You are on page 1of 9

Advances in psychiatric treatment (2013), vol. 19, 259–267  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.112.

010363

Boredom among psychiatric article

in-patients: does it matter?


Rachel Steele, Paul Henderson, Frances Lennon & Donna Swinden

(Laudet 2004), in substance misuse more generally Rachel Steele is a clinical librarian
Summary with Tees, Esk and Wear Valleys
(Corvinelli 2005) and in absconding from
Many psychiatric in-patients report boredom. Such NHS Foundation Trust. Her main
psychiatric hospitals (Bowers 1999). Although interests include the emerging
complaints may appear trivial, but this litera­ture
the possible association between boredom and role of the clinical librarian/
review by a clinical librarian suggests that boredom
psychiatric disorders is complex, Newell et al informationist in leading and
is more complex than may initially appear and relates facilitating access to the evidence
to wider areas of importance for in-patient psychi­ (2012) found a correlation between depression
base in all aspects of mental health
atric teams. Boredom may relate to the internal and boredom, and Todman (2003) argues that services, as well as educational/
experience of meaning, which itself encompasses boredom may be a prodromal marker in psychosis. CPD interventions to develop
meaningful relationships and roles and a sense of The experience of boredom therefore has much health professionals’ skills in
using evidence in their practice.
control. Although meaningful therapeutic activities wider implications than may initially appear.
Paul Henderson is a consultant
are vital, mental health profes­sionals should focus psychiatrist, Frances Lennon
on the internal as well as the external dimensions Why ask a clinical librarian? is an occupational therapist and
of boredom. Medications, particu­larly dopamine Donna Swinden is a modern
antagonist anti­psychotics, may be a contributing As this article is one of many outputs from the matron, all based at the time of
factor. This article highlights the benefits of the TEWV Clinical Librarian Project, it is appropriate writing in an adult acute in-patient
to highlight briefly the rationale for the initiative. service of Tees, Esk and Wear
clinical librarian role in synthesising research
Valleys NHS Foundation Trust.
in mental health. Research can illuminate psy­ Clinical librarians have been in existence for
Correspondence  Rachel Steele,
chiatric practice in a more holistic way than purely around 40 years. They provide research summaries Library and Information Service,
by applying ‘scientific’ evidence in the practice of to support clinical decision-making and to inform Education Centre, Lanchester Road
‘biological’ psychiatry. strategy and changes. Hospital, Durham DH1 5RD, UK.
Email: rachelsteele@nhs.net
Declaration of interest There have been very few clinical librarian
None. projects in mental health. A notable exception is
a study by Gorring et al (2010), which concluded
that the clinical librarian role can be effective in
This critical litera­ture review on boredom arose out implementing evidence-based psychiatric practice
of a pilot clinical librarian/informationist project in clinical teams.
in Tees, Esk and Wear Valleys (TEWV) NHS Davidoff & Florance (2000) acknowledge the
Foundation Trust. At the time of writing, the lead contributions that clinical librarians can make in
author (R.S.) was clinical librarian/informationist stimulating access to the evidence base, pointing
for four pilot teams in the trust. One of these out that many now read the literature and write
teams includes a consultant psychiatrist (P.H.), brief synopses of papers to present on ward rounds
a modern matron (D.S.) and an occupational and at conferences.
therapist (F.L.) in an adult acute in-patient service. During the TEWV Clinical Librarian Project,
The consultant psychiatrist suggested that R.S. it soon became apparent that clinicians valued
consult the research evidence base to illuminate the role of an information specialist/librarian
in-patients’ reports of ‘boredom’. The body of this who could critically appraise and synthesise
article, including the conclusions, has been written research, as well as the more traditional role of the
by R.S., with commentaries by P.H., F.L. and D.S. healthcare librarian, which focuses on information
searching.
Is in-patient boredom important?
Boredom is a topic that has received little
Benefits and uses of evidence syntheses
empirical attention in clinical settings (Goldberg in mental health
2011). Although patients’ complaints that they are This article is an instructive example to consul­
‘bored’ may sound trivial, there is some (although tant psychiatrists of the benefits of the clinical
limited) evidence that boredom is associated with a librarian’s evidence-synthesising role, and
range of illnesses and dysfunctional behaviour, for consequently of research evidence synthesis, to
example as a trigger for relapse in dual diagnosis mental healthcare.

259
https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Steele et al

The argument that evidence-based medicine What is boredom?


is not restricted to randomised trials and meta-
Patients on many psychiatric wards report
analyses is not new (see Sackett 1996), and the
boredom (National Institute for Health and
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence
Clinical Excellence 2011). The Royal College of
(as NICE is now named) believes that all practice,
Psychiatrists (2011: p. 10) states that boredom
not just scientific approaches, should be evidence
impedes recovery and can lead to unsafe and
based. Nevertheless, there is anecdotal evidence
violent behaviour. The College has explicitly linked
that evidence-based practice is seen as more
the ‘therapeutic ward’ and structured activities
relevant for ‘biological’ psychiatry than for the
to reduction of patient boredom, arguing that
more subtle, greyer areas of clinical practice
all staff, not just occupational therapists, should
(Cooper 2003; Hannes 2010; Jainer 2011).
consider keeping patients engaged ‘as important a
These ‘greyer’ areas might include a myriad of
function as administering medication’ (p. 12).
factors relating to the holistic patient/service
user experience and to questions of diagnosis,
Situation-dependent and situation-independent
prognosis and aetiology: factors that do not so
boredom
easily lend themselves to the collection of scientific
(particularly randomised controlled trial, RCT) The notion that boredom is due to inadequate
evidence in assessing the efficacy of treatments outside stimulation is intuitively appealing. As
(particularly drug interventions) more commonly Newell (2009) argues, ‘The general consensus in
associated with evidence-based practice. psychology and occupational science literature is
Geddes et al (1996) found that the majority that boredom is a drive of low arousal’ (p. 25).
of primary interventions for psychiatric in- There are two major, non-mutually exclusive,
patients were based on ‘high-level’ experimental conceptions of boredom – situation-dependent
evidence. They did, however, note that most such boredom and situation-independent boredom
interventions were pharmacological and that (Todman 2003). Proponents of situation-dependent
RCT evidence is more likely to be available in this boredom believe that boredom is caused by
area than for non-pharmacological approaches. monotonous environmental conditions, that once
However, our study shows how non-RCT evidence these are removed, boredom will be alleviated, and
(mainly qualitative research, cross-sectional that individual differences in boredom reflect a
studies, case studies and a literature review) can differing level of vulnerability to environ­mental
also have an impact on holistic patient care and stimuli, with ‘high-risk’ individuals employing
services. The study’s search strategy is outlined certain (usually maladaptive) behaviours or traits.
in Box 1. In contrast, proponents of situation-independent
boredom (who tend to be psychoanalytically
oriented or existential theorists) argue that bore­
dom ‘results from an inability to utilize the coping
Box 1 The boredom search strategy
mechanisms needed to provide escape from mono­
R.S. searched bibliographic databases The challenge in setting inclusion and tony […] (i.e. fantasy, anticipation, etc.)’, which is
(AMED, BNI, CINAHL, the Cochrane Library, exclusion criteria was to provide a broad independent of environmental conditions (Todman
Embase, MEDLINE and PsycINFO) using enough perspective to give a useful 2003: p. 149).
‘boredom’ and synonymous keywords (e.g. overview but to be related enough to in- Situation-dependent boredom is therefore a
‘monotony’) and search strings relating to patient psychiatry to be of practical use. The view of boredom as ‘externally’ driven, whereas
in-patient psychiatry, such as (ward* OR initial inclusion criteria were that papers
situation-independent boredom emphasises
inpatient* OR in-patient* OR hospital* OR had to consider boredom in its entirety in
‘internal’ factors. There is no clear consensus
acute) AND (psych* OR mental*). Database the in-patient context. The review was
thesaurus terms relating to boredom or then widened to cover boredom and adult
on whether situation-dependent or situation-
in-patient psychiatry were combined with mental illness more globally (although independent theories of boredom are more
keyword searches. excluding older adult populations), because accurate. The bulk of our article, however, follows
Supplementary literature searches included a tight focus on the in-patient setting omits Binnema’s (2004) ‘internal’, sociological view of
keywords and thesaurus terms relating to theoretical frameworks which can illuminate boredom because his theoretical proposition is
therapeutic groups in in-patient psychiatry, broader understanding. more closely based on the psychiatric in-patient
the third (voluntary) sector providing in- There is a separate evidence base which has population than most other research reports.
reach into psychiatric hospitals, in-patient considered leisure or occupational boredom, However, in a wider sense it is helpful, as Newell
‘hearing-voices’ approaches, and medica- but this was not included in the study. (2009: p. 25) believes, to recognise that sources
tions and boredom. References identified by of stimulation can be either internal or external
electronic searching were consulted. to the individual, and therefore that there are
internal and external components of boredom.

260 Advances in psychiatric treatment (2013), vol. 19, 259–267  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363
https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Boredom among psychiatric in-patients

Boredom is therefore more complex than simply


‘not having enough to do.’
Box 2 A service user’s experience of the therapeutic benefits of activity-
based groups in in-patient units
The ‘internal’ experience of meaning ‘One of the main problems for people with The Thai boxing classes and mindfulness
and boredom mental health issues is social exclusion. sessions were exceptionally helpful to me.
Often met with stigma, our needs to Thanks to the hospital I was able to use
Boredom and the experience of meaning develop and maintain jobs, relationships, the Thai boxing classes to beat out the
A key piece of evidence in relation to psychiatric interests and other roles can lead to negativity stored up and replace it with
patients’ experiences of boredom is Binnema’s rejection causing a breakdown in our daily positive thinking by applying mindfulness.
(2004) literature review. Binnema argues that lives. I believe that the activities provided I am truly grateful for the help and support
by the hospital are essential tools in from the activities team, who became a
boredom is an emotion interconnected with the
counterattacking this breakdown. huge part of my treatment.
experience of meaning. It should be noted that this
internally driven theory is one of several competing The opportunity to participate in activity- Since my discharge I have become a
based groups in the hospital setting is member at a mixed martial arts gym where
theories of boredom. According to Binnema’s
the first step towards social inclusion. I train in Brazilian ju-jitsu and Thai boxing.
view, complaints of boredom represent patients’
In my experience it has been the This is something I would never have done
emotional response to a decreased sense of meaning.
foundation to rebuilding my confidence had it not been for the activities team
The notion that boredom is related to ‘life meaning’ and self-esteem. Physical activities have a introducing me to the sport and helping me
(i.e. having a purpose in life) is supported by phenomenal effect on our minds as well as to recognise my potential.’
Fahlman (2009), who found that boredom and life our bodies. (Published with permission of the service user)
meaning share a closer relation­ship with each other
than with depression or anxiety in undergraduates.
Binnema identifies three interrelated concepts
Being in meaningful relationships
which he argues are central to the experience of
meaning in life: Binnema (2004) asserts that social relationships
are empowering and help create experiences of
•• being in meaningful relationships
meaning, but that psychiatric in-patients have few
•• having meaningful roles
opportunities for meaningful social interactions.
•• having a sense of control.
The significance of social relationships is
The centrality of these dimensions to the experience supported by Thomas et al ’s (2002) qualitative
of meaning is highlighted in Box 2, in which a study of eight in-patients. All of the participants
service user links participation in therapeutic thought that ‘therapy’ administered by their peers
groups to decreased stigma and promotion of was the most beneficial aspect of their time in
social inclusion. This individual also found hospital, and they ‘expressed longing for a deeper
mindfulness helpful, which ties in with another connection with staff’. Evidence has also been put
internally driven theory of boredom currently forward by NICE that some in-patients feel they
gaining interest within cognitive neuroscience – receive more support from fellow patients than
that boredom is associated with inattention. staff (National Institute for Health and Clinical
Eastwood et al (2012) believe that boredom Excellence 2011).
should be defined in terms of attention, and Mal­ Thomas et al ’s findings are supported by
kovsky et al  (2012) have explored the relationship Higgins et al ’s (1999) study of acute psychiatric
between sustained attention and proneness to wards, which reported that nurses’ activity was
boredom. The Malkovsky team identify two types focused on a minority of severely ill patients,
of boredom: apathetic, where the person is un­ which significantly limited their opportunity
concerned with their environment; and agitated, to implement coordinated programmes of care
where the person tries to engage in meaningful for all patients. Although patients valued their
activities but these fail to satisfy. In their study of named nurse’s support, many had ‘only passing
undergraduates, the team found that proneness to relationships with nurses’, were bored and ‘felt
apathetic boredom was associated with attention they were often left to their own devices’.
lapses, and proneness to agitated boredom was Nevertheless, there are examples of healthcare
associated with reduced sensitivity to errors of professionals addressing in-patients’ needs for
sustained attention and increased symptoms of meaningful relationships. In their report on in-
adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. patient nurses using Romme & Escher’s approach
Martin (2008: p. 154) theorises that boredom is to help voice-hearers develop a narrative account
associated with a lack of mindfulness and argues of their voices, Place et al (2011: p. 841) note the
that ‘it might be possible to overcome a propensity strong relationship that develops between nurse
to boredom through learning how to be mindful’. and patient. Ruddle et al  (2011) argue that group

Advances in psychiatric treatment (2013), vol. 19, 259–267  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 261
https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Steele et al

treatment offers advantages over individual Caddy et al  (2011) reported that participation in
therapy for voice-hearers. Place et al ’s successful creative activity groups positively correlated with
therapeutic partnership approach could therefore improved mental health in an acute in-patient
be replicated in ‘hearing-voices groups’ which psychiatric hospital in Australia. However, they
could address in-patients’ needs for meaningful pointed out that not everyone finds such groups
relationships with staff and other patients. meaningful or beneficial, so group allocation
should reflect the needs of individual patients
Having meaningful roles (p. 331).
There may also be a role for partner organis­
Binnema (2004: p. 839) argues that offering
ations who could ‘in-reach’ to meet patients’
psychiatric patients the opportunity to take on
needs for meaningful activities while in hospital
meaningful roles while in hospital may give them
and provide continuity of therapeutic input after
a sense of meaning and therefore empowerment.
discharge. There are limited examples of partner/
Such roles could be provided through purposeful
voluntary organisations working in this way, but
activities. Indeed, NICE stresses that psychiatric
Thomas et al (2012) report a successful voluntary
in-patients should have access to ‘meaningful
activities programme that complements statutory
and culturally appropriate’ activities (National
child and adolescent mental health services. The
Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence 2011:
service user whose experiences are described in
p. 118). Antoniou’s (2007) experience as a service
Box 2 also highlights the benefits of continuing
user supports Binnema’s assertion that boredom
therapeutic activities pre- and post-discharge.
may be linked to the absence of meaningful roles
or roles that patients are used to when not in
Having a sense of control
hospital. She writes:
The need of patients to have a say in the activities
‘There are two sorts of “boring” on the ward. One
and occupations offered on the wards has been
is caused by having lots of hours to fill and not
having the things one usually has to fill them up recognised by NICE (National Institute for
with because one is not at home […] It is possible to Health and Clinical Excellence 2011). Binnema
be bored on the ward even when there are activities (2004) likewise argues that ‘Loss of control […]
planned every hour, and not to be bored when doing is related to decreased mental health. Conversely,
nothing’ (Antoniou 2007: p. 33).
empower­ment […] and thus decreased boredom,
This argument is potentially relevant to ward- is related to increased mental health’ (p. 836). He
based group activities. Simpson et al (2005) acknowledges that loss of control is intrinsically
reported that some nurses, ward managers and related to the nature of psychiatric illnesses, but
psychiatrists saw the occupational therapist’s also argues that authoritarian psychiatric nursing
role as largely focused on group activities: ‘Often may encourage patient dependency. The challenge
these were seen as predominantly diversionary is to empower patients by harnessing the potential
or time-filling activities, with the more explicitly of the hospital therapeutic environment.
therapeutic functions […] rarely acknowledged’ Farnworth et al (2004) cite the example of
(p. 547–8). Newell (2009: p. 26) believes that a service user who was dissatisfied with ward
organised group activities deal only with external occupations but felt that he had learnt how to gain
components of boredom, so the provision of more control over his time. Interestingly, they note that
activities does not necessarily reduce complaints he was the only participant interviewed who said
of boredom. that he was ‘quite happy with [his] life’ (p. 435).
If activities do not provide opportunities to If a lack of control is related to boredom, we
experience meaningful roles, then it is unlikely might expect there to be an association between
that they will exert their full therapeutic effect. autonomous activities and reduced boredom.
Farnworth et al  (2004), in a naturalistic inquiry Indeed, Newell et al  (2012) reported that, although
study involving eight patients in an Australian there was an association between in-patients’
secure forensic psychiatric unit, found that engagement in autonomous activity and decreased
popular groups offered ‘a specific purpose and self-reports of boredom, ‘There was no evidence of
outcome’ or encompassed ‘novelty, creativity or any relationship between boredom proneness and
nonverbal components to performance’ (p. 435). organised activities’ (p. 6).
For example, the ‘cook your own meal’ group had Given the importance of autonomy, Newell
a waiting list. However, many of the activities et al (2012) hypothesised that involuntary in-
on offer seemed to lack ‘novelty, challenge and patients would be more prone to boredom.
personal meaning’ and patients thought them However, their study indicated the opposite. A
boring or ‘old hat’ (p. 435). potential confounder was that more voluntary

262 Advances in psychiatric treatment (2013), vol. 19, 259–267  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363
https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Boredom among psychiatric in-patients

than involuntary patients were diagnosed with may contribute to a lack of drive, and consequently
depression and ‘depression is highly correlated boredom, in people with schizophrenia because
with proneness to boredom’. We return to the of the nature of the negative symptoms of the
complex relationship between depression and disorder. However, studies of healthy volunteers
boredom in ‘Associations between boredom and offer indications. Artaloytia et al (2006) reported
psychiatric diagnoses’, below. that single doses of haloperidol, and particularly
As numerous authors (e.g. Simpson 2005; Hitch of risperidone, produced negative symptoms in
2009) argue, occupational therapists are often healthy volunteers. Park et al (2012) likewise found
dismayed to receive referrals of patients who that a single dose of risperidone induced negative
are ‘bored’, believing that this shows a limited symptoms (including increased avolition), although
understanding of their role. Shattell (2007) also they concluded that haloperidol, amisulpride and
notes that many nurses react negatively to patients’ aripiprazole did not.
boredom, saying that their role is not ‘to entertain It is therefore worth considering medication as
patients’. However, Newell (2009: p. 27) believes a possible contributor to boredom when reviewing
that ultimately ‘we should be helping our service drug choice and dosage.
users to address boredom for themselves’.
Associations between boredom and psychiatric
Medication and boredom diagnoses
In examining boredom, it is difficult to disentangle Newell et al (2012) investigated proneness to
the effects of medication from those of psychiatric boredom in a cross-sectional sample of 55
illness, but there is some evidence that medication psychiatric in-patients. The study should be
could be a contributing factor independent of psy­ regarded as preliminary because of its design
chiatric disorder. Healey (2009) notes that almost and small sample, but as far as we know it is the
all anti­depressants ‘can be somewhat sedative’ and only study that has investigated the prevalence
that with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors of boredom in adult psychiatric in-patients. The
(SSRIs) ‘there may be a paradoxical coexistence of authors concluded that the three variables most
feeling drowsy or fatigued’ (p. 70). He also points predictive of boredom were having depression,
out that the sedative effect of all benzodiazepines anxiety or perhaps a personality disorder.
except clobazam is well documented (p. 154). If The possible association/relationship between
we consider theories of situation-dependent bore­ boredom and psychiatric disorders is complex.
dom, which stress the centrality of environmental Clinicians may mistake boredom for depression
conditions, then the sedative/attention-impairing or negative symptoms of schizophrenia, but
effects of these medications could contribute to boredom does seem to be a distinct phenomenon.
patients’ experiences of environmental sameness Goldberg et al (2011), in a cross-sectional sample
and thus feelings of boredom. Internally driven of under­g raduates, reported higher correlations
theories positing a link between boredom and between boredom and depression than between
in­attention may also be significant (Eastwood bore­dom and apathy or anhedonia, but found that
2012; Malkovsky 2012). If medications induce depression and boredom were empirically distinct.
sedation, and consequently impair attention or Todman (2003) argues, on the basis of clinical
concentration, this may also be a contributor to and non-clinical empirical studies and case studies
boredom. of out-patients with psychosis, that boredom may
The antipsychotics are also a potentially be a prodromal marker in psychosis but that this
significant contributor because of their blocking may work in a heterogeneous and sometimes
of the D2 dopamine receptor. The mesolimbic non-intuitive manner. He argues against the
dopamine pathway is important for motivation, implicit assumption of the vulnerability–stress–
pleasure and reward, as well as the positive coping model of schizophrenia, i.e. that external
symptoms of psychosis (Stahl 2008: p. 272). Wise stimulation should be reduced. His reasoning is
(2004) concludes that ‘brain dopamine seems to that, although some patients may be motivated
“stamp-in” response–reward and stimulus–reward to avoid excessive stimulation, others (especially
associations that are essential for the control of in partially or fully remitted states) may be as
motivated behaviour by past experience’ (p. motivated to avoid unstimulating environments
10). Flaherty (2011) also argues that ‘Creativity as non-psychotic individuals. However, as this
depends on goal-driven approach motivation from theoretical assertion is based on single cases
midbrain dopaminergic systems’ (p. 132). (albeit with reference to prior research), further
It is difficult to determine the extent to which investigation is needed about the potential value
the dopamine antagonist effects of antipsychotics of boredom as a prodromal marker in psychosis.

Advances in psychiatric treatment (2013), vol. 19, 259–267  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 263
https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Steele et al

Implications for practice in adult acute questions that arise during MDT discussions. The
in-patient services exploration of boredom is a particular example of
this, as we have an extensive activity programme
Specialist commentary by Paul Henderson, for our acute wards, with a variety of group and
consultant psychiatrist individual sessions, plus an on-site gymnasium.
Before commenting on the clinical content of the Our intensive support team also access community
article, I think it is important to highlight the facilities, so we were rather puzzled when we often
TEWV Clinical Librarian Project in general. As received negative feedback citing lack of activities.
a newly qualified consultant working in a busy We regularly ask during patient ward meetings
general adult ward, the variety of presentations whether there are any other activities that patients
I see, the rapidity with which they present, and would like to participate in, but we receive very
the multitude of pressures on my time mean that few suggestions. This led us to consider a number
it has simply not been feasible to be as up to of explanations – for example, lack of usual
date across as wide a variety of topics as I would routine, that illness may prevent patients engaging
like. This has probably been the greatest source in activities, or that some people will be bored
of professional and personal discomfort in my whatever activities are on offer.
short consultant career thus far. As an addition The evidence base produced by the clinical
to the multidisciplinary team (MDT), the clinical librarian confirms to some degree our suspicions
librarian has made a significant impact in this and led to some useful discussions of how we
area, both for myself and the wider team, with might address the problem of boredom. It has
junior colleagues, nursing staff, occupational prompted the team to make more use of the
therapists and psychologists all making use of patient workbook so that patients can state their
the service she provides. When compared with all own interests and ideas of what would help them.
the other aspects of clinical governance the MDT We have also implemented a handover between
practises, I have no doubt that the addition of the nursing staff and intensive support staff every
clinical librarian to the team has made the single morning making use of the patient’s workbook,
largest improvement. so that intensive support staff are better informed
My motivation to ask the clinical librarian as to what activities/interventions might be best
to provide an evidence summary on boredom for the individual on that particular day. Our
was that I had become increasingly aware of the intensive support team also monitor the uptake
apparent dichotomy that ward staff appear to be of activities, recording when patients have been
constantly rushing, but many patients complain of offered an activity and have declined it. We are
being bored; and that, because of time pressures, waiting to see whether our patient experience
my own practice was becoming more and more surveys will reflect these changes. We have also
focused on the biological aspects of treatment. In introduced more formalised protected therapeutic
the context of constant pressure to promote a more time so that patients can have more one-to-one
therapeutic environment, with a view to shorter time with the nursing team. We managed this by
hospital stays, it appeared to me that a broader limiting MDT meetings to specific times of the
perspective would be helpful. day. This was introduced at the start of 2012 and
While I am not suggesting that a sole focus on is working well.
reducing patients’ boredom is sufficient, or indeed A further benefit of the TEWV Clinical Librarian
necessary, the richness and nuances of the con­ Project is that the whole team, from nursing
cepts in this article have certainly provided fresh assistant through to consultant psychiatrist,
and interesting ideas for the whole in-patient MDT become involved in discussions and generate ideas.
to work on together. The insights into internal This has enabled staff who do not traditionally
and external factors, as well as the probable access research, or who do not have the time to do
superiority of autonomously driven activities, as so, to become familiar with literature reviews and
opposed to generic group activities, have certainly experience their usefulness.
significantly informed the ceaseless process of
change that we are engaged in. Specialist commentary by Frances Lennon,
occupational therapist
Specialist commentary by Donna Swinden, We would not usually see our own activities as
modern matron simply ‘occupying’ or ‘filling in’ time. We can be
The TEWV Clinical Librarian Project has been just as ‘bored’ when doing an activity that has no
very beneficial to the clinical ward teams as it has meaning or purpose to us as doing nothing at all.
enabled us to quickly access the evidence base for Occupational therapists aim to empower people

264 Advances in psychiatric treatment (2013), vol. 19, 259–267  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363
https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Boredom among psychiatric in-patients

to develop skills to plan their own time in a


meaningful, individual way, and to transfer these
Box 3 Team approaches to alleviating boredom
skills into everyday life. Internal aspects of boredom • Use Romme & Escher’s voice-hearing
Occupational therapist assessment and treatment approach to provide meaningful
• Use a patient workbook to coordinate a
includes graded use of activities (i.e. facilitating personalised activity plan: meaningful therapeutic interventions for individual
activities at an appropriate level in the person’s activities can help patients experience patients who hear voices
journey) to reduce and gain control of mental meaningful roles • Facilitate peer networks
health symptoms and to support recovery. We work
• Use the workbooks daily to plan individual External aspects of boredom
jointly with clients in developing their skills for
and group activities (and involve all staff
life. Graded use of activities on leave can promote • Offer popular groups that are meaningful
groups)
daily living skills and can counteract the reduced to individuals
• Monitor the uptake of activities and Use a graded approach to activities (i.e.
opportunity for usual routines when on the ward. •
review patients’ views on relevance and facilitate activities at an appropriate level
Linking with community resources relevant to
satisfaction in the patient’s journey)
the individual can assist in the transition between
hospital and home. • Protect one-to-one therapeutic time • Use Romme & Escher’s voice-hearing
Therapeutic groups on and off the ward are with patients by reducing meetings and approach to facilitate therapeutic groups
administration for voice-hearers
aimed at helping people overcome mental health
symptoms at each stage of being acutely unwell. • Regularly review potential negative • Partner with third-sector (voluntary-sector)
These groups should not be characterised as effects of medication on motivation and groups to increase the variety of activities
‘give them something to do’. ‘Doing’ needs to be concentration available from the ward
meaningful, purposeful and individually relevant.
If it is not, the groups will only address external
dimensions of boredom.
that boredom as a phenomenon distinct from
The TEWV Clinical Librarian Project has
psychiatric illness is worth researching. Further
enabled a reflective step back to review what we
original research (particularly with a longitudinal
do and how we do it, to confirm that something
design and larger samples) is needed to investigate
works, or to plan change.
boredom on adult psychiatric wards. It would be
especially helpful if studies could investigate:
Conclusions
associations/relationships between boredom and
Boredom among in-patients psychiatric diagnosis; the efficacy of psychosocial
Boredom among psychiatric in-patients is consid­ interventions designed to alleviate in-patients’
erably more complex than simply a response experiences of boredom; and the extent to which
to a lack of external stimulation that can best medications (particularly dopamine antagonist
be tack­­led by diversionary activities. Boredom antipsychotics) may be a contributor to boredom,
relates to the experience of meaning, which itself
encompasses relationships, roles and a sense of
control. Furthermore, medication, particularly Box 4 Key learning points for promoting a therapeutic environment that
antipsychotics that block dopamine (which is im­ addresses patients’ boredom in a holistic way
portant for experiencing motivation, pleasure and • Be aware that there are internal and the experience of having a sense of
reward), may contribute to it. external dimensions to boredom. Boredom control
Meaningful therapeutic activities are vital, but may be due to a lack of stimulation • Generic ‘diversionary’ activities (either
mental health professionals should be aware of (external) but is also likely to be caused by individual or group based) are likely to
the internal and external dimensions of boredom. a lack of experience of meaning (internal) address only the external dimensions of
These can be linked, for example by a recognition • ‘Internal’ and ‘external’ approaches to the boredom, so may not provide long-lasting
that activities (external) are important, but are treatment of boredom can be combined, solutions
more likely to be successful if patients’ needs for for example by recognising that (external) • All staff, not just occupational therapists,
autonomy (internal) and meaning (internal) are activities are important but they should should work together to maintain a
met. Box 3 gives some practical suggestions for be (internally) meaningful for patients and therapeutic environment to keep patients
alleviating boredom on the ward. Box 4 outlines autonomously driven engaged and alleviate boredom
the key clinical learning points of this article, • The lack of experience of meaning is likely • Medications (particularly antipsychotics
which have implications for multi­d isciplinary to encompass three dimensions: that block dopamine) may contribute to
adult acute in-patient ward teams. the experience of being in meaningful boredom
relationships • Clinicians may mistake boredom for
Further research the experience of being in meaningful depression or negative symptoms of
The possible association between boredom and roles schizophrenia
psychiatric disorders is complex, but it does seem

Advances in psychiatric treatment (2013), vol. 19, 259–267  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 265
https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Steele et al

independent of the psychiatric conditions they are Hannes K, Pieters G, Goedhuys J, et al (2010) Exploring barriers to the
MCQ answers implementation of evidence-based practice in psychiatry to inform health
designed to treat.
1 e 2 b 3 a 4 e 5 a policy: a focus group based study. Community Mental Health Journal
46: 423–32.
The role of clinical librarian and the complexity of Healey D (2009) Psychiatric Drugs Explained. Churchill Livingstone.
‘evidence’
Higgins R, Hurst K, Wistow G (1999) Nursing acute psychiatric patients:
By developing the role of the clinical librarian as a quantitative and qualitative study. Journal of Advanced Nursing 25:
evidence synthesiser, the full potential of ‘evidence’ 833–42.
(which uses a multiplicity of research designs, not Hitch T (2009) Thoughts on… Boredom, mental health inpatients and
just ‘scientific’ approaches) to illuminate clinical occupational therapy by Sue Newell. Mental Health Occupational
Therapy 14: 58.
practice can be realised.
Jainer AK, Teelukdharry SR, Onalaja D, et al (2011) Understanding the
obstacles to evidence based practice in psychiatry: qualitative study of
Acknowledgements staff perceptions. International Medical Journal 18: 163–6.
R.S. would like to thank Catherine Ebenezer and Laudet AB, Magura S, Vogel HS, et al (2004) Perceived reasons for
Bryan O’Leary for their support and encourage­ substance misuse among persons with a psychiatric disorder. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry 74: 365–75.
ment in the writing of this article. All four authors
Malkovsky E, Merrifield C, Goldberg Y, et al (2012) Exploring the
thank the service user who kindly provided a relationship between boredom and sustained attention. Experimental
patient perspective in Box 2, and Advances peer Brain Research 221: 59–67.
reviewers for their valuable comments. Martin M (2008) The phenomenon of boredom and its relationship to
mindfulness. British Journal of Occupational Therapy 71: 154.
References National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (2011) Service User
Antoniou J (2007) Bored on the ward. In Experiences of Mental Health Experience in Adult Mental Health: Improving the Experience of Care for
In-Patient Care: Narratives from Service Users, Carers and Professionals People Using Adult NHS Mental Health Services (NICE Clinical Guideline
(eds M Hardcastle, D Kennard, S Grandison, et al): 33–40. Routledge. 136). NICE.
Artaloytia JF, Arango C, Lahti A, et al (2006) Negative signs and symptoms Newell S (2009) Boredom, mental health inpatients and occupational
secondary to antipsychotics: a double-blind, randomized trial of a single therapy. Mental Health Occupational Therapy 14: 25–7.
dose of placebo, haloperidol, and risperidone in healthy volunteers.
Newell SE, Harries P, Ayers S (2012) Boredom proneness in a psychiatric
American Journal of Psychiatry 163: 488–93.
inpatient population.  International Journal of Social Psychiatry 58:
Binnema DJ (2004) Interrelations of psychiatric patient experiences of 488–95.
boredom and mental health. Issues in Mental Health Nursing 25: 833–42.
Park CH, Park TW, Yang JC, et al (2012) No negative symptoms in
Bowers L, Jarrett M, Clark N, et al (1999) Absconding: why patients healthy volunteers after single doses of amisulpride, aripiprazole, and
leave. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 6: 199–205. haloperidol: a double-blinded placebo-controlled trial. International
Caddy L, Crawford F, Page AC (2011) ‘Painting a path to wellness’: Clinical Psychopharmacology 27: 114–20.
correlations between participating in a creative activity group and Place C, Foxcroft R, Shaw J (2011) Telling stories and hearing voices:
improved measured mental health outcomes. Journal of Psychiatric and narrative work with voice hearers in acute care. Journal of Psychiatric
Mental Health Nursing 19: 327–33. and Mental Health Nursing 18: 837–42.
Cooper B (2003) Evidence-based mental health policy: a critical appraisal. Royal College of Psychiatrists (2011) Do the Right Thing: How to Judge
British Journal of Psychiatry 183: 105–13. a Good Ward. Ten Standards for Adult In-Patient Mental Healthcare
Corvinelli A (2005) Alleviating boredom in adult males recovering from (Occasional Paper OP79). Royal College of Psychiatrists.
substance use disorder. Occupational Therapy in Mental Health 21: 1–11. Ruddle A, Mason O, Wykes T (2011) A review of hearing voices groups:
Davidoff F, Florance V (2000) The informationist: a new health evidence and mechanisms of change. Clinical Psychology Review 31:
professional? Annals of Internal Medicine 132: 996–8. 757–66.
Eastwood J, Frischen A, Fenske M, et al (2012) The unengaged mind: Sackett D, Rosenberg W, Muir Gray J, et al (1996) Evidence based
defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological medicine: what it is and what it isn’t. BMJ  312: 71.
Science 7: 482–95.
Shattell M (2007) Boredom in acute psychiatric care. Issues in Mental
Fahlman SA, Mercer KB, Gaskovski P, et al (2009) Does a lack of life Health Nursing 28: 661–2.
meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and
experimental analyses. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 28: Simpson A, Bowers L, Alexander J, et al (2005) Occupational therapy
307–40. and multidisciplinary working on acute psychiatric wards: the
Tompkins Acute Ward Study. British Journal of Occupational Therapy
Farnworth L, Nikitin L, Fossey E (2004) Being in a secure forensic 68: 545–52.
psychiatric unit: every day is the same, killing time or making the most
of it. British Journal of Occupational Therapy 67: 430–8. Stahl SM (2008) Stahl’s Essential Psychopharmacology: Neuroscientific
Basis and Practical Applications. Cambridge.
Flaherty AW (2011) Brain illness and creativity: mechanisms and
treatment risks. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry  56: 132–43. Thomas N, Pilgrim D, Street C, et al (2012) Supporting young people with
mental health problems: lessons from a voluntary sector pilot. Mental
Geddes J, Game D, Jenkins N, et al (1996) What proportion of primary Health Review Journal 17: 14–25.
psychiatric interventions are based on evidence from randomised
controlled trials? Quality in Health Care 5: 215–7. Thomas S, Shattell M, Martin T (2002) What’s therapeutic about the
therapeutic milieu. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing 16: 99–107.
Goldberg YK, Eastwood JD, Laguardia J, et al (2011) Boredom: an
emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia or depression. Todman M (2003) Boredom and psychotic disorders: cognitive and
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 30: 647–66. motivational issues. Psychiatry 66: 146–67.
Gorring H, Turner E, Day E, et al (2010) A clinical librarian pilot project in Wise RA (2004) Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature Reviews
psychiatry. Psychiatrist 34: 65–8. Neuroscience  5: 483–94.

266 Advances in psychiatric treatment (2013), vol. 19, 259–267  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363
https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Boredom among psychiatric in-patients

MCQs 3 Which of these statements most d protecting one-to-one therapeutic time by


Select the single best option for each question stem accurately describes how antipsychotics reducing meetings and administration
may be a significant contributor to e all of the above.
1 Alleviating patient bore­dom in adult acute
boredom?
psychiatric wards is the function of:
a Antipsychotics block dopamine, a 5 Which of the following statements best
a occupational therapists
neurotransmitter which is important for describes the role of a clinical librarian?
b nurses
motivation, pleasure and reward. a Clinical librarians facilitate access to the
c psychiatrists
b Antipsychotics stimulate dopamine, a evidence base to support clinical decision-
d psychologists
neurotransmitter which is important for making and to inform strategy and changes.
e all of the above.
motivation, pleasure and reward. b Clinical librarians are exclusively concerned
c It is impossible to say whether antipsychotics with teaching evidence-based practice skills to
2 Regarding the organisation of activities on could be a boredom contributor because no health professionals.
psychiatric wards: research studies have separated their effects c Clinical librarians are well established in uk
a patients need their time to be filled from the negative symptoms of schizophrenia. mental health services.
b generic ‘diversionary’ activities are likely d Risperidone is the antipsychotic which is least d Clinical librarians only support the practice of
to address only the external dimensions of likely to be a potential boredom contributor. hospital consultants.
boredom e Haloperidol is the antipsychotic which is most e Clinical librarians can only provide evidence
c it does not matter whether or not activities likely to be a potential boredom contributor. summaries to assist with clinical decision-
stimulate patients’ perceptions of a sense of making.
control 4 A successful team approach to boredom
d generic ‘diversionary’ activities are likely might involve:
to address only the internal dimensions of a using a patient workbook to coordinate a
boredom personalised activity plan
e it does not matter whether activities provide b using a graded approach to activities
patients with an opportunity to occupy c regularly reviewing negative effects of
meaningful roles. medication on motivation and concentration

Advances in psychiatric treatment (2013), vol. 19, 259–267  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 267
https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.112.010363 Published online by Cambridge University Press

You might also like