You are on page 1of 11

Postural control system

Jean Massion

Laboratory of Neurobiology and Movements, CNRS, Marseille, France

The postural control system has two main functions: first, to build up posture
against gravity and ensure that balance is maintained; and second, to fix the
orientation and position of the segments that serve as a reference frame for
perception and action with respect to the external world. This dual function
of postural control is based on four components: reference values, such
as orientation of body segments and position of the center of gravity (an
internal representation of the body or postural body scheme); multisensory
inputs regulating orientation and stabilization of body segments; and flexible
postural reactions or anticipations for balance recovery after disturbance, or
postural stabilization during voluntary movement. The recent data related to
the organization of this system will be discussed in normal subjects (during
ontogenesis), the elderly and in patients with relevant deficits.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1994, 4:877-887

Introduction body orientation with respect to verticality. The postu-


ral reactions, like the ‘anticipatory’ postural adjustments
Body posture is built up by a set of assembled segments, associated with voluntary movements, are organized on
each with its own mass, that are linked together by the basis of this internal representation, as is the whole set
flexible joints controlled by the neuromuscular system. of interactions involved in perception and action towards
The central organization of posture involves interactions the external world.
between external forces, such as gravity, the mechanical
Because of the complexity of the postural control system,
properties of the body and the neuromuscular forces.
many aspects have given rise to some debate. How are
In order to understand the present trends in studies on posture and equilibrium centrally organized? How does
postural organization, one should bear in mind that pos- the organism adapt to changes, such as microgravity, in
ture serves two main functions. First, it has a mechani- the environment? How is the system built up during on-
cal antigravity function whereby the reference posture togenesis? What impairments affect it in the elderly and
(stance) is built up; equilibrium also depends on this diseased?
antigravity function, which requires the center of grav- Progress in research often depends on new methodologi-
ity (CG) projection to remain inside the supporting cal approaches. A new method has recently been used
surface under static conditions. Second, it serves as a in this field to analyze postural control in the absence
reference frame for perception and action with respect of external disturbances. Classical methods of analysis,
to the external world. The position and orientation of such as recording the center of pressure oscillations from
body segments such as the head, trunk or arms serve as a a force platform under normal stance, have been re-ex-
reference frame for calculating target locations in the ex- amined. The stationary properties of postural sway have
ternal world and for organizing movements toward these been questioned [l]. New techniques of analysis using
targets. the chaotic dynamic approach have been proposed in
In line with the complexity of the functions mediated order to identify putative ‘attractors’ in postural sway
by posture, central organization of its control system in- [2]. Collins and De Luca [3’] have carried out stochas-
volves many interacting elements. On the sensory side, tic analyses on the center of pressure oscillations during
multisensory (visual, labyrinthine, proprioceptive and quiet stance to identify the open-loop and closed-loop
cutaneous) inputs contribute to orienting the postural components of these oscillations. A cross-correlation
segments with respect both to each other and the ex- method has been used on simultaneously recorded kine-
ternal world (vertical gravity vector). These classes of matic parameters at various levels of the multijoint chain
sensors monitor any mismatch between the intended involved in erect posture, with a view to identif+ng the
and actual positions. A so-called ‘postural body scheme’ kinematic strategies used under various sensory condi-
provides an internal representation of the body geom- tions ([4*], see also IS]). Several mathematical analyses
etry, the body dynamics (support conditions) and the have been carried out on multijoint changes to elucidate

Abbreviations
CC-center of gravity; EMG-electromyograph,

0 Current Biology Ltd ISSN 0959-4388 877


878 Neural control

Postural control

__..-,_- _.
1 Orienta’;‘;’ 1 1 ;;““ility 1 fX3y

Feedback control
of posture

Cunent Opinion in Neurobiology

Fig. 1. Central organization of postural control. This schematic diagram summarizes the main components involved in the central organization
of postural control. There are two sets of reference values, one is related to body segment orientation and the other is related to whole body
stability (equilibrium control). These references values, and their maintenance against external or internal disturbances, are based on a body
schema or internal representation of the body, which include several components, namely, body geometry and kinetics, representation of
verticality and reference frame. In addition, postural networks contribute to the execution of the postural tasks. Multisensory inputs are used for
building up the body schema. These inputs also act as error-detecting sensors for evaluating the mismatch between the prescribed orientation
and stability, and the actual posture. The postural reactions in the presence of an error message, as well as the anticipatory postural adjustments
in association with voluntary movements, are exerted through the postural networks on one or several segments. The execution of the postural
reactions or anticipations are controlled on line by local feedbacks.

the control processes involved in the kinematic control In the former situation, the fictive point (CG), which is
of posture [6*,7*]. Biomechanical modelling is another the result of the masses of the individual body segments,
interesting means of studying the postural strategies de- would be directly regulated, whereas in the latter, this
veloped, depending on the constraints [8*,9’,10-121. fictive point would be regulated secondarily as a result
This review will focus on four aspects of the postural of body geometry control.
control system, where the main contributions have been Lacquaniti has provided evidence (see [13]) that in the
made recently. These advances are related to the control cat under normal stance, it is the body geometry rather
of the center of gravity, the concept of postural body than the CG that is regulated. First, the length and an-
schema, postural reactions and anticipation, and age-re- gle of the limb axis with respect to the vertical were kept
lated changes. constant when the supporting platform was tilted in the
sagittal plane. Second, the CG moved when a load was
added in front of the CG position, indicating that no
direct regulation of the CG position occurred. In a re-
Center of gravity control versus body geometry cent study, Lacquanti and Maioli [14**] identified two
separate control systems, one regulating limb length and
When stance is disturbed in bipeds or quadru- axis with respect to the vertical, and the other the hori-
peds, the resulting postural reactions tend to move the zontal contact forces (lever component) exerted by fore
body back toward its initial position, as long as the and hindlimbs in order to stabilize the body. This kinetic
imposed disturbance does not overcome given limits. control system presumably plays a role in maintaining the
During stance, there is thus a reference position that CG within the support area. In fact two parallel control
is stabilized. Some controversy has arisen as to whether systems therefore exist, the one focusing on the body
the reference value stabilized to preserve balance is the geometry and the other on stability [14*-l (Fig. 1).
center of gravity position with respect to the ground, or Is the limb axis, with respect to the vertical, the ge-
whether the body geometry is the main value regulated. ometrical reference value which is regulated during
Postural control system Massion 879

stance? An alternative explanation has been proposed ing, via visual and vestibular inputs, the head and body
by Macpherson [15**] and by Fung and Macpherson movements with respect to the environment. Head- and
[PI. By changing the interlimb distance in the sagit- trunk-centered reference Ii-ames are also used for target
tal and frontal plane, they concluded that the length location and movement trajectory planning (see [21]).
and the orientation of the trunk with respect to the The trunk axis is actively stabilized during locomotion
supporting surface, i.e. an external reference frame, is ac- in both the frontal and sagittal planes, due to the ac-
tually regulated, the legs being used as levers to maintain tion of the hip muscles [22*,23]. The trunk stabilization
the trunk length constant, while the interlimb distance in the frontal plane is increased in labyrinthine defective
is varied. These authors concluded that the preferred in- patients [24]. The trunk axis also remains vertically ori-
terlimb distance during natural stance corresponds to the ented during leg raising, especially in dancers [25]. As
distance involving the least energy consumption [Y]. the trunk axis serves as an egocentric reference frame
for calculating leg position [26*], vertical orientation of
Is the CG regulated in human? In human bipedal stance,
the trunk may be used for direct calculation of the leg
the narrow support surface provided by the foot area and
position with respect to space.
height of the body with respect to the ground make
for a much smaller safety margin than in quadrupeds. To conclude, the results of recent investigations indicate
It has been established previously that after stance dis- that in cats and humans, two control systems can be
turbances, one of the strategies used, namely the hip identified: one fixes the orientation of the body seg-
strategy, consists of flexing or extending the hip, in or- ments with respect to the external world, while the
der to keep the CG within the stability limits [lb]. The other ensures the stability of the body and contributes
same change in body geometry occurs when a voluntary to stabilizing the CG (Fig. 1).
movement of the upper trunk is performed; this would
entail a movement of the CG in the same direction if no
corrective processes intervened. Opposite displacements
of the lower segments then occur however, which re- Postural body scheme
sults in the maintenance of CG within the support area.
This has been termed ‘synergy’ by Babinski [17] but a According to Gurfinkel [27], there is an internal repre-
more appropriate name might be ‘kinematic strategy’, in sentation of the body, or postural body scheme, which
line with the hip strategy known to counteract imposed is not primarily based on sensory information and deals
disturbances. These findings indicate that in humans, the with the body kinematics and kinetics as well as the ori-
body geometry changes in order to regulate the CG po- entation of the body with respect to the vertical. This
sition. representation is “used for the perception of body posi-
The question then arises as to how the CG is regulated, tion and its orientation in space and is also used for mo-
tor control, including reactions directed towards main-
especially when a voluntary movement is performed
which would induce imbalance. The kinematic strat- taining stable body position”[27].
egy observed during upper trunk bending has been ana- The body scheme remains quite stable during dras-
lyzed using the principal components analysis method. tic changes in environmental conditions, such as those
Surprisingly, both the movement and the CG regula- occurring under microgravity, where the vestibular and
tion carried out using the kinematic strategy were ex- proprioceptive inputs are greatly modified (see [28-l). As
pressed by the first principal component as fully as 99% ascertained by testing subjects’ perception of complex
in the case of forward upper trunk movements [7’] and tactile stimuli [29] or by asking them to draw ellipses
96% in backward movements. This indicates that a single with prescribed orientations [30], the egocentric refer-
central control fixes the ratios between angular changes ence frame is still used in the absence of gravity to per-
in the hip, knee and ankle joints and synchronizes their form perceptual and spatial orientation tasks efficiently.
changes with time. The axial kinematic changes associ- The amplitude of forearm movements in the vertical and
ated with upper trunk movements thus constitute an au- horizontal planes remains roughly unchanged under mi-
tomatic control, probably learned in childhood, whereby crogravity, except in the case of slow movements, which
the movement and the CG position are controlled simul- are disturbed because they are controlled by propriocep-
taneously This would explain why angular changes asso- tive feedbacks, which are depressed under microgravity
ciated with upper trunk movements remain unchanged [28*]. The ability of subjects to point to remembered
under microgravity, in the absence of any equilibrium target positions deteriorates in space [31].
constraints [ 181.
The sensors that contribute to estimating the body’s
In addition to the stabilization of the CG position, the orientation, configuration and support conditions have
orientation of several body segments has also been re- been further investigated. One of the questions currently
ported to be regulated simultaneously during stance, under debate concerns the estimation of the gravity vec-
movement or locomotion. For example, the head axis tor. Besides the well-known contribution of labyrinthne
is often stabilized with respect to the vertical during lo- inputs and vision to monitoring the vertical axis, a pu-
comotion [19,20]. This stabilization provides a reference tative role of body graviceptors has been suggested in
value aligned with the vertical axis, used for monitor- the past [32,33], and this idea has recently been indi-
880 Neural control

rectly supported. By adding loads under water, where (due to the nuccal afferents). As soon as a fixed visual or
the body’s weight is cancelled by the water pressure, or tactile reference frame was used, the same patients with
by adding a horizontal force equal to the body weight vestibular deficits perceived the real trunk rotation. Sim-
while the subject is lying down, the previously absent ilar results were obtained by Guxfinkel and Levik [43-l
postural reaction to ‘support’ disturbances can be re- in normal subjects, using very slow trunk rotations that
stored. These results argue in favour of the presence of were not perceived at all with the head f?ee. With the
graviceptors distributed among the segments. head fixed, illusory head rotation is perceived; whereas
when the hand is in contact with a rigid handle fixed to
Which sensors are used to monitor the load? One
the wall, the real trunk rotation is perceived [43*]. How
possible explanation might be that the muscle effort
the proprioceptice input is interpreted by the subject is
opposing the effects of gravity or other forces may be
therefore highly dependent on the reference frame used
estimated by the Golgi tendon organs, whose discharge
at the same time.
pattern correlates with the number of active motor units
[34,35]. U sm. g a d’1ff erent approach, based on a subjective Postural responses are also markedly context-depen-
estimation of verticality, Riccio, Martin and Stoffregen dent. This is illustrated by the responses induced by
[36] reached similar conclusions by artificially dissociat- labyrinthine stimulation [44*]. A standing subject’s re-
ing the gravity vector horn the axis of the ground reac- sponse to galvanic stimulation of the labyrinth consists
tion forces that have to be controlled in order to maintain of a postural sway in the direction of the ear behind
equilibrium. These authors concluded that the subjective which the anode is placed. As previously established,
vertical depends on both the gravity vector and the di- the direction of the sway changes with the head posi-
rection of the ground reaction forces needed to control tion with respect to the trunk, and that of the trunk
balance. Interactions between the orientation of the vi- with reference to the legs; the direction of sway therefore
sual reference frame and somatosensory inputs relating depends on the body geometry at the time of stimula-
to the body orientation with respect to the vertical in tion. The electrbmyograph (EMG) response associated
the perceived subjective vertical have also been found with the sway (the late component of the response) de-
to occur by Nemire and Cohen [37], indicating the pends on the support conditions: it occurs in the arm
importance of the trunk axis (idiotropic vector) in the muscles when part of the body support is exerted by the
perceived vertical [33]. Under microgravity, the percep- hands, and disappears in the soleus in the sitting sub-
tion of the subjective vertical in the absence of vision ject. It therefore occurs in the muscles that are actively
may depend on the ‘saccular’ Z bias, that is, on the engaged in balance. Moreover, reduced responses were
difference between the mean resting discharges of sac- observed when other sources of afferent information are
cular units polarized in the rostra1 and caudal directions. available, for example when the subjects touched a fixed
Other cues - such as tactile, visual and proprioceptive support or when their eyes were open.
- also play an important role in body orientation under
microgravity [38].
One of the most clearly emerging properties of the sen-
sory inputs that contributes to the body scheme is the Postural reactions
marked dependence of these inputs on context, partic-
ularly on the reference frame used for their analysis (see Postural reactions are elicited on the basis of sensory sig-
discussion in DiZio it a/. [39-l). For example, the illu- nals that indicate a disturbance ofposture and/or equilib-
sory or effective body sways that result horn artificial Ia rium. Experimental stance disturbances are usually pro-
inputs produced by vibrating bilaterally the tendon of duced by moving the supporting platform. Disturbances
either the gastrocnemius or tibialis anterior muscle in restricted to the hip level have also been tested and con-
a standing subject tend to disappear under microgravity pared between normal and hemiparetic patients [45].
[40*]. These ‘postural’ illusions or reactions are therefore Discrete disturbances have usually been used; pseudo-
gravity-dependent. random disturbances have also been tested [46]. The
central organization of the postural reactions will not
The role played by vestibular and proprioceptive inputs
be specifically discussed here (see Jankowska and Ed-
in human self-motion perception in space also depends
gley [47] on the spinal cord organization). It is worth
on the reference frame. Mergner ETal. [41*], using vari-
mentioning however that chronic spinal cats show very
ous combinations of head, trunk and feet rotation, have
poor postural control [48].
proposed a model in which the vestibular signal and the
proprioceptive input arising along the whole body axis One of the emerging ideas about the organization of
were used to reconstruct the perception of head, trunk postural reactions is that the central nervous system is
and feet position in space. In vestibular patients, the unable to control individual muscles separately and that
perception of trunk rotation in space with the head it controls only a small number of degrees of freedom by
free is deficient contrary to normal subjects, indicat- activating functional synergies, involving a set of muscles
ing that vestibular inputs are normally responsible for regulated as a whole [49,50]. Synergies have commonly
this perception [42*]. With the head fixed during trunk been observed in postural reactions. They appear to be
rotation, patients perceived an illusory head movement flexible and depend on external constraints such as the
Postural control system Massion 881

direction of the forces disturbing posture. The bifunc- There is also some flexibility in compensatory stepping
tional muscles that extend across two joints contribute observed in response to stance disturbance. This response
largely to this flexibility, because their activity depends depends on the velocity of the disturbance as well as on
more strongly on sensory inputs than the single joint the instructions given to the subjects. Aborted reactions
muscles, and they might serve to orient the force vec- are also observed where only the initial weight transfer
tor exerted by the single joint muscles depending on the toward the supporting limb occurs [57,58]. The step-
requirements of the postural task [51*]. ping response is thus composed of several stages, each of
It has been suggested by Horak and Nashner [16] that which includes a decision made on the basis of sensory
a higher level of organization may be involved in the cues.
postural reactions to stance disturbance: at this level
strategies are selected that each define a given type of
action for restauring balance. Hip strategy (torques and Role of sensory inputs
movement at the hip joint), ankle strategy (torques and Some recent investigations have been carried out on the
movement at the ankle joint), and stepping are different respective roles of the various categories of sensory in-
ways of restoring balance, depending on the intensity of puts involved in postural stability.
the balance disturbance and the constraints. The mus-
To what extent do the vestibular inputs contribute to the
cle synergy may be a lower level of organization, imple-
postural reactions to stance disturbance? In cats, with
menting the strategy by providing the appropriate muscle
bilateral labyrinthectomy, no change in the force con-
forces.
straint strategy nor in the muscle synergy afier stance
Recent data tend to show that the strategy level is also disturbance was detected [59]. Hypermetric responses
flexible and adaptable to task constraints. For exam- are seen early after the operation. In humans, a spe-
ple, the hip and ankle strategies are not ‘all or none’ cial device for producing phasic vestibular linear stim-
reactions but rather form a continuum under progres- ulation by displacing the head was tested. This stimula-
sively changing external constraints. Horak and Moore tion induced postural responses in leg and trunk muscles
[52*] have observed that a continuum occurs in postural in standing subjects. These responses were rather weak,
changes involving gradually more hip strategy and less however, suggesting that vestibular inputs do not con-
ankle strategy when leaning forward is increased. The tribute strongly to early postural reactions to balance
postural response to stance disturbance is also adapted stance disturbances [60*]. The vestibular system plays
when a disturbance to equilibrium is used as a sig- a role in organizating hip strategy [61]. Imposed ankle
nal for gait initiation; in this situation, goal-directed joint rotation induces a hip strategy in normal subjects,
changes in postural responses were found to occur [53]. which disappears in vestibular patients and is replaced by
Conclusions on the same lines were reached by Al- a rather inefficient ankle strategy [54*]. It should also be
lum et al. [54-l, who disturbed standing posture by mentioned that no vestibular deficits were detected in
random combinations of rotation (triggering the hip Parkinsonian patients with impaired balance [62], and
strategy) and translation (triggering the ankle strategy) that, generally speaking, the usual multisensory inputs
of a supporting surface. On the basis of kinematic and are properly integrated in these patients [63]: this suggests
EMG analysis, they established that one out of two that the disorder affects only the control of the postural
discrete muscle synergies was selected on the basis of reactions.
leg afferent input. In addition, segmental velocity re-
The role of leg somatosensory inputs in postural stabil-
lated inputs continuously update the initial pattern. A
ity has also been investigated. For example, longitudinal
comparable updating of the basic pattern by segmental
platform oscillations at 8-24 Hz markedly increased the
inputs was also reported by Forssberg and Hirschfeld
subjects’ postural instability [64]. The somatosensory
[55-l during disturbance of the sitting position. A depen-
inputs are not only involved in postural stability: so-
dence on the biomechanical constraints was also found
matosensory inputs Gem the lower leg also contribute
in the case of the ‘force constraint strategy’ observed in
directly to the stabilization of the head as do the vi-
the cat, whatever the direction of the stance disturbance
sual and vestibular inputs 1651. Are the somatosensory
[9*,15**]. This strategy does not depend on prior experi-
inputs from the leg needed to trigger and scale human
ence and is therefore part of the animal’s repertoire [56].
automatic postural responses? In patients with lower limb
It is characterized by the fact that the force directions ex-
neuropathy, the postural reactions to stance disturbance
erted to restore balance under individual paws are always
are still present, but their latency increases (2@3Oms)
oriented forward and backward whatever the direction
[66*]. This suggests that leg somatosensory inputs are
of the balance disturbance. This strategy disappears with
actually used to trigger and scale the postural reactions;
small interpaw distances. On the basis of a biomechani-
however, other inputs (trunk, vestibular and visual) may
cal analysis, Macpherson and Fung [9*] have proposed
replace the missing inputs.
that this strategy may be aimed at preventing a lateral
bending of the trunk, thus maintaining the back length How are somatosensory inputs used to scale the postural
invariant when lateral disturbing forces are present. With response in ccrebellar patients [67’]! Interestingly, in pa-
shorter interlimb distances, the back incurvation seems tients with anterior lobe pathology, feedback-controlled
to be prevented by an increase in back stiffiless [9*]. scaling, depending on the velocity of the disturbance, is
882 Neural control

still possible. In contrast, scaling of the response am- chanical impact. This inhibition may minimize impact
plitude, which depends on presetting before the dis- effect on forearm posture by reducing muscle stiffness
turbance onset, disappears and is replaced by tendency in advance of the disturbance [76]. The mechanisms re-
towards hypermetria. sponsible for anticipatory postural adjustments have been
investigated. In a bimanual load lifting task, where one
Concerning the role of vision in postural control, it has arm was supporting the load and the other voluntary
been shown that postural sway is equally controlled by lifting the load, anticipatory postural adjustments were
peripheral and central visual fields [68].The effects of a observed in the postural arm (see [77]). Interestingly,
moving visual environment on postural oscillations have the sarne adjustments were observed in a patient with
been investigated by Previc et al. [69] and Dijkstra et forearm-afferent deprivation, indicating that these ad-
al. [70]. The latter paper concludes that retinal slip justments result from feedforward control. The limb
minimization does not explain the coupling between afferents are necessary, however, for new anticipatory
a moving visual environment and postural sway, and postural adjustrnent when unloading the postural fore-
that a dynamic coupling between the two must exist. arm is triggered by new movement [78].
Vision has been found to shorten the latency of postural
responses [71]. Vection has been shown to shorten or de- Anticipatory postural adjustments aimed at maintaining
lay the onset of balance recovery in unexpected forward balance have been observed with upper trunk move-
falls, depending on the direction of the moving scene ments (see [77]). These adjustments are impaired in
[72]. Lastly, the respective roles of the somatosensory cerebellar anterior lobe patients; the main deficit involves
and visual inputs in stabilizating body motion depend a lack of feedforward activation in the thigh muscles at
on the stance width. With a larger supporting base, the the onset of the movement [79]. Respiratory oscillations
role of vision in body stabilization in the frontal plane can also lead to balance disturbance. As the respiratory
decreases in favor of somatosensory inputs [ll]. Visual oscillation estimated from the center of pressure sway
motion compensates in the cat for loss of vestibular in- path recorded from a force platform is larger in sitting
put during the early stages of recovery after unilateral than in standing subjects, anticipatory control seems to
labyrinthectomy. The vestibular nucleus response to op- be more efficient when standing than when sitting [80].
tokinetic stimuli on the deafferented side shows an in-
The last specific type of postural adjustment occurs with
creased band width [73].
movements involving the legs, such as gait initiation,
standing on tip toe or the heels and lifting or raising
a leg. With motor acts of this kind, the movement is
preceded by postural changes that shift the CG toward a
Anticipatory postural adjustments new position compatible with equilibrium maintenance
during the leg movement. These types ofpostural adjust-
Unlike the postural reactions in response to the onset of ments are impaired in patients with hemiparesis. When
posture or balance disturbances, the anticipatory postural asked to lift a leg, they are not able to use the paralyzed
adjustments precede the disturbance onset and therefore limb to exert the appropriate horizontal ground reaction
minimize the effects of the forthcoming disturbance in forces needed to move the CG toward the supporting
a feedforward manner. leg [81]. When asked to stand on tip toe, the temporal
sequence is disturbed on both sides [82]. Standing on
Anticipatory postural adjustments usually occur in asso-
tip toe is also markedly impaired in anterior lobe cere-
ciation with voluntary movements, which are one of the
bellar patients [83] and Parkinsonian [84]. The scaling of
main sources of posture and balance disturbance. Antici-
the amplitude and duration of the preparatory postural
patory adjustments can also be observed, however, when
phase is abnormal in cerebellar patient (low amplitude,
imposed disturbance is recurrent, as in the case of hu-
prolonged duration) and the temporal relationship be-
man stance on a sinusoidally translating platform. The
tween the postural and movement phase is lost.
anticipatory adjustments which are then observed serve
to orient the body so as to minimize the effects of grav-
itoinertial forces. When the frequency of the platform
translation is changed, the feedforward mode is replaced
by a feedback mode of control for a few oscillations Age-related changes
and the feedforward control then reappears [74’]. This
Ontogenesis
anticipatory postural control is impaired in Parkinsonian
Successive steps have been described during the devel-
patients [75].
opment of posture: postural control of the head, postural
Anticipatory postural adjustments before the disturbance control of the trunk, sitting position, standing and loco-
of a single joint position caused by the voluntary move- motion. The emergence of these various stages depends
ment of another segment have been described. For ex- on the evolutional state of several systems, such as the
ample, when a subject is tapping with a hammer on the musculo-skeletal system, the sensori-motor system, the
radial muscle of his other forearm, there is a silent period level of motivation and of behavioral development, and
in the muscle response pattern before the actual me- the internal and environmental constraints [85].
Postural control system Massion 883

Both the visual and somatosensory systems are crucial for Ageing
stabilization of head, trunk and whole body posture, but The balance deficits that occur with ageing have been
vision is effective earlier than the somatosensory system. extensively studied over recent years. Authors of three
recent papers [94-961 have emphasized the multiple
At each stage in postural development, moving visual in- sources of balance deficits in the elderly and the need
formation contributes to maintaining posture and equi- to adopt a systemic approach. For example, sensory
librium. It has been established that vision and optic receptors deteriorate with ageing. Sensory deprivation
flow influence spontaneous head oscillation as early as or sensory conflicts have more drastic effects on balance
2 to 3 days after birth, and head posture as early as in the elderly than in younger subjects [97]. Mechanical
5 months of age [86]. A response to optic flow oc- properties of the tendons and muscles are also affected
curs with a supported standing posture as early as 5 in terms of their force and elasticity. Postural responses
months of age and increases considerably later on. The are delayed and weaker with more co-activation in the
somatosensory system is mainly involved in reactions to elderly [98]. Ability to adapt changing external perturba-
support disturbances. Head stabilization is observed from tions such as shifting from a translation to a rotational dis-
3 to 4 months of age and head-trunk stabilization in sit- turbance is impaired. Postural responses associated with
ting posture at 5 months [87]. Hirschfeld and Forssberg voluntary movements are also impaired (increased laten-
[88*] have observed that in sitting children who undergo ties, excessive co-contractions, etc.) [99]. Furthermore,
a forward horizontal balance disturbance, the EMG the attentional demands of postural tasks are increased
pattern is roughly comparable to the adult one [55*], in the elderly [loo]. Generally speaking, the impairment
since the activation of the front muscles induced by of balance that develops in the elderly results from the
backward trunk imposed displacement is similar to what degradation of multiple systems that participate, either
occurs in adults, although this is not so in the case of the directly or indirectly, in the task of balance control.
back muscles. These authors suggested that two levels of
control might exist: basic one present in children, and
that which develops in adults as the result of learning.
At 9 months of age, the classical distal-proximal EMG
pattern present in adults subjects to platform disturbances Conclusions
is present in standing children. It is preceded by a stage
where only the ankle joint muscles are activated and The overall picture of postural organization that emerges
standing without support is not possible [89,90]. The horn recent investigations is a long way off the picture
vestibular contribution to postural reactions has turned of classical postural reflexes presented by the Sherring-
out to be apparently less important than the somatosen- tonian School. While the old description of these re-
sory one early in life, but further investigations will be flexes is still valid and their analysis is still a useful means
necessary to elucidate this point. of experimentation and neurological evaluation, the en-
phasis now is on the flexibility of postural control and its
Special attention has been paid to equilibrium control adaptability to different contexts.
during locomotion. As soon as locomotion starts, stabi-
Such flexibility is reflected in the multisensory integra-
lization of the hip in the frontal plane with respect to
tion which is involved in postural orientation and sta-
space is observed [91*,92]. After two months of walk-
bilization. The multisensory aspect of postural control
ing experience, stabilization of the shoulders improves.
was first pointed out as early phylogenetically as in the
This suggest that a hip-centered temporal organization of
lamprey [lOl*], but its ability to adapt to context and
balance control occurs while walking. Long-term mat-
task is characteristic of higher vertebrates. This explains
uration of locomotor balance has also been investigated.
the large range of compensatory possibilities available in
During the first stage (3-6 years), the head is stabilized
the case of selective sensory deficits, and also the fact that
on the trunk whenever constraints makes equilibrium
a given sensory input can induce various perceptions or
difficult. By 7-8 years, the head is also stabilized in space
postural reactions, depending on the reference frame se-
during locomotion when facing equilibrium constraints.
lected and the external constraints such as gravity This
Adults use similar stabilization of the head in the frontal
flexibility is under the control of the internal representa-
plane while walking on a narrow support (see [85]).
tion of the body or postural body scheme, which remains
quite stable under changing conditions.
Does the head stabilization in space depend on visual in-
puts or is it based on vestibular cues? Although peripheral Postural reactions show a similar degree of flexibility.
vision and movement visual cues play an important part Some invariant aspects of postural reactions, such as
in balance control, especially in young children, head strategies and synergies, appear to be less fixed than
stabilization in space does not depend to any great ex- was first thought to be the case, and change depend-
tent on visual cues and may therefore mostly depend on ing on the constraints. The external constraints, as well
vestibular cues [20]. Head stabilization in space in chil- as the biomechanical properties of the body segments,
dren as in adults [93] is a basis for ‘top-down’ postural impose choice of control. One of the main criteria for
control on basis of visual and vestibular cues. the emergence of a postural pattern after training is that
884 Neural control

it should be as economical as possible in terms of en- lenburg DJ, Harris FP, Probst R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science
Publishers B.V.; 1993:349-358.
ergy consumption. This does not mean that the brain is
The authors proposed a theoretical framework for studying coordination
merely a passive player in the game. Its role is to utilize strategies in standing posture. This framework consists of a musculoskele-
passive forces to organize the most suitable spatiotempo- tal model of human lower extremity in the sag&al plane and a technique
to visualize, geometrically, how constraints internal and external to the
ral pattern in order to carry out a task. It plays a crucial
body affect the movement. The model predicts that the hip strategy is
role in coordinating postural tasks with various aspects of most effective at controlling the center of mass with minimal muscle ac-
the ongoing action. The coordination between posture, tivation.

equilibrium and movement is certainly one of the main 9. Fung J, Macpherson JM: Determinants of postural orientation
functions of the postural control system. . in quadrupedal stance. J Neurosci 1994, in press.
The determinants of postural orientation in the standing cat were inves-
tigated by examining stance kinematics and kinetics at various interpaw
distances. The authors show that the trunk orientation is kept constant
due to maintaining a constant intralimb geometry.

Acknowledgements 10. Lee WA, Russo AM: Constraints and coordination in whole-
body actions. In lnterlimb coordination: neural, dynamical, and
cognitive constrainfs. Edited by Swinnen S, Heuer Ii, Massion
The author wish to thank S Zakarian for the very efficient J, Casaer P. San Diego: Academic Press; 1994537-569.
help in the preparation of this review paper, and F Horak and
J Macpherson for their critical reading of the manuscript. The 11. Day BL, Steiger MJ, Thompson PD, Marsden CD: Effect of
vision and stance width on human body motion when stand-
CNES (Centre National d’etudes Spatiales) is acknowledged for
ing: implications for afferent control of lateral sway. J Physiol
its support. (Londl 1993, 469:479-499.

12. Merfeld DM, Young LR, Oman CM, Shelhamer MJ: A mul-
tidimensional model of the effect of gravity on the spatial
orientation of the monkey. J Vest Res 1993, 3:141-161.
References and recommended reading 13. Lacquaniti F: Automatic control of limb movement and pos-
ture. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1992, 2:807-814.
Papers of particular interest, published within the annual period of
14. Lacquaniti F, Maioli C: Independent control of limb posi-
review, have been highlighted as: .. tion and contact forces in cat posture. / Neurophysiol 1994,
. of special interest 72:1476-l 495.
.. of outstanding interest The main aim of this study was to assess whether or not control of limb
1. Carroll JP, Freedman W: Nonstationary properties of postural position in the standing cat is independent of control of contact forces
sway. ) Biomech 1993, 26:409-416. at the feet. By tilting the support surface, changing interfeet distance and
head orientation, or by adding a load to the back, the authors showed
2. Newell KM, Van Emmerik REA, Lee D, Sprague RL: On postural that two independent controls do exist, one addressed to limb length and
stability and variability. Cair & Posture 1993, 4:225-230. orientation, the other to the lever component oftangential contact forces.

3. Collins JJ, De Luca CJ: Open-loop and closed-loop control of 15. Macpherson JM: Changes in a postural strategy with inter-paw
. posture: a random-walk analysis of center of pressure trajec- .. distance. J Neurophysiol 1994, 71:931-940.
tory. Exp Brain Res 1993, 95:308-318. The postural strategy during stance disturbance in the cat is characterized
The center of pressure trajectories during quiet stance (steady states by a forward or backward orientation of the ground reaction forces. This
behavior) are modelled as fractional Brownian motion. This analysis strategy is lost for short interlimb distance. The author suggests that the
reveals two modes of postural control: open loop and feedback. strategy is aimed at controlling the back orientation.

4. Amblard 8, Assaiante C, Lekhel H, Marchand A: A statistical 16. Horak FB, Nashner LM: Central programming of postural
. approach to somatosensorimotor strategies: conjugate cross- movements: adaptation to altered support surface configura-
correlations. / Motor Behav 1994, 26: 103-l 12. tions. / Neurophysiol 1986, 55: 1369-l 381.
A method based on cross-correlation function between two time series of
17. Babinski J: De I’asynergie c&+belleuse. Rev Neural 1899,
kinematic measurements is proposed for the analysis of multisegmental
7:806-816.
movements. It can be used for defining postural strategies during stance.
18. Massion J, Curfinkel V, Lipshits M, Obadia A, Popov K: Axial
5. Allum JHJ, Honegger F: Synergies and strategies underlying
synergies under microgravity conditions. J Vestibular Res 1993,
normal and vestibulary deficient control of balance: implica-
3~275-287.
tion for neuroprosthetic control. In Progress in brain research,
vol 97. Edited by Allurn JHJ, Allum-Mecklenburg DJ, Harris 19. Berthoz A, Pozzo T: Head and body coordination during lo-
FP, Probst R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.; comotion and complex movements. In Interlimb coordination:
1993:33 l-348. neural, dynamical, and cognitive constraints. Edited by Swin-
nen S, Heuer H, Massion J, Casaer P. San Diego: Academic
6. Lacquaniti F, Maioli C: Coordinate transformations in the con-
Press; 1994: 147-l 65.
. trol of cat posture. / Neuro,ohysio/ 1994, 72: 1496-l 515.
The authors have tested a number of different hypotheses on the nature 20. Assaiante C, Amblard B: Ontogenesis of head stabilization in
of the processing stage that transforms end point coordinates of the limbs space during locomotion in children: influence of visual cues.
specified by limb length and orientation into the angular coordinates of Exp Brain Res 1993, 93:499-515.
the joints (inverse mapping).
21. Andersen RA, Snyder LH, Li CS, Stricanne 6: Coordinate trans-
7. Alexandrov A, Frolov A, Massion J: Voluntary forward bending formations in the representation of spatial information. Curr
. movement in human: a principal component analysis of axial Opin Neurobiol 1993, 3:171-l 76.
synergies. In Vestibular and neural iron&. Edited by Taguchi K,
Igarashi M, Mozi S. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1994:345-348. 22. Mackinnon CD, Winter DA: Control of whole body balance in
Principal component analysis was used to determine the intersegmen- . the frontal plane during human walking. / Biomechanics 1993,
tat constraints during upper trunk movement. A single component (PClJ 26:633-644.
fixes the ratio of angle changes, and the amplitude and time course of The authors use a whole-body inverted pendulum model to investigate
the movement. control of balance and posture in the frontal plane during human walk-
ing.
8. Kuo AD, Zajac FE: Human standing posture: multi-joint move-
. ment strategies based on biomechanical constraints. In Progress 23. Winter DA, Mackinnon CD, Ruder CK, Wieman C: An in-
in brain research, vol 97. Edited by Allum JHJ, Allum-Meck- tegrated EMC/biomechanical model of upper body balance
Postural control system Massion 885

and posture during human gait. In Progress in brain research, vibrated simulatneously, the perceived distance between the targets in-
vol 97. Edited by Allum JHJ, Allum-Mecklenburg DJ, Harris creases significantly. These findings indicate that brachial muscle spindle
FP, Probst R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.; signals can contribute to an independent representation of felt target lo-
1993:359-367. cation in head-centric coordinates. The authors propose a model of these
representations.
24. Glasauer S, Amorim MA, Vitte E, Berthoz A: Coal-directed lin-
ear locomotion in normal and labyrinthine-defective subjects. 40. Roll JP, Popov K, Gurfinkel V, Lipshits M, Andre-Deshays C,
Exp Brain Res 1994, 98:323-335. . Gilhodes JC, Quoniam C: Sensorimotor and perceptual func-
tion of muscle proprioception in microgravity. 1 Vesfibular Res
25. Mouchnino L, Aurenty R, Massion J, Pedotti A: Coordina- 1993, 3:259-273.
tion between equilibrium and head-trunk orientation during During long-term exposure to microgravity, the postural responses or the
leg movement: a new strategy built up by training. / Neuro- illusory postural displacements induced by vibratory stimulation of Tib-
physiol 1992, 67:1587-l 598. ialis anterior or Gastrocnemius tendons are reduced or disappear and are
replaced by body lift illusion. They reappear by artificially increasing the
26. Mouchnino L, Aurenty R, Massion J, Pedotti A: Is the trunk a
. reference frame for calculating leg position? Neuroreporf 1993, foot support by means of braces.
4:125-l 27. 41. Mergner T, Hlavacka F, Schweigart G: Interaction of vestibular
By comparing leg angle with respect to trunk during leg raising in dancers . and proprioceptive inputs. / Vestibular Res 1993, 3:41-57.
(keepingthe trunk axis vertical) and untrained subjects (trunk inclination). The study investigates the interaction of leg proprioceptive and vestibular
The authors concluded that leg position is estimated with respect to the affe’rents for human self perception in space.
trunk axis.
42. Schweigart G, Heimbrand S, Mergner T, Becker W: Perception
27. Curfinkel VS: The mechanisms of postural regulation in man. . of horizontal head and trunk rotation: modification of neck
Soviet Scientific Reviews F Phys Gen Biol 1994, 7:59-89. input following loss of vestibular function. Exp Brain Res 1993,
Fisk j, Lackner JR, Dizio P: Gravitoinertial force level 95:533-546.
28.
The horizontal head and trunk rotation perception is investigated in pa-
. influences arm movement control. / Neurophysiol 1993,
69:504-511. tients with chronic vestibular loss. In these patients, the trunk rotation
with respect to a stationary head is perceived as a rotation of the head
The ability to move the forearm between remembered elbow joint angle
with respect to a stationary trunk. The normal perception is restored when
after rapid changes of the gravitoinertial forces (G) level was measured. C
changes only influenced the movement amplitude for slow movements visual or somatosensory space reference is presented.
that rely on proprioceptive information, suggesting a decrease of spindle 43. Gurfinkel VS, Levik YuS: The suppression of the cervico-ocular
activity in 0 C. . response by the haptokinetic information about the contact
with a rigid, immobile object. Fxp Brain Res 1993, 95:359-364.
29. Curfinkel VS, Lestienne F, Levik YuS, Popov KE: Egocentric
Super slow trunk rotation with respect to a stationary head is perceived
references and human spatial orientation in microgravity. I.
as a head rotation with respect to a stationary trunk. Eye movements in
Perception of complex tactile stimuli. Exp Brain Res 1993,
phase with the illusory head rotation are seen. Grasping a rigid handle
95:339-342.
(haptic contact) restores perception of trunk rotation with respect to the
30. Curfinkel VS, Lestienne F, Levik YuS, Popov KE, Lefort L: Ego- head.
centric references and human spatial orientation in micrograv-
ity. II. Body-centred coordinates in the task of drawing ellipses 44. Britton TC, Day BL, Brown P, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Mars-
. den CD: Postural electromyographic responses in the arm and
with prescribed orientation. Fxp Brain Res 1993, 95:343-348.
leg following galvanic vestibular stimulation in man. Fxp Brain
31. Young LR, Oman CM, Merfeld D, Watt D, Roy S, Deluca C, Res 1993, 94:143-151.
Balkwill D, Christie J, Groleau N, Jackson DK ef al.: Spatial The effect of galvanic vestibular stimulation in man depends on support
orientation and posture during and following weightlessness: conditions (feet, hands). Early and late EMG responses are described,
human experiments on spacelab life sciences 1. / Vestibular which are computed independently.
Res 1993, 3:231-239.
45. Wing AM, Goodrich S, Virji-Babul N, Jenner JR, Clapp S:
32. Gurfinkel VS, Lipshits YuS, Popov KE: Stabilization of body Balance evaluation in hemiparetic stroke patients using lat-
position as the main task of postural regulation. Fiziologya eral forces applied to the hip. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1993,
Cheloveka 1981, 7:40&410. 74:292-299.

33. Mittelstaedt H: A new solution to the problem of the subjective 46. Maki BE, Ostrovski G: Do postural responses to transient and
vertical. Nafurwissenschaften 1983, 70:272-281. continuous perturbations show similar vision and amplitude
dependence? / Biomech 1993, 26: 1181-l 190.
34. Dietz V: Neuronal basis of stance regulation. Interlimb coor-
dination and antigravity receptor function. In lnferlimb coor- 47. Jankowska E, Edgley S: Interaction between pathways control-
dination: neural, dynamical, and cognifive constraints. Edited ling posture and gait at the level of spinal interneurones. in
by Swinnen SP, Heuer H, Massion J, Casaer P. New York: Aca- Progress in brain research, vol 97. Edited by Allum JHJ. Allum-
demic Press; 1994:167-l 78. Mecklenburg DJ, Harris FP, Probst R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Sri-
ence Publishers B.V.; 1993:161-l 71.
35. Dietz V, Gollhofer A, Kleiber M, Trippel M: Regulation of
bipedal stance: dependency on ‘load’ receptors. Exp Brain Res 48. Pratt CA, Fung J, Macpherson JM: Stance control in the chronic
1992, 89:229-231. spinal cat. J Neurophysiol 1994, 71:1981-1985.

36. Riccio GE, Martin El, Stoffregen T: The role of balance dynam- 49. Bernstein N: The coordination and regulafion oi movemenf.
ics in the active perception of orientation. / Exp Psycho/ (HPPJ London: Pergamon Press; 1967.
1992, 18~624-644.
50. Macpherson JM: How flexible are muscle synergies? In Mofor
37. Nemire K, Cohen MM: Visual and somesthetic influences on control: concepts and issues. Edited by Humphrey DR, Freund
postural orientation in the median plane. Perception 8. Psy- H-J. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 1991:33-47.
chophysics 1993, 53:106-l 16.
51. Van lngen Schenau GJ, Pratt CA, Marpherson JM: Differential
38. Mittelstaedt H, Glasauer S: Crucial effects of weightlessness on . use and control of mono- and biarticular muscles. Human MOV
human orientation. / Vesfibular Res 1993, 3:307-314. Sci 1994, In press.
The respective role of mono- and biarticular muscles In multIJoIn’s move-
39. Dizio P, Lathan CE, Lackner JR: The role of brachial muscle ments is analyzed in various tasks, in cat and human. As a whole, the
. spindle signals in assignment of visual direction. / Neurophysiol bialticular muscle activity is more flexible under the influence of motion-
1993, 70:1578-l 584. related feedback and aimed at fine regulation of the distribution of net
The illusion of target light displacement (oculobrachial illusion) is per- moments over the limb joints.
ceived when a target light is attached to the unseen stationary hand
and an illusory forearm motion is elicited by brachial muscle vibra- 52. Horak FB, Moore SP: The effect of prior leaning on human
tion. When two targets are fixed, one on each hand, and both biceps . postural responses. C;ait & Posfure 1993, 1:203-210.
886 Neural control

This study examines how human postural responses to stance disturbance normal subjects. The main deficit was the lack of anticipatory scaling of
are altered by leaning about the ankles to five different initial stance PO- the amplitude of the responses.
sitions.
68. Straube A, Krafczyk S, Paulus W, Brandt T: Dependence of
53. Burleigh AL, Horak FB, Malouin F: Modification of postural re- visual stabilization of postural sway on the cortical magnifi-
sponses and step initiation: evidence for goal directed postural cation factor of restricted visual fields. Exp Brain Res 1994,
interactions. J Neurophysiol 1994, in press. 99:501-506.

54. Allum JHJ, Honegger F, Schicks H: Vestibular and propriocep- 69. Previc FH, Kenyon RV, Boer ER, Johnson BH: The effects of
. tive modulation of postural synergies in normal subjects. J background visual roll stimulation on postural and manual con-
Vestibular Res 1993, 3:59-85. trol and self-motion perception. Perception & Psychophysics
The different muscle synergies underlying human balance control were 1993, 54:93-l 07.
tested by combining rotation and translation of the support surface.
70. Dijkstra TMH, Schoner C, Cielen CUM: Temporal stability of
55. Forssberg H, Hirschfeld H: Postural adjustments in sitting hu- the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving
. mans following external perturbations: muscle activity and visual environment. Exp Brain Res 1994, 97:477-486.
kinematics. Exp Braain Res 1994, 97:515-527.
71. Timmann D, Belting C, Schwarz M, Diener HC: Influence of
Translation and rotation disturbances were tested in sitting subjects. The
visual and somatosensory input on leg EMG responses in dy-
results suggest that two levels of control can be identified: first, a pattern
namic posturography in normals. Electroencephalogr C/in Neu-
of muscle activation is generated centrally, and second, the pattern is
rophysiol 1994, 93:7-l 4.
shaped and timed by interaction from the entire somatosensory, vestibu-
lar and visual input. 72. Hoshiyama M, Watanabe S, Kaneoke Y, Koike Y, Takahashi A:
Effecl of optokinetic stimulation on human balance recovery
56. Macpherson JM: The force constraint strategy for stance
in unexpected forward fall. Neurosci Res 1993, l&121-127.
is independent of prior experience. Exp Brain Res 1994,
101:397-405. 73. Zennou-Azogui Y, Xerri C, Harlay F: Visual sensory substitution
in vestibular compensation: neuronal substrates in the alert
57. Mcllroy WE, Maki BE: Task constraints on foot movements and cat. Exp Brain Res 1994, 98:457-473.
the incidence of compensatory stepping following perturbation
of upright stance. Brain Res 1993, 616:30-38. 74. Dietz V, Trippel M, lbrahim IK, Berger W: Human stance on
. a sinusoidally translating platform: balance control by feed-
58. Maki BE, Whitelaw RS, Mcllroy WE: Does frontal-plane asym- forward and feedback mechanisms. Exp Brain Res 1923,
metry in compensatory postural responses represent prepara- 93:352-362.
tion for stepping? Neurosci Lett 1993, 149:87-90. Body inclination and EMC activity was tested in subjects standing on
a treadmill moving sinusoidally backward and forward. Both feedback
59. Macpherson JM, lnglis JT: Stance and balance following bi-
reactions to changes in sinusoidal frequency and feedforward control
lateral labyrinthectomy. In Progress in brain research, vol 97.
during repeated cycles at the same frequency were analyzed. The au-
Edited by Allum JHJ, Allum-Mecklenburg DJ, Harris FP, Probst
thors conclude that feedforward control acts on CC position relative to
R. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.; 1993:219-228.
the feet in order to minimize forces acting on the body during treadmill
60. Horak FB, Shupert CL, Dietz V, Horstmann C: Vestibular and movement.
. somatosensory contributions to responses to head and body
75. Dietz V, Zijlstra W, Assaiante C, Tripple M, Berger W: Balance
displacements in stance. Exp Brain Res 1994, 100:93-l 06.
control in Parkinson’s disease. Gait & Posture 1993, 1:77-84.
The relative contribution of vestibular and somatosensory information to
triggering postural responses to external body displacements was inves- 76. Struppler A, Gerilovsky L, Jakob C: Self-generated rapid taps
tigated by comparing responses to displacements of the head alone with directed to opposite forearm in man: anticipatory reduction
responses to displacement of head and body, in healthy subjects and pa- in the muscle activity of the target arm. Neurosci Lett 1993,
tients with vestibular deficits. 159:115-118.

61. Horak FB, Nashner LM, Diener HC: Postural synergies asso- 77. Massion J: Movement, posture and equilibrium: interaction and
ciated with somatosensory and vestibular loss. Exp Brain Res coordination. Progress Neurobiol 1992, 38:35-56.
1990, 82:167-l 77.
78. Forget R, Lamarre Y: Postural adjustments associated with dif-
62. Pastor MA, Day BL, Marsden CD: Vestibular induced postural ferent unloading of the forearm: effects of proprioceptive and
responses in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1993, 116:1177-l 190. cutaneous afferent deprivation. Can J Pbysiol Pharmacol 1994,
in press.
63. Wasterton )A, Hawken MB, Tanyeri S, Jantti P, Kennard
C: Influence of sensory manipulation on postural control 79. Viallet F, Massion J, Bonnefoi-Kyriacou B, Aurenty R, Obadia
in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Neurosurg Psychiatry 1993, A, Khalil R: Approche quantitative de I’asynergie posturale en
56:1276-1281. pathologie cMbelleuse. Rev Neural 1994, 150:55-60.

64. Pyykko I, Aalto H, Starck J, lshizaki H: Postural stabilization on 80. Bouisset S, Duchene JL: Is body balance more perturbated by
a moving platform oscillating at high frequencies. Aviat Space respiration in seating than in standing posture! Neuroreport
Environ Med 1993,64:300-305. 1994, 5:957-960.

65. Di Fabio RP, Anderson JH: Effect of sway-referenced visual and 81. Rogers MW, Hedman LD, Pai YC: Kinetic analysis of dy-
somatosensory inputs on human head movement and postural namic transitions in stance support accompanying voluntary
patterns during stance. J Vestibular Res 1993, 3:409-417. leg flexion movements in hemiparetic adults. Arch Phys Med
Rehabil 1993, 74:19-25.
66. lnglis IT, Horak FB, Shupert CL, Jones-Rycewicz C: The impor-
. tance of somatosensory information in triggering and scaling 82. Diener HC, Bather M, Guschlbauer B, Thomas C, Dichgans
automatic postural responses in humans. fxp Brain Res 1994, J: The coordination of posture and voluntary movement in
100:159-164. patients with hemiparesis. J Neural 1993, 240:161-167.
The role of lower limb somatosensory information in triggering and scal-
83. Diener HC, Dichgans J, Guschlbauer 8, Bather M, Rapp H,
ing the postural responses in patients with sensory peripheral neuropathy
Klockgether T: The coordination of posture and voluntary
was tested. The postural response was delayed by 20-30ms and the scal-
movement in patients with cerebellar dysfunction. Mov Dis
ing of the response to both velocity and amplitude of surface translation
1992, 7:14-22.
impaired. The lower limb sensory afferents play a role in both triggering
and scaling the postural responses. 84. Horak F, Frank J: Three separate postural systems affected
in Parkinsonism. In Motor control VII. Edited by Stuart DC,
67. Horak FB, Diener HC: Cerebellar control of postural scaling
Cantcher GN, Curfinkal VS, Wiesendangez M. Tucson, Ari-
. and central set in stance. J Neurophysiol 1994, in press. zona: Motor Control Press; in press.
The scaling of the automatic postural response was studied in patients
with anterior cerebellar deficits. Although the responses were hyperme- 85. Woollacott M, Assaiante C, Amblard B: Development of bal-
tric, the earliest postural responses were scaled to platform velocity as ance and gait control. In Clinical aspects oi balance and gait
Postural control system Massion 887

disorders. Edited by Bronstein A, Brandt T, Wooliacott M. City?: 93. Pozzo T, Berthoz A, Lefort L, Vitte E: Head stabilization during
Arnold Publ; 1994, in press. various locomotor tasks in humans. II. Patients with bilateral
peripheral vestibular deficits. Exp Brain Res 1991, 85:208-2 17.
86. Jouen F: Head position and posture in newborn infants. In The
head-neck sensory-motor system. Vol 7: Evolution, biomechan- 94. Woollacott MH: Age-related changes in posture and move-
its and basic aspects. Edited by Berthoz A, Graf W, Vidal PP. ment. / Gerontology 1993, 48:5&60.
Chichester: John Wiley; 1994, in press.
95. Alexander NB: Postural control in older adults. / Am Geriat
87. Woollacott MH, Sveistrup H: The development of sensorimo- 50~ i 994, 42:93-i 08.
tor integration underlying posture control in infants during
the transition to independent stance. In hterlimb coordina- 96. Horak FB, Shupert CL, Mirka A: Components of postural
tion: neural, dynamical, and cognitive constrain&. Edited by dyscontrol in the elderly: a review. Neurobiol Aging 1989,
Swinnen S, Heuer H, Massion J, Casaer P. San Diego: Aca- io:727-738.
demic Press; 1994:371-389.
97. Whipple R, Wolfson L, Derby C, Singh D, Tobin I: Altered
aa. Hirschfeld H, Forssberg H: Epigenetic development of postu- sensory function and balance in older persons. / Geronrology
. ral responses for sitting during infancy. Exp Brain Res 1994, 1993, 48:71-76.
97:52a-540.
The ontogenesis of the postural reactions to platform translation is ana- 98. Keshner EA, Allum JHJ, Honegger F: Predictors of less sta-
lyzed in sitting toddlers. ble postural responses to support surface rotation in healthy
human elderly. / Vestibular Res 1993, 3:419-429.
89. Sveistrup H, Woollacott MH: Systems contributing to emer-
gence and maturation of stability in postural development. In 99. Maki BE: Biomechanical approach to quantifying anticipatory
The development of coordination in infancy. Edited by Savels- postural adjustments in the elderly. Med Go/ Eng Comput
bergh GJP. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1993:319-336. 1993, 31:355-362.
90. Woollacott MH, Sveistrup H: Changes in the sequencing and
100. Teasdale N, Bard C, Larue J, Fleury M: On the cognitive pen-
timing of muscle response coordination associated with devel-
.. etrability of posture control. Exp Aging Res 1993, 19:1-13.
opmental transItIons in balance abilities. Human Movement
Science 1992, 11:23-36. 101. Deliagina TG, Grillner S, Orlovsky CN, Ullen F: visual input
. affects the response to roll in reticulospinal neurons of the
91. Assaiante C, Thomachot B, Aurenty R: Hip stabilization and
lamprey. Exp Brain Res 1993, 95:421-428.
. lateral balance control in toddlers during the first four months
The role of visual input in body orientation and on reticulospinal neurons
of autonomous walking. Neuroreport 1993, 4:875-878.
activity is investigated in the lamprey.
The stabilization on hip, shoulder and head is analyzed during the first
four months of autonomous walking in toddlers.

92. Breniere Y, Bril B, Fontaine R: Analysis of the transition from


upright stance to steady state locomotion in children with
under 200 days of autonomous walking. / Motor Behav 1989, J Massion, Laboratory of Neurobiolob? and Movements, CNRS
21:20-37. 31, Chemin Joseph Aiguier, 13402 Marseille Cedrx 20, France.

You might also like