You are on page 1of 18

Gifted Education Intemational2000 VoltS, pp 4-21

©2000 A B Academic Publishers

Robert J. Sternberg
Yale University, USA*

the theory of successful


intelligence
Abstract
This article describes the theory of successful intelligence and haw it can be applied to gifted educa-
tion. The article opens with an introduction to the issue of abilities in gifted education. It continues
by discussing the inadequacy of notions of IQ or general ability for fully characterizing intellectual
giftedness. Then it describes the themy of successful intelligence. Next it presents evidence in favor of
the statistical validity and usefulness of the theory in gifted education. Finally it draws conciusions.

Some educators in the field of the gifted The argument of this article is that iden-
believe that IQ and related constructs give an tification, instruction, and assessment of
adequate assessment of whether a child is achievement of gifted children should be
intellectually gifted. These educators may more broadly construed. In particular, I hope
use other criteria, such as motivation or to replace the conventional model of intelli-
school achievement in identification of gifted gence with a model of successful intelligence.
children, but rely on tests of IQ to assess the
"intellectual" portion of giftedness. These Successful intelligence is the ability to
educators also may believe that instruction achieve success in life, given one's personal
for gifted children is adequate when it teach- standards, within one's sociocultural context
es in a way that is attuned to high IQ in other (Sternberg, 1997, 1999a, 1999d). One's ability
words, when it emphasizes memory and to achieve success depends on one's capital-
analytical skills. For example, most accelera- izing on one's strengths and correcting or
tion programs· require for success the same compensating for one's weaknesses through
IQ-based skills as do regular instructional a balance of analytical, creative, and practical
programs, only more so. Often assessment of abilities in order to adapt to, shape, and
achievement of gifted children then is also in select environments. Gifted people do these
terms of knowledge accumulated via memo- things at a higher level than do othets.
ry and analytical skills. But what about the
highly creative child who writes short sto- The remainder of this article is divided
ries, paints, or devises science experiments? into four main parts. First I argue that con-
How about the highly practical child who ventional and some other notions of intelli-
starts a business, or repairs electronic gence are, at best, incomplete, and, at worst,
devices, or can convince almost anyone to wrong. Second I suggest an alternative
adopt his or her point of view? Are they not notion of successful intelligence that
intellectually gifted too, even if their IQs are expands upon conventional notions of intel-
not above some predetermined level? ligence. Third I present evidence in favor of
• Dept of Psychology, Yale University, Box 208205, New Haven, CT 06520-8205, USA E-mail: robert.sternbergCa.yale.edu
4, Gifted Education International

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
the theory and its usefulness in classrooms. medicating themselves and others. Thus,
Finally I draw some conclusions about the tests of how to use these medicines consti-
nature of intelligence and its relationship to tute effective measures of one aspect of prac-
gifted education. tical intelligence as defined by the villagers
as well as their life circumstances in their
The Conventional Notion of Intelligence is environmental contexts. Middle-class
Inadequate Westerners might find it quite a challenge to
thrive or even survive in these contexts, or,
In this section I argue that conventional for that matter, in the contexts of urban ghet-
notions of intelligence are incomplete. My tos often not distant from their comfortable
argument is based on research conducted homes.
among the members of my research group at
Yale and collaborators from all over the We measured the Kenyan children's abil-
world. ity to identify the medicines, where they
come from, what they are used for, and how
There now has accumulated a substantial they are dosed. Based on work we had done
body of evidence suggesting that, contrary elsewhere, we expected that scores on this
to conventional notions, intelligence is not a test would not correlate with scores on con-
unitary construct. Thus, giftedness is not ventional tests of intelligence. In order to test
unitary either. This evidence is of a variety of this hypothesis, we also administered to the
different kinds, most of which suggest that 85 children the Raven Coloured Progressive
the positive manifold (pattern of positive Matrices Test, which is a measure of fluid or
correlations) among ability tests is likely not abstract-reasoning-based abilities, as well as
to be a function of some inherent structure of the Mill Hill Vocabulary Scale, which is a
intellect. Rather, it reflects interactions measure of crystallized or formal knowl-
among the kinds of individuals tested, the edge-based abilities. In addition, we gave the
kinds of tests used in the testing, and the sit- children a comparable test of vocabulary in
uations in which the individuals are tested. their own Dholuo language. The Dholuo lan-
guage is spoken in the home, English in the
In a study in Usenge, Kenya, near the schools.
town of Kisumu, we were interested in
school-age children's ability to adapt to their We did indeed find no correlation
indigenous environment. We devised a test between the test of indigenous tacit knowl-
of practical intelligence for adaptation to the edge and scores on the fluid-ability tests. But
environment (see Sternberg & Grigorenko, to our surprise, we found statistically signif-
1997; Sternberg et al., in press). The test icant correlations of the tacit-knowledge
measured children's informal tacit knowl- tests with the tests of crystallized abilities.
edge for natural herbal medicines that the The correlations, however, were negative. In
villagers believe can be used to fight various other words, the higher the children scored
types of infections. At least some of these on the test of tacit knowledge, the lower they
medicines appear to be effective (Dr. scored, on average, on the tests of crystal-
Frederick Okatcha, personal communica- lized abilities. This surprising result can be
tion), and most villagers certainly believe in interpreted in various ways, but based on
their efficacy, as shown by the fact that chil- the ethnographic observations of the cultur-
dren in the villages use their knowledge of al anthropologists on our team, Geissler and
these medicines an average of once a week in Prince, we concluded that a plausible see-

Volume 15 No 1 2000, 5

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
nario takes into account the expectations of which children learn a craft from an early
families for their children. age. They learn what they will need to know
in order to succeed in a trade, but not a lot
Children generally drop out of school more. They are not simultaneously engaged
before graduation, and most families in the in tasks that require the development of the
village do not particularly value formal particular blend of skills measured by con-
Western schooling. There is no reason they ventional intelligence tests. Hence it is less
should, as their children will for the most likely that one would observe a general fac-
part spend their lives farming or engaged in tor in their scores, much as we discovered in
other occupations that make little or no use Kenya.
of Western schooling. These families empha-
size teaching their children the indigenous The test of practical intelligence we
informal knowledge that will lead to suc- developed for use in Kenya, as well as some
cessful adaptation in the environments in of the other practically-based tests described
which they will really live. Children who in this essay, may seem more like tests of
spend their time learning the indigenous achievement or of developing expertise (see
practical knowledge of the community gen- Ericsson, 1996; Howe, Davidson, & Sloboda,
erally do not invest themselves heavily in 1998) than of intelligence. But I have argued
doing well in school, whereas children who that intelligence is itself a form of developing
do well in school generally do not invest expertise -that there is no clearcut distinc-
themselves as heavily in learning the indige- tion between the two constructs (Sternberg,
nous knowledge- hence the negative corre- 1998a, 1999b). Indeed, all measures of intelli-
lations. gence, one might argue, measure a form of
developing expertise. Crystallized ability
The Kenya study suggests that the iden- tests, such as tests of vocabulary and general
tification of a general factor of human intelli- information, certainly measure developing
gence or of a single IQ may tell us more and developed knowledge base. And avail-
about how abilities interact with patterns of able data suggest that fluid-ability tests, such
schooling and especially Western patterns of as tests of abstract reasoning, measure devel-
schooling than it does about the structure of oping and developed expertise even more
human abilities. In Western schooling, chil- strongly than do crystallized-ability tests.
dren typically study a variety of subject mat- Probably the best evidence for this claim is
ters from an early age and thus develop that fluid-ability tests have shown much
skills in a variety of skill areas. This kind of greater increases in scores over the last sev-
schooling prepares the children to take a test eral generations than have crystallized abili-
of intelligence, which typically measures ty tests (Flynn, 1984, 1987; Neisser, 1998).
skills in a variety of areas. Often intelligence The relatively brief period of time during
tests measure skills that children were which these increases have occurred (about 9
expected to acquire a few years before taking points of IQ per generation) suggests an
the intelligence test. But as Rogoff ( 1990) environmental rather than a genetic cause of
and others have noted, this pattern of school- the increases. And the substantially greater
ing is not universal and has not even been increase for fluid than for crystallized tests
common for much of the history of suggests that fluid tests, like all other tests,
humankind. Throughout history and in actually measure an expertise acquired
many places still, schooling, especially for through interactions with the environment.
boys, takes the form of apprenticeships in This is not to say that genes do not influence

6, Gifted Education International

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
intelligence: Almost certainly they do the tests dynamically rather than statically
(Bouchard, 1997; Plomin, 1997; Scarr, 1997). (Brown & Ferrara, 1985; Budoff, 1968; Day,
Rather, the point is that the environment Engelhardt, Maxwell, & Bolig, 1997;
always mediates their influence and tests of Feuerstein, 1979; Grigorenko & Sternberg,
intelligence measure gene-environment 1998; Guthke, 1993; Haywood & Tzuriel,
interaction effects. We are seeking out people 1992; Lidz, 1987, 1991; Tzuriel, 1995;
who are gifted by virtue of very high levels Vygotsky, 1978). Dynamic testing is like con-
of developing expertise. ventional static testing in that individuals
are tested and inferences about their abilities
The forms of developing expertise that made. But dynamic tests differ in that chil-
are viewed as practically or otherwise intelli- dren are given some kind of feedback in
gent may differ from one society to another order to help them improve their scores.
or from one sector of a given society to Vygotsky (1978) suggested that the chil-
another. For example, procedural knowledge dren's ability to profit from the guided
about natural herbal medicines, on the one instruction the children received during the
hand, or Western medicines, on the other, testing session could serve as a measure of
may be critical to survival in one society, and children's zone of proximal development
irrelevant to survival in another (e.g., where (ZPD), or the difference between their devel-
one or the other type of medicine is not avail- oped abilities and their latent capacities. In
able). Whereas what constitutes components other words, testing and instruction are
of intelligence is universal, the content that treated as being of one piece rather than as
constitutes the application of these compo- being distinct processes. This integration
nents to adaptation to, shaping, and selec- makes sense in terms of traditional defini-
tion of environments is culturally and even tions of intelligence as the ability to learn
subculturally variable. ("Intelligence and its measurement," 1921;
Sternberg & Detterman, 1986). What a
The developing world provides a partic- dynamic test does is directly measure
ularly interesting laboratory for testing theo- processes of learning in the context of test-
ries of intelligence because many of the ing rather than measuring these processes
assumptions that are held as dear in the indirectly as the product of past learning.
developed world simply do not apply. A Such measurement is especially important
study we have done in Tanzania (see when not all children have had equal oppor-
Sternberg & Grigorenko, 1997; Sternberg et tunities to learn in the past.
al., 1999) points out the risks of giving tests,
scoring them, and interpreting the results as In our assessments, children were first
measures of some latent intellectual ability given the ability tests. Then they were given
or abilities. We administered to 358 young a brief period of instruction in which they
school children near Bagamoyo, Tanzania, were able to learn skills that would poten-
tests including a form-board classification tially enable them to improve their scores.
test, a linear syllogisms test, and a Twenty Then they were tested again. Because the
Questions Test, which measure the kinds of instruction for each test lasted only about 5-
skills required on conventional tests of intel- 10 minutes, one would not expect dramatic
ligence. Of course, we obtained scores that gains. Yet, on average, the gains were statis-
we could analyze and evaluate, ranking the tically significant. More importantly, scores
children in terms of their supposed general on the pretest showed only weak although
or other abilities. However, we administered significant correlations with scores on the

Volume 15 No 1 2000, 7

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
post-test. These correlations, at about the .3 The Nature of Successful Intelligence
level, suggested that when tests are adminis-
tered statically to children in developing The theory of successful intelligence has 4
countries, they may be rather unstable and key elements (see also Sternberg, 1997).
easily subject to influences of training. The
reason could be that the children are not 1. Intelligence is defined in terms of the ability
accustomed to taking Western-style tests, to achieve success in life in terms of one's per-
and so profit quickly even from small sonal standards, within one's sociocultural
amounts of instruction as to what is expect- context. The field of intelligence has at
ed from them. Of course, the more important times tended to put "the cart before the
question is not whether the scores changed horse," defining the construct conceptu-
or even correlated with each other, but rather ally on the basis of how it is operational-
how they correlated with other cognitive ized rather than vice versa. This practice
measures. In other words, which test was a has resulted in tests that stress the aca-
better predictor of transfer to other cognitive demic aspect of intelligence, as one might
performance, the pretest score or the post- expect, given the origins of modern intel-
test score? We found the post-test score to be ligence testing in the work of Binet and
the better predictor. Simon (1916) in designing an instrument
that would distinguish children who
Dynamic testing can be used in devel- would succeed from those who would
oped as well as in developing countries. In fail in school. But the construct of intelli-
one of our studies, we devised a test of for- gence needs to serve a broader purpose,
eign-language learning ability that dynami- accounting for the bases of success in all
cally measured participants' ability to learn of one's life.
an artificial language at the time of test. The
language was quite complex and required The use of societal criteria of success
learning of many different facets, presented (e.g., school grades, personal income) can
both orally and visually (Grigorenko, obscure the fact that these criteria often
Sternberg, & Ehrman, in press). We found do not capture people's personal notions
that scores on our test correlated more high- of success. Some people choose to con-
ly with a test of foreign-language learning centrate on extracurricular activities such
ability (the Modern Language Aptitude as athletics or music and pay less atten-
Test- MLAT) than with a test of general tion to grades in schoo~ others may
ability. Scores also significantly predicted choose occupations that are personally
success in foreign language classrooms at the meaningful to them but that never will
U.S. Foreign Services Institute (PSI), an insti- yield the income they could gain doing
tute for teaching languages to foreign-serv- work that is less personally meaningful.
ice officers and military personnel. In the theory of successful intelligence,
however, the conceptualization of intelli-
· What, then, is intelligence? Who is gift- gence is always within a sociocultural
ed? These questions are addressed in the context. Although the processes of intelli-
next section. gence may be common across such con-
texts, what constitutes success is not.
Being a successful member of the clergy
of a particular religion may be highly

8, Gifted Education International

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
rewarded in one society and viewed as a Definitions of intelligence traditionally
worthless pursuit in another culture. have emphasized the role of adaptation
to the environment (Intelligence and its
2. One's ability to achieve success depends on measurement, 1921; Sternberg &
one's capitalizing on one's strengths and cor- Detterman, 1986). But intelligence
recting m· compensating for one's weakness- involves not only modifying oneself to
es. Theories of intelligence typically spec- suit the environment (adaptation), but
ify some relatively fixed set of abilities, also modifying the environment to suit
whether one general factor and a number oneself (shaping), and sometimes, find-
of specific factors (Spearman, 1904), ing a new environment that is a better
seven multiple factors (Thurstone, 1938), match to one's skills, values, or desires
or eight multiple intelligences (Gardner, (selection).
1999). Such a specification is useful in
establishing a common set of skills to be Not all people have equal opportunities
tested. But people achieve success, even to adapt to, shape, and select environ-
within a given occupation, in many dif- ments. In general, people of higher
ferent ways. For example, successful socioeconomic standing tend to have
teachers and researchers achieve success more opportunities and people of lower
through many different blendings of socioeconomic standing have fewer. The
skills rather than through any single for- economy or political situation of the soci-
mula that works for all of them. ety also can be factors. Other variables
that may affect such opportunities are
3. Success is attained through a balance of ana- education and especially literacy, politi-
lytical, creative, and practical abilities. cal party, race, religion, and so forth. For
Analytical abilities are the abilities pri- example, someone with a college educa-
marily measured by traditional tests of tion typically has many more possible
abilities. But success in life requires one career options than does someone who
not only to analyze one's own ideas as has dropped out of high school in order
well as the ideas of others, but also to to support a family. Thus, how and how
generate ideas and to persuade other well an individual adapts to, shapes, and
people of their value. This necessity selects environments must always be
occurs in the world of work, as when a viewed in terms of the opportunities the
subordinate tries to convince a superior individual has.
of the value of his or her plan; in the
world of personal relationships, as when
a child attempts to convince a parent to Validity and Educational Utility of
do what he or she wants or when a the Theory of Successful Intelligence
spouse tries to convince the other spouse
to do things his or her preferred way; and An important foundation of the theory of
in the world of the school, as when a stu- successful intelligence is the importance of
dent writes an essay arguing for a point analytical, creative, and practical abilities to
of view. intellectual functioning. A number of the
studies described below show both the inter-
4. Balancing of abilities is achieved in order to nal validity and the external validity of these
adapt to, shape, and select environments. constructs. We differ from some other theo-

Volume 15 No 1 2000, 9

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
rists in our emphasis on the predictive Students have to say what number
empirical validation of the theory. In other should come next in·a series of numbers.
words, we wish to show not only that the
theory is plausible, but that it can predict 3. Analytical-Figural: Matrices. Students
data-in other words, that it is scientifically see a figural matrix with the lower right
as well as educationally supportable. entry missing. They have to say which of
the options fits into the missing space.
The Relative Independence of
Analytical, Creative, and Practical 4. Practical-Verbal: Everyday reasoning.
Students are presented with a set of
Abilities. everyday problems in the life of an ado-
lescent and have to select the option that
Three separate factor-analytic studies sup-
best solves each problem.
port the relative independence of the three
abilities posited by the theory of successful
5. Practical-Quantitative: Everyday math.
intelligence. Such studies examine the men-
Students are presented with scenarios
tal abilities that underlie performance on requiring the use of math in everyday life
tests of intelligence or other psychological
(e.g., buying tickets for a ballgame), and
constructs.
have to solve math problems based on
the scenarios.
In one study (Sternberg, Grigorenko,
Ferrari, & Clinkenbeard, 1999), we used the
6. Practical-Figural: Route planning.
so called Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test
Students are presented with a map of an
(STAT- Sternberg, 1993) to investigate the
area (e.g., an entertainment park) and
internal validity of the theory. Three hun- have to answer questions about navigat-
dred twenty-six high school students who
ing effectively through the area depicted
had been identified by their schools as gift-
by the map.
ed, primarily from diverse parts of the
United States, took the test, which com-
7. Creative-Verbal: Novel analogies.
prised 12 subtests in all. There were four
Students are presented with verbal analo-
subtests each measuring analytical, creative, gies preceded by counterfactual premises
and practical abilities. For each type of abili- (e.g., money falls off trees). They have to
ty, there were three multiple-choice tests and
solve the analogies as though the counter-
one essay test. The multiple-choice tests, in
factual premises were true.
tum, involved respectively, verbal, quantita-
tive, and figural content. Consider the con-
8. Creative-Quantitative: Novel number
tent of each test:
operations. Students are presented with
rules for novel number operations, for
1 . Analytical-Verbal: Figuring out mean-
example, "flix," which involves numeri-
ings of neologisms (artificial words) from
cal manipulations that differ as a func-
natural contexts. Students see a novel tion of whether the first of two operands
word embedded in a paragraph, and is greater than, equal to, or less than the
have to infer its meaning from the con- second. Participants have to use the
text. novel number operations to solve pre-
sented math problems.
2. Analytical-Quantitative: Number series.

10, Gifted Education International

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
9. Creative-Figural: In each item, partici- found that the creative tests are moderately
pants are first presented with a figural correlated with each other and the practical
series that involves one or more transfor- tests are highly correlated with each other.
mations; they then have to apply the rule The two kinds of tests are distinct from one
of the series to a new figure with a differ- another, however. Interestingly, the perform-
ent appearance, and complete the new ance-based assessments tend to cluster sepa-
series. rately from multiple-choice assessments
measuring the same skills (similar to our ear-
We found that a confirmatory factor lier findings of essay measures tending to be
analysis on the data was supportive of the distinctive from multiple-choice measures).
triarchic theory of human intelligence, yield- These results further suggest the need for
ing separate and uncorrelated analytical, cre- measuring not only a variety of abilities, but
ative, and practical factors. The lack of corre- also, for measuring these abilities through
lation was due to the inclusion of essay as various modalities of testing.
well as multiple-choice subtests. Although
multiple-choice tests tended to correlate sub- In a second and separate study, conduct-
stantially with multiple-choice tests, their ed with 3278 students of varying ages in the
correlations with essay tests were much United States, Spain, and Finland, we used
weaker. We found the multiple-choice ana- the multiple-choice section of the STAT to
lytical subtest to load most highly on the compare five alternative models of intelli-
analytical factor, but the essay creative and gence, again via confirmatory factor analy-
performance subtests to load most highly on sis. A model featuring a general factor of
their respective factors. Thus, measurement intelligence fits the data relatively poorly.
of creative and practical abilities probably The triarchic model, allowing for intercorre-
ideally should be accomplished with other lation among the analytic, creative, and prac-
kinds of testing instruments that comple- tical factors, provided the best fit to the data
ment multiple-choice instruments. (Sternberg, Castejon, Prieto, Hautakami, &
Grigorenko, in press).
We have now developed a revised version
of this test, which, in a preliminary study of In a third study, we tested 511 Russian
53 college students, shows outstanding inter- school children (ranging in age from 8 to 17
nal and external validation properties years) as well as 490 mothers and 328 fathers
(Grigorenko, Gil, Jarvin, & Sternberg, 2000). of these children. We used entirely distinct
This test supplements the creative and practi- measures of analytical, creative, and practi-
cal measures described above with perform- cal intelligence. In this study, factor analyses
ance-based measures. For example, creative yielded clearcut analytical, creative, and
abilities are additionally measured by having practical factors for the tests. Thus, with a
people write and tell short stories, by having sample of a different nationality (Russian), a
them do captions for cartoons, and by having different set of tests, and a different method
them use computer software to design a vari- of analysis (exploratory rather than confir-
ety of products. Practical skills are measured matory analysis) again supported the theory
additionally by an everyday situational judg- of successful intelligence. Now consider in
ment inventory and a college-student tacit more detail each of three major aspects of
knowledge inventory. These tests require successful intelligence: analytical, creative,
individuals to make decisions about every- and practical.
day problems faced in life and in school. We

Volume 15 No 12000, 11

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
The Theory of Successful Intelligence We used the Sternberg Triarchic Abilities
in Gifted Education Test, as described above. The test was
administered to 326 children identified as
We have done three studies that look simul- gifted around the United States and in some
taneously at the relevance of analytical, cre- other countries who were identified by their
ative, and practical abilities for schooling. schools as gifted by any standard whatsoev-
These studies were based on the notion that er. Children were selected for a summer pro-
teachers can teach for successful intelligence. gram in (college-level) psychology if they fell
We have written a book for teachers that into one of five ability groupings: high ana-
describes in detail how it is done (Sternberg lyticaL high creative, high practical, high bal-
& Grigorenko, 2000).
anced (high in all three abilities), or low bal-
anced (low in all three abilities). Students
Basically, teaching for successful intelli- who came to Yale were then divided into
gence involves helping students recognize four instructional groups. Students in all
and capitalize upon their strengths at the four instructional groups used the same
same time the teacher helps students recog- introductory-psychology textbook (a prelim-
nize and correct or compensate for weak- inary version of Sternberg [1995]) and lis-
nesses. Instructional activities, as well as tened to the same psychology lectures. What
assessments of achievement, involve a bal- differed among them was the type of after-
ance among memory, analytical, creative, noon discussion section to which they were
and practical skills. For example, with regard assigned. They were assigned to an instruc-
to memory, students are asked to recall and tional condition that emphasized either
recognize material that they have learned. memory, analytical, creative, or practical
With regard to analytical thinking, students instruction. For example, in the memory con-
are asked to analyze, critique, judge, com- dition, they might be asked to describe the
pare and contrast, and evaluate what they main tenets of a major theory of depression.
have learned. With regard to creative think- In the analytical condition, they might be
ing, students are asked to create, invent, asked to compare and contrast two theories
explore, discover, and imagine things based of. depression. In the creative condition, they
on, but that go beyond, what they have IIDght be asked to formulate their own theo-
learned. And with regard to practical think- ry of depression. In the practical condition,
ing, students are asked to apply, implement, they might be asked how they could use
and put into practice what they have what they had learned about depression to
learned. help a friend who was depressed.

In the first set of studies, we explored the Students in all four instructional condi-
question of whether conventional education tions were evaluated in terms of their per-
in school systematically discriminates formance on homework, a midterm exam, a
against children with creative and practical final exam, and an independent project. Each
strengths (Sternberg & Clinkenbeard, 1995; type of work was evaluated for memory,
Sternberg, Ferrari, Clinkenbeard, & analytical, creative, and practical quality.
Grigorenko, 1996; Sternberg, Grigorenko, Thus, all students were evaluated in exactly
Ferrari, & Clinkenbeard, 1999). Motivating the same way.
this work was the belief that the systems in
schools strongly tend to favor children with Our results suggested the utility of the
strengths in memory and analytical abilities. theory of successful intelligence. First, we

12, Gifted Education International

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
observed when the students arrived at Yale in school. Children with creative and practi-
that the students in the high creative and cal abilities, who are almost never taught or
high practical groups were much more assessed in a way that matches their pattern
diverse in terms of racial, ethnic, socioeco- of abilities, may be at a disadvantage in
nomic, and educational backgrounds than course after course, year after year.
were the students in the high-analytical
group, suggesting that correlations of meas- In a follow-up study (Sternberg, Torff, &
ured intelligence with status variables such Grigorenko, 1998a, 1998b), we looked at
as these may be reduced by using a broader learning of social studies and science by
conception of intelligence. Thus, the kinds of third-graders and eighth-graders. The 225
students identified as strong differed in terms third-graders were students in a very low-
of populations from which they were drawn income neighborhood in Raleigh, North
in comparison with students identified as Carolina. The 142 eighth graders were stu-
strong solely by analytical measures. More dents who were largely middle to upper-
importantly, just by expanding the range of middle class studying in Baltimore,
abilities we measured, we discovered intel- Maryland, and Fresno, California. In this
lectual strengths that might not have been study, students were assigned to one of three
apparent through a conventional test. instructional conditions. In the first condi-
tion, they were taught the course that basi-
We found that all three ability tests-ana- cally they would have learned had we not
lytical, creative, and practical- significantly intervened. The emphasis in the course was
predicted course performance. When multi- on memory. In a second condition, they were
ple-regression analysis was used, at least taught in a way that emphasized critical
two of these ability measures contributed (analytical) thinking. In the third condition,
significantly to the prediction of each of the they were taught in a way that emphasized
measures of achievement. Perhaps as a analytical, creative, and practical thinking.
reflection of the difficulty of deemphasizing All students' performance was .assessed for
the analytical way of teaching, one of the sig- memory learning (through multiple-choice
nificant predictors was always the analytical assessments) as well as for analytical, cre-
score. (However, in a replication of our study ative, and practical learning (through per-
with low-income African-American students formance assessments).
from New York, Deborah Coates of the City
University of New York found a different As expected, we found that students in
pattern of results. Her data indicated that the the successful-intelligence (analytical, cre-
practical tests were better predictors of ative, practical) condition outperformed the
course performance than were the analytical other students in terms of the performance
measures, suggesting that what ability test assessments. One could argue that this result
predicts what criterion depends on popula- merely reflected the way they were taught.
tion as well as mode of teaching.) Most Nevertheless, the result suggested that
importantly, there was an aptitude-treatment teaching for these kinds of thinking succeed-
interaction whereby students who were ed. More important, however, was the result
placed in instructional conditions that better that children in the successful-intelligence
matched their pattern of abilities outper- condition outperformed the other children
formed students who were mismatched. In even on the multiple-choice memory tests. In
other words, when students are taught in a other words, to the extent that one's goal is
way that fits how they think, they do better just to maximize children's memory for

Volume 15 No 1 2000, 13

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
information, teaching for successful intelli- difference not only in laboratory tests, but in
gence is still superior. It enables children to school classrooms and even the everyday life
capitalize on their strengths and to correct ~r of adults as well.
to compensate for their weaknesses, and 1t
allows children to encode material in a vari- Conclusion
ety of interesting ways.
The time has come to move beyond conven-
We have now extended these results to tional theories of intelligence. In this essay I
reading curricula at the middle-school a~d have provided data suggesting that conven-
the high-school level. In a study of 871 mld- tional theories and tests of intelligence are
dle-school students and 432 high school stu- incomplete. Hence conventional notions of
dents, we taught reading either triarchically giftedness are incomplete. The general factor
or through the regular curriculum. At the is an artifact of limitations in populations of
middle-school level, reading was taught individuals tested, types of materials with
explicitly. At the high school level, read~ng which they are tested, and types of methods
was infused into instruction in mathematics, used in testing. Indeed, our studies show
physical sciences, social sciences, English, that even when one wants to predict school
history, foreign languages, and the arts. In all
performance, the conventio~a~ tests. ~re
settings, students who were taught tri- somewhat limited in their predictive validity
archially substantially outperformed stu- (Sternberg & Williams, 1997). I have pro-
dents who were taught in standard ways posed a theory of successful intelligence and
(Grigorenko, Jarvin, & Sternberg, 2000). its development that fares well in construct
validations, whether one tests in the labora-
In the third study-the Grigorenko- tory, in schools, or in the work place. The
Sternberg (in press) study described above, greatest obstacle to our moving on is in vest-
the analytical, creative, and practical tests we ed interests, both in academia and in the
employed were used to predict mental and world of tests, where testing companies are
physical health among the Russian adults. doing well financially with existing tests. We
Mental health was measured by widely used now have ways to move beyond convention-
paper-and-pencil tests of depression and al notions of intelligence and giftedness; we
anxiety and physical health was measured need only the will.
by self-report. The best predictor. of ~ent~l
and physical health was the prachcal-mtelh- What is especially interesting is that lay
gence measure. Analytical intelligence came conceptions of intelligence are quite a bit
second and creative intelligence came third.
broader than ones of psychologists who
All three contributed to prediction, however.
believe in general ability (g) (Berry 1974;
Thus, we again concluded that a theory of
Sternberg & Kaufman, 1998). For example, in
intelligence encompassing all three elements a study of people's conceptions of intelli-
provides better prediction of success in life gence (Sternberg, Conway, Ketron, &
than does a theory comprising just the ana- Bernstein, 1981; see also Sternberg, 1985b),
lytical element. we found that lay persons had a three factor
view of intelligence as comprising practical
Thus the results of three sets of studies problem solving, verbal, and social co.~~e­
suggest that the theory of successful intelli- tence abilities. Only the first of these abihtles
gence is valid as a whole. Moreover, the is measured by conventional tests. In a study
results suggest that the theory can make a of Taiwanese Chinese conceptions of intelli-

14, Gifted Education International

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
gence (Yang & Sternberg, 1997a, 1997b), we as administered by the Office of Educational
found that although Taiwanese conceptions Research and Improvement, U.S.
of intelligence included a cognitive factor, ·Department of Education. Grantees under-
they also included factors of interpersonal taking such projects are encouraged to
competence, intrapersonal competence, intel- express freely their professional judgment.
lectual self assertion, and intellectual self- This article, therefore, does not necessarily
effacement. In a study of Kenya conceptions represent the position or policies of the
of intelligence (Grigorenko et al., in press), Office of Educational Research and
we found that four distinct terms constitute Improvement of the U.S. Department of
rural Kenyan conceptions of intelligence - Education, and no official endorsement
rieko (knowledge and skills), luoro (respect), should be inferred. Preparation of the article
winjo (comprehension of how to handle real- was further supported by funds from the
life problems), paro (initiative - with only Partnership for Child Development.
the first directly referring to knowledge- Requests for reprints should be sent to
based skills (including but not limited to the Robert J. Sternberg, Department of
academic). Even more importantly, perhaps, Psychology, Yale University, Box 208205,
we discovered in a study among different New Haven, CT 06520-8205 USA.
ethnic groups in San Jose, California, that
although the 359 parents in different ethnic
groups have different conceptions of intelli- References
gence, the more closely their conception
matches that of their children's teachers, the Barnes, M. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Social
best the children do in school (Okagaki & intelligence and decoding of nonverbal cues.
Sternberg, 1993). In other words, teachers Intelligence, 13, 263-287.
value students who do well on the kinds of
attributes that the teachers associate with Berry, J. W. (1974). Radical cultural relativism and
intelligence. The attributes they associate the concept of intelligence. In J. W. Berry & P. R.
Dasen (Eds.), Culture and cognition. Readings in
with intelligence are too limited.
cross-cultural psychology (pp. 225-229). London:
Methuen.
In sum, intelligence involves far more
than just the memory and analytical abilities Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1916). The development of
that have played such an important part in intelligence in children. Baltimore: Williams &
gifted education. These abilities matter for Wilkins. (Originally published 1905)
gifted education, but so should creative and
practical ones. The time has come to expand Bouchard, T. J. (1997). IQ similarity in twins
gifted education to recognize all the gifts that reared apart: Findings and responses to critics. In
R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.),
students can bring to their schoolwork and
Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 126-160).
to their work throughout their lives. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Brand, C. (1996). The g factor. general intelligence


Author Notes and its implications. Chichester, England: Wiley.

Preparation of this article was supported by Brody, N. (2000) History of theories and measure-
ments of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),
Grant REC-9979843 from the National Handbook of intelligence (pp. 16-33). New York:
Science Foundation and by a grant under the Cambridge University Press.
Javits Act Program (Grant No. R206R950001)

Volume 15 No 1 2000, 15

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
Brown, A. L., & Ferrara, R. A. (1985). Diagnosing Day, J.D., Engelhard, J. L., Maxwell, S. E., & Bolig,
zones of proximal development. In J. V . Wertsch E. E. (1997). Comparison of static and dynamic
(Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition. assessment procedures and their relation to inde-
Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 273-305). New York: pendent performance. journal of Educational
Cambridge University Press. Psychology, 89,358-368.

Budoff, M. (1968). Learning potential as a supple- Embretson, S., & McCollam, K. (2000).
mentary assessment procedure. In J. Hellmuth Psychometric approaches to the understanding
(Ed.), Learning disorders (Vol. 3, pp. 295-343). and measurement of intelligence. In R. J.
Seattle, WA: Special Child. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 423-
444). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Carraher, T. N., Carraher, D., & Schliemann, A. D.
(1985). Mathematics in the streets and in schools. Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.). (1996). The road to excellence.
British journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 21 -29. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A Feuerstein, R. (1979). The dynamic assessment of
survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: retarded performers: The learning potential assessment
Cambridge University Press. device, theory, instrument, and techniques. Baltimore,
MD: University Park.
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities. Their structure,
growth and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Flynn, J. R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans:
Massive gains 1932 to 1978. Psychological Bulletin,
Cattell, R. B., & Cattell, A. K. (1973). Test of g: 95,29-51.
Culture Fair, Level 2. Champaign, IL: Institute for
Personality and Ability Testing. Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations:
What IQ tests really measure. Psychological
Ceci, S. J. (1996). On intelligence: A bioecological Bulletin, 101, 171 - I 91.
treatise on intellectual development (expanded ed.).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of
multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.
Ceci, S. J., & Liker, J. (1986). Academic and
nonacademic intelligence: an experimental sepa- Gardner, H. (1999). Are there additional intelli-
ration. In R. J. Sternberg & R. K. Wagner (Eds.), gences? The case for naturalist, spiritual, and exis-
Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of compe- tential intelligences. In J. Kane (Ed.), Education,
tence in the everyday world (pp. 119-142). New York: information, and transformation (pp. 111 - 131).
Cambridge University Press. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Ceci, S.J., & Roazi, A. (1994). The effects of context Gardner, H., Krechevsky, M., Sternberg, R. J., &
on cognition: Postcards from Brazil. In R. J. Okagaki, L. (1994). Intelligence in context:
Sternberg & R. K. Wagner (Eds.}, Mind in context. Enhancing students' practical intelligence for
Interactionist perspectives on human intelligence (pp. school. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons.
74-101). New York: Cambridge University Press. Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice
(pp. 105-127). Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books.
Davidson, J. E., & Sternberg, R. J. (1984). The role
of insight in intellectual giftedness. Gifted Child Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1998).
Quarterly, 28, 58-64. Dynamic assessment. Psychological Bulletin, 124,
75-111.
Davies, M., Stankov, L., & Roberts, R. D. (1998).
Emotional intelligence: In search of an elusive Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1999).
construct. journal of Personality and Social Parental influences on children's analytical, cre-
Psychology, 75, 985- 1015. ative, and practical intelligence. Manuscript sub-
mitted for publication.

16, Gifted Education International

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (in press). for training. Paper presented at the Society for
Analytical, creative, and practical intelligence as Industrial and Organizational Psychology
predictors of self-reported adaptive functioning: Conference, Dallas, Texas.
A case study in Russia. Intelligence.
Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell
Grigorenko, E. L., Gil, G., Jarvin, L., & Sternberg, curve. New York: Free Press.
R. J. (2000). Toward a validation of aspects of the
theory of successful intelligence. Manuscript sub- Horn, J. L. (1994). Theory of fluid and crystallized
mitted for publication. intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The encyclope-
dia of human intelligence (Vol. I, pp. 443-451). New
Grigorenko, E. L., Jarvin, L., & Sternberg, R. J. York: Macmillan.
(2000). School-based tests of the triarchic theory of
intelligence: Three settings, three samples, three Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A.
syllabi. Manuscript submitted for publication. (1998). Innate talents: Reality or myth? Behavioral
& Brain Sciences, 21, 399-442.
Grigorenko, E. L., Geissler, W., Prince, R.,
Okatcha, F., Nokes, C., Kenny, D., Bundy, D. & Intelligence and its measurement: A symposium.
Sternberg, R. J., (in press). The organization of (1921). Journal of Educational Psychology, 12, 123-
Luo conceptions of intelligence: A study of 147, 195-216,271-275.
implicit theories in a Kenyan village. International
Journal of Behavioral Development. Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor. The science of men-
tal ability. Westport, CT: Praeger/Greenwoood.
Grigorenko, E. L., Sternberg, R. J., & Ehrman, M.
(in press). A theory-based approach to the meas- Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathe-
urement of second-language learning ability: The matics, and culture in everyday life. New York:
CANAL-S theory and test. The Modern Language Cambridge University Press.
Journal.
Lidz, C. S. (Ed.). (1987). Dynamic assessment: An
Gustafsson, J. E. (1994). Hierarchical models of interactional approach to evaluating learning poten-
intelligence and educational achievement. In A. tial. New York: Guilford.
Demetriou & A. Efk.lides (Eds.), Intelligence, mind
and reasoning: Structure and development (pp. 45- Lidz, C. S. (1991). Practitioner 's guide to dynamic
73). Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier Science assessment. New York: Guilford
Publishers.
Lubart, T. I., & Sternberg, R. J. (1995). An invest-
Guthke, J. (1993). Current trends in theories and ment approach to creativity: Theory and data. In
assessment of intelligence. In J. H. M. Hamers, K. S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The
Sijtsma, & A. J. J. M. Ruijssenaars (Eds.), Learning creative cognition approach (pp. 269-302).
potential assessment (pp. 13-20). Amsterdam: Swets Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
& Zeitlinger.
Neisser, U. (Ed.). (1998). The rising curve.
Guyote, M., & Sternberg, R. J. ( 1981 ). A transitive- Washington, DC: American Psychological
chain theory of syllogistic reasoning, Cognitive Association.
Psychology, 13,461-525.
Nunes, T. (1994). Street intelligence. In R. J.
Haywood, H. C., & Tzuriel, D. (Eds.). (1992). Sternberg (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human intelligence
Interactive assessment. New York: Springer-Verlag. (Vol. 2, pp. 1045-1049). New York: Macmillan.

Hedlund, J., Sternberg, R. J., Horvath, J. A., & Nunes, T., Schliemann, A. D., & Carraher, D. W.
Dennis, M. (1998, April). The acquisition of tacit (1993). Street mathematics and school mathematics.
knowledge for military leadership: Implications New York: Cambridge University Press.

Volume 15 No 12000, 17

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
Okagaki, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Parental Sternberg, R. J. (1982). Natural, unnatural, and
beliefs and children's school performance. Child supernatural concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 14,
Development, 64, 36-56. 451-488.

Piaget, J. (1972). The psychology of intelligence. Sternberg, R. J. (1983). Components of human


Totowa, NJ: Littlefield Adams. intelligence. Cognition, 15, 1-48.

Plomin, R. (1997). Identifying genes for cognitive Sternberg, R. J. ( 1984). Toward a triarchic theory
abilities and disabilities. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. of human intelligence. Behavioral and Brain
Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and envi- Sciences, 7, 269-287.
ronment (pp. 89- 104). New York: Cambridge
University Press. Sternberg, R. J. (1985a). Beyond IQ. A triarchic theo-
ry of human intelligence. New York: Cambridge
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking. University Press.
Cognitive development in social context. New York:
Oxford University Press. Sternberg, R. J. (1985b). Implicit theories of intelli-
gence, creativity, and wisdom. journal of
Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., & Salovey, P. (2000). Personality and Social Psychology, 49,607-6-7.
Emotional intelligence meets traditional stan-
dards for an intelligence. Intelligence, 27, 267-298 Sternberg, R. J. (1987a). Most vocabulary is
learned from context. In M. G. McKeown & M. E.
Scarr, S. (1997). Behavior-genetic and socialization Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition
theories of intelligence: Truce and reconciliation. (pp. 89-105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.),
Intelligence, heredity and environment (pp. 3-41). Sternberg, R. J. (1987b). The psychology of verbal
New York: Cambridge University Press. comprehension. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in
instructional psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 97-151).
Schmidt, F., & Hunter, J. (1998). The validity and Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
utility of selection methods in personnel psychol-
ogy: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of mind.
years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, Conceptions of the nature of intelligence. New York:
124,262-274. Cambridge University Press.

Spearrnan, C. E. (1904). "General intelligence" Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Sternberg Triarchic


objectively determined and measured. American Abilities Test (STAT). Unpublished test.
journal of Psychology, 15, 201 -293.
Sternberg, R. J. (1998a). Abilities are forms of
Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information pro- developing expertise. Educational Researcher, 27,
cessing, and analogical reasoning. The componential 11-20.
analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sternberg, R. J. (1998b). A balance theory of wis-
Sternberg, R. J. (1980a). Representation and dom. Review of General Psychology, 2, 347-365.
process in linear syllogistic reasoning. journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 119-159. Sternberg, R. J. (1999a). Human intelligence: A
case study of how more and more research can
Sternberg, R. J. (1980b). Sketch of a componential lead us to know less and less about a psychologi-
subtheory of human intelligence. Behavioral and cal phenomenon, until finally we know much less
Brain Sciences, 3, 573-584. than we did before we started doing research. In
E. Tulving (Ed.), Memory, consciousness, and the
Sternberg, R. J. (1981). Intelligence and nonen- brain. The Tallinn Conference (pp. 363-373).
trenchment. journal of Educational Psychology, 73, Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
1-16.

18, Gifted Education International

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
Sternberg, R. ]. (1999b). Intelligence as developing Sternberg, R. ]., & Gastel, J. (1989a). Coping with
expertise. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24, novelty in human intelligence: An empirical
259-375. investigation. Intelligence, 13, 187-197.

Sternberg, R. ]. (1999c). A propulsion model of Sternberg, R. ]., & Gastel, J. (1989b). If dancers ate
creative contributions. Review of General their shoes: Inductive reasoning with factual and
Psychology, 3, 83- 100. counterfactual premises. Memory and Cognition,
17, 1-10.
Sternberg, R. ]. (1999d). The theory of successful
intelligence. Review of General Psychology, 3, 292- Sternberg, R. ]., & Grigorenko, E. L. (1997, Fall).
316. The cognitive costs of physical and mental ill
health: Applying the psychology of the developed
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.). (2000). Handbook of intelli- world to the problems of the developing world.
gence. New York: Cambridge University Press. Eye on Psi Chi, 2, 20-27.

Sternberg, R. ]., Castej6n, ]., & Prieto, M. D., Sternberg, R. ]., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2000).
Hautakami, ]., & Grigorenko, E. L. (in press). Teaching for successful intelligence. Arlington
Confirmatory factor analysis of the Sternberg Heights, IL: Skylight.
Triarchic Abilities Test (multiple-choice items) in
three international samples: An empirical test of Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E.L., Ferrari, M. &
the triarchic theory of intelligence. European Clinkenbeard, P. (1999). A triarchic analysis of an
journal of Psychological Assessment. aptitude-treatment interaction. European journal of
Psychological Assessment, 15, 1-11.
Sternberg, R. ]., Clinkenbeard, P. R. (1995). A tri-
archic model of identifying, teaching, and assess- Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., Ngorosho, D.,
ing gifted children. Roeper Review, 17, 255-260. Tantubuye, E., Mbise, A., Nokes, C., & Bundy, D.
A. (1999). Hidden intellectual potential in rural
Sternberg, R. ]., Conway, B. E., Ketron, ]. L., & Tanzanian school children. Manuscript submitted
Bernstein, M. (1981). People's conceptions of intel- for publication.
ligence. journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
41,37-55. Sternberg, R. ]., & Kalmar, D.A. (1997). When will
the milk spoil? Everyday induction in human
Sternberg, R. ]., & Detterman D. K. (Eds.). (1986). intelligence. Intelligence, 25, 185-203.
What is intelligence? Contemporary viewpoints on its
nature and def nition. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Sternberg, R. ]., & Kaufman, ]. C. (1998). Human
abilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 479-502.
Sternberg, R. ]., Ferrari, M., Clinkenbeard, P.R., &
Grigorenko, E. L. (1996). Identification, instruc- Sternberg, R. ]., & Lubart, T. I. (1991). An invest-
tion, and assessment of gifted children: A con- ment theory of creativity and its development.
struct validation of a triarchic model. Gifted Child Human Development, 34, 1-31.
Quarterly, 40, 129- 137.
Sternberg, R. ]., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the
Sternberg, R. ]., Forsythe, G. B., Horvath, ]., crowd. Cultivating creativity in a culture of conformi-
Hedlund,]., Snook, S., Williams, W. M., Wagner, ty. New York: Free Press.
R. K., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2000). Practical intelli-
gence in everyday life. New York: Cambridge Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T.l. (1996). Investing in
University Press. creativity. American Psychologist, 51, 677-688.

Sternberg, R. ]., & Gardner, M. K. (1983). Unities Sternberg, R. ]., & Nigro, G. (1980).
in inductive reasoning. journal of Experimental Developmental patterns in the solution of verbal
Psychology: General, 112, 80- 116. analogies. Child Development, 51, 27-38.

Volume 15 No 1 2000, 19

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
Sternberg, R. J., Nokes, K., Geissler, P. W., Prince, Sternberg, R. J., Wagner, R. K., Williams, W. M., &
R., Okatcha, F., Bundy, D. A., & Grigorenko, E. L. Horvath, J. A. (1995). Testing common sense.
(in press). The relationship between academic and American Psychologist, 50, 912-927.
practical intelligence: A case study in Kenya.
Intelligence. Sternberg, R. ]., & Williams, W. M. (1996). How to
develop student creativity. Alexandria, VA:
Sternberg, R. J., Okagaki, L., & Jackson, A. (1990). Association for Supervision and Curriculum
Practical intelligence for success in school. Development.
Educational Leadership, 48, 35-39.
Sternberg, R. ]., & Williams, W. M. (1997). Does
Sternberg, R. ]., & Powell, J. S. (1982). Theories of the Graduate Pecord Examination predict mean-
intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of ingful success in the graduate training of psychol-
human intelligence (pp. 975- 1005). New York: ogists? A case study. American Psychologist, 52,
Cambridge University Press. 630-641.

Sternberg, R. J., Powell, J. S., & Kaye, D. B. (1982). Tetewsky, S. J., & Sternberg, R. J. (1986).
The nature of verbal comprehension. Poetics, 11, Conceptual and lexical determinants of nonen-
155-187. trenched thinking. journal of Memory and
Language, 25, 202-225.
Sternberg, R. ]., Powell, C., McGrane, P. A., &
McGregor, S. (1997). Effects of a parasitic infection Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities.
on cognitive functioning. journal of Experimental Chicago: University of Chicago
Psychology: Applied, 3, 67-76.
Tzuriel, D. (1995). Dynamic-interactive assess-
Sternberg, R. ]., & Rifkin, B. (1979). The develop- ment. The legacy of L. S. Vygotsky and current
ment of analogical reasoning processes. journal of developments. Unpublished manuscript.
Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 195-232.
Vernon, P. E. (1971 ). The structure of human abilities.
Sternberg, R. J., & Smith, C. (1985). Social intelli- London: Methuen.
gence and decoding skills in nonverbal communi-
cation. Social Cognition, 2, 168 - 192. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. The develop-
ment of higher order processes. Cambridge, MA:
Sternberg, R. J., Torff, B., & Grigorenko, E. L. Harvard University Press.
(1998a). Teaching for successful intelligence raises
school achievement. Phi Delta Kappan, 79,667-669. Wagner, R. K. (1987). Tacit knowledge in everyday
intelligent behavior. journal of Personality and
Sternberg, R. ]., Torff, B., & Grigorenko, E L. Social Psychology, 52, 1236-47.
(1998b). Teaching triarchically improves school
achievement. journal of Educational Psychology, 90, Wagner, R. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (1986). Tacit
374-384. knowledge and intelligence in the everyday
world. In R. J. Sternberg & R. K. Wagner (Eds.),
Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (1993). The geo- Practical intelligence. Nature and origins of compe-
centric view of intelligence and job performance tence in the everyday world (pp. 51-83). New York:
is wrong. Current Directions in Psychological Cambridge University Press.
Science, 2, 1-4.
Williams, W. M., Blythe, T., White, N., Li, J.,
Sternberg, R. ]., Wagner, R. K., & Okagaki, L. Sternberg, R. J., & Gardner, H. I. (1996). Practical
(1993). Practical intelligence: The nature and role intelligence for school: A handbook for teachers of
of tacit knowledge in work and at school. In H. grades 5-8. New York: HarperCollins.
Reese & J. Puckett (Eds.), Advances in lifespan
development (pp. 205-227). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Yang, S., Sternberg, R. J. (1997a) Conceptions of
intelligence in ancient Chinese philosophy.

20, Gifted Education International

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015
journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, Yang, S., & Sternberg, R. J. (1997b). Taiwanese
17, 101-119. Chinese people's conceptions of intelligence.
Intelligence, 25, 21-36.

The Sheer Splendour Of It

The Universe in all its beauty can be described in entirely physical terms. This is a
concept which was initiated by Galileo when he asserted that science is concerned
only with primary qualities, those qualities of the external world that could be
weighed and measured. Secondary qualities - beauty, love and value, fell outside
the realm of Science. This concept is beginning to change as modern science tries to
find a grand unified theory which not only predicts events within the universe, but
also predicts the very existence of the universe itself.

As the scientists strip nature of its beauty when they formulate laws and theories to
describe it, they find that they can explain life, once the structure of the DNA mole-
cule is known, as the most probable set of chemical replications which the DNA
molecule can undergo. Or they can explain the appearance of the universe in terms
of the anthropic principle which states that the universe only looks the way it does,
because it is the most likely appearance it can have. Had it appeared differently,
then we wouldn't be here to observe the difference. Thus nature is reduced to sets
of formulae and statistics.

The problem with this concept appears not when individual segments of knowl-
edge are added to the growing mountain which already exists, by scientists who
endeavour to study in ever-increasingly specialized fields, but when an overall
look at the knowledge (formulae and statistics) is taken when trying to formulate a
grand unified theory, only that it exists as a secondary quality, and according to
Galileo, therefore should have no place in science.

What is this so-called secondary quality that predicts the existence of the universe?
It is the intelligence the system appears to have. For instance, we know that the Big
Bang Theory explains the formation of the universe, but why did the Big Bang
occur at that particular instant, when no time existed before. In other words, what
makes one particular point in the infinite vast sea of 'no time' so special? Surely, if
it was the most probable event to happen, then it could have happened at any ran-
dom instant in the infinite amount of instants. But we have evidence that, had it
not happened exactly as it did, then no universe would exist today.

This intelligence of the system is subjective because it cannot be weighed or meas-


ured. Nontheless, it is an integral part of science. Modern quantam physics recog-
nizes that objective and subjective cannot be separated, as theorists of quantum
mechanics include the observer in the observations. Thus nature has a sheer splen-
dour within itself, once all the superficial beauty is removed by the physics. Science
is a way of getting to this hidden splendour of nature.

P. duPlessis (age 15 years)


Volume 15 No 1 2000, 21

Downloaded from gei.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on February 14, 2015

You might also like