You are on page 1of 26

r Academy of Management Journal

2018, Vol. 61, No. 1, 220–244.


https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2016.0315

COLLABORATIVE STAKEHOLDER ENGAGEMENT: AN


INTEGRATION BETWEEN THEORIES OF ORGANIZATIONAL
LEGITIMACY AND LEARNING
VINIT M. DESAI
University of Colorado Denver

Organizations often collaborate with stakeholders such as customers, communities, and


other groups to pursue shared goals, and these partnerships are known to affect an
organization’s legitimacy with those groups as well as its access to information from
them. While these concerns could be examined within each of their own independent
literatures, existing theories are ill equipped to handle this process in tandem. Thus,
studying these collaborations provides an opportunity to more broadly explore how
organizations balance knowledge search or exploration efforts with their needs to
manage organizational legitimacy. Accordingly, I suggest that collaboration facilitates
access to external information, and that organizations pursue it when the information is
needed to solve related problems. However, I also argue that collaborations reciprocally
allow stakeholders to more directly scrutinize organizational practices. Thus, I predict
that organizations suppress their use of collaborations to solve problems when their
legitimacy is potentially or already at risk, such as when powerful stakeholders can
hold an organization accountable for inappropriate acts or when some organizational
actions have already been deemed as controversial. I test related predictions on the
community policing efforts of a law enforcement agency panel, finding general support
for several of the study’s predictions.

It is well known that organizations often collaborate distribute new products, engage in social media
or work directly with external stakeholders to pursue campaigns or viral marketing efforts, crowdsource for
goals that would otherwise be difficult to achieve in- new ideas or novel information, collectively create or
ternally. These partnerships come in a wide variety of refine software, manage their reputation among dis-
forms, ranging from strategic alliances with peer or- tributed outsiders, or facilitate access to external re-
ganizations (Haleblian & Finkelstein, 1999; Sampson, sources, as well as for a large variety of other reasons
2005), or joint arrangements with trade associations (e.g., Estelles-Arolas & Gonzalez-Ladron-de-Guevara,
and other agencies (Hiatt & Park, 2013; Julian, Ofori- 2012; Julian et al., 2008; Lerner & Tirole, 2002).
Dankwa, & Justis, 2008), to collaborative engage- A commonly studied motive for these forms of col-
ments with more distributed groups of stakeholders laborative engagement involves gaining, maintaining,
such as customers, communities, and other third- or repairing organizational legitimacy (Lamin &
party groups (Estelles-Arolas & Gonzalez-Ladron-de- Zaheer, 2012; Oliver, 1991; Suchman, 1995). Here,
Guevara, 2012; Lerner & Tirole, 2002). Of these, the “legitimacy” refers to generalized perceptions or as-
latter efforts—to engage with broad and dispersed sumptions that “the actions of an entity are desirable,
groups of outsiders—are uniquely interesting, given proper, or appropriate within some socially con-
their diversity and increasing prevalence in practice. structed system of norms, values, beliefs, and defi-
For instance, organizations may collaborate with nitions” (Suchman, 1995: 574). Much research
customers and other diverse groups to market and recognizes that organizations typically depend on
external stakeholder groups for access to vital re-
sources, and that outside observers are more likely
I am extremely thankful for valuable feedback on this
to supply these resources to organizations that ap-
project received from participants of the 2016 Carnegie
School of Organizational Learning conference. I am also pear legitimate (Crilly, Zollo, & Hansen, 2012;
grateful for invaluable assistance from Shelley Hyland DiMaggio & Powell, 1983; Lamin & Zaheer, 2012).
(U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, While a myriad of strategies exist for managing or-
Bureau of Justice Statistics) for fielding numerous ques- ganizational legitimacy (e.g., Julian et al., 2008; Oliver,
tions related to the data used in this project. 1991; Suchman, 1995), collaborative engagements with
220
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s express
written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.
2018 Desai 221

groups such as customers and community organiza- external information through them (e.g., Baum &
tions provide greater transparency and allow these Dahlin, 2007; Cyert & March, 1963; Gavetti, Greve,
outsiders to more effectively monitor and evaluate Levinthal, & Ocasio, 2012). In contrast to existing
organizational activities, perhaps conferring espe- conceptualizations of organizational search (Cyert &
cially enduring legitimacy to organizations with so- March, 1963; Levitt & March, 1988), organizations
cially desirable practices (Hawdon, 2008; Sinha, that seek new information through these external
Daellenbach, & Bednarek, 2015). Conversely, many collaborations are likely to consider the implications
examples exist of organizations with socially un- for organizational legitimacy when forging these
desirable practices that attempt to preserve their partnerships (Pache & Santos, 2013; Sinha et al.,
legitimacy by concealing those activities from out- 2015; Suchman, 1995). This is an important distinc-
siders, again suggesting the relevance of open tion, as little research on organizational search—and
collaboration—or its avoidance—to organizational on organizational learning more generally—examines
legitimacy (Crilly et al., 2012; Meyer & Rowan, the potential risks posed by search or other learning
1977; Piazza & Perretti, 2015). efforts (Barnett & Hansen, 1996; Yang, Phelps, &
However, notwithstanding the importance of or- Steensma, 2010), and even less work examines
ganizational legitimacy to this process, another mo- whether organizations might temper their search ef-
tive may also prompt external collaboration (Roloff, forts based on these risks. Studying collaborative en-
2008; Roome & Wijen, 2006). Specifically, organi- gagement with external stakeholders provides the
zations may collaborate or engage with these exter- opportunity to blend related but typically distinct ar-
nal groups in order to access previously unavailable guments from research on organizational search and
information that is presumably useful for the orga- organizational legitimacy, and to examine whether and
nization, such as through crowdsourcing (Estelles- how organizations balance their searches for novel
Arolas & Gonzalez-Ladron-de-Guevara, 2012), open information with considerations regarding the related
source development (Lerner & Tirole, 2002), or other risks that these external searches might pose.
approaches (Morabito, 2010; Roine, Ohinmaa, & Thus, I predict that external collaborations benefit
Hailey, 2001). Indeed, a large body of research in organizational search efforts by increasing the orga-
the organizational learning tradition suggests that nization’s access to outside knowledge, and I adapt
organizations pursue external information to refine search theories to suggest that organizations pursue
their internal routines or procedures, amend ap- these collaborations when attempting to solve
proaches, and expand their repository of knowledge problems involving this external information (Baum
beyond more localized experiences (e.g., Baum & & Dahlin, 2007; Cyert & March, 1963; Greve, 1998).
Dahlin, 2007; Kim & Miner, 2007; Levitt & March, However, in contrast to extent theories, I argue that
1988; March, 1991), and this recognition has infused this more transparent form of search places organi-
work on topics such as organizational search (Cyert & zations at risk of disclosing internal performance
March, 1963), vicarious learning (Levitt & March, problems, operational challenges, and proprietary
1988), and exploration (March, 1991). And, although practices, which external stakeholders could then
some research examines collaboration with consol- use to inform their perceptions of the organization’s
idated exchange partners, such as through strategic legitimacy and appropriateness (Meyer & Rowan,
alliances or board interlocks (e.g., Rosenkopf & 1977; Suchman, 1995). Therefore, I suggest that or-
Almeida, 2003; Sampson, 2005), collaborative en- ganizations suppress their use of external collabo-
gagements with more dispersed groups, such as rations to solve problems when their legitimacy is
customers and communities, involve legitimacy potentially or already at risk, such as when more
motives alongside their potential to enhance in- powerful stakeholders can hold an organization di-
formation access. Existing theories, which typically rectly accountable for inappropriate acts or when
focus independently on either learning (Cyert & some of the organization’s actions have already been
March, 1963; Levitt & March, 1988) or legitimacy deemed as controversial by outside observers
considerations (Oliver, 1991; Suchman, 1995), are ill (Hudson, 2008; Mitchell, Agle, & Wood, 1997; Piazza
equipped to handle these concerns in tandem since & Perretti, 2015).
legitimacy considerations may affect or temper I test related hypotheses on a panel of local law
learning efforts. enforcement agencies across the United States, and I
Thus, I adapt theories of organizational search and examine their efforts to engage the citizens they serve
exploration in order to examine these collaborations through community policing initiatives (Morabito,
and the conditions that prompt organizations to seek 2010; Zhao, Lovrich, & Thurman, 1999). In the law
222 Academy of Management Journal February

enforcement context, a primary motivation behind However, regardless of the purpose that un-
community policing involves the search for external derlies many of these relationships, research
information about shared public safety issues and suggests that outsiders use the partnerships to ob-
their solutions (Morabito, 2010). However, these serve and evaluate the legitimacy or appropriateness
initiatives can also help to shape external percep- of organizational actions, since doing so allows them
tions regarding law enforcement agencies, and the to hold organizations accountable for acts that have
study’s theoretical framework predicts that agencies violated expectations as well as gauge the likelihood
with currently or potentially contested legitimacy— that an organization might act inappropriately in the
such as those with powerful stakeholders (in this case, future (Mitchell et al., 1997; Suchman, 1995). Fur-
operating through civilian complaint review boards, thermore, the relatively close and sustained contact
or CCRBs, that can hold agencies independently ac- among participants suggests that collaborative
countable) or controversial practices (as reflected engagements—again, regardless of their explicit
through complaints about excessive force, among purpose—are often also rich vehicles for knowledge
other attributes)—would be less likely to rely on exchange, information search, and other organiza-
community policing as a search tool. The study’s tional learning efforts (Crilly et al., 2012). These search
empirical context is described in more detail below, and legitimacy considerations are described below.
following a theoretical discussion of collaborative Collaborative engagement and organizational
engagement. search. While organizational learning research is
characterized by many distinct perspectives, a com-
mon theme in this literature is that organizations
THEORY AND HYPOTHESES learn through their members’ efforts to seek or access
internal and external information or experiences,
Collaborative Engagement:
and interpret as well as codify lessons from these
Search and Legitimacy Considerations
experiences into routines or procedures that guide
Here, “collaborative engagement” refers specifi- subsequent action (Levitt & March, 1988). Learning
cally to organizational partnerships with dispersed research recognizes the complexity and importance
stakeholder groups, such as customers, communi- of both the knowledge search and codification sub-
ties, advocacy groups, or other parties, to pursue or processes; for instance, organizations may learn in-
accomplish shared goals. These are distinct from effectively by searching incorrectly (Cyert & March,
collaborations or alliances with consolidated ex- 1963; Gavetti et al., 2012), or they may misapply
change partners such as peer organizations, suppliers, lessons from newly obtained knowledge (Argote &
distributors, or other similar entities, although orga- Miron-Spektor, 2011). Knowledge search, in partic-
nizations that collaborate with organized represen- ular, often unfolds collaboratively across organiza-
tatives of dispersed stakeholder groups, such as tional boundaries (Lerner & Tirole, 2002; Roine et al.,
community organizations and advocacy groups, 2001; Sampson, 2005). Collaborative engagement
would fall under the present scope. These collabo- holds significant promise as a form of search because
rative engagements could be formal or informal; even it features close and ongoing contact with outside
when informally governed, though, organizations stakeholder groups (Crilly et al., 2012), potentially
that engage collaboratively with stakeholders often providing the organization with access to richer in-
establish supportive internal structures such as cul- formation than that available publicly, through vi-
tural norms or incentive schemes that reward em- carious observation, or from other, more general
ployees for community engagement (Morabito, 2010; forms of environmental search (Cyert & March, 1963;
Zhao et al., 1999). March, 1991).
Organizations might collaborate with external Collaborative engagement and organizational
stakeholder groups for a variety of reasons. For in- legitimacy. Collaborative engagements with external
stance, these external partnerships could help to re- stakeholders also typically increase the transparency
duce operational costs or enhance the organization’s among parties (Estelles-Arolas & Gonzalez-Ladron-
efficiency (Ang & Cummings, 1997; Martinez & de-Guevara, 2012; Lerner & Tirole, 2002). Even when
Dacin, 1999), prevent imitation or maintain other companies attempt to solicit external information
competitive advantages (Montiel, Husted, & while disclosing as little as possible, at least some
Christmann, 2012), or directly preserve access enhanced information about an organization’s plans
to outside resources (Hillman & Keim, 2001; or activities often becomes available to engaged
Julian et al., 2008; Pfeffer & Salancik, 1978). stakeholders. Similarly, these efforts also make it
2018 Desai 223

more difficult for an organization to conceal or dis- police-centric enforcement efforts and instead partner
tract attention away from controversial practices, es- with external stakeholders within the community to
pecially when such collaborations are sustained over collectively identify and solve shared public safety
long periods of time (Pache & Santos, 2013; Sinha problems (Morabito, 2010). Furthermore, involved
et al., 2015). Indeed, Crilly et al. (2012: 1431) suc- stakeholders also gain rich insights into the policies,
cinctly suggested that close collaboration with exter- practices, and approaches of participating law en-
nal stakeholders facilitates outsiders’ “participation forcement agencies through close and ongoing contact
in, and direct observation of, internal operations” as with members of those organizations. Accordingly,
well as the exchange of private information and re- law enforcement organizations commonly use com-
duction in information asymmetries. Since this access munity policing to manage their external legitimacy;
allows stakeholders to more directly scrutinize and indeed, doing so also facilitates their access to external
evaluate organizational practices, concerns regarding information from citizens within their jurisdictions
organizational legitimacy are likely central to the use (Hawdon, 2008; Morabito, 2010; Zhao et al., 1999).
of collaborative engagement as a tool for organiza- These dual purposes are directly engrained in
tional search. community policing through its evolution. In many
At organizations that act in accordance with ex- agencies, the traditional approach to law enforce-
ternal expectations, collaborative engagement may ment historically emphasized a high degree of police
succeed as a search tool because it reinforces and independence in operational decisions regarding
solidifies an organization’s legitimacy with external crime prevention, patrols and investigations, and
parties, who then become more inclined to share arrests and enforcement (Morabito, 2010; Zhao et al.,
information with the organization (Hawdon, 2008; 1999). Yet, shifting public perceptions regarding
Montiel et al., 2012; Suchman, 1995). However, the policing, partly stemming from controversial law
greater transparency afforded through these collab- enforcement responses to civic unrest during the
orations may make it more difficult for organizations 1960s and 1970s (Zhao et al., 1999), strained com-
to recover when their legitimacy is potentially or munity relationships and led to the evolution of
already at risk, as newly disclosed information might community policing during the 1980s and 1990s
be interpreted negatively, further threatening the (Morabito, 2010). Despite its earlier roots, the ap-
organization’s legitimacy and access to critical re- proach flourished during the 1990s and 2000s, fol-
sources (Hudson, 2008; Suchman, 1995). A number lowing several supportive legal and environmental
of studies suggest that organizations with contro- changes (Zhao et al., 1999). However, regardless of
versial practices often attempt to avoid transparency its general acceptance and overall proliferation,
by concealing their actions, using distracting rheto- community policing was, in practice, far from
ric, or disengaging from observers (Crilly et al., 2012; widespread in terms of substantive implementa-
Fiss & Zajac, 2006; Piazza & Perretti, 2015). There- tions. Although many agencies enacted a few rela-
fore, organizations likely suppress their use of ex- tively topical changes or otherwise claimed to follow
ternal collaborations for external search purposes a community oriented approach, far fewer law en-
when their legitimacy is potentially or already at forcement organizations actually met routinely with
risk, since greater transparency could backfire by distinct stakeholder groups, rewarded officers for
reducing stakeholders’ propensities to share in- engaging with the community in shared problem-
formation with the organization. Related arguments solving efforts, or made other substantive changes to
are formalized further below, following a discussion support these relationships (Morabito, 2010).
of the study’s empirical context. Why did such substantive variation arise across
agencies? Although some answers to this question
have been explored (Hawdon, 2008; Morabito, 2010;
Context: Community Policing among Law
Zhao et al., 1999), a potentially neglected possibility
Enforcement Agencies
involves the uncertainties that accompany this and
This study’s empirical tests examine the commu- other forms of collaborative engagement. While law
nity policing efforts of a longitudinal panel of enforcement agencies can use a community oriented
U.S. local law enforcement agencies operating be- approach to facilitate their own knowledge search
tween 1997 and 2007. Community policing repre- efforts, close collaborations with external stakeholders
sents a dramatic shift in the routines and practices can simultaneously reveal much detail regarding po-
of law enforcement organizations, since agencies lice practices, technologies, and approaches. Though
that adopt this model transition from traditional these disclosures could enhance public trust and civic
224 Academy of Management Journal February

participation in the public safety process (Hawdon, individuals and community groups, as well as in-
2008), some uncertainty certainly exists regarding how directly by increasing transparency and legitimacy
external stakeholders may perceive or react to specific with other third-party observers within the agency’s
law enforcement approaches, especially when a law jurisdiction. Accordingly, I posit the following hy-
enforcement agency’s external legitimacy is poten- pothesis regarding the outcome of collaborative
tially or already at risk. Thus, considerations regarding engagement:
the external evaluations of policing practices, and the
Hypothesis 1. Collaborative engagement will
resulting effects on organizational legitimacy, may in-
increase the volume of relevant information re-
fluence community policing decisions in practice
ceived from participants and observers.
(Hawdon, 2008). Below, I forward each of the study’s
specific hypotheses. While many reasons exist for collaborative en-
gagement, the study’s theoretical framework empha-
sizes its relationship with organizational learning,
Hypotheses
and specifically its role in providing the organization
As discussed above, collaborative engagement (in with access to new information from external stake-
this case, through community policing practices and holders. Learning theory maintains that organizations
initiatives) facilitates the organization’s access to search most intensively for new information in re-
external information from direct participants, such sponse to performance problems or other similar is-
as involved individuals and community groups, as sues, since these challenges prompt decision makers
well as from third-party observers who may in- to mobilize resources and actively scan their envi-
directly witness increased organizational trans- ronments for corrective solutions (Baum & Dahlin,
parency. This is predicted to occur for two closely 2007; Cyert & March, 1963; Greve, 1998). Collabora-
related reasons. First, close collaborations provide tive engagement can be thought of as a particularly
the organization with direct opportunities to request interactive form of organizational search, since orga-
or obtain information from participants that is rele- nizations engaged in these efforts aim to generate
vant to solving particular problems or addressing rich, novel insights from external stakeholder groups
specific organizational issues. Second, these rela- (Crilly et al., 2012).
tively transparent arrangements allow participants Learning theory also maintains that organizations
as well as third parties to more directly observe the search most actively in the neighborhood or domain
organization and evaluate the legitimacy or appro- of their performance problems (Cyert & March, 1963;
priateness of its practices—thus increasing their Greve, 2003), meaning that organizations would be
willingness to share relevant information with the most prone to collaborate with external stakeholder
organization. While organizations whose legitimacy groups when problems either involve those groups
is potentially or already at risk may eschew these or when information held by those groups is seem-
relationships (as hypothesized further below), orga- ingly relevant to their resolution. Although organi-
nizations that act in accordance with external expec- zations may respond to minor problems or small
tations likely enter and build further goodwill through performance declines by making local procedural
these engagements, facilitating their success. changes or other internal refinements, greater per-
With specific regard to the law enforcement con- formance downturns or persistent problems typi-
text, police agencies fundamentally depend on the cally lead organizations to consider a broader or
public’s trust and cooperation in providing in- more novel range of solutions (Baum & Dahlin, 2007;
formation about recent or ongoing crimes and Greve, 1998). Thus, externally focused changes,
other public safety concerns (Hawdon, 2008; Zhao such as collaborative engagement with outsiders,
et al., 1999), and a number of studies have identi- may become more likely when organizations face
fied or alluded to citizens’ reluctance in supplying increasing problems or continued performance de-
this information to agencies with poor legitimacy clines in categories of performance that involve
or low civic engagement (Kraska & Kappeler, 1997; those outside groups.
McGregor, 2016; Meeks, 2006; Morabito, 2010; While various organizational problems exist in the
Taylor, Alpert, Kubu, Woods, & Dunham, 2011). law enforcement setting, one of the most salient
Thus, community policing initiatives likely facilitate metrics in this context is the crime rate within a law
an agency’s access to external information about enforcement agency’s jurisdiction. While agencies
public safety concerns within the community, do not have direct control over these rates, they are
both from direct participants such as involved certainly evaluated by communities based on their
2018 Desai 225

abilities to identify and respond to crimes within Organizations with controversial practices risk the
their jurisdictions and to generally maintain a safe possibility that stakeholders may object to their tac-
environment for citizens. Increases in the crime rate tics, approach, or plans to resolve the crisis, perhaps
within an agency’s jurisdiction reflect a clear and further threatening the organization’s legitimacy and
salient organizational problem, with pressure for access to critical resources (Hudson, 2008; Suchman,
resolution often coming from the public, elected of- 1995). Thus, organizational decision makers might
ficials, and other external stakeholders. One poten- presume that the risks of open collaboration to
tial solution under these circumstances, among their organization’s legitimacy might outweigh any
others, involves improving community relation- search-related benefits that could arise through these
ships in order to enhance the agency’s response ef- partnerships. As a result, controversial practices
forts. Therefore, the crime rate within an agency’s likely suppress the direct relationship between
jurisdiction is an ideal reflection of organizational problems and collaborative engagement.
problems involving community members and other Similarly, organizations with relatively powerful
external stakeholders within this setting, and stakeholders risk the possibility that those outsiders
agencies with increasing crime rates are more likely could impose stronger sanctions on the organization
to explore stakeholder-based options such as com- if inappropriate or unacceptable actions come to
munity policing. These arguments lead to the fol- light through close collaborative engagements. Even
lowing hypothesis: when organizations do not engage in inappropriate
practices, greater stakeholder power allows out-
Hypothesis 2. Organizational problems in-
siders to hold the organization accountable if dis-
volving community members will increase the
agreements arise about the organization’s planned
use of collaborative engagement.
approach to solving particular problems. Thus, or-
Although learning theory robustly posits that orga- ganizational decision makers may again presume
nizations respond to performance problems and other that the risks of collaboration might outweigh its
operational challenges by intensifying their search benefits. Accordingly, stakeholder power also likely
and knowledge collection efforts (Gavetti et al., 2012; suppresses the direct relationship between problems
Greve, 2003), collaborative relationships place stake- and collaborative engagement.
holders in a better position to observe these problems In the law enforcement context, some research
and gauge the appropriateness of organizations’ re- supports the notion that agencies require public trust
sponses (Crilly et al., 2012). Thus, some organizations to realize benefits from community policing initia-
may be less likely than others to engage collabora- tives; by extension, agencies whose legitimacy is al-
tively with outsiders when problems arise. ready impaired may be more reluctant to seek
On the one hand, this transparency likely has little solutions through these efforts when problems arise
impact on organizations whose practices already (Hawdon, 2008; Morabito, 2010; Zhao et al., 1999).
conform to external expectations, since outsiders’ Similarly, agencies may also avoid these initiatives
perceptions of organizational legitimacy might not when powerful stakeholders exist. Although pow-
be affected through closer collaboration in these erful stakeholders could pressure organizations to
cases. In addition, organizations may also collabo- engage under some circumstances, such collabora-
rate openly when outsiders have limited power to tions might be relatively limited or symbolic. In-
impose sanctions on the organization, since stake- stead, organizations with powerful stakeholders may
holders would face greater difficulty in holding the be less inclined to substantively engage, given that
organization accountable for declining legitimacy or stakeholders could hold them more accountable for
questionable practices in these situations (Chiu & lapses or more strongly dissent to specific organiza-
Sharfman, 2011; Mitchell et al., 1997). tional tactics and approaches (McGregor, 2016;
However, conversely, organizations with contro- Morabito, 2010). Collectively, these arguments lead
versial practices—or those with more powerful to the following two hypotheses:
stakeholders—may be more reluctant to use collab-
Hypothesis 3. Stakeholder power will reduce the
orative engagement as a form of organizational
positive influence of organizational problems
search when problems arise or intensify. In these
on collaborative engagement.
situations, organizations that collaborate openly
with stakeholders must often share the details of Hypothesis 4. Controversial practices will re-
those problems and expose their planned solutions duce the positive influence of organizational
to greater external scrutiny (Sutton & Galunic, 1996). problems on collaborative engagement.
226 Academy of Management Journal February

METHODS (Tables 3 and 4) as compared with its first analysis


(Table 2). In the first analysis, the community polic-
Setting and Data
ing practices variable is used as a lagged predictor
The data set used for this study includes in- (absorbing the 1997 reporting period, where it was
formation from a periodic census conducted by the first collected) and the calls for service measure is
U.S. Department of Justice, referred to as the Law used as an unlagged outcome variable (thus exclud-
Enforcement Management and Administrative Sta- ing the 2007 reporting period). This treatment is re-
tistics (LEMAS) survey, that collects information versed in Tables 3 and 4, where calls for service is
about the structure and operations of police de- used as a lagged control and community policing
partments, sheriffs’ departments, and other local law practices as an unlagged outcome variable. As a re-
enforcement organizations with 100 or more sworn sult, the analysis in Table 2 is conducted for out-
officers. The data are made available through the comes measured in 2000 and 2003, while the
Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social analyses in Tables 3 and 4 use a larger estimation
Research (e.g., U.S. Department of Justice, 2007). The panel with outcomes measured in 1997, 2000, 2003,
LEMAS survey is conducted roughly every three to and 2007. For these reasons, the sample size for each
four years, and the panel assembled for this study model is reported individually along with results in
includes the 1993, 1997, 2000, 2003, and 2007 the study’s tables.
reporting periods, with observations from the 1993
reporting period absorbed to construct the initial set
Measures
of lagged variables. Supplemental data on area crime
rates were also obtained through the Federal Bureau Calls for service. This study uses two different
of Investigation’s Uniform Crime Reporting program dependent variables to test its respective hypotheses.
and are available more frequently; thus, the study The first of these dependent variables is used to test
incorporates crime data from the year just prior to Hypothesis 1, regarding the volume of information
each observation. received from external stakeholders (the second de-
The unit of analysis is the agency-year, and the full pendent variable is discussed in the next section). In
estimation sample includes a total of 2,380 agency- this context, members of the general public provide
year observations (an average of 595 agencies per information about crime events, emergencies, and
year). However, the number of agencies included in other service issues directly to law enforcement
the estimations tends to vary across years. First, a few agencies through 9-1-1 call systems. The volume of
agencies entered or exited the data set when they calls received by an agency through its 9-1-1 system
crossed the reporting threshold of 100 sworn officers may reflect a number of factors, with the most obvious
at some point during the study period. Second, a few influences being the crime rate and population of
other agencies were either created or disbanded citizens within the agency’s jurisdiction.
during the study window. Third, although most However, these and other relevant influences are
variables were available (i.e., collected through the directly controlled, as described below. Aside from
LEMAS survey) for all of the reporting periods noted these factors, the public’s willingness to use 9-1-1
above, two important variables were reported on call systems partly reflects their general trust and
a more limited basis. First, the calls for service perceptions of law enforcement (e.g., Hawdon, 2008;
measure (described further below) was not collected Morabito, 2010; Zhao et al., 1999). Indeed, several
at all during the 2007 survey, and was also frequently studies consistently indicate that a primary reason
missing (reported by some but not all agencies) behind public reluctance to call 911 is the distrust of
during the 2000 period. In the latter case, the missing law enforcement within some communities (Kessell,
values were spread across agencies during the pe- Alvidrez, McConnell, & Shumway, 2009; Sasson
riod, with no discernable differences in organiza- et al., 2015; Tobin, Davey, & Latkin, 2005). Agencies
tional demographic characteristics among reporters that improve community relations through commu-
and non-reporters. In addition, the community po- nity policing practices should experience an increase
licing practices variable (also described further be- in the community’s willingness to use the 9-1-1 call
low) was not collected during the 1993 survey. A few system. Thus, the calls for service variable measures
other measures employed in the analyses also have the volume of calls received by the agency through its
occasional missing values for some observations. 9-1-1 system.
Collectively, this data structure results in a larger Community policing practices. The study’s sec-
estimation sample within the study’s later analyses ond dependent variable tracks each agency’s
2018 Desai 227

collaborative engagement, which translates into problems and collaborative engagement. Law en-
their use of community policing within this par- forcement agencies are subject to varying degrees of
ticular setting. While various definitions of com- formal and informal external oversight and gover-
munity policing exist, the central elements of this nance from policymakers and legislators, judicial
approach tend to involve agency collaborations systems, advocacy organizations, and citizens, with
with key stakeholders to identify and solve law greater governance and oversight leading agencies to
enforcement problems, as reflected through re- operate less autonomously and therefore signifying
lated changes to organizational procedures such as increased stakeholder power.
assigning patrol officers to specific geographic A particularly important form of external over-
areas, training or encouraging officers to collabo- sight within this context involves independent
rate with community members to solve problems, monitoring by CCRBs, which are typically tasked
and including such collaborations in officers’ with reviewing complaints against the police re-
performance evaluation criteria (Hawdon, 2008; garding use of force, misconduct, or other procedural
Morabito, 2010). Accordingly, I track organiza- lapses. This form of oversight is especially relevant
tional efforts to collaborate through a variable, to the study’s framework, given that CCRBs are often
community policing practices, that measures the composed of local citizens from various stakeholder
number of these procedures (geographic patrols, groups such as civic organizations, businesses, ad-
formal encouragement of collaboration, and sup- vocacy organizations, schools, and other external
portive incentive structures) in place at the focal communities. Therefore, CCRBs provide a relevant
agency. The variable could take a value from form of oversight and transparency with members of
0 (reflecting no supportive procedures) to 3 the same stakeholder groups that are often involved
(reflecting the full mix of supportive procedures). in community policing actions, although the specif-
This measure is used as a dependent variable when ically involved individuals do not necessarily over-
examining the antecedents of collaborative en- lap among these distinct efforts.
gagement, and as a predictor variable when ex- CCRBs often play a more direct and immediate
amining its outcome. oversight role than other entities, such as legal in-
Crime rate. As discussed above, the crime rate stitutions, since they are able to review and in-
within an agency’s jurisdiction reflects organiza- vestigate complaints against police officers that may
tional problems involving external stakeholders not have yet entered (or may never enter) the legal
within this setting. Accordingly, the crime rate var- system. Furthermore, their adoption has historically
iable tracks the rate of reportable crime events per been resisted by many law enforcement agencies
person within the reporting agency’s jurisdiction. over fears regarding the loss of police autonomy
Here, reportable crime events refer to the specific (Bartels & Silverman, 2005; de Guzman & Frank,
criminal offenses tracked by the Federal Bureau of 2004; McGregor, 2016). Accordingly, stakeholder
Investigation’s Uniform Crime Reporting program. power is measured through a dichotomous indicator
These offenses include violent crimes such as ho- (civilian oversight board) that is set to 1 if this form of
micide, robbery, aggravated assault, and sexual as- external oversight exists at the focal law enforcement
sault, as well as property crimes such as burglary, agency, and set to 0 if an external review board does
motor vehicle theft, and larceny. These offenses, in not exist. The relevant moderating hypothesis is
particular, were selected by the Uniform Crime tested through an interaction between this variable
Reporting program due to their severity, because and the crime rate measure described above.
they occur with regularity in all areas of the country, Controversial practices: Nonlethal patrol gear. I
and since they are most likely to be reported to po- employed two different variables to separately proxy
lice. The variable is missing for a small number of for distinct aspects of controversy in this setting.
observations in respect of which the presiding First, I tracked the use of a specific set of practices
agency could not be uniquely identified. As noted that met with controversy from some stakeholders
above, crime data were available more frequently during the study period. While this approach bene-
than measures from the LEMAS survey; thus, these fits by examining formally sanctioned procedures,
data are collected for the year just prior to each focal the actual controversy surrounding particular prac-
observation. tices likely varies over time and across agencies.
Civilian oversight board. One of the study’s hy- Thus, the second measure, described later, more di-
potheses predicts a moderating effect of stakeholder rectly examines the level of controversy surrounding
power on the direct relationship between organizational particular practices.
228 Academy of Management Journal February

A number of activities conducted by some their use on cooperative individuals, have occurred
agencies, such as racial profiling, the use of military- in some situations (Adams & Jennison, 2007; Sousa
grade weapons, or other similar tactics, have et al., 2010; Vilke & Chan, 2007; Vilke, Johnson,
attracted controversy (Ajilore, 2015; Campbell & Castillo, Sloane, & Chan, 2009). Fourth, human rights
Campbell, 2016; Meeks, 2006). However, reliable groups have periodically issued reports challenging
data are unfortunately unavailable in many of these these practices over the past several decades (Ferdik,
cases. Instead, I focussed on another set of contested Kaminski, Cooney, & Sevigny, 2014; Sousa et al.,
practices, given their distinct importance, reliable 2010). Collectively, the controversy has led some
measurement, and widespread proliferation. Spe- departments to avoid using these technologies amid
cifically, much current and historical research has concerns regarding police–community relations
noted the controversy surrounding nonlethal and (Adams & Jennison, 2007: 450).
less-than-lethal law enforcement technologies dur- Thus, the nonlethal patrol gear variable measures
ing this study’s analysis period (Adams & Jennison, the number of these technologies available for rou-
2007; Azadani, Tseng, Ermakov, Marcus, & Lee, tine use at the focal agency. The variable specifically
2011; Bailey, 1996; Crow & Adrion, 2011; Jauchem, tracks whether the agency formally allows patrol
2015; Lee et al., 2009; MacDonald, Kaminski, & officers to use conductive energy devices (stun guns
Smith, 2009; Pidgeon, Bragg, Ball, & Meltzer, 2008). or Tasers), rubber bullets, neck restraints (carotid
Perhaps surprisingly, the controversy has persisted, holds or choke holds), tear gas, pepper spray, soft
despite some medical studies reflecting their po- projectiles (e.g., bean bag rounds), or requires that
tential safety (Bozeman et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2009; they wear body armor or other tactical gear on rou-
MacDonald et al., 2009), as well as anecdotal asser- tine patrols. The variable ranges from 0 (no nonlethal
tions that the technologies have reduced civilian equipment available for routine patrols) to 7 (all
fatalities by providing officers with reliable alterna- equipment categories described above are available).
tives to lethal force (Brandl & Stroshine, 2012; Sousa, As with the other moderator variable, the respective
Ready, & Ault, 2010; Taylor et al., 2011; Taylor & hypothesis was tested through an interaction be-
Woods, 2010). tween this variable and the crime rate variable de-
Yet, the safety of this equipment was often called scribed above.
into question over the study period (Adams & Controversial practices: Use of force complaints.
Jennison, 2007; Bailey, 1996; Chowaniec, Kobek, The study’s second measure of controversial practices
Jablonski, Kabiesz-Neniczka, & Karczewska, 2008; more directly tracks the number of complaints about
Mendelson et al., 2010; Pidgeon et al., 2008; Taylor & excessive force involving an agency’s officers that are
Woods, 2010). For instance, some studies refer to the received by the agency during the year (Bartels &
controversy regarding conductive energy devices Silverman, 2005; Crow & Adrion, 2011; de Guzman &
(e.g., Tasers and stun guns) stemming from several Frank, 2004; Hawdon, 2008; Taylor et al., 2011;
fatalities following their use (Adams & Jennison, Taylor & Woods, 2010). These complaints can be
2007; Jauchem, 2015; Lee et al., 2009; Pidgeon et al., initiated by citizens or on their behalf, and they are
2008). Similar concerns have also been raised re- typically investigated by the law enforcement agency
garding other technologies such as tear gas and or by the CCRB if one exists.
pepper sprays (Mendelson et al., 2010; Vilke & Chan, Crucially, the measure does not track legal actions
2007), impact projectiles such as rubber bullets and against departments for excessive force; further-
bean-bag rounds (MacDonald et al., 2009; Vilke & more, it likely underreports the true rate of excessive
Chan, 2007), and neck restraint techniques (Panzar, force incidents, given that many citizens may be re-
2014). luctant to file complaints with the presiding law
This controversy likely persists for several rea- enforcement agency. However, while the variable is
sons. First, despite their potential to reduce fatalities, certainly a poor measure of actual excessive force
these technologies may have caused or contributed incidents, it is likely an appropriate reflection of the
to deaths in certain cases (Chowaniec et al., 2008; visible public concern surrounding a law enforce-
Mendelson et al., 2010; Pidgeon et al., 2008). Second, ment agency’s policing practices. Decision makers in
these deaths may be particularly notable given that law enforcement agencies with a higher number of
the technologies are intended to avoid such an out- excessive force complaints can directly infer that
come (Bailey, 1996; MacDonald et al., 2009; Pidgeon more citizens within their jurisdiction believe offi-
et al., 2008; Taylor et al., 2011; Vilke & Chan, 2007). cers have acted inappropriately, whether or not the
Third, police abuses of the technologies, such as underlying complaints are ultimately substantiated
2018 Desai 229

(Crow & Adrion, 2011; Taylor et al., 2011). Thus, separately controlled for the percent of female offi-
this is an important indicator regarding the ex- cers and percent of nonwhite officers employed by
tent of controversy that decision makers believe the agency. Similarly, I controlled for the percent of
their agencies likely face among members of the female residents and percent of nonwhite residents
community. within the focal agency’s county using demographic
Accordingly, I constructed a use of force com- data from the U.S. Census Bureau’s intercensal
plaints variable that tracks the number of these ex- county population estimates. I also controlled for the
cessive force complaints that were reported to the population of citizens (in millions) within that geo-
focal agency during the year. Unfortunately, this in- graphic area. Finally, I included year (i.e., period)
formation was only requested by the underlying law effects in all models to address unobserved differ-
enforcement agency survey for two of the survey ences affecting all agencies over time. All control and
administration periods (2003 and 2007) rather than predictor variables were lagged, as described above,
for all surveys over this study’s entire window. In to reduce the possibility of reverse causality.
each case, the survey requested use of force in-
formation for the prior year, although other survey
Analysis
metrics were requested for the current survey ad-
ministration year. So, for instance, the 2007 survey The study’s first dependent variable (calls for
requested the number of excessive force complaints service) is bounded on its lower end by 0; thus, I used
from 2006, but tracked all other agency data in 2007 random-effects Tobit regression for the models re-
(and the 2003 survey uses the same collection ported in Table 2. The second dependent variable,
structure). While the reason for this unique approach community policing practices, is an ordinal variable
with the focal variable is unknown, the current study reflecting ordered categories (i.e., the number of
benefitted through this structure by incorporating relevant practices). Given this structure, ordered lo-
use of force data for the year immediately preceding gistic regression is ideal (Cameron & Trivedi, 1998;
each available observation. Thus, 2006 force com- Wooldridge, 2002), and all relevant models were
plaint data were used to predict 2007 outcomes, and estimated using random-effects ordered logistic re-
2002 force complaint data were used to predict 2003 gression with robust standard errors (through the
outcomes. Finally, as in the other cases, the re- xtologit command in Stata/SE version 13).
spective moderating hypothesis was tested through Note, however, that ordered logistic estimation
an interaction between this variable and the crime places a restrictive assumption on the data by pre-
rate variable described above. suming that any given shift in a predictor must have
Control variables. I controlled for the scope of an the same impact across the dependent variable’s
agency’s services through an organizational units entire range. To test this assumption, Brant tests were
variable that counts the number of distinct special- performed following each of the regressions in-
ized units (such as units for domestic violence, drug volving this outcome variable (Williams, 2006). This
education, juvenile crimes, and other specific areas) test determines whether the proportional odds as-
operated by the agency. To more directly address the sumption, as it is referred to, has been violated
effects of agency size, I controlled for the agency’s within a given ordered logistic regression model. A
operating expenses (logged to reduce skewness). review of test results following each regression
Similarly, I also controlled for the agency’s volume of model indicated that regressions estimating use of
calls for service (again, logged to reduce skewness) in force (those displayed in Table 4) uniformly do not
the analyses reported in Tables 3 and 4, although the violate the proportional odds assumption. However,
control is omitted in Table 2 in which calls for service models estimating the other moderators (displayed
is used as an outcome measure. in Table 3) do tend to violate the assumption.
I also separately controlled for the number (in This condition suggests that ordered logistic esti-
thousands) of full-time sworn officers employed by mation, which assumes that predictors lead to simi-
the agency, as well as the number of hours of class- lar shifts across various categories of the outcome
room and field training, respectively, required by the variable, might discard some potentially pertinent
agency for its new officer candidates. I also included information regarding differences among these cat-
a dichotomous indicator that was set to 1 if collective egories. To determine whether this influences any of
bargaining was authorized for the agency’s officers. the relevant hypothesis tests, affected models (those
To address the effects of diversity on community displayed in Table 3) were replicated using gener-
policing and other law enforcement practices, I alized ordered logistic regression, which relaxes the
230 Academy of Management Journal February

proportional odds assumption (see Williams, 2006). the second measure controls for the rate of reportable
Results were similar across these approaches. Given property crimes (which include burglaries, motor ve-
this similarity, and the fact that ordered logistic re- hicle thefts, and larceny). In this model, the coefficient
gression estimates can be presented more parsimo- estimates for the community policing practices vari-
niously, final study models use ordered logistic able are positive and significant for agencies that em-
estimation rather than its generalized counterpart. ploy any one or two practices, although the coefficient
Finally, as noted above, data on use of force com- estimate is only significant at the p , .10 level for
plaints were only available for two of the study’s agencies that employ all three practices.
observation periods. Accordingly, including this Recall that Hypothesis 1 predicted that collabora-
variable or its interaction within models estimating tive engagement would increase the volume of in-
the other hypothesized predictors resulted in the loss formation received from external stakeholders.
of more than half of the study’s observations, risking Given that the models displayed in Table 2 rely on
the possibility that a significant amount of relevant a nonlinear estimator, marginal effects must be ex-
information was being discarded by the study’s other amined in order to infer support for this hypothesis
tests. Thus, I separately tested the moderating effect (Greene, 2010; Hoetker, 2007; Karaca-Mandic,
of use of force complaints on community policing Norton, & Dowd, 2012). For these models, marginal
outcomes in its own set of models (reported in effects plots corresponded directly to the results
Table 4), while the other predictors of community displayed in these tables, with the sign and signifi-
policing outcomes are instead nested in the distinct cance of effects remaining consistent over the range
models reported through Table 3. of plotted values. For brevity, these plots are not
presented here, although marginal effects plots from
the models in Tables 3 and 4 are displayed and
RESULTS
interpreted, given that the latter plots do affect the
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics and pair- interpretation of results from the corresponding
wise correlations among the study’s variables. High models. Collectively, the results from Table 2 and
positive correlations exist between the control for associated marginal effects provide support for Hy-
full-time sworn officers and the variables tracking pothesis 1 for agencies that employ either one or two
calls for service (r 5 0.79) or use of force complaints community policing practices, although the hy-
(r 5 0.86). To determine whether these correlations pothesis is not supported for agencies that employ all
influence estimation results, the control for full-time three practices in combination.
officers was omitted from preliminary models, with Tables 3 and 4 present models estimating the
similar results obtained. Given its conceptual im- number of community policing practices employed
portance, the control was retained in final analyses. by a focal agency. Models in Table 3 respectively
Table 2 displays models estimating the calls for estimate the moderating effects of civilian oversight
service outcome variable. Model 1 includes only board and nonlethal patrol gear, while the moder-
controls. Model 2 adds the community policing ating effect of use of force complaints is separately
practices variable (categorically, with 0 practices as estimated in Table 4 due to its more restricted
the omitted level). The variable’s coefficient estimate availability, as described above. Turning to Table 3,
is positive and significant for agencies that employ Model 1 includes only controls. Model 2 adds the
two out of the three community policing practices, main effect of crime rate. The coefficient estimate of
while the coefficient estimate is not significant (at this variable is positive and significant in this and
the p , .05 level) for agencies that either employ only later models (as well as in the models within Table 4).
one, or all three, community policing practices. This is consistent with Hypothesis 2, which pre-
However, 911 calls are likely influenced by the type of dicted that problems would increase an organiza-
crime rather than (or in addition to) its overall rate. For tion’s use of collaborative engagement. However,
instance, citizens may be more likely to initiate calls again, examinations of marginal effects are required
involving serious or violent incidents rather than to infer support for the hypotheses tested through
for property crimes. Thus, Model 3 in Table 2 de- these models (Greene, 2010; Hoetker, 2007; Karaca-
composes the overall crime rate control into two Mandic et al., 2012). I follow this approach further
separate controls. The first measure controls for the below, after discussing the study’s remaining esti-
rate of reportable violent crimes within the focal mation results.
agency’s jurisdiction (these include homicides, rob- Model 3 in Table 3 introduces the main effect of
beries, aggravated assaults, and sexual assaults), while civilian oversight board, and Model 4 adds its
2018 Desai 231

TABLE 1
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
Variable Obs. Mean SD Min. Max. 1 2 3 4

1. Calls for service (000s) 1,088 306.32 716.06 0.00 11500


2. Community policing practices 2,380 1.65 1.03 0.00 3.00 0.12*
3. Crime rate (per capita) 2,030 0.05 0.03 0.00 0.26 0.17* 0.10*
4. Civilian oversight board (indicator) 2,195 0.14 0.35 0.00 1.00 0.21* 0.09* 0.20*
5. Nonlethal patrol gear 2,194 2.62 1.46 0.00 7.00 0.15* 0.13* 20.10* 0.05*
6. Use of force complaints 973 36.53 271.20 0.00 7663.00 0.60* 0.00 0.01 0.14*
7. Organizational units 2,380 7.49 3.44 0.00 11.00 0.14* 0.17* 0.02 0.09*
8. Operating expenses (logged) 2,380 3.13 0.99 0.99 8.20 0.59* 0.11* 20.02 0.25*
10. Full-time sworn officers (000s) 2,380 0.50 1.52 0.10 38.33 0.79* 0.05* 0.02 0.21*
11. Classroom training hours 2,380 0.64 0.28 0.00 3.60 0.21* 0.06* 0.03 0.13*
12. Field training hours 2,380 0.47 0.33 0.00 3.52 0.02 0.15* 0.09* 0.04*
13. Collective bargaining (indicator) 2,380 0.61 0.49 0.00 1.00 0.06* 0.05* 0.00 0.08*
14. % Nonwhite 2,380 0.18 0.16 0.00 1.00 0.14* 0.00 0.21* 0.19*
15. % Female 2,380 0.11 0.06 0.00 0.43 0.09* 20.04 20.07* 0.11*
16. Area % nonwhite 2,380 0.20 0.14 0.01 0.79 0.13* 20.07* 0.20* 0.09*
17. Area % female 2,380 0.51 0.01 0.45 0.55 0.01 20.14* 0.20* 0.00
18. Area population (millions) 2,380 0.91 1.56 0.01 9.77 0.13* 0.10* 20.06* 0.09*
19. Year (categorical) 2,380 2001.67 4.06 1997.00 2007.00 0.03 0.01 20.17* 0.02

interaction with crime rate. The coefficient estimate coefficient estimate of this interaction term is nega-
of this interaction term is negative and significant. tive and significant. Although this is consistent with
This is consistent with Hypothesis 3, which pre- Hypothesis 4, marginal effects must be evaluated in
dicted that stakeholder power would reduce the this and the other cases described above.
positive influence of organizational problems on
collaborative engagement. Support for this hypoth-
Marginal Effects
esis is evaluated further below using marginal effect
plots from this model. Model 5 removes the prior Marginal effects for ordered choice models must
terms and adds the main effect of nonlethal patrol be plotted and evaluated separately for each outcome
gear, and the interaction with this term is introduced category aside from the base outcome (in this case,
in Model 6. The coefficient estimate of that in- a value of 0 for the dependent variable). Thus, each
teraction term is not significant in this model. Hy- relevant model typically yields three corresponding
pothesis 4 predicted that controversial practices figures. The first figure in each case plots the mar-
would reduce the direct effect of organizational ginal effect of a one-unit change in the relevant co-
problems on collaborative engagement, with non- variate on a value of one for the community policing
lethal patrol gear being one of the study’s two mea- outcome variable, the second figure plots the mar-
sures of controversy. Although the coefficient ginal effect on a value of two, and the third figure
estimate on this interaction term is not significant, plots the marginal effect on the highest value of three
this hypothesis is also evaluated using marginal ef- community policing practices. The plots display
fects further below. Finally, Model 7 includes all whether the confidence interval around a predicted
prior variables and their respective interactions, and effect line crosses 0 along some or all of its length. For
the results are similar to those in prior models. values in respect of which the confidence interval is
Table 4 presents models estimating the moderat- above 0, an increase in the covariate appears to raise
ing effect of use of force complaints. Again, Model 1 the outcome’s likelihood. Conversely, when the
includes only controls, and Model 2 adds the crime confidence interval is below 0, an increase in the
rate variable. As before, the coefficient estimate of covariate appears to reduce the likelihood of that
this variable is positive and significant in this and particular outcome.
later models. Model 3 introduces the use of force In each of the relevant figures, I plotted a range of
complaints measure, and Model 4 incorporates values from one standard deviation below to one
its interaction term. In contrast with results using standard deviation above each predictor’s (or mod-
the other measure of controversial practices, the erator’s) mean, unless the relevant variable was
232 Academy of Management Journal February

TABLE 1
(Continued)
5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17

0.00
0.31* 0.04
0.23* 0.28* 0.23*
0.06* 0.86* 0.07* 0.54*
0.15* 0.10* 0.27* 0.41* 0.17*
0.21* 0.00 0.18* 0.12* 20.03 0.15*
0.05* 0.04 0.06* 0.14* 0.07* 0.20* 0.05*
0.05* 0.12* 0.04 0.23* 0.16* 0.10* 20.01 20.01
0.07* 0.08* 0.03 0.15* 0.11* 20.17* 20.08* 20.20* 0.27*
20.03 0.08* 0.01 0.17* 0.13* 0.07* 20.04* 20.16* 0.47* 0.18*
20.26* 0.06* 20.09* 20.09* 0.06* 20.08* 20.14* 20.02 0.06* 0.01 0.28*
0.19* 0.06 0.08* 0.20* 0.16* 0.13* 0.11* 0.19* 0.17* 20.06* 0.11* 20.15*
0.38* 0.01 0.56* 0.19* 0.00 0.30* 0.15* 0.00 0.04 0.05* 0.08* 20.10* 0.00

Notes: Pairwise correlations.


*Significant at the p , .05 level

dichotomous or truncated within that range. Figures in tandem, and increases over most of the variable’s
1a, 1b, and 1c use information from Model 2 in range also tend to raise the likelihood of using two
Table 3 to plot the main effect of crime rate on the use out of the three practices. However, the hypothesis
of one community policing practice, two practices, does not appear supported in the case of using only
or three practices, respectively. Recall that Hypoth- one out of the three practices—to the contrary, in-
esis 2 predicted that organizational problems (in this creases in the crime rate tend to make agencies less
case, the crime rate) would increase the use of col- likely to use only one practice in isolation.
laborative engagement. Figure 1a plots the marginal Figures 2a, 2b, and 2c examine the moderating
effect of crime rate, from one standard deviation be- effect of civilian oversight board from Model 4 in
low its mean to one standard deviation above it, on Table 3. The first figure plots the moderating effect
the likelihood of using one community policing on the use of one community policing practice in
practice. The confidence interval around the pre- isolation. Now, the solid gray band around the
dicted marginal effect (represented by the gray band) dashed line displays the marginal effect (and confi-
is consistently below 0, indicating that a one-unit dence interval) of crime rate when the moderator is at
increase for any crime rate values within this range its lower value (in this case, a value of 0 reflecting
will uniformly reduce—rather than increase—the that no civilian board exists). In addition, a new
likelihood that an agency employs one community dotted black pattern has been added; in each figure
policing practice in isolation. where this pattern appears, the pattern displays the
Figure 1b plots the effect on using two practices, marginal effect (and confidence interval) of crime
showing that the marginal effect of crime rate turns rate when the relevant moderator is at its higher
positive and significant as the variable increases in value (here, a value of 1 reflecting that a civilian
value around and above 0.04. Finally, Figure 1c board exists). The dotted line through the middle of
shows that the marginal effect of crime rate on the this new transparent area shows the predicted mar-
use of all three practices is consistently positive over ginal effect, while the top and bottom dotted lines
the entire plotted range. Collectively, the results show the confidence interval around this prediction.
from these figures display an interesting and com- Hypothesis 3 predicted that stakeholder power
plex pattern of partial support for Hypothesis 2, would reduce the positive influence of problems on
whereby increases in the crime rate tend to raise an collaboration. In Figure 2a, the marginal effect of
agency’s likelihood of employing all three practices crime rate when no civilian board exists (i.e., low
2018 Desai 233

TABLE 2
Tobit Regression Models of Calls for Service (in 000s)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Community policing practices 5 1a 55.821† 57.612*


(29.261) (29.262)
Community policing practices 5 2a 65.161* 66.549*
(30.041) (30.028)
Community policing practices 5 3a 53.243† 56.199†
(31.269) (31.289)
Crime rate 0.014*** 0.014***
(0.003) (0.003)
Crime rate: Violent 0.060*
(0.027)
Crime rate: Property 0.008†
(0.005)
Organizational units 20.402*** 18.539*** 18.352***
(3.816) (3.944) (3.943)
Operating expenses 140.601*** 139.811*** 137.986***
(11.732) (11.736) (11.762)
Full-time sworn officers 284.337*** 284.268*** 283.934***
(5.962) (5.954) (5.947)
Classroom training hours 62.863† 58.742† 57.769
(35.320) (35.517) (35.490)
Field training hours 74.641** 72.132** 74.277**
(25.204) (25.249) (25.255)
Collective bargaining 241.707* 243.108* 246.591**
(17.741) (17.733) (17.817)
% Nonwhite 258.067 259.308 276.367
(58.026) (58.081) (58.827)
% Female 2210.575 2195.307 2210.984
(144.181) (144.292) (144.398)
Area % nonwhite 241.028 233.244 234.623
(71.843) (71.927) (71.779)
Area % female 2153.743 292.484 2291.897
(872.938) (875.648) (881.630)
Area population 211.423* 210.402† 210.587†
(5.729) (5.757) (5.744)
Model N 946 946 946
F test of period effects 25.260*** 19.510*** 18.910***

Notes: Standard errors in parentheses.


a
Community policing practice 5 0 is used as baseline (omitted) category.

p , .10
* p , .05
** p , .01
*** p , .001 (two-tailed)

stakeholder power) is uniformly negative (the upper Hypothesis 3 is obtained with respect to using one
bound of the confidence interval, reflected through practice in isolation.
the filled-in gray area, is always negative), and the Figure 2b plots the relevant marginal effects on
marginal effect when a board exists (i.e., high power) using two out of the three practices. Here, the mar-
is not necessarily lower (the dotted black lines ap- ginal effect of crime rate when no board exists (the
pear higher, although partially overlapping). This solid gray area) is positive and significant across its
pattern indicates that organizational problems (as range. When a board does exist, the marginal effect of
measured here through crime rates) generally do crime rate (the unfilled area bounded by dotted black
not exert a positive influence on this outcome, and lines) generally appears lower, although its upper
that stakeholder power does not appear to reduce confidence interval overlaps with 0 as well as the
this direct influence. Accordingly, no support for other predicted range. This pattern requires some
234 Academy of Management Journal February

TABLE 3
Random-Effects Ordered Logistic Regression Models of Community Policing Practices
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 Model 7

Crime rate 4.388* 4.623* 6.001** 4.779* 4.980* 6.309**


(1.995) (2.134) (2.178) (2.114) (2.377) (2.408)
Civilian oversight board 0.065 0.722† 0.722
(0.185) (0.374) (0.374)
Crime rate 3 Civilian board 211.579* 211.571*
(5.719) (5.731)
Nonlethal patrol gear 0.039 0.039 0.039
(0.044) (0.044) (0.044)
Crime rate 3 Nonleth. gear 0.254 0.325
(1.244) (1.228)
Organizational units 0.134*** 0.116*** 0.136*** 0.137*** 0.133*** 0.133*** 0.136***
(0.022) (0.026) (0.029) (0.029) (0.029) (0.029) (0.029)
Operating expenses 20.090 20.010 0.008 0.025 0.011 0.012 0.022
(0.081) (0.098) (0.102) (0.102) (0.101) (0.101) (0.102)
Calls for service 0.328*** 0.353*** 0.334*** 0.337*** 0.331*** 0.331*** 0.334***
(0.051) (0.061) (0.063) (0.063) (0.063) (0.063) (0.063)
Full-time sworn officers 0.008 20.001 20.005 20.014 20.004 20.004 20.014
(0.029) (0.026) (0.027) (0.027) (0.027) (0.027) (0.027)
Classroom training hours 20.221 0.058 0.063 0.081 0.083 0.083 0.089
(0.203) (0.231) (0.238) (0.237) (0.238) (0.238) (0.238)
Field training hours 0.553*** 0.611*** 0.674*** 0.674*** 0.655*** 0.654*** 0.653***
(0.147) (0.167) (0.180) (0.180) (0.181) (0.181) (0.181)
Collective bargaining 0.026 20.112 20.109 20.103 20.105 20.105 20.107
(0.118) (0.136) (0.146) (0.145) (0.145) (0.144) (0.145)
% Nonwhite 0.063 20.384 20.440 20.443 20.422 20.422 20.440
(0.358) (0.403) (0.452) (0.449) (0.448) (0.448) (0.446)
% Female 21.470 22.095* 21.867† 21.800† 21.875† 21.882† 21.841†
(0.911) (1.041) (1.095) (1.091) (1.091) (1.091) (1.090)
Area % nonwhite 20.864† 21.121* 21.097† 21.079† 21.078† 21.081† 21.070†
(0.466) (0.515) (0.561) (0.559) (0.554) (0.555) (0.555)
Area % female 221.623*** 224.008*** 224.541** 224.225** 223.694** 223.682** 223.318**
(5.862) (6.499) (7.106) (7.084) (7.136) (7.138) (7.138)
Area population 0.099† 0.085 0.082 0.077 0.075 0.075 0.071
(0.051) (0.057) (0.058) (0.058) (0.058) (0.058) (0.058)
Model N 2,380 2,030 1,872 1,872 1,872 1,872 1,871
F test of period effects 46.530*** 33.770*** 36.270*** 37.240*** 37.190*** 37.110*** 37.490***

Notes: Standard errors in parentheses.



p , .10
* p , .05
** p , .01
*** p , .001 (two-tailed)

explanation and supplemental analysis, as it can be What can be inferred from Figure 2b, first, is that
difficult to interpret. A common assumption might the effect of crime rate on the relevant outcome is
be that the two effects (crime rate with no board, and uniformly positive when no civilian board exists,
crime rate with board) are not statistically distin- and, second, that the effect becomes indistinguish-
guishable because their confidence intervals over- able from 0 when a board does exist. There are two
lap. However, the confidence intervals plotted here different explanations for this pattern. First, the
reflect tests of the predicted marginal effects with crime rate slope (i.e., the marginal effect of crime rate
respect to 0 (i.e., no effect) rather than tests of on the outcome) is positive when no board exists, but
whether each prediction differs from the other. As this slope is reduced when a board exists—in fact, be-
a result, no useful information is provided by ex- coming indistinguishable from 0. This is consistent
amining whether confidence intervals overlap with with the moderating effect predicted in Hypothesis 3.
each other in this figure. However, alternatively, the pattern could arise because
2018 Desai 235

TABLE 4
Random-Effects Ordered Logistic Regression Models of Community Policing Practicesa
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

Crime rate 10.650** 10.595** 11.898**


(3.872) (3.879) (3.981)
Use of force complaints 0.000 0.001
(0.001) (0.001)
Crime rate 3 Force complaints 20.050*
(0.024)
Civilian oversight board 0.556† 0.401 0.402 0.445
(0.286) (0.292) (0.292) (0.297)
Nonlethal patrol gear 0.051 0.063 0.064 0.063
(0.070) (0.070) (0.070) (0.070)
Organizational units 0.309*** 0.293*** 0.294*** 0.292***
(0.065) (0.065) (0.065) (0.066)
Operating expenses 0.151 0.250 0.230 0.291†
(0.151) (0.154) (0.164) (0.171)
Calls for service 0.148 0.064 0.061 0.074
(0.099) (0.102) (0.102) (0.103)
Full-time sworn officers 20.068* 20.063* 0.007 20.105
(0.033) (0.031) (0.188) (0.200)
Classroom training hours 0.320 0.332 0.334 0.334
(0.447) (0.450) (0.451) (0.449)
Field training hours 0.938** 0.809** 0.830** 0.806*
(0.299) (0.305) (0.314) (0.315)
Collective bargaining 20.388† 20.430† 20.429† 20.421†
(0.227) (0.227) (0.227) (0.227)
% Nonwhite 20.285 20.613 20.603 20.550
(0.662) (0.679) (0.682) (0.688)
% Female 22.571 22.409 22.479 22.375
(2.083) (2.073) (2.088) (2.098)
Area % nonwhite 20.571 20.895 20.893 20.780
(0.897) (0.896) (0.898) (0.915)
Area % female 218.690 221.194† 221.357† 220.805†
(11.790) (11.689) (11.726) (11.752)
Area population 0.096 0.113 0.108 0.110
(0.079) (0.080) (0.081) (0.082)
Model N 877 877 877 877
F test of period effects 12.360*** 10.750** 10.650** 10.800**

a
Standard errors in parentheses.

p , .10
* p , .05
** p , .01
*** p , .001 (two-tailed)

of insufficient statistical power; the prediction a significant difference (p , .05, x2, 1 df) between the
when a board exists might appear closer to 0 be- marginal effect of crime rate (at each of the values
cause its confidence interval has expanded to in- plotted in this figure) when the moderator is at its low
clude 0, not necessarily because it is statistically plotted value (i.e., no board) versus when it is at the
distinct from the originally predicted effect without high plotted value (i.e., board exists). Results of the
a board. Thus, a more careful test would involve tests can be graphed to facilitate their interpretation.
determining whether the moderation itself is sta- In the resulting figure, a confidence interval that does
tistically significant. not overlap with 0 indicates that the moderating effect
This can be accomplished through testing the con- is statistically significant at the relevant values of the
trasts of these predicted marginal effects (using the crime rate variable. Figure 2b(i) (notice the “i” suffix)
“contrast” option of the margins command in Stata/SE plots the contrasts of marginal effects for Figure 2b. As
13). Stated briefly, the tests evaluate whether there is shown in the figure, these contrasts are statistically
236 Academy of Management Journal February

FIGURE 1
Main Effect of Crime Rate (Table 3, Model 2)

A B
Avg. marginal effect on community policing

Avg. marginal effect on community policing


0

.6
–.5

.4
–1

.2
–1.5

0
.02 .04 .06 .08 .02 .04 .06 .08
Crime rate Crime rate
Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 1. Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 2.

C
Avg. marginal effect on community policing
1.5
1
.5
0

.02 .04 .06 .08


Crime rate
Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 3.

significant for most values of crime rate (below significant over roughly half of the plotted range (for
roughly 0.06). Combined with the information crime rate values above 0.05). Again, this pattern of
from Figure 2b, this series of findings suggests results is consistent with Hypothesis 3, for those
that the positive effect of crime rate (on employ- values of crime rate that achieve significance as
ing two out of three practices) is moderated shown in the figure. To summarize, then, Hypothesis
by board existence over much of its range. 3 is not supported in the domain of using one practice
This pattern of results is directly consistent with in isolation, but it does receive support in many cases
Hypothesis 3. with respect to using two or all three practices in
Moving on, Figure 2c plots the relevant effects on combination.
using all three practices. Here, the marginal effect of Figures 3a, 3b, and 3c examine the moderating
crime rate is positive when no board exists, and ap- effect of nonlethal patrol gear from Model 6 in
pears reduced when a board does exist. A test of Table 3. Here, this measure of controversy does not
contrasts, displayed in the supplemental Figure 2c appear to moderate the direct influence of crime
(i), reveals that this moderating effect is statistically rate—the predicted marginal effects of crime rate
2018 Desai 237

FIGURE 2
A, B and C, Moderating Effect of Civilian Board (Table 3, Model 4). Bi and Ci, Contrasts of Civilian Board
Moderation (Table 3, Model 4)

A B C

ffect on community policing


ffect on community policing

f ct on community policing
1

2
2

1
.5
1

–3 –2 –1 0
0
0

–.5

Avg. marginal effe


–1

Avg. marginal eff


Avg. marginal eff

–1
–2

.02 .04 .06 .08 .02 .04 .06 .08 .02 .04 .06 .08
Crime rate Crime rate Crime rate
civil. board = 0 civil. board = 1 civil. board = 0 civil. board = 1 civil. board = 0 civil. board = 1

Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 1. Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 2. Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 3.

Bi Ci
1.5
Contrast of marginal effects from

Contrast of marginal effects from


low to high moderator values

4
low to high moderator values
1

3
.5

1 2
0

0
–.5

.02 .04 .06 .08 .02 .04 .06 .08


Crime rate Crime rate
Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 2. Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 3.

(more specifically, their confidence intervals) Hypothesis 4, given that the effect of crime rate is
typically overlap with 0 at low as well as high negative (rather than positive) and does not appear to
values of the moderator in each of these figures. become less positive as the moderator increases.
Separate tests of contrasts (not displayed) did not re- Figure 4b plots the effects on using two out of three
veal significant differences among the relevant effects practices. Here, the predicted marginal effect of
in any of these cases. Thus, no support for Hypothesis crime rate at the low moderator value appears posi-
4 is obtained through any results using this measure of tive over much of its range (below roughly 0.06), and
controversy. the marginal effect at the high moderator value ap-
Figures 4a, 4b, and 4c use the alternative use of pears indistinguishable from 0. Again, this pattern
force complaints measure, plotting the results from requires supplemental analyses of contrasts, which
Model 4 in Table 4. As before, the first of these figures appear in Figure 4b(i). As seen in the supplemental
(Figure 4a) plots the predicted marginal effects on figure, the moderating effect achieves significance
using one out of the three community policing only at low levels of crime rate (below roughly 0.03).
practices. The marginal effect of crime rate when the Thus, while some support for Hypothesis 4 can be
moderator is at its lower bound (0 force complaints) inferred with respect to using two out of three prac-
is negative, and its confidence interval does not tices, this support is extremely limited in scope.
overlap with 0, indicating that increases in the crime Finally, Figure 4c plots the predicted marginal
rate will suppress (rather than raise) the likelihood of effects on using all three practices. Here, the pre-
using one practice in isolation when there are no dicted effect of crime rate at the low moderator value
complaints. The marginal effect of crime rate when the is uniformly positive at all plotted values, while the
moderator takes a value of one standard deviation effect becomes indistinguishable from 0 at the high
above its mean (307.73 force complaints) appears moderator value. Figure 4c(i) displays the results of
higher, with its confidence interval spanning 0. This supplemental contrast tests in this case, which show
pattern of results does not provide support for that the moderating effect achieves acceptable
238 Academy of Management Journal February

FIGURE 3
Moderating Effect of Nonlethal Gear (Table 3, Model 6)

A B
Avg. marginal effect on community policing

Avg. marginal effect on community policing


.8
0

.6
–.5

.4
.2
–1

0
–1.5

–.2
.02 .04 .06 .08 .02 .04 .06 .08
Crime rate Crime rate
nonleth. gear = 1.16 nonleth. gear = 4.08 nonleth. gear = 1.16 nonleth. gear = 4.08

Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 1. Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 2.

C
Avg. marginal effect on community policing
2
1.5
1
.5
0

.02 .04 .06 .08


Crime rate

nonleth. gear = 1.16 nonleth. gear = 4.08

Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 3.

significance at all plotted levels of the crime rate not supported with respect to using one collaborative
variable above 0.02. This pattern of results provides practice in isolation, but is typically supported with
strong support for Hypothesis 4, with controversial respect to using two or all three practices in combi-
practices measured through use of force complaints nation. Hypothesis 3, regarding the moderating effect
and with respect to using all three community po- of stakeholder power, is also not supported for one
licing practices in combination. practice, but does receive some support with respect
To collectively summarize results of all the study’s to using two or all three practices in combination.
hypothesis tests, Hypothesis 1 (predicting that col- Finally, Hypothesis 4, regarding the moderating effect
laborative engagement will increase the volume of of controversial practices, receives no support when
information received from external stakeholders) is controversy is measured through the existence of
supported from the analyses reported in Table 2 for particular practices (nonlethal patrol gear). However,
agencies using one or two practices, although it is not when measuring controversy more directly through
supported for agencies using all three practices in stakeholder complaints (use of force complaints), the
combination. Hypothesis 2 (predicting that problems hypothesis is unsupported with respect to using one
will increase the use of collaborative engagement) is collaborative practice in isolation, partially (within an
2018 Desai 239

FIGURE 4
A, B and C, Moderating Effect of Force (Table 4, Model 4). Bi and Ci, Contrasts of Force Moderation
(Table 4, Model 4)

A B C
Avg. marginal effect on community policing

Avg. marginal effect on community policing


Avg. marginal effect on community policing
4

4
2
2

2
1
0

0
–2

–1

–2
–4

–2

–4
.02 .04 .06 .08 .02 .04 .06 .08 .02 .04 .06 .08
Crime rate Crime rate Crime rate
force = 0 force = 307.73 force = 0 force = 307.73 force = 0 force = 307.73

Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 1. Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 2. Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 3.

Bi Ci

5
2

Contrast of marginal effects from


Contrast of marginal effects from

low to high moderator values


low to high moderator values

4
1

3
0

2
–1

1
–2

.02 .04 .06 .08 .02 .04 .06 .08


Crime rate Crime rate
Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 2. Notes: For community policing practices outcome = 3.

extremely limited range) supported for two practices, at least two, of the supporting practices, rather than
and strongly supported with respect to using all three only one in isolation). In hindsight, this pattern is
practices in combination. perhaps unsurprising, given that some law enforce-
ment agencies may adopt community policing on
a superficial rather than substantive level (Morabito,
DISCUSSION
2010; Zhao et al., 1999). It may be the case that fairly
Collectively, this study’s findings suggest that or- topical community engagement practices are rela-
ganizations using collaborative engagements, mea- tively common in this setting, but that more thorough
sured here through community policing practices engagements are driven by the study’s theorized pro-
within the law enforcement context, may benefit cesses. And, perhaps counterintuitively, Hypothesis 1
through increased access to outside information from is only supported for agencies that employ one or two
external stakeholders. Organizations generally appear (rather than all three) practices, suggesting that agencies
to use these engagements in response to problems re- with small to moderate implementations may receive
quiring outside information, as predicted by theories of the greatest benefits from their community policing
organizational search (Cyert & March, 1963; Gavetti efforts (in terms of service calls received). In hind-
et al., 2012). However, results also suggest that decision sight, it may be the case that extremely thorough
makers take risks to their organization’s legitimacy into implementations of community policing, which in-
account, becoming less likely to engage collaboratively clude changes to internal organizational attributes
when stakeholders are relatively powerful or when past such as officers’ performance evaluation criteria,
practices have directly been called into question. In- may not gain much external public visibility beyond
terestingly, Hypotheses 2 through 4 only appear more moderate adoptions of community policing.
supported for fairly thorough implementations of The study’s findings contribute directly to orga-
community policing (involving all three, or sometimes nizational search and learning theories, and also
240 Academy of Management Journal February

raise implications for theories of organizational le- 2012; Oliver, 1991; Suchman, 1995), but that con-
gitimacy. First, learning theories increasingly at- cerns about legitimacy may also indirectly affect
tempt to identify the precise mechanisms and other organizational activities—such as organiza-
processes involved in the organizational search and tional search and learning (Cyert & March, 1963;
knowledge accumulation process (Argote & Miron- Gavetti et al., 2012; Greve, 2003). Indeed, while
Spektor, 2011; Gavetti et al., 2012). While much work much work has examined the legitimacy motives
has examined organizational search and exploration that often underlie stakeholder engagement (Oliver,
(Argote & Miron-Spektor, 2011; Gavetti et al., 2012; 1991; Piazza & Perretti, 2015), and some work has
Levitt & March, 1988), little research addresses also acknowledged the benefits for organizational
whether organizations may abstain from particular learning (Roloff, 2008; Roome & Wijen, 2006), these
search or learning processes due to the potential risks concerns are rarely examined simultaneously de-
associated with those activities (Chen & Miller, 2012; spite their strong potential to interactively influence
Hawk, Pacheco-de-Almeida, & Yeung, 2013). The organizational activity in practice.
present study provides a theoretical framework to Second, hypotheses about the antecedents of
examine whether, and when, organizations may ab- community policing were supported for fairly com-
stain from certain search activities, given their re- prehensive or substantive implementations, rather
ciprocal potential to reveal proprietary information than for topical commitments to one or a few prac-
to outsiders. The study’s findings regarding stake- tices in isolation. This pattern indirectly contributes
holder power and controversy, which generally to broader research on the institutionalization of or-
conform to predictions derived from theories of or- ganizational practices, which generally suggests
ganizational legitimacy (Suchman, 1995), suggest that widespread or longstanding practices are in-
that organizational decision makers might weigh the creasingly adopted for ceremonial reasons or in rel-
possible risks prior to collaboratively engaging with atively superficial ways (e.g., Crilly et al., 2012;
external stakeholders. This is important evidence, DiMaggio & Powell, 1983; Tolbert & Zucker, 1983).
because it raises the possibility that organizations in By this study’s analysis period, community polic-
other settings, and with respect to other modes of ing practices had existed for decades, were fairly
organizational learning, may pursue a sophisticated widespread, and were increasingly expected of po-
approach regarding the possible consequences of lice departments by some of their communities
exploration and learning activities. This has broad (Morabito, 2010; Zhao et al., 1999). Yet, many of the
implications for theories of vicarious learning, study’s results applied more strongly to substantive
which routinely examine organizations’ external adoptions, and not to more widespread but relatively
searches of their environments (Baum & Dahlin, topical commitments. Based on the study’s findings,
2007; Yang et al., 2010). Future research, for in- it is possible that some organizations pursue collab-
stance, should determine whether common pre- orative engagements in substantive ways when they
dictions such as nonlocal search in response to poor need access to new information, even when other
performance (Baum & Dahlin, 2007), observation organizations pursue the collaborations for more
and learning through the actions of similar organi- symbolic or topical reasons. This suggests a complex
zations under uncertainty (Haunschild & Miner, adoption process that differs from the widely held
1997), and a variety of other relevant findings belief that later adopters are primarily motivated for
from this stream of research may be tempered in ceremonial reasons, and future research on practice
cases in which such learning might be reciprocal institutionalization would be wise to explore these
in nature. nuances.
The present study also contributes indirectly to the In addition to those contributions, this study also
literature on organizational legitimacy, in two ways suggests the need for additional research regarding
(Oliver, 1991; Piazza & Perretti, 2015; Suchman, how communities or other third-party outsiders can
1995). First, considerations regarding organizational influence the nature of organization–stakeholder
legitimacy seem to influence organizations’ pursuit collaboration, or even shape specific organizational
of collaborative engagements, since the engagements processes such as exploration and learning. While
could adversely affect external stakeholders’ judg- some research already highlights the role that third
ments regarding some organizations’ practices and parties play in shaping the information, resources, and
approaches. This finding suggests that organizations legitimacy available to organizations (e.g., Deephouse
not only manage their legitimacy directly through & Heugens, 2009; Hiatt & Park, 2013; Rhee & Valdez,
various reputation-building tactics (e.g., Crilly et al., 2009), this study’s findings suggest a complex and
2018 Desai 241

iterative process, whereby organizational decision regarding these engagements. The study’s frame-
makers balance judgments regarding the benefits and work and findings suggest the need for future work
risks of collaborating—while external parties concur- examining collaborative engagement in other
rently evaluate whether and how to engage with the contexts, as well as additional research that in-
organization. The reciprocal character of these ex- tegrates theories of learning and legitimacy more
changes raises a number of interesting and important generally.
questions that the present study unfortunately could
not address, such as whether outside parties modify
their interactions with an organization based on their REFERENCES
own learning about the organization, how resource or Adams, K., & Jennison, V. 2007. What we do not know
power disparities between organizations and their about police use of Tasers. Policing, 30: 447–465.
stakeholders affect the likelihood or nature of collabo- Ajilore, O. 2015. The militarization of local law enforce-
rations, and ultimately how collaborations between ment: Is race a factor? Applied Economics Letters, 22:
organizations and complex or fragmented stakeholder 1089–1093.
communities iteratively unfold over time.
Ang, S., & Cummings, L. L. 1997. Strategic response to in-
This study also contains several limitations. First,
stitutional influences on information systems out-
law enforcement agencies are not exposed to the
sourcing. Organization Science, 8: 235–256.
same competitive pressures as organizations in other
market-oriented settings; thus, the processes that Argote, L., & Miron-Spektor, E. 2011. Organizational
drive collaborative engagement here may differ from learning: From experience to knowledge. Organiza-
tion Science, 22: 1123–1137.
those in other contexts. To that end, care was taken to
develop generalizable theory regarding the motives Azadani, P. N., Tseng, Z. H., Ermakov, S., Marcus, G. M., &
for and against collaboration, and to control for Lee, B. K. 2011. Funding source and author affiliation
a variety of idiosyncratic factors within the study’s in Taser research are strongly associated with a con-
empirical analysis. Nonetheless, additional work clusion of device safety. American Heart Journal,
that extends this theoretical framework into other 162: 533–537.
settings would certainly provide value. In particular, Bailey, W. C. 1996. Less-than-lethal weapons and police-
some research suggests that crowdsourcing (Estelles- citizen killings in U.S. urban areas. Crime and De-
Arolas & Gonzalez-Ladron-de-Guevara, 2012), open linquency, 42: 535–552.
source software development (Lerner & Tirole, Barnett, W. P., & Hansen, M. T. 1996. The red queen in
2002), and telemedicine (Roine et al., 2001) may organizational evolution. Strategic Management
provide appealing contexts for further research into Journal, 17: 139–157.
organizational collaborations with broad and diverse Bartels, E. C., & Silverman, E. B. 2005. An exploratory
stakeholder groups. study of the New York City civilian complaint review
Second, the study’s data are admittedly coarse, with board mediation program. Policing, 28: 619–630.
many measures tracked only once every three to four
Baum, J. A., & Dahlin, K. 2007. Aspiration performance and
years on the corresponding law enforcement agency
railroads’ patterns of learning from train wrecks and
(LEMAS) survey. For this reason, great care was taken crashes. Organization Science, 18: 368–385.
to inform the study with anecdotal evidence and in-
dustry research in order to adhere carefully with dy- Bozeman, W. P., Hauda, W. E., Heck, J. J., Graham, D. D.,
Martin, B. P., & Winslow, J. E. 2009. Safety and injury
namics in this setting. Nonetheless, intervening issues
profile of conducted electrical weapons used by law
within unobservable years may affect the study’s tests,
enforcement officers against criminal suspects.
and replication in settings with more frequent mea-
Annals of Emergency Medicine, 53: 480–489.
surement is certainly advisable.
In conclusion, the present study integrates theo- Brandl, S. G., & Stroshine, M. S. 2012. The physical hazards
ries of organizational search and learning with le- of police work revisited. Police Quarterly, 15:
gitimacy management to examine collaborative 262–282.
stakeholder engagement. Given its reciprocal nature, Cameron, C., & Trivedi, P. 1998. Regression analysis of
this form of engagement is distinct from other com- count data. New York, NY: Cambridge University
monly researched search activities. To that end, the Press.
study combines theories of organizational search Campbell, D., & Campbell, K. 2016. Police/military con-
and learning with research on organizational le- vergence in the USA as organisational mimicry. Po-
gitimacy in order to generate novel predictions licing and Society, 26: 332–353.
242 Academy of Management Journal February

Chen, M. J., & Miller, D. 2012. Competitive dynamics: Greve, H. R. 1998. Performance, aspirations, and risky or-
Themes, trends, and a prospective research platform. ganizational change. Administrative Science Quar-
The Academy of Management Annals, 6: 135–210. terly, 43: 58–86.
Chiu, S.-C., & Sharfman, M. 2011. Legitimacy, visibility, Greve, H. R. 2003. Organizational learning from perfor-
and the antecedents of corporate social performance: mance feedback: A behavioral perspective on in-
An investigation of the instrumental perspective. novation and change. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge
University Press.
Journal of Management, 37: 1558–1585.
Haleblian, J., & Finkelstein, S. 1999. The influence of or-
Chowaniec, C., Kobek, M., Jablonski, C., Kabiesz-
ganizational acquisition experience on acquisition
Neniczka, S., & Karczewska, W. 2008. Case-study of
performance: A behavioral learning perspective. Ad-
fatal gunshot wounds from non-lethal projectiles.
ministrative Science Quarterly, 44: 29–56.
Forensic Science International, 178: 213–217.
Haunschild, P. R., & Miner, A. S. 1997. Modes of in-
Crilly, D., Zollo, M., & Hansen, M. T. 2012. Faking it or terorganizational imitation: The effects of outcome
muddling through? Understanding decoupling in re- salience and uncertainty. Administrative Science
sponse to stakeholder pressures. Academy of Man- Quarterly, 42: 472–500.
agement Journal, 55: 1429–1448.
Hawdon, J. 2008. Legitimacy, trust, social capital, and po-
Crow, M. S., & Adrion, B. 2011. Focal concerns and police licing styles a theoretical statement. Police Quarterly,
use of force: Examining the factors associated with 11: 182–201.
Taser use. Police Quarterly, 14: 366–387. Hawk, A., Pacheco-de-Almeida, G., & Yeung, B. 2013.
Cyert, R. M., & March, J. G. 1963. A behavioral theory of Fast-mover advantages: Speed capabilities and
the firm. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. entry into the emerging submarket of Atlantic
basin LNG. Strategic Management Journal, 34:
de Guzman, M. C., & Frank, J. 2004. Using learning as
1531–1550.
a construct to measure civilian review board impact
on the police: The Philippine experience. Policing, Hiatt, S. R., & Park, S. 2013. Lords of the harvest: Third-
party influence and regulatory approval of genetically
27: 166–182.
modified organisms. Academy of Management
Deephouse, D. L., & Heugens, P. 2009. Linking social issues Journal, 56: 923–944.
to organizational impact: The role of infomediaries
Hillman, A. J., & Keim, G. D. 2001. Shareholder value,
and the infomediary process. Journal of Business
stakeholder management, and social issues: What’s
Ethics, 86: 541–553. the bottom line? Strategic Management Journal, 22:
DiMaggio, P. J., & Powell, W. W. 1983. The iron cage 125–139.
revisited: Institutional isomorphism and collective Hoetker, G. 2007. The use of logit and probit models in
rationality in organizational fields. American Socio- strategic management research: Critical issues. Stra-
logical Review, 48: 147–160. tegic Management Journal, 28: 331–343.
Estelles-Arolas, E., & Gonzalez-Ladron-de-Guevara, F. Hudson, B. A. 2008. Against all odds: A consideration of
2012. Towards an integrated crowdsourcing defini- core-stigmatized organizations. Academy of Man-
tion. Journal of Information Science, 38: 189–200. agement Review, 33: 252–266.
Ferdik, F. V., Kaminski, R. J., Cooney, M. D., & Sevigny, Jauchem, J. R. 2015. Taser-conducted electrical weapons:
E. L. 2014. The influence of agency policies on con- Misconceptions in the scientific/medical and other
ducted energy device use and police use of lethal literature. Forensic Science, Medicine, and Pathol-
force. Police Quarterly, 17: 328–358. ogy, 11: 53–64.

Fiss, P. C., & Zajac, E. J. 2006. The symbolic management Julian, S. D., Ofori-Dankwa, J. C., & Justis, R. T. 2008. Un-
derstanding strategic responses to interest group
of strategic change: Sensegiving via framing and
pressures. Strategic Management Journal, 29:
decoupling. Academy of Management Journal, 49:
963–984.
1173–1193.
Karaca-Mandic, P., Norton, E. C., & Dowd, B. 2012. In-
Gavetti, G., Greve, H. R., Levinthal, D. A., & Ocasio, W.
teraction terms in nonlinear models. Health Services
2012. The behavioral theory of the firm: assessment Research, 47: 255–274.
and prospects. The Academy of Management An-
Kessell, E. R., Alvidrez, J., McConnell, W. A., & Shumway,
nals, 6: 1–40.
M. 2009. Effect of racial and ethnic composition of
Greene, W. 2010. Testing hypotheses about interaction neighborhoods in San Francisco on rates of mental
terms in nonlinear models. Economics Letters, 107: health-related 911 calls. Psychiatric Services
291–296. (Washington, D.C.), 60: 1376–1378.
2018 Desai 243

Kim, J. Y., & Miner, A. S. 2007. Vicarious learning from the Montiel, I., Husted, B. W., & Christmann, P. 2012. Using
failures and near-failures of others: Evidence from private management standard certification to reduce
the U.S. commercial banking industry. Academy of information asymmetries in corrupt environments.
Management Journal, 50: 687–714. Strategic Management Journal, 33: 1103–1113.
Kraska, P., & Kappeler, V. 1997. Militarizing American Morabito, M. S. 2010. Understanding community policing
police: The rise and normalization of paramilitary as an innovation: Patterns of adoption. Crime and
units. Social Problems, 44: 1–18. Delinquency, 56: 564–587.
Lamin, A., & Zaheer, S. 2012. Wall Street vs. Main Street: Oliver, C. 1991. Strategic responses to institutional processes.
Firm strategies for defending legitimacy and their Academy of Management Review, 16: 145–179.
impact on different stakeholders. Organization Sci- Pache, A.-C., & Santos, F. 2013. Inside the hybrid organi-
ence, 23: 47–66. zation: Selective coupling as a response to competing
Lee, B. K., Vittinghoff, E., Whiteman, D., Park, M., Lau, institutional logics. Academy of Management Jour-
L. L., & Tseng, Z. H. 2009. Relation of Taser (electrical nal, 56: 972–1001.
stun gun) deployment to increase in in-custody sud- Panzar, J. 2014, December 9. Police wrestle with
den deaths. The American Journal of Cardiology, definition of chokehold. Los Angeles Times. Avail-
103: 877–880. able at: www.latimes.com/nation/la-na-chokehold-
Lerner, J., & Tirole, J. 2002. Some simple economics of 20141210-story.html
open source. The Journal of Industrial Economics, Pfeffer, J., & Salancik, G. R. 1978. The external control of
50: 197–234. organizations: A resource dependence perspective.
Levitt, B., & March, J. G. 1988. Organizational learning. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Annual Review of Sociology, 14: 319–340. Piazza, A., & Perretti, F. 2015. Categorical stigma and firm
MacDonald, J. M., Kaminski, R. J., & Smith, M. R. 2009. The disengagement: Nuclear power generation in the
effect of less-lethal weapons on injuries in police use- United States, 1970–2000. Organization Science, 26:
of-force events. American Journal of Public Health, 724–742.
99: 2268–2274. Pidgeon, K. C., Bragg, S., Ball, K., & Meltzer, T. 2008. Un-
March, J. G. 1991. Exploration and exploitation in common cause of death: The use of Taser guns in
organizational learning. Organization Science, 2: South Florida. Journal of Emergency Nursing: JEN,
71–87. 34: 305–307.
Martinez, R. J., & Dacin, M. T. 1999. Efficiency motives Rhee, M., & Valdez, M. E. 2009. Contextual factors sur-
and normative forces: Combining transactions costs rounding reputation damage with potential implica-
and institutional logic. Journal of Management, 25: tions for reputation repair. Academy of Management
75–96. Review, 34: 146–168.
McGregor, A. 2016. Politics, police accountability, and Roine, R., Ohinmaa, A., & Hailey, D. 2001. Assessing
public health: Civilian review in Newark, New Jersey. telemedicine: A systematic review of the literature.
Journal of Urban Health: Bulletin of the New York Canadian Medical Association Journal, 165:
Academy of Medicine, 93: S141–S153. 765–771.
Meeks, D. 2006. Police militarization in urban areas: The Roloff, J. 2008. Learning from multi-stakeholder networks:
obscure war against the underclass. The Black Issue-focussed stakeholder management. Journal of
Scholar, 35: 33–41. Business Ethics, 82: 233–250.
Mendelson, J. E., Tolliver, B. K., Delucchi, K. L., Baggott, Roome, N., & Wijen, F. 2006. Stakeholder power and or-
M. J., Flower, K., Harris, C. W., Galloway, G. P., & ganizational learning in corporate environmental
Berger, P. 2010. Capsaicin, an active ingredient in management. Organization Studies, 27: 235–263.
pepper sprays, increases the lethality of cocaine. Rosenkopf, L., & Almeida, P. 2003. Overcoming local
Forensic Toxicology, 28: 33–37. search through alliances and mobility. Management
Meyer, J. W., & Rowan, B. 1977. Institutionalized organi- Science, 49: 751–766.
zations: Formal-structure as myth and ceremony. Sampson, R. C. 2005. Experience effects and collaborative
American Journal of Sociology, 83: 340–363. returns in R&D alliances. Strategic Management
Mitchell, R. K., Agle, B. R., & Wood, D. J. 1997. Toward Journal, 26: 1009–1031.
a theory of stakeholder identification and salience: Sasson, C, et al 2015. Barriers to calling 911 and learning
Defining the principle of who and what really counts. and performing cardiopulmonary resuscitation for
Academy of Management Review, 22: 853–886. residents of primarily Latino, high-risk neighborhoods
244 Academy of Management Journal February

in Denver, Colorado. Annals of Emergency Medicine, U.S. Department of Justice. 2007. Law enforcement man-
65: 545–552. agement and administrative statistics (ICPSR31161-v1).
Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Politi-
Sinha, P., Daellenbach, U., & Bednarek, R. 2015. Legiti-
cal and Social Research.
macy defense during post-merger integration: Be-
tween coupling and compartmentalization. Strategic Vilke, G. M., & Chan, T. C. 2007. Less lethal technology:
Organization, 13: 169–199. Medical issues. Policing, 30: 341–357.
Sousa, W., Ready, J., & Ault, M. 2010. The impact of Tasers Vilke, G. M., Johnson, W. D., Castillo, E. M., Sloane, C., &
on police use-of-force decisions: Findings from a ran- Chan, T. C. 2009. Tactical and subject considerations
domized field-training experiment. Journal of Ex- of in-custody deaths proximal to use of conductive
energy devices. The American Journal of Forensic
perimental Criminology, 6: 35–55.
Medicine and Pathology, 30: 23–25.
Suchman, M. C. 1995. Managing legitimacy: Strategic and
Williams, R. 2006. Generalized ordered logit/partial pro-
institutional approaches. Academy of Management
portional odds models for ordinal dependent vari-
Review, 20: 571–610.
ables. The Stata Journal, 6: 58–82.
Sutton, R. I., & Galunic, D. C. 1996. Consequences of public
Wooldridge, J. M. 2002. Econometric analysis of cross
scrutiny for leaders and their organizations. Research section and panel data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
in Organizational Behavior, 18: 201–250.
Yang, H. Y., Phelps, C., & Steensma, H. K. 2010. Learning
Taylor, B., Alpert, G., Kubu, B., Woods, D., & Dunham, R. G. from what others have learned from you: The effects of
2011. Changes in officer use of force over time: A de- knowledge spillovers on originating firms. Academy
scriptive analysis of a national survey. Policing, 34: of Management Journal, 53: 371–389.
211–232.
Zhao, J. H., Lovrich, N. P., & Thurman, Q. 1999. The status
Taylor, B., & Woods, D. J. 2010. Injuries to officers and sus- of community policing in American cities: Facilitators
pects in police use-of-force cases: A quasi-experimental and impediments revisited. Policing, 22: 74–92.
evaluation. Police Quarterly, 13: 260–289.
Tobin, K. E., Davey, M. A., & Latkin, C. A. 2005. Calling
emergency medical services during drug overdose: An
examination of individual, social, and setting corre- Vinit Desai (vinit.desai@ucdenver.edu) is an associate
lates. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 100: 397–404. professor of management at the University of Colorado
Tolbert, P. S., & Zucker, L. G. 1983. Institutional sources of Denver. His research examines issues related to organiza-
change in the formal-structure of organizations: The tional learning, failures, and organizational legitimacy.
diffusion of civil-service reform, 1880–1935. Admin-
istrative Science Quarterly, 28: 22–39.
Copyright of Academy of Management Journal is the property of Academy of Management
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.

You might also like