tables 1 and 2 show the complete wording
mother ratings, given the similarities between their network results. of the symptoms
The colored groups represent the walktrap results. Orange = ADHD—

Average within-domain (e.g., IN symptom with IN symp- had the strongest EI for the three sources (mothers = 2.36;
tom) edge weights were 0.06-0.07, whereas the average fathers = 1.69; teachers = 3.02). For fathers, the SCT symp-
between-domain edge weight was 0.005 or less. This indi- toms lost in a fog (1.59) and easily confused (1.18) along
cates connections were within symptom sets. Edge weights, with the ADHD-IN symptoms forgetful in daily activities
edge weight comparisons, and global strength values are (1.49) and easily distracted (1.42) also had EI values above
available in the supplemental materials. one standard deviation. For mothers, the SCT symptom eas-
ily confused (1.50) and the ADHD-IN symptom easily dis-
Centrality tracted (1.63) had EI values above one standard deviation.
The other SCT symptom with an elevated EI was gets lost
Supplemental Fig. S1 shows expected influence (EI) z-scores in own thoughts (1.79) for teachers. None of the depressive
for the symptom networks for mothers and fathers with symptoms had elevated EI values. ADHD-IN symptoms has
Fig. S2 showing EI z-scores for teachers. ADHD-IN symp- difficulty keeping attention focused during tasks and easily
tom has difficulty keeping attention focused during tasks distracted are consistently emerging as central symptoms

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

C. Teachers MGM

Stares

Daydreams
Fog

Lost Sluggish
in
Thoughts
Drowsy
Spaces out

Easily Underactive
distracted
Easily
Tired

Train
of
Thought
Sustain
attention
Forgetful

Apathetic
Close
attention Forgets Confused
Slow Thinking
Loses things

Mixed up

Organization
skills Tongue−Tied
Concentration
Follow
through

Lacks energy

Lonely

Listen
Anhedonia

Hopeless

Sad

Feels Worthless

Fig. 3  Mixed graphical model network graph of teacher ratings. The groups represent the walktrap results. Orange = ADHD—Inattention
nodes represent symptoms of SCT, ADHD-IN and Depression and symptoms + symptom Apathetic (lacks motivation to complete tasks
the edges represent the statistical association between the symptoms symptom, outlined in a thick black border); green = depression symp-
after statistically controlling for the other symptoms. Thicker edges toms. Light green, pink, and blue = SCT symptom communities. For
represent stronger associations, with green edges representing posi- ease of interpretation, edges lower than .05 are not displayed in the
tive associations. This network is on the same layout as mother rat- network. For context, the largest edge weight is Mixed Up – Confused
ings, given the similarities between their network results. The colored (.46). Tables 1 and 2 show the complete wording of the symptoms

(Martel et al. in press; Preszler & Burns, 2019; Preszler and social impairment. For mother and father ratings, SCT
et al., 2020). symptoms easily confused, spaces or zones out, and diffi-
The case-dropping bootstrapped results (using 5000 boot- culty expressing thoughts had nonzero edge weights with
straps) indicated that the correlation stability (CS) coefficient academic impairment, while ADHD-IN symptoms difficulty
for EI was above 0.50 (range: 0.59—0.67) for all three raters. keeping attention focused during tasks and avoids, dislikes
Epskamp et al. (2018) recommend that the CS-coefficient or is reluctant to engage in tasks that require sustained
should not be below 0.25, and ideally above 0.50 to provide mental efforts had nonzero edge weights with academic
evidence for stability of the EI values. impairment. For mother ratings only, ADHD-IN symptom
easily distracted had a nonzero edgeweight with academic
impairment. For ratings by teachers, SCT symptoms easily
Impairment confused and difficulty expressing thoughts and ADHD-IN
symptoms fails to give close attention to details or makes
Table 3 shows the SCT, ADHD-IN, and depressive symp- careless mistakes, has difficulty keeping attention focused
toms with nonzero edge weights with academic impairment during tasks, shows poor organization skills, and avoids,

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

Table 3  SCT, ADHD-IN, Symptoms Academic Impairment Social Impairment


and Depressive Symptoms
with Nonzero Edgeweights Mothers Fathers Teachers Mothers Fathers Teachers
with Academic and Social
Impairment SCT symptoms
Easily confused 0.07 0.08 0.11
Spaces or zones out 0.05 0.06
Difficulty expressing thoughts 0.05 0.05 0.09
ADHD-inattention symptoms
Close attention 0.10
Sustaining attention 0.12 0.13 0.05
Does not listen 0.06
Poor organization skills 0.08
Sustained mental effort tasks 0.15 0.13 0.10
Easily distracted 0.05
Depressive symptoms
Seems sad, unhappy, or depressed 0.05 0.04 0.08
Seems lonely 0.05 0.11

All values are regularized edge weights. Blank spaces indicate no relationship. SCT sluggish cognitive
tempo, ADHD attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

dislikes or is reluctant to engage in tasks that require sus- Tables S1 to S3 show the primary and secondary standard-
tained mental effort had nonzero edge weights with aca- ized loadings for the three sources.
demic impairment (i.e., four of nine ADHD-IN symptoms Higher scores on ADHD-IN and SCT factors were
had non-zero edgeweights with academic impairment). In uniquely associated with significantly (ps < 0.01) higher
addition, the item lacks motivation to complete tasks (see scores on the academic impairment factor (all three sources
footnote 4) had a nonzero edge weight with academic for ADHD-IN and mothers and teachers for SCT).6 ADHD-
impairment for teachers. No depressive symptoms had a IN’s unique association with academic impairment was
non-zero edge weight with academic impairment. stronger than SCT’s unique association with academic
For social impairment, the depressive symptom sad, impairment (partial standardized regression coefficients of
unhappy, or depressed had a nonzero edge weight with 0.60 to 0.62 vs. 0.09 to 0.25, respectively). Although the
social impairment for mothers, fathers, and teachers, while depressive factor did not have a significant unique associa-
the depressive symptom lonely had a nonzero edge weight tion with the academic impairment factor, its unique asso-
for fathers and teachers. In contrast, no SCT symptoms had a ciation with the social impairment factor was significant
nonzero edge weight with and only one ADHD-IN symptom (ps < 0.001) for each source (partial standardized regression
for fathers (does not seem to listen when spoken to directly) coefficients from 0.30 to 0.43). In contrast, the SCT factor
had a nonzero edge weight. did not have a significant unique association with the social
impairment factor while the ADHD-IN factor did have a sig-
nificant (ps < 0.001) unique association with social impair-
Latent Variable Model Results ment for fathers and teachers (partial standardized regression
coefficients of 0.16 and 0.18, respectively). The depression
A daydreams, mental confusion, hypoactive, ADHD-IN, and factor thus had a stronger unique relation with social impair-
depressive five-factor model resulted in a close fit with no ment than the ADHD-IN factor. Table S4 shows the regres-
localized ill-fit for each source (mothers: χ2 (320) = 1123, sion results and Table S5 shows the correlations of SCT,
p < 0.001, CFI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.039 (0.037, 0.042), and ADHD-IN, and depressive factors with academic and social
SRMR = 0.02; fathers: χ2 (320) = 842, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.99, impairment factors.
RMSEA = 0.035 (0.032, 0.038), and SRMR = 0.02;
and teachers: χ 2 (320) = 1545, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.99,
RMSEA = 0.046 (0.044, 0.049), and SRMR = 0.01). Pri- 6
  The SCT factor was not separated into the three SCT subfactors for
mary loadings were moderate to substantial with only 12 of the exploratory structural regression analyses Additional research is
348 cross-loadings greater than 0.30 (nearly all of the cross- required to determine the best SCT items to represent each subfactor
and if the three SCT subfactors have unique and different associations
loadings were less than an absolute 0.10). Supplemental with impairment relative to ADHD-IN and depression.

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

Discussion central symptoms. Factor loadings for these symptoms were


substantial (if not the highest loadings on their factor). As
Network and latent variable models were applied to mother, expected, under the assumption of a factor data-generating
father, and teacher ratings of SCT, ADHD-IN, and depres- model, there is correspondence between NM central symp-
sive symptoms in a large sample of Spanish children. Given toms and LVM strongest symptoms and this occurred in this
our assumption that a factor rather than a network model study.
generated the covariances among the symptom ratings, To summarize, the interpretation of central symptoms
the expectation was for the two models to yield equivalent should consider these issues. Researchers should be cau-
results (Christensen & Golino, 2021; Christensen et al., tious when interpretating central symptoms if (1) symptoms
2020; van Bork et al., 2019). Support was found for this within a syndrome have consistently high inter-symptom
prediction. We now summarize these similarities and then correlations, (2) symptoms have highly similar content/
discuss the implications for the selection of network and wording, and (3) the data-generating model is a factor
latent variable models to evaluate the construct validity of model, especially the third point. (Christensen & Golino,
SCT, ADHD, and depressive symptom ratings as well as 2021; Christensen et al., 2020). In this study, centrality val-
the study of child and adolescent psychopathology more ues are not providing useful information from a network
broadly. perspective (one symptom directly related to others), but
instead indentifying symptoms with the highest communal-
ity, findings redundant with LVM results.
Network Model Symptom Communities and Latent
Variable Model Factors Network Symptom‑Impairment and Latent
Variable‑Impairment Relations
Walktrap community analyses yielded SCT, ADHD-IN, and
depressive symptom communities. Within the overall SCT Earlier studies with this sample and others found that SCT
symptom community, there were also three distinct subcom- and ADHD-IN factors had significant unique associations
munities (daydreams, mental confusion, and hypoactive with academic and social impairment factors (Becker et al.,
behaviors). Factor analyses found support for daydreams, 2020; Burns & Becker, 2021; Sáez et al., 2019). In this study,
mental confusion, hypoactive, ADHD-IN, and depressive a depressive factor was added to this regression analysis to
factors. The symptoms within each symptom community expand on these earlier results (e.g., Would SCT predict
were also the same symptoms on the matching factor with academic impairment even after controlling for ADHD-IN
only a few exceptions. These five symptom communities and and depression?). The ADHD-IN factor continued to have a
their respective symptoms from a NM thus closely matched significant unique association with the academic impairment
the five factors and their respective symptoms from a LVM. factor for three sources and the SCT factor for two sources.
The symptom communities from a NM were similar As expected, the depression factor did not have a significant
across mother, father, and teacher ratings, especially moth- unique association with the academic impairment factor
ers and fathers. Teacher symptom ratings had stronger edge while it did have a significant unique association with the
weights and higher factor loadings than parents. Invariance social impairment factor for three sources. SCT did not have
analyses on the same data also found invariance of like-item a unique association with social impairment while ADHD-
loadings and thresholds for SCT, ADHD-IN, and depressive IN did show such a unique association for two of three
symptoms across mother and father symptom ratings in an sources. These LVM results also indicated that ADHD-IN
earlier study (Burns, Preszler et al., 2021; Burns, Servera had a stronger unique association than SCT with academic
et al., 2021). Network and latent variable model analyses impairment with depression having a stronger unique asso-
again yielded similar results. ciation than ADHD-IN and SCT with social impairment.
Do individual symptom-impairment relations from a NM
offer unique insights relative to latent variable-impairment
Network Model Central Symptoms and Latent relations from a LVM? We first review the individual symp-
Variable Model Strong Symptoms toms with unique relations with impairment and then com-
pare the relative usefulness of network and latent variable
For this discussion, a symptom is considered a central symp- model results on impairment. We mostly focus on the indi-
tom if such occurred for at least two of the three sources. vidual symptom-impairment relations that occurred for all
The ADHD-IN symptom difficulty keeping attention focused three sources for this discussion.
during tasks had high EI values for all three sources. For Two ADHD-IN symptoms (difficulty keeping attention
mother and father ratings, the ADHD-IN symptom easily focused during tasks and avoids, dislikes, or is reluctant to
distracted and the SCT symptom easily confused were also engage in tasks that require sustained mental efforts) and

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

two SCT symptoms (easily confused and difficulty express- data generating model is a small-world network model, then
ing thoughts) showed unique relations with academic the use of a NM is more appropriate than a LVM. Statisti-
impairment whereas one depressive symptoms (seems sad, cal tests are currently available to provide guidance on the
unhappy, or depressed) showed a unique relation with social nature of the data-generating model (Christensen & Golino,
impairment. Given these individual symptom-impairment 2021; Christensen et al., 2020). A failure to consider the
unique effects replicated across all three sources, these data-generating model can result in problematic interpreta-
results were robust. Two more ADHD-IN symptoms, fails tions, especially if a NM is applied to symptom ratings when
to give close attention to details or makes careless mistakes the data-generating model is a factor model. In this situa-
and shows poor organization skills, had unique associations tion, as demonstrated in the current study, the two models
with academic impairment for teachers (thus four of nine yield equivalent results.
ADHD-IN symptoms showed unique associations with aca- It would also be useful to have hybrid latent variable and
demic impairment for teachers). network models where this single model could be applied to
How would a NM interpret these results? A NM would the data simultaneously (e.g., Bringmann, 2021). The ability
argue for the importance of specific individual symptoms to apply such a hybrid model simultaneously to the same data
as bridge symptoms to impairment. This focus would would allow for the exploration of more complex questions.
therefore argue for the importance of the individual symp- Such developments, however, imply increasing complexity
toms for screening, assessment, and treatment (e.g., Goh and it is always important to remember the reason for the use
et al., 2020; Martel et al. in press). In other words, rather of such models (e.g., understanding psychopathology and
than a focus on the summary score on the nine ADHD-IN the development of more specific individualized treatments).
symptoms for assessment and treatment, the specific indi- More specifically, by embracing complexity and thereby sac-
vidual symptoms would be the focus. This interpretation of rificing parsimony in such models, researchers risk reducing
the individual symptom-impairment depends on the data- clinical applicability or comprehension.
generating model being a small-world network model rather
than a factor model. This interpretation also perhaps reflects
a failure to consider that the strong individual symptoms Limitations and Future Directions
represent a much larger response class of behavior relevant
to impairment and, with highly correlated symptoms with a It is important to note several limitations. First, and probably
high level of redundancy in content and wording, it is prob- the most important, the study involved cross-sectional data.
ably the communality in the larger response class responsi- Intensive longitudinal data, that is, daily measurement of
ble for the symptom/syndrome to impairment association. SCT, ADHD-IN, and depressive symptoms along with daily
To summarize, if a factor model was the data-generating measures of impairment, would allow a more informative
model, then a LMV interpretation of the individual symptom- comparison of a NM with its greater focus on individual
impairment relations would be more appropriate. A LVM symptoms and a LVM with its greater focus on latent vari-
interpretation would argue that such individual symptoms ables. Equivalent results across the two longitudinal models
best reflect the latent variable (strongest loadings) and thus would yield additional support for a factor data-generation
probably the aspect of the latent variable most strongly asso- model. Second, this study included school-aged children in
ciated with the latent impairment variable. The LVM would ­3rd-6th grades and it is unknown if similar results would occur
also view the symptoms with the strongest loadings as rep- with adolescents. Third, although a key strength of this study
resentative of a sample of behavior reflecting a much larger was the use of three sources (mothers, fathers, and teachers),
class of behavior relevant to impairment with treatment plans future research should include self-report assessment of SCT
based on this assumption (treatment with a focus on the larger (Becker, 2021) and depression. Fourth, the depression measure
response class). did not assess complete range of depressive symptoms (e.g.,
the factor data-generating model might only apply to these
Selection of Network or Latent Variable Models depressive symptoms while a small-world network model
for Study of SCT and ADHD Symptoms might apply to the complete range of depressive symptoms).
Fifth, the social impairment measure was a global measure of
This section offers tentative guidelines for the decision to the quality of interactions with others. Previous research has
select a network or latent variable model to seek an under- consistently found SCT symptoms to be uniquely associated
standing of SCT and ADHD symptoms. It is important to with shyness, social withdrawal and isolation (Becker, Garner
take into consideration more the data-generating model (i.e., et al., 2019; Burns & Becker, 2021; Servera et al., 2018). We
the model responsible for the covariation among the symp- did not include such measures, and it will be important for
toms). If the data generating model is a factor model, then future research to include them. And, finally, it is important to
the use of a LVM is more appropriate than a NM. If the remember the findings so far are specific to the CABI scales.

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

Recommendations Bansal, P. S., Goh, P. K., Lee, C. A., & Martel, M. M. (2020). Con-
ceptualizing callous-unemotional traits in preschool through con-
firmatory factor and network analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child
Network and latent variable models were applied to mother, Psychology. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10802-​019-​00611-9
father, and teacher ratings of SCT, ADHD-IN, and depres- Başay, O., Çiftçi, E., Becker, S. P., & Burns, G. L. (2021). Validity
sive symptoms in a large sample of Spanish children. Given of sluggish cognitive tempo in Turkish children and adolescents.
Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 52, 191–199. https://​
the assumption that the data-generating model for symptom doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10578-​020-​01110-5
covariation was a factor model, the two different models of Becker, S. P. (2021). Systematic review: Assessment of sluggish cogni-
psychopathology were expected to yield equivalent results. tive tempo over the past decade. Journal of the American Academy
Both models yielded equivalent results, thus providing support of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 60, 690–709. https://​doi.​org/​
10.​1016/j.​jaac.​2020.​10.​016
for the data-generating assumption, along with support for the Becker, S. P., Garner, A. A., Tamm, L., Antonini, T. N., & Epstein,
construct validity of SCT symptoms relative to ADHD-IN and J. N. (2019). Honing in on the social difficulties associated with
depressive symptoms. Prior to the application of a NM to child sluggish cognitive tempo in children: Withdrawal, peer ignoring,
and adolescent symptom ratings, researchers should consider and low engagement. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Psychology, 48, 228–237. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 080/1​ 53744​ 16.2​ 017.​
and, if possible, determine the data-generating model. If the 12865​95
data-generating model is a factor model, then it is probably Becker, S. P., Leopold, D. R., Burns, G. L., Jarrett, M. A., Langberg,
best to use a LVM rather than a NM. J. M., Marshall, S., & A., ... Willcutt, E. G. (2016). The internal,
external, and diagnostic validity of sluggish cognitive tempo: A
Supplementary Information  The online version contains supplemen- meta-analysis and critical review. Journal of the American Acad-
tary material available at https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 007/s​ 10802-0​ 21-0​ 0890-1. emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55, 163–178. https://d​ oi.​
org/​10.​1016/j.​jaac.​2015.​12.​006
Becker, S. P., Mossing, K. W., Zoromski, A. K., Vaughn, A. J., Epstein,
Acknowledgements  The authors would like to acknowledge the dis-
J. N., Tamm, L., & Burns, G. L. (2020). Assessing sluggish cogni-
cussions with Alexander P. Christensen on network and latent variable
tive tempo and ADHD inattention in elementary students: Empiri-
models. These discussions were helpful in the revisions of this paper.
cal differentiation, invariance across sex and grade, and meas-
urement precision. Psychological Assessment, 32, 1047–1056.
Funding  This research was supported by two grants from the Ministry https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​pas00​00946
of Economy and Competitiveness of Spanish Government under award Bringmann, L. F. (2021). Person-specific networks in psychopathology:
numbers PSI2014-52605-R and PSI2017-82550-R (AEI/FEDER, UE), Past, present, and future. Current Opinions in Psychology, 41,
and a predoctoral fellowship co-financed by MINECO (Spanish Gov- 59–64. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​copsyc.​2021.​03.​004
ernment) and the European Social Fund (BES-2015–075142). The Bringmann, L. F., Elmer, T., Epskamp, S., Krause, R. W., Schoch, D.,
content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not neces- Wichers, M., Wigman, J., & Snippe, E. (2019). What do cen-
sarily represent the official views of the Spanish Government. We thank trality measures measure in psychological networks? Journal of
Cristina Trias for assistance with the study. Abnormal Psychology, 128, 892–903. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​
Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad,PSI2014-52605-R abn00​0044
PSI2017-82550-R, Mateu Servera, National Institutes of Health, Bringmann, L. F., & Eronen, M. I. (2018). Don’t blame the model:
K23MH108603, Stephen Becker. Reconsidering the network approach to psychopathology. Psycho-
logical Review, 125, 606–615. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 037/r​ ev000​ 0108
Compliance with Ethical Standards  Burns, G. L., Geiser, C., Servera, M., Becker, S. P., & Beauchaine, T.
P. (2020). Application of the bifactor S – 1 model to multisource
ratings of ADHD/ODD symptoms: An appropriate bifactor model
Ethical Approval  The research protocol was approved by the Research
for symptom ratings. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 48,
Ethics Committee (Institutional Review Board [IRB]) of the University
881–894. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10802-​019-​00608-4
of the Balearic Islands.
Burns, G. L., & Becker, S. P. (2021). Sluggish cognitive tempo and
ADHD symptoms in a nationally representative sample of U.S.
Informed Consent  Mothers, fathers, and teachers provided written
children: Differentiation using categorical and dimensional
informed consent.
approaches. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
50, 267–280. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​15374​416.​2019.​16781​65
Conflict of Interest  The authors of the current study declare that they Burns, G. L., Preszler, J., & Becker, S. P. (2021). Psychometric and
have no conflicts of interest. normative information on the Child and Adolescent Behavior
Inventory in a nationally representative sample of United States
children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology.
References Advance online publication. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​15374​416.​
2020.​18529​43
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2009). Exploratory structural equation Burns, G. L., Servera, M., & Becker, S. P. (2021). Psychometric prop-
modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 16, 397–438. https://d​ oi.​ erties and normative information on the Child and Adolescent
org/​10.​1080/​10705​51090​30082​04 Behavior Inventory with ratings for Spanish children from parents
Barkley, R. A. (2013). Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from and teachers. Psicothema, 33, 139–145. https://​doi.​org/​10.​7334/​
ADHD in children and adolescents: Executive functioning, impair- psico​thema​2020.​267
ment, and comorbidity. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Christensen, A. P., & Golino, H. (2021). On the equivalency of factor
Psychology, 42, 161–173. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​15374​416.​2012.​ and network loadings. Behavioral Research Methods. https://​doi.​
734259 org/​10.​3758/​s13428-​020-​01500-6

13
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology

Christensen, A. P., Golino, H., & Silvia, P. J. (2020). A psycometric Jung, S. H., Lee, S. Y., Burns, G. L., & Becker, S. P. (2020). Internal
network perspective on the validity and validation of personality and external validity of self-report and parent-report measures of
trait questionnaires. European Journal of Personality. Advance sluggish cognitive tempo in South Korean adolescents. Journal
online. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​per.​2265 of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. Advance online
Csardi, G., & Nepusz, T. (2006). The igraph software package for com- publication. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10862-​020-​09821-8
plex network research. InterJournal, Complex Systems, 1695, 1–9. Martel, M. M., Goh, P. K., Lee, C. A., Karalunas, S. L., & Nigg, J. T.
Eadeh, H-M., Markon, K. E., Nigg, J. T., & Nikolas, M. A. (2021). (in press). Longitudinal attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
Evaluating the viability of neurocognition as a transdiagnostic symptom networks in childhood and adolescence: Key symptoms,
construct using both latent variable models and network analysis. stability, and predictive validity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology. https://​doi.​ https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​abn00​00661
org/​10.​1007/​s10802-​021-​00770-8 McNally, R. J. (2016). Can network analysis transform psychopathol-
Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., & Fried, E. I. (2018). Estimating psycho- ogy? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 86, 95–104. https://​doi.​
logical networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Behavior org/​10.​1016/j.​brat.​2016.​06.​006
Research Methods, 50, 195–212. Mullarkey, M. C., Marchetti, I., & Beevers, C. G. (2019). Using net-
Epskamp, S., & Fried, E. I. (2018). A tutorial on regularized partial work analysis to identify central symptoms of adolescent depres-
correlation networks. Psychological Methods, 23, 617–634. sion. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 48,
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​met00​00167 656–668. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​15374​416.​2018.​14377​35
Fried, E. I., van Borkulo, C. D., Cramer, A. O., Boschloo, L., Preszler, J., & Burns, G. L. (2019). Network analysis of ADHD and
Schoevers, R. A., & Borsboom, D. (2017). Mental disorders as net- ODD symptoms: Novel insights or redundant findings with the
works of problems: A review of recent insights. Social Psychiatry latent variable model? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47,
and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 52, 1–10. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​ 1599–1610. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10802-​019-​00549-y
s00127-​016-​1319-z Preszler, J., Burns, G. L., Becker, S. P., & Servera, M. (2020). Multi-
Garner, A. A., Mrug, S., Hodgens, B., & Patterson, C. (2013). Do source longitudinal network and latent variable model analyses
symptoms of sluggish cognitive tempo in children with ADHD of ADHD symptoms in children. Journal of Clinical Child and
symptoms represent comorbid internalizing difficulties? Journal Adolescent Psychology. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​15374​416.​2020.​
of Attention Disorders, 17, 510–518. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​ 17562​97
10870​54711​431456 Sáez, B., Servera, M., Becker, S. P., & Burns, G. L. (2019). Optimal items
Goh, P. K., Martel, M. M., & Barkley, R. A. (2020). Clarifying ADHD for assessing sluggish cognitive tempo in children across mother,
and sluggish cognitive tempo item relations with impairment: A father, and teacher ratings. Journal of Clinical Child and Adoles-
network analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 48, cent Psychology, 48, 1–15. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​15374​416.​2017.​
1047–1061. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10802-​020-​00655-2 14166​19
Goh, P. K., Smith, T. E., Lee, C. A., Bansal, P. S., Eng, A. G., & Servera, M., Saez, B., Burns, G. L., & Becker, S. P. (2018). Clinical
Martel, M. M. (2021). Etiological networks of attention-deficit/ differentiation of sluggish cognitive tempo and attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder during childhood and adolescence. Journal hyperactivity disorder in children. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. https://​doi.​org/​10.​ ogy, 127, 818–829. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​abn00​00375
1080/​15374​416.​2021.​19468​20 van Bork, R., Rhemtulla,M., Waldorp, L. J., Kruis, J., Rezvanifar, S.,
Golino, H. F., & Epskamp, S. (2017). Exploratory graph analysis: A & Borsboom, D. (2019). Latent variable models and networks:
new approach for estimating the number of dimensions in psy- Statistical equivalence and testability. Multivariate Behavioral
chological research. PLoS One, 12, e0174035. https://​doi.​org/​10.​ Research, 1–24. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​00273​171.​2019.​16725​15
1371/​journ​al.​pone.​01740​35
Haslbeck, J., & Waldorp, L. J. (2015). mgm: Estimating time-varying Publisher's Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
mixed graphical models in high-dimensional data. arXiv pre- jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
print. https://​arxiv.​org/​abs/​1510.​06871

13

View publication stats

You might also like