You are on page 1of 15

Development and Psychopathology, 11 (1999), 127–141

Copyright  1999 Cambridge University Press


Printed in the United States of America

Early conduct problems and later risk of


teenage pregnancy in girls

LIANNE J. WOODWARD AND DAVID M. FERGUSSON


Christchurch School of Medicine

Abstract
The relationship between conduct problems at age 8 years and teenage pregnancy by the age of 18 years was
analyzed in a birth cohort of 491 girls. A statistically significant association was found between early conduct
problems and later risk of teenage pregnancy, with girls in the most disturbed 10% of the cohort having a pregnancy
rate that was 5.3 times higher (p < .001) than the rate found in the least disturbed 50% of the cohort. The elevated
risk of teenage pregnancy amongst girls with early conduct problems was in part, explained by social and family
factors that were correlated with early conduct problems, and in part, by a causal chain process in which early
conduct problems were associated with increased rates of risk taking behaviors in adolescence, which in turn led to
an increased risk of teenage pregnancy. These results suggest that the higher rate of teenage pregnancy among girls
with early conduct problems reflected both their relatively disadvantaged family backgrounds and their tendencies to
risk taking behavior in adolescence.

In recent years there has been increasing con- berg, 1993; Card & Wise, 1981; Upchurch &
cern about the rising rates of teenage preg- McCarthy, 1990); socioeconomic disadvan-
nancy and the consequences of early parent- tage and welfare dependence (Black & De-
hood for young women and their children (for Blassie, 1985; Furstenberg, Brooks–Gunn, &
reviews of this issue see Brooks–Gunn & Morgan, 1987); increased likelihood of an
Chase–Lansdale, 1995; Coley & Chase–Lans- early marriage and subsequent marital break-
dale, 1998; Furstenberg, Brooks–Gunn, & down (Furstenberg et al., 1987); maternal de-
Chase–Lansdale, 1989; Hechtman, 1989). Ev- pression (Colletta, 1983); and less competent
idence suggests that teenage pregnancy can parenting (Brooks–Gunn Chase–Lansdale,
have far reaching physical, social, and emo- 1995; Luster & Mittelstaedt, 1993; Haskett,
tional consequences for young women, in- Johnson, & Miller, 1994; Nagin, Pogarsky &
cluding increased risk of antenatal complica- Farrington, 1997). Children born to teenage
tions and mortality (Black & DeBlassie, 1985; mothers are also at greater risk of adverse
Zuckerman et al., 1984); failure to complete cognitive, behavioral, and social develop-
schooling (Brooks–Gunn, Guo, & Fursten- mental outcomes (Brooks–Gunn & Chase–
Lansdale, 1995; Brooks–Gunn & Furstenberg,
1986; Furstenberg et al., 1987, 1990; Hardy
This research was funded by grants from the Health Re-
search Council of New Zealand, the National Child et al., 1978). Although the consequences of
Health Research Foundation, the Canterbury Medical Re- teenage pregnancy and parenthood are well
search Foundation and the New Zealand Lottery Grants established, much less is known about the risk
Board. factors and life pathways that place young
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: women at increased risk of teenage pregnancy
Lianne Woodward, Christchurch Health & Development
Study, Christchurch School of Medicine, PO Box 4345, (Zabin, 1990). An understanding of the life
Christchurch, New Zealand; E-mail: lwoodwrd@chmeds. processes that lead to early pregnancy and
ac.nz. motherhood will be important in identifying

127

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


128 L. J. Woodward and D. M. Fergusson

strategies for breaking intergenerational cy- duct difficulties (Kovacs et al., 1994). Two
cles of poor mental health and social func- possible models may account for this relation-
tioning. ship. First, it is possible that associations be-
One factor that has been associated with tween early conduct problems and teenage
an increased risk of teenage pregnancy is the pregnancy may be noncausal and arise be-
presence of early conduct problems and diffi- cause many of the social and contextual fac-
culties (Bardone, Moffitt, Caspi, Dickson, & tors associated with early onset conduct prob-
Silva, 1996; Kessler et al., 1997; Kovacs, lems are also independently related to an
Krol, & Voti, 1994; Robins & Price, 1991; elevated risk of teenage pregnancy. There is
Quinton, Pickles, Maughan, & Rutter, 1993). considerable indirect support for this explana-
For example, Quinton et al. (1993), in a fol- tion from mental health and teenage parenting
low-up study of 81 institutionally raised girls research which suggests that substantial over-
and a comparison sample of 41 girls from lap exists in the risk factors associated with
similar social backgrounds but raised at home, these two outcomes. For example, both the in-
found conduct disorder to be a major risk fac- cidence of childhood conduct problems and
tor for subsequent unsupported pregnancy be- teenage pregnancy are higher among girls
fore the age of 20 years. Similarly, Bardone from disadvantaged family backgrounds
et al. (1996), in a longitudinal prospective where their own mother was a young single
study of the mental health and social out- parent (Furstenberg, Levine, & Brooks–Gunn,
comes of conduct disordered adolescent girls, 1990; Kovacs et al., 1994; McLanahan &
found that girls identified as conduct disor- Bumpass, 1988). It could therefore be plausi-
dered at the age of 15 years were 4 times more bly argued that the relationship between early
likely than healthy control girls to have be- conduct problems and later teenage pregnancy
come pregnant by 21 years of age. is spurious and can be accounted for by con-
Given women’s reproductive role and tra- founding preexisting child and family factors.
ditional responsibility for child care, the rela- To demonstrate that childhood conduct prob-
tionship between early conduct problems and lems lead to an increased risk of pregnancy
later teenage pregnancy is likely to have im- during adolescence, it is necessary to show
portant gender specific consequences for the that the observed relationship persists even
opportunities and life pathways of girls with after confounding factors known or suspected
early conduct problems. An early teenage to be associated with both early conduct prob-
pregnancy is likely to set in motion a life lems and later teenage pregnancy have been
course characterized by reduced opportunities taken into account (Baron & Kenny, 1986).
and increased psychosocial stress from which On the assumption that it may be shown
escape will be difficult (Harris, Brown, & Bi- that early onset conduct problems are likely
fulco, 1987). This is consistent with research to be causally related to later teenage preg-
findings which suggest that teenage preg- nancy, a further issue that arises is that of doc-
nancy is part of a broader constellation of umenting the causal processes that lead girls
compromised psychosocial outcomes, includ- with early onset conduct problems to be at in-
ing lower educational achievement, socio- creased risk of teenage pregnancy. This raises
economic disadvantage, single parenthood, the issue of the risk factors and life processes
and cohabitation with an unsupportive and/or that may intervene between early conduct
physically abusive partner (Bardone et al.; problems and subsequent teenage pregnancy.
Brooks–Gunn & Chase–Lansdale, 1995; Quin- Empirical evidence suggests that children
ton et al., 1993). who show early conduct problems are at in-
While associations between early conduct creased risk, not just for teenage pregnancy
problems and teenage pregnancy have been and early parenting, but also for a broad range
adequately documented, little is known about of adverse adolescent outcomes including:
the risk factors and life pathways that lead to continued mental health difficulties; substance
an increased risk of teenage pregnancy among use and abuse; affiliations with delinquent
adolescent girls who show signs of early con- peers and partners; and school behavior prob-

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


Conduct problems and pregnancy in girls 129

lems (Bardone et al., 1996; Farrington et al., behavior such as school truancy and diffi-
1990; Fergusson & Lynskey, 1998; Fergus- culties with school authorities.
son, Horwood, & Lynskey, 1995; Fergusson &
Horwood, 1996; Quinton et al., 1993; Rob-
Method
ins & Price, 1991; Storm–Mathisen & Vag-
lam, 1994). It is therefore possible that the The data reported here were collected during
higher rates of teenage pregnancy among girls the course of the Christchurch Health and De-
with early conduct difficulties might be ex- velopment Study (CHDS). The CHDS is a
plained by mediating adolescent behaviors re- longitudinal study of a birth cohort of 1,265
flecting a tendency toward risk taking and children born in the Christchurch (New Zea-
norm violation. These adolescent behaviors land) urban region during mid-1977. These
may, in turn, serve to heighten the risk of children have been studied at birth, 4 months,
teenage pregnancy among girls with early at annual intervals to 16 years, and again at
conduct problems. To demonstrate that the re- 18 years of age. An overview of the study
lationship between early conduct problems aims and design has been given previously
and later risk of teenage pregnancy is ex- (Fergusson, Horwood, Shannon, & Lawton,
plained by intervening adolescent behavior, it 1989). The analysis reported here is restricted
is necessary to show that when due allowance to the 491 female members of the cohort for
is made for these behaviors, early conduct whom complete data were available up to the
problems are no longer related to teenage age of 18 years. The variables analyzed in this
pregnancy. report were measured in the following ways.
This paper reports the results of an 18-year
longitudinal study of the relationship between
Conduct problems (8 years)
early onset conduct problems and later risk of
teenage pregnancy in a birth cohort of New At age 8 years, parallel parent and teacher re-
Zealand born girls studied to the age of 18 ports of girls’ tendencies to disruptive or con-
years. The specific aims of the study were as duct disordered behaviors were obtained us-
follows: ing instruments which combined items from
the Rutter (Rutter, Tizard, & Whitmore, 1970)
1. To document the extent to which early con- and Conners (1969, 1970) parent and teacher
duct problems were associated with an in- questionnaires. These items spanned a range
creased risk of teenage pregnancy by the of behaviors relating to disobedience and defi-
age of 18 years. ance of authority, fits of temper and irritabil-
ity, aggression or cruelty towards others, de-
2. To examine the extent to which associa-
struction of property, lying, stealing, and
tions between early conduct problems and
similar behaviors (Fergusson, Horwood, &
subsequent teenage pregnancy could be ex-
Lloyd, 1991). Each item was scored on a 3-
plained by a series of social and contextual
point scale ranging from not at all to a great
factors that may be correlated with early
deal. A scale score representing the extent to
conduct problems and risk of teenage preg-
which the child was described as exhibiting
nancy. These confounding factors included
disruptive, oppositional, or conduct disor-
measures of social background and disad-
dered behavior was created by summing pa-
vantage, measures of child characteristics,
rental and teacher item scores for each child.
and measures of early parenting and family
This procedure was used since it helps to min-
functioning.
imize the effects of situational and rater bias.
3. To explore possible intervening factors that The reliability of this scale, assessed using co-
may explain the association between early efficient α, was .93. For the purposes of the
conduct problems and later risk of teenage present analysis, cutpoints were placed on the
pregnancy. These included early sexual de- scale to divide the sample into four class in-
velopment and behavior, deviant peer affili- tervals ranging from those with few reports of
ations, early substance use, and disruptive disruptive behavior to those with many re-

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


130 L. J. Woodward and D. M. Fergusson

ports of these behaviors. These class intervals follows: 1 = Levels 1, 2 (professional, mana-
were: the lowest 50% of the distribution; gerial); 2 = Levels 3, 4 (clerical, technical,
those whose conduct problems scores placed skilled); and 3 = Levels 5, 6 (semiskilled, un-
them in the 51st to 70th percentiles; those skilled, unemployed).
whose conduct problems scores placed them Ethnicity. Girls were classified as being of
in the 71st to 90th percentiles; and those either Pakeha (European) or Maori/Pacific Is-
whose conduct problems scores placed them land descent on the basis of parental definition
in the 91st to 100th percentiles. of the child’s ethnic identification at birth.
Family living standards (0–8 years). At
each annual assessment, the living standards
Teenage pregnancy (18 years)
of the family were rated by survey interview-
Each year from age 15 years, female sample ers on a 5-point scale that ranged from very
members were asked whether they had be- good to very poor. Interviewer ratings for the
come pregnant since the last interview. At period from the girl’s birth to age 8 years
age 14, parents were questioned about their were summed and then divided by 10 to give
daughter’s prior pregnancy history. In total, a measure of the typical living standards of
8.8% of the sample reported becoming preg- the family over this period.
nant by the age of 18 years. The median age
of first pregnancy was 16 years. Individual characteristics.
Intelligence. At age 8 years, sample mem-
bers were administered the revised Wechsler
Confounding factors
Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-R;
To assess the extent to which the association Wechsler, 1974). The full scale score was
between early conduct problems and teenage used in the present analysis. The reliability of
pregnancy outcome could be explained by the this score, assessed using split half methods,
effects of potentially confounding factors, the was .93.
following measures were selected from the Attentional problems. Concurrent with the
database for inclusion in the analysis. assessment of conduct problems at age 8
years, parent and teacher reports of girls’
Family social background. tendencies to inattentive, restless, or hyperac-
Maternal age. This was assessed in whole tive behaviors were obtained using instru-
years at the time of the sample member’s birth. ments which combined items from the Rutter
Family type. Whether the sample member (Rutter et al., 1970) and Conners (1969, 1970)
entered a single-parent family or a two-parent parent and teacher questionnaires. These
family at birth. items spanned a range of behaviors relating to
Maternal education. This was assessed at inattention, poor concentration, short attention
the time of the sample member’s birth using span, distractability, restlessness, and hyper-
a 3-point scale that reflected the highest level activity (Fergusson et al., 1991); each item
of educational attainment. This scale was 1 = was scored on a 3-point scale ranging from
mother lacked educational qualifications, 2 = not at all to a great deal. A scale score repre-
mother had secondary (high school) qualifi- senting the extent to which the child was de-
cations, and 3 = mother had tertiary (college) scribed as exhibiting inattentive, distractible,
qualifications. restless, or hyperactive behavior was created
Socioeconomic status. Family socioeco- by summing the parent and teacher item
nomic status at the time of the sample mem- scores for each child. The reliability of this
ber’s birth was assessed using the Elley and scale, assessed using coefficient α, was .82.
Irving (1976) scale of socioeconomic status
for New Zealand. This index ranks families Parenting and family functioning.
into six levels on the basis of paternal occupa- Maternal emotional responsiveness. This
tion. For the purposes of the present analysis, measure was an index of the frequency with
the scale was collapsed into three levels as which the girl’s mother was observed to make

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


Conduct problems and pregnancy in girls 131

positive emotional responses toward the child. without qualifications, and unemployment),
This scale was based on the HOME inventory psychological well-being, cannabis and other
(Bradley & Caldwell, 1977; Elardo, Brad- substance use, affiliation with delinquent peers,
ley, & Caldwell, 1977) and assessed when the parental physical punishment, and childhood
sample member was 3 years old. sexual abuse (Fergusson & Horwood, 1996,
Maternal avoidance of punishment. This 1997; Fergusson, Horwood, & Lynskey, 1997a,
was a measure of the frequency with which 1997b; Fergusson & Lynskey, 1997).
the girl’s mother was observed to make puni-
tive responses to her child’s behavior. This Intervening factors
scale was based on the HOME inventory
To examine the extent to which the associa-
(Bradley & Caldwell, 1977; Elardo, Brad-
tion between childhood conduct problems
ley, & Caldwell, 1977) and assessed when the
measured at age 8 years and subsequent teen-
sample member was 3 years old.
age pregnancy was influenced by mediating
Changes of parents. As part of the study,
adolescent behavior, the following measures
comprehensive data on changes of parents
were included in the analysis.
were collected at annual intervals (Fergusson,
Horwood, & Lynskey, 1992). These data were Sexual development and behavior.
used to construct a measure of the number of Early onset sexual activity. At ages 15 and
parental changes the child had experienced 16 years, young women were questioned
during the interval from birth to age 8 years. about whether they had ever engaged in con-
A change of parents was counted if a parent sensual sexual intercourse. Those responding
left the family as a result of parental separa- positively were then asked to provide an esti-
tion or death, or entered the family as a result mate of the age at which they initiated con-
of remarriage or reconciliation. sensual intercourse. A young women was
Parental conflict. Parents were questioned classified as having consensual early onset
annually on three items which described the sexual activity if she reported having sexual
quality of marital relations. These items were intercourse before the age of 16 years. On the
(a) whether the parents had engaged in pro- basis of this classification, 25.6% of the sam-
longed arguments during the last 12 months, ple were classified as reporting early onset
(b) whether the child’s mother reported being sexual activity.
assaulted by her spouse in the last 12 months, Multiple sexual partners. At ages 15 and
and (c) whether the child’s mother had re- 16, young women who reported being sexu-
ported experiencing sexual difficulties in the ally active were asked a series of further ques-
last 12 months. Parental responses to these tions including their number of sexual part-
items were averaged across the 10 assess- ners since the preceding interview. Young
ments to produce a scale measure of the ex- women who reported having more than one
tent to which a child was exposed to parental sexual partner before the age of 16 years were
conflict in the period from birth to age 8 classified as having multiple sexual partners.
years. The three items comprising the parental On the basis of this definition, 17.5% of the
conflict scale were derived from a factor anal- sample were classified as having multiple sex-
ysis of items measuring family change and ual partners.
parental discord (Fergusson et al., 1992). This
analysis showed that these items were signifi- Peer relationships.
cantly intercorrelated, r = .19 to .45, and ap- Peer affiliations. To measure the extent to
peared to load on a single factor. The reliabil- which female sample members affiliated with
ity of this scale, assessed using coefficient α, delinquent or substance using peers at age 15
was .74. In addition, subsequent analyses us- years, an index of peer affiliations was con-
ing this scale have shown that it is strongly structed. This index was based on girls’ re-
predictive of a wide range of child and family ports of the extent to which their best friend
outcomes, including reduced life opportuni- and other friends used tobacco, alcohol, and
ties (early school leaving, leaving school cannabis, truanted, or broke the law. These

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


132 L. J. Woodward and D. M. Fergusson

items were summed to produce a scale mea- so, on how many occasions; 23.4% of girls
sure of the extent to which the sample mem- in the sample reported having truanted from
ber reported affiliating with delinquent or school on at least one occasion between the
substance using peers. The construction of ages of 14 and 15 years.
this delinquent peer scale has been described Suspension from school. Parallel to the
previously (Fergusson & Horwood, 1996). measure of truancy, sample members were
This scale was of moderate reliability, having also asked whether over the past year they had
an α coefficient of .74. been suspended from school for any reason.
Peer attachment. The quality of peer at- A total of 1.6% of girls in the cohort had been
tachments was assessed at age 15 years using suspended from school between the age of 14
the peer attachment scale developed by Arms- and 15 years.
den and Greenberg (1987). The full peer at-
tachment scale was used in this analysis and
Sample size
was found to have good reliability, α = .85.
The sample studied in this paper consisted of
Substance use. 491 young women for whom complete data
Tobacco use (15 years). At age 15 years, were available up to the age of 18 years. This
sample members were asked whether or not sample consisted of 77.9% of the original
they had used tobacco during the preceding CHDS cohort of 630 young women and 87.5%
year: 37.9% of the girls reported that they had of the cohort of young women who were still
used tobacco on at least one occasion during alive and resident in New Zealand at the age
the past year. of 18. Losses arose from emigration from
Cannabis use. At age 15 years, teenagers New Zealand (54%), refusal to participate
and parents were questioned, in separate inter- (37%), and mortality (9%).
views, about the young persons use of canna- To examine the effects of sample loss on
bis during the preceding year. Those with self the representativeness of the sample, compari-
reports or parental reports of cannabis use sons were made between the 491 subjects in-
were classified as having used cannabis, cluded in the analysis and the remaining 139
whereas those with neither parental reports or subjects on a range of social background mea-
self reports of cannabis use were classified as sures collected at birth. These analyses showed
not having used cannabis. By the age of 15 that sample losses were not associated with
years, 9.1% of the sample were reported to maternal age, maternal educational qualifica-
have used cannabis. tions, ethnicity, family socioeconomic status,
Alcohol use. At age 15 years, sample mem- or size. However, there was a small but statis-
bers were asked a series of questions concern- tically detectable, p < .01, tendency for the fi-
ing their use of alcohol during the preceding nal sample to underrepresent girls from single
year. First, they were asked whether or not parent families. These results suggest that the
they had consumed alcohol at any time during sample used in this analysis was largely repre-
the past year. Second, those young people sentative of the original birth cohort of girls.
who reported having consumed alcohol were
then asked to provide an estimate of the
Results
largest amount of alcohol consumed on a sin-
gle occasion during the previous 3-month pe-
Associations between early conduct problems
riod. A total of 10.1% of the sample reported
and teenage pregnancy
having consumed the equivalent of 90 ml of
pure alcohol on a single occasion (Fergusson, Table 1 shows the sample classified into four
Horwood, & Lynskey, 1995). groups according to the extent of child con-
duct problems reported at age 8 years. These
School difficulties. groups ranged from those young women
School truancy. At age 15 years, sample whose conduct problems score placed them in
members were asked whether they had tru- the least disturbed 50% of the sample, to
anted from school over the past year and, if those whose conduct problems score placed

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


Conduct problems and pregnancy in girls 133

Table 1. Rates of teenage pregnancy at age 18 by extent of


earlier conduct problems at age 8 years

Conduct Problems (8 years)

1 (Low) 2 3 4 (High)
Measure 1–50 a 51–70 a 71–90a 91–100 a p

Teen pregnancy by age


18 years (%) 3.5 10.8 12.0 18.4 <.0001
n 257 93 92 49
a
Percentiles.

them in the most disturbed 10% of the sam- functioning measures (parental emotional re-
ple. For each group, the table shows the per- sponsiveness, punitive parent–child interac-
centage of young women who had experi- tion, parental changes, and parental conflict).
enced a teenage pregnancy by the age of 18 For ease of display, all measures were ex-
years. This comparison was tested for signifi- pressed in dichotomous form. This conven-
cance using the Mantel–Haenszel χ2 test of tion makes it possible to construct a profile of
linearity. The table shows that with increasing the backgrounds of children with varying lev-
levels of early conduct problems, there is a els of conduct problems by scoring all vari-
corresponding increase in the risk of subse- ables on a common metric. Associations be-
quent teenage pregnancy, p < .0001. Young tween each variable and the level of reported
women whose conduct problems at age 8 conduct problems was tested using the Man-
years placed them in the most disturbed 10% tel–Haenszel χ2 test of linearity.
of the cohort had a 5.3 times greater risk of The table shows that increasing rates of
falling pregnant before the age of 18 years child conduct problems were related to a wide
compared to young women whose conduct range of social, individual, and family factors.
problems score placed them in the least dis- Girls with early conduct problems were more
turbed 50% of the cohort. No evidence was likely to (a) come from socially disadvantaged
found for a nonlinear trend in the relationship family backgrounds characterized by teenage
between early conduct problems and subse- parenthood, p < .05, single parenting, p < .01,
quent risk of teenage pregnancy, χ2(2) = .45; lower maternal educational achievement, p <
p > .70. .001, lower socioeconomic status, p < .01, and
below average living standards, p < .0001; (b)
report higher rates of comorbid attentional dif-
Factors associated with conduct problems
ficulties, p < .001, and have lower IQ scores
The results in Table 1 suggest that increases on the WISC-R, p < .001, at 8 years of age;
in the level of conduct problems reported at (c) have been exposed to greater family dys-
age 8 years were associated with increasing function, including early punitive parenting, p
risks of later teenage pregnancy. However, it < .001, parental change, p < .001, and parental
could be suggested that this association was conflict, p < .001.
due to confounding factors that may be corre-
lated with both early conduct problems and
Association between early conduct
increased risk of teenage pregnancy. Table 2
problems and later teenage pregnancy after
shows the relationships between the level of
adjustment for the effects of confounding
reported conduct problems at age 8 years and
psychosocial factors
a series of social background (maternal age,
single parenthood, maternal education, socio- To examine the extent to which the associa-
economic status, ethnicity, and family living tion between early conduct problems and sub-
standards); individual adjustment (child IQ sequent teenage pregnancy could be explained
and early attentional problems); and family by the effects of social and contextual factors

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


134 L. J. Woodward and D. M. Fergusson

Table 2. Associations between extent of conduct problems at 8 years and measures of


sociodemographic background, family functioning, and individual factors

Conduct Problems (8 years)

1 (Low) 2 3 4 (High)
Measure 1–50 a 51–70 a 71–90 a 91–100 a p

Sociodemographic background (birth)


Mother aged <19 years at birth of first child 7.8 9.7 10.7 21.4 <.01
Child born into single parent family 4.8 4.9 14.6 10.7 <.01
Mother had no formal educational qualifications 39.6 58.3 58.3 80.4 <.0001
Family of semiskilled/unskilled socioeconomic status 19.3 23.3 24.3 37.5 <.01
Child of Maori/Pacific Island ethnicity 11.1 7.8 19.4 16.1 <.10
In below average living standards 14.3 37.9 31.1 49.6 <.0001
Individual characteristics (8 years)
In lowest quartile of IQ distribution 15.7 24.1 35.7 48.8 <.0001
In highest quartile of ADD problems score 9.6 24.3 43.7 82.1 <.0001
Parenting and family functioning (birth–8 years)
In lowest quartile of maternal emotional responsiveness
(3 years) 18.9 21.8 24.0 23.2 >.10
In highest sextile of maternal punishment score
(3 years) 8.9 13.9 22.0 30.4 <.0001
Experiencing parental change (0–8 years) 18.6 28.6 34.7 50.0 <.001
In highest quintile of parental conflict score (0–8
years) 9.3 18.4 28.6 46.0 <.001
Note: Values are in percents.
a
Percentiles.

associated with early conduct problems, the analysis showed that the conclusions drawn
relationship between early conduct problems were robust to changes in the covariates in-
and later teenage pregnancy shown in Table 1 cluded in the model, provided that the mini-
was adjusted for the effects of the potentially mum set of significant covariates shown in
confounding social, individual, and family Table 3 was included in the final model. The
factors shown in Table 2. A logistic regres- results of this analysis are presented in Table
sion model was fitted in which the log odds 3, which shows the estimated dose/response
of experiencing a teenage pregnancy by the relationship between early conduct problems
age of 18 years was modeled as a linear func- and later teenage pregnancy after adjustment
tion of the extent of conduct difficulties at age for potentially confounding covariates. The
8 and the social, individual, and family factors table also lists the covariate factors found to
shown in Table 2. In fitting this model the be significant in each equation.
covariate factors were not treated as dichoto- The results show that controlling for con-
mies as shown in Table 2 but were scored in founding factors reduced the strength of the
the units described in the method section. association between early conduct problems
Model fitting was conducted using forward and later risk of teenage pregnancy. After ad-
and backward variable elimination to identify justment for the effects of confounding fac-
the best fitting and most parsimonious model. tors, the relative risk of a subsequent preg-
The sensitivity of the analysis to methods of nancy for girls with early conduct problems
variable selection was examined by compar- decreased from 5.3 to 2.6. Nonetheless, the
ing the regression estimates obtained using association between early conduct problems
different methods of covariate control, rang- and teenage pregnancy remained statistically
ing from a model in which all covariates were significant, p < .05. This would suggest that
controlled to a model in which only a core some, but not all, of the relationship between
set of significant factors were controlled. This early conduct problems and later teenage

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


Conduct problems and pregnancy in girls 135

Table 3. Rates of teenage pregnancy by extent of earlier conduct


problems at age 8 years after adjustment for covariates

Conduct Problems (8 years)

1 (Low) 2 3 4 (High)
Measure 1–50 a 51–70 a 71–90 a 91–100 a p

Teenage pregnancy by
age 18 years (%) 5.3 7.4 10.1 13.6 <.05
n 257 93 92 49

Note: Covariate factors are maternal educational qualifications, β = 0.98, p < .01; num-
ber of parental changes from birth to 8 years, β = 0.26, p < .05; and maternal endorse-
ment of punishment at 3 years, β = .35; p < .10.
a
Percentiles.

Table 4. Rates of early adolescent sexual activity, substance use, delinquent


peer affiliations, and behavior by extent of conduct problems at age 8 years

Conduct Problems (8 years)

1 (Low) 2 3 4 (High)
Measure 1–50 a 51–70 a 71–90 a 91–100 a p

Sexual development and behavior


First sexual intercourse before age 16 15.6 23.7 38.0 49.0 <.0001
More than one sexual partner by 16 years 14.1 16.5 20.2 29.4 <.01
Peer relationships (15 years)
Affiliation with delinquent peers 27.5 29.7 30.7 41.7 <.10
Upper quartile of peer attachment 35.6 29.7 27.3 12.5 <.01
Early substance use (15 years)
Smoking cigarettes (at least once/week) 10.9 13.2 23.9 22.9 <.01
Early cannabis use 8.1 4.4 11.4 18.8 <.05
Early alcohol use 7.7 9.9 17.0 10.4 <.10
School difficulties (15 years)
Truanted from school in past 12 months 16.6 20.7 33.0 45.1 <.0001
Suspended from school 0.8 2.2 2.2 3.9 <.10

Note: Values are in percents.


a
Percentiles.

pregnancy was explained by the social and which shows the relationship between the ex-
family backgrounds of these girls. The signifi- tent of early conduct problems and a series of
cant confounding factors included maternal adolescent behaviors, including sexual activ-
educational qualifications, parental changes, ity (early onset sexual intercourse and multi-
and punitive early mother child interaction. ple sexual partnerships), delinquent peer affil-
iations, substance use (cigarettes, cannabis,
and alcohol), and school difficulties (truancy
Factors mediating between early conduct
and school suspension). The table shows the
problems and teenage pregnancy
presence of a pervasive relationship between
Results, thus far, suggest a possible causal re- early conduct problems and rates of risk tak-
lationship between the extent of conduct prob- ing and norm violating behaviors in adoles-
lems in middle childhood and later risk of cence. Girls with high rates of early conduct
teenage pregnancy. This conclusion raises the problems at age 8 years were in adolescence
issue of the life processes and pathways that more likely to have experienced early sexual
lead to higher risk in girls with early conduct intercourse, p < .0001, and had multiple sex-
problems. This issue is examined in Table 4, ual partners, p < .01, when compared to girls

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


136 L. J. Woodward and D. M. Fergusson

Table 5. Rates of teenage pregnancy by extent of earlier conduct problems


at age 8 years after adjustment for confounding covariates and intervening factors

Conduct Problems (8 years)

1 (Low) 2 3 4 (High) Factors In


1–50 a 51–70 a 71–90 a 91–100 a p Equation

Model 1
Excludes adolescent sexual behaviors as
intervening variable 5.8 7.3 9.0 11.1 >.10 1–3, 6
Model 2
Includes adolescent sexual behavior as
intervening variable 6.1 7.4 8.9 10.5 >.15 1–5

Covariate factors are 1 = maternal educational qualifications, 2 = number of parental changes from birth to 8
years, and 3 = maternal endorsement of punishment at 3 years. Intervening factors are 4 = first sexual
intercourse before age 16 years, 5 = number of sexual partners, and 6 = cigarette smoking.
a
Percentiles.

with low rates of early conduct problems. In ing and intervening factors. The table also
addition, girls with high rates of early conduct lists the covariate and intervening factors
problems also reported that they smoked ciga- found to be significant in each equation.
rettes, p < .05, used cannabis, p < .05, had These results indicate that after adjustment
lower peer attachments, p < .01, and more of- for the effects of both covariate and interven-
ten truanted from school, p < .0001, compared ing factors, early conduct problems were no
to girls with low rates of early conduct prob- longer significantly associated with teenage
lems. pregnancy risk. The first model, which ex-
To examine the extent to which associa- cluded early sexual behavior, identified ciga-
tions between early conduct problems and rette smoking in adolescence as a possible
later teenage pregnancy could be explained by mediating factor, whereas the second model,
intervening adolescent behaviors, the data which included early sexual behavior, identi-
were analyzed using a logistic regression fied early onset of sexual intercourse and the
model in which the log odds of teenage preg- number of sexual partnerships reported by age
nancy was modelled as a linear function of 16 years as possible mediating factors. When
early conduct problems, the confounding fac- taken in conjunction with the findings in Ta-
tors shown in Table 3, and the measures of ble 4, these results clearly suggest that early
adolescent behavior shown in Table 4. Two conduct problems are linked to later teenage
models were fitted. The first model included pregnancy by a causal chain process in which
measures of peer relations, early substance early conduct problems are associated with an
use, and school difficulties but did not in- increased risk of a range of adolescent risk
clude measures of adolescent sexual behavior. taking behaviors, including sexual activity,
The second model considered all of the fac- substance use, peer relations, and school diffi-
tors listed in Table 4. Model fitting was con- culties. In turn, these risk taking behaviors,
ducted using methods of forward and back- and particularly early sexual risk taking, suc-
ward variable elimination to identify the best cessfully account for the higher rates of teen-
fitting and most parsimonious model. In both age pregnancy found among girls with early
cases, control for confounding and interven- conduct problems.
ing factors reduced the association between Collectively, these results suggest that the
early conduct problems and later teenage statistical linkages between early conduct
pregnancy to statistical nonsignificance. These problems and teenage pregnancy shown in
results are presented in Table 5, which shows Table 1 were explained by two processes:
the dose/response relationship between early
conduct problems and later teenage pregnancy 1. A substantial component of the relationship
after adjustment for the effects of confound- was explained by preexisting family factors

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


Conduct problems and pregnancy in girls 137

associated with both increased risks of have on the future life opportunities and
childhood conduct problems and teenage choices available to these young women
pregnancy. These factors included maternal (Brooks–Gunn & Chase–Lansdale, 1995;
educational qualifications, number of pa- Furstenberg et al., 1987, 1989; Horwitz, Kler-
rental changes, and early punitive mother– man, Kuo, & Jekel, 1991). Finally, while it is
child interaction. clear from these findings that girls with early
conduct problems are at increased risk of fall-
2. The remaining association was explained
ing pregnant during their teenage years, it is
by a causal chain process in which early
important to acknowledge that a substantial
conduct problems were associated with in-
proportion of girls with early conduct prob-
creased risk taking and particularly sexual
lems avoid this outcome.
risk taking during adolescence, which in
turn accounted for the elevated risk of teen-
age pregnancy among girls with high rates The role of confounding psychosocial factors
of early conduct problems compared to girls
While early conduct problems were related to
with low rates of early conduct problems.
increased risks of teenage pregnancy, subse-
quent analyses also showed that these prob-
Discussion lems were related to a broad range of adverse
individual, family, and social factors. Girls
In this study we have examined the relation- with early onset conduct problems more often
ship between early conduct problems at 8 came from socially disadvantaged family
years of age and the risk of experiencing a backgrounds characterized by low socioeco-
teenage pregnancy in a birth cohort of New nomic status, lower levels of maternal educa-
Zealand girls studied to the age of 18 years. tion, young and single parenthood, and below-
The major findings of this study and their im- average family living standards. As children,
plications are reviewed below. they were themselves more susceptible to co-
morbid attentional difficulties and lower intel-
ligence. Finally, their early parenting and
Relationship between early conduct problems
family experiences were characterized by
and teenage pregnancy
early punitive parenting, parental conflict, and
In agreement with previous research (Bardone family instability. Given this exposure to a
et al., 1996; Kessler et al., 1997; Kovacs et wide range of psychosocial adversities, it
al., 1994; Quinton et al., 1993), findings sug- could therefore be plausibly argued that the
gest that girls with early conduct problems are higher rate of teenage pregnancy found
an at-risk group for later teenage pregnancy. among girls with early conduct problems may
Girls in the most disturbed 10% of the cohort not reflect the direct effect of early conduct
had rates of teenage pregnancy that were over problems. Rather, it may be that the individ-
5 times higher than the rate of teenage preg- ual and family characteristics associated with
nancy for girls in the least disturbed 50% of early conduct problems also place these girls
the cohort. This estimate is similar to that re- at increased risk of teenage pregnancy. This
ported by Bardone et al. (1996) in a study of hypothesis was partially supported in the pres-
another New Zealand birth cohort. They ent analysis, because it was found that after
found that girls meeting criteria for conduct controlling for the effects of pre-existing con-
disorder at age 15 years had rates of teenage textual factors (maternal education, family
pregnancy that were 4 times higher than girls change, and early exposure to punitive child
who did not meet criteria for conduct disor- rearing practices), the association between
der. The weight of the evidence clearly favors early conduct problems and teenage preg-
the view that teenage pregnancy represents a nancy was reduced substantially. After adjust-
significant life course outcome for girls with ment for the confounding effects of family
early onset conduct problems. This conclu- factors, girls in the most disturbed 10% of the
sion is of considerable concern given the cohort were 2.6 times more likely to become
long-term effects that teenage pregnancy may pregnant by the age of 18 years, compared to

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


138 L. J. Woodward and D. M. Fergusson

girls in the least disturbed 50% of the cohort. Yamaguchi & Kandel, 1987; Zabin, 1984,
However, even after extensive efforts to con- 1990). Given this convergence of findings, it
trol for the effects of confounding factors, appears that the relationship between early
childhood conduct problems remained a sig- conduct problems and subsequent teenage
nificant predictor of subsequent teenage preg- pregnancy may be mediated by a pattern of
nancy. This result suggests the presence of a risk taking behavior in adolescence.
possible cause and effect relationship between These findings can also be interpreted
the extent of early conduct problems and later within the context of problem behavior theory
risk of teenage pregnancy. (Donovan & Jessor, 1985) which maintains
that associations between adolescent external-
Adolescent adjustment as a izing behavior problems reflect a more gen-
mediating process eral predisposition to problem behavior, and
that deviant activities such as cigarette smok-
Given the suggestion of a cause and effect ing, illicit drug use, drinking, precocious sex-
relationship between early adjustment and ual activity, and delinquent behavior are func-
teenage pregnancy, we explored possible tionally equivalent for the adolescent. The
mechanisms that might mediate between early present results suggest that for girls, early
adjustment and later risk. This analysis sug- pregnancy may be one manifestation of this
gested that the observed association between tendency to problem behavior, and that early
early conduct problems and later pregnancy conduct difficulties predispose children to
was mediated by a generalized pattern of risk problem behavior in adolescence.
taking and norm violating behavior in adoles- More generally, the results of this study
cence. Specifically, girls with higher rates of suggest that the association between early
early conduct problems reported initiating conduct problems and later teenage pregnan-
sexual intercourse at an earlier age and were cy may arise by two general pathways. First,
more likely to have had multiple sexual part- girls with early conduct problems tended to
ners by the age of 16 years, compared to girls come from family backgrounds characterized
with lower rates of conduct problems. They by higher rates of social disadvantage, family
also reported lower peer attachments and change, and punitive childrearing practices. In
higher rates of early cigarette smoking and part their elevated risk of teenage pregnancy
cannabis use. Finally, girls with early conduct reflects these background factors. However,
problems were more prone to problems with independently of these social and contextual
school authorities such as school truancy, factors, girls with early onset conduct prob-
compared to girls with low rates of early con- lems also reported higher rates of adolescent
duct problems. In turn, these risk taking be- risk taking behavior, including sexual risk
haviors, and particularly sexual risk taking, taking, substance use, and problems with
appeared to explain the higher rates of teen- school authorities. Together these two pro-
age pregnancy found among girls with early cesses accounted for the observed association
conduct problems. between early conduct problems and later
These findings are generally consistent teenage pregnancy in this cohort.
with previous research and theory in the area Some striking similarities exist between
of teenage pregnancy and parenthood which the present findings and those obtained from a
suggest that risks of teenage pregnancy are el- previous study, in which the linkages between
evated among adolescents who engage in a early onset conduct problems and later life
series of risk taking behavior including the opportunities, including school dropout and
early onset of sexual intercourse, multiple unemployment, were examined (Fergusson &
sexual partners, adolescent substance use, and Horwood, 1998). Both analyses led to very
affiliation with deviant peer groups or part- similar conclusions. First, to a considerable
ners (Harvey & Spigner, 1995; Holden, Nel- extent the elevated rates of school dropout,
son, Velasquez, & Ritchie, 1993; Morgan, unemployment, and teenage pregnancy found
Chapar, & Fisher, 1995; Quinton et al., 1993; among children with early conduct problems

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


Conduct problems and pregnancy in girls 139

appear to reflect the effects of psychosocial which measures of all outcomes were assessed
disadvantage associated with early conduct from questionnaire responses. It is likely that
problems. However, even after controlling for such data will contain measurement errors and
the confounding effects of these factors it was imperfections that may threaten the validity of
evident that early conduct problems continued the conclusions drawn. Given the nature of
to have a significant association with all three the analyses reported, statistical adjustment
social outcomes. In all cases, the subsequent for possible measurement error was not possi-
social adjustment of these children during ad- ble and the extent to which the results are ad-
olescence was also found to significantly con- versely affected by measurement error cannot
tribute to their increased social risk in later be assessed. Nonetheless, to the extent that
life. As noted above, children with early con- the analysis appears to provide a coherent and
duct problems showed clearly elevated rates theoretically plausible account of the risk fac-
of risk-taking and norm-violating behaviors in tors and life processes linking early conduct
adolescence. In turn, these behavior problems problems to later teenage pregnancy, there is
were found to be associated with increased good reason to believe that this analysis has
rates of teenage pregnancy, school dropout, captured the essential features of the pro-
and unemployment. cesses that link these variables.
The convergence of these findings sug- Finally, because many of these pregnant
gests that the elevated social risks of children teenagers are likely to become young moth-
with early conduct problems may reflect the ers, these findings clearly have important im-
cumulative effects of two factors that conspire plications not just for the women themselves,
to place these children at greater future risk. but also for their offspring. Substantial evi-
First, the general linkages between conduct dence now exists to show that while individ-
problems and psychosocial disadvantage mean ual variability exists, in general, teenage par-
that children with early conduct problems are enthood has adverse effects not only on the
an at-risk population for later teenage preg- life chances of young parents but also on the
nancy and other adverse social outcomes, future health and development of their off-
such as school dropout and unemployment, spring (Brooks–Gunn & Chase–Lansdale,
independent of their tendencies to conduct 1995; Coley & Chase–Lansdale, 1998; Furs-
disorder. Secondly, these pre-existing risks tenberg et al., 1987). In light of the present
appear to be further increased by the develop- finding that girls with early conduct problems
ment of patterns of risk taking and norm vio- are at increased risk of teenage pregnancy, it
lating behaviors in adolescence. Together is likely that the mental health and social
these two processes of early psychosocial dis- functioning of these women during childhood
advantage and predisposition to risk taking and adolescence may well account for some
during adolescence appear to account for the of the parenting and child developmental dif-
greater vulnerability of children with early ficulties encountered in their own families of
conduct problems to later adverse social out- procreation. Future studies of this cohort may
comes such as teenage pregnancy. provide opportunities to examine the linkages
It should be borne in mind that the data between the prospectively assessed early ex-
analyzed in this study were collected in the periences and life histories of teenage mothers
context of a survey based investigation in and their later behavior as parents.

References
Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inven- erations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
tory of parent and peer attachment: Individual differ- ogy, 51, 1173–1182.
ences and their relationship to psychological well- Bardone, A. M., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Dickson, N., &
being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Silva, P. A. (1996). Adult mental health and social
Adolescence, 16, 427–454. outcomes of adolescent girls with depression and con-
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator– duct disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 8,
mediator variable distinction in social psychological 811–829.
research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical consid- Black, C., & DeBlassie, E. R. (1985). Adolescent preg-

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


140 L. J. Woodward and D. M. Fergusson

nancy: Contributing factors: consequences, treatment conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and
and plausible solutions. Adolescence, 20, 281–290. Psychiatry, 32, 257–274.
Bradley, R. H., & Caldwell, B. M. (1977). Home obser- Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T.
vation for measurement of the environment: A valida- (1992). Family change, parental discord and early of-
tion study of screen efficiency. American Journal of fending. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Mental Deficiency, 81, 417–424. 33, 1059–1075.
Brooks–Gunn, J., & Chase–Lansdale, P. L. (1995). Ado- Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T.
lescent parenthood. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook (1995). The prevalence and risk factors associated
on parenting: Vol. 3. Who is the parent? (pp 113– with abusive or hazardous alcohol consumption in 16
149). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. year olds. Addiction, 90, 935–946.
Brooks–Gunn, J., & Furstenberg, F. F., Jr. (1986). The Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T.
children of adolescent mothers: Physical, academic (1997a). The effects of unemployment on psychiatric
and psychological outcomes. Developmental Review, illness during young adulthood. Psychological Medi-
6, 224–251. cine, 27, 371–381.
Brooks–Gunn, J., Guo, G., & Furstenberg, F. F., Jr. Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T.
(1993). Who drops out and who continues beyond (1997b). Childhood sexual abuse, adolescent sexual
high school? A 20-year follow-up Black urban youth. behaviors and sexual revictimization. Child Abuse &
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 3, 271–294. Neglect, 21, 789–803.
Card, J. J., & Wise, L. L. (1981). Teenage mothers and Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., Shannon, F. T.,
teenage fathers: The impact of early childbearing & Lawton J. M. (1989). The Christchurch Child De-
on the parents’ personal and professional lives. In velopment Study: A review of epidemiological
F. F. Furstenberg, Jr., R. Lincoln, & J. Menken (Eds.), findings. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 3,
Teenage sexuality, pregnancy and childbearing 278–301.
(pp. 211–222). Philadelphia: University of Pennsyl- Fergusson, D. M., & Lynskey, M. T. (1997). Physical
vania. punishment/maltreatment during childhood and ad-
Coley, R. L., & Chase–Lansdale, P. L. (1998). Adoles- justment in young adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect,
cent pregnancy and parenthood. American Psycholo- 21, 617–630.
gist, 53, 152–166. Fergusson, D. M., & Lynskey, M. T. (1998). Conduct
Colletta, N. D. (1983). At risk for depression: A study of problems in childhood and psychosocial outcomes in
young mothers. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 142, young adulthood: A prospective study. Journal of
301–310. Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 6, 2–18.
Conners, C. K. (1969). A teacher rating scale for use in Fergusson, D. M., Lynskey, M. T., & Horwood, L. J.
drug studies with children. American Journal of Psy- (1995). Truancy in adolescence. New Zealand Jour-
chiatry, 126, 884–888. nal of Educational Studies, 30, 25–38.
Conners, C. K. (1970). Symptom patterns in hyperkinetic, Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Brooks–Gunn, J., & Chase–Lans-
neurotic and normal children. Child Development, 41, dale, L. (1989). Teenaged pregnancy and childbear-
667–682. ing. American Psychologist, 44, 313–320.
Donovan, J. E., & Jessor, R. (1985). Structure of problem Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Brooks–Gunn, J., & Morgan, S. P.
behavior in adolescence and young adulthood. Jour- (1987). Adolescent mothers in later life. New York:
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 890– Cambridge University Press.
904. Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Levine, J. A., & Brooks–Gunn, J.
Elardo, R., Bradley, R. M., & Caldwell, B. M. (1977). (1990). The daughters of teenage mothers: Patterns of
A longitudinal study of the relation of infant home early childbearing in two generations. Family Plan-
environments to language development at age three. ning Perspectives, 22, 54–61.
Child Development, 48, 595–603. Hardy, J. B., Welcher, D. W., Standley, J., & Dallasm,
Elley, W. B., & Irving, J. C. (1976). Revised socio-eco- T. R. (1978). Long range outcome of adolescent preg-
nomic index for New Zealand. New Zealand Journal nancy. Clinical Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 21,
of Educational Studies, 11, 25–36. 1215–1232.
Farrington, D. P., Loeber, R., Elliott, D. S., Hawkins, Harris, T., Brown, G., & Bifulco, A. (1987). Loss of par-
J. D., Kandel, D. B., Klein, M. W., McCord, J., Rowe, ent in childhood and adult psychiatric disorder: The
D. C., & Tremblay, R. E. (1990). Advancing knowl- role of social class position and premarital pregnancy.
edge about the onset of delinquency and crime. In B. Psychological Medicine, 17, 163–183.
B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical Harvey, S. M., & Spigner, C. (1995). Factors associated
child psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 383–442). New York: with sexual behavior among adolescents: A multivari-
Plenum. ate analysis. Adolescence, 30, 253–262.
Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (1996). The role of Haskett, M., Johnson, C., & Miller, J. (1994). Individual
adolescent peer affiliations in the continuity between differences in risk of child abuse by adolescent moth-
childhood behavioral adjustment and juvenile offend- ers: Assessment in the perinatal period. Journal of
ing. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 205– Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 461–476.
221. Hechtman, L. (1989). Teenage mothers and their chil-
Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (1997). Early onset dren: Risks and problems: A review. Canadian Jour-
cannabis use and psychosocial adjustment in young nal of Psychiatry, 34, 569–575.
adults. Addiction, 92, 279–296. Holden, G. W., Nelson, P. B., Velasquez, J., & Ritchie,
Ferguson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (1998). Early conduct K. L. (1993). Cognitive, psychosocial, and reported
problems and later life opportunities. Journal of Child sexual behavior differences between pregnant and
Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 1097–1108. nonpregnant adolescents. Adolescence, 28, 557–572.
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lloyd, M. (1991). Horwitz, S. M., Klerman, L. V., Kuo, H. S., & Jekel, J. F.
Confirmatory factor models of attention deficit and (1991). School-age mothers: Predictors of long-term

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press


Conduct problems and pregnancy in girls 141

educational and economic outcomes. Pediatrics, 87, from the NIMH epidemiological catchment area proj-
862–867. ect. Psychiatry, 54, 116–132.
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P. A., Foster, C. L., Saunders, Rutter, M., Tizard, J., & Whitmore, K. (1970). Education,
W. B., Stang, P. E., & Walters, E. (1997). Social con- health and behavior. London: Longmans.
sequences of Psychiatric Disorders, II: Teenage par- Storm–Mathisen, A., & Vaglum, P. (1994). Conduct dis-
enthood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1405– order patients 20 years later: A personal follow-up
1411. study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 89, 416–420.
Kovacs, M., Krol, R., & Voti, L. (1994). Early onset psy- Upchurch, D. M., & McCarthy, J. (1990). The timing of
chopathology and the risk for teenage pregnancy first birth and high school completion. American So-
among clinically referred girls. Journal of the Ameri- ciological Review, 55, 224–234.
can Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, Wechsler, D. (1974). WISC-R Wechsler Intelligence Scale
106–113. for Children-Revised. New York: The Psychological
Luster, T., & Mittelstaedt, M. (1993). Adolescent moth- Corporation.
ers. In T. Luster & L. Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting: An Yamaguchi, K., & Kandel, D. (1987). Drug use and other
ecological perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Elbaum. determinants of premarital pregnancy and its out-
McLanahan, S., & Bumpass, L. (1988). Intergenerational come: A dynamic analysis of competing life events.
consequences of family disruption. American Journal Journal of Marriage and the Family, 49, 257–270.
of Sociology, 94, 130–152. Zabin, L. S. (1984). The association between smoking
Morgan, C., Chapar, G. N., & Fisher, M. (1995). Psycho- and sexual behavior among teens in U.S. contracep-
social variables associated with teenage pregnancy. tive clinics. American Journal of Public Health, 74,
Adolescence, 30, 277–287. 261–263.
Nagin, D. S., Pogarsky, G., & Farrington, D. P. (1997). Zabin, L. S. (1990). Adolescent pregnancy and early sex-
Adolescent mothers and the criminal behavior of their ual onset. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Ad-
children. Law & Society Review, 31, 137–162. vances in Clinical Child Psychology (Vol. 13, pp.
Quinton, D., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., & Rutter, M. 247–278). New York: Plenum.
(1993). Partners, peers and pathways: Assortative Zuckerman, B. S., Walker, D. K., Frank, D. A., Chase,
pairing and continuities in conduct disorder. Develop- C., & Hamburg, B. (1984). Adolescent pregnancy
ment and Psychopathology, 5, 763–783. biobehavioral determinants of outcome. Journal of
Robins, L. N., & Price, R. K. (1991). Adult disorders Pediatrics, 105, 857–863.
predicted by childhood conduct problems: Results

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499001984 Published online by Cambridge University Press

You might also like