You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(4), 465–470, 2014

Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC


ISSN: 0022-3891 print / 1532-7752 online
DOI: 10.1080/00223891.2014.891524

The Dispositional Flow Scale–2 as a Measure of Autotelic


Personality: An Examination of Criterion-Related Validity
JARROD A. JOHNSON,1 HEIDI N. KEISER,2 EVAN M. SKARIN,3 AND SCOTT R. ROSS2,4
1
Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University
2
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota–Twin Cities
3
The Guildhall, Southern Methodist University
4
Department of Psychology, DePauw University

The Dispositional Flow Scale–2 (DFS–2; Jackson & Eklund, 2002) may be one of the most promising measures for assessing Csikszentmihalyi’s
(1990) construct of “autotelic personality.” Despite strong internal validity, external validity of the DFS–2 remains open. We used 2 methods
to provide evidence for external validity: (1) multiple-time assessments of experience sampling (1,856 entries generated over 7 days) to derive
aggregate indices of criterion validity; and (2) single-time assessments of flow and personality for additional criterion-related validity. For single-time
assessments of flow, we used a modified version of the Flow Questionnaire (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1984). To assess personality, we included
a measure of the Five-factor traits using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992). A path model of NEO domains, DFS–2
global scores, and experience sampling aggregates fit the data well.

Flow is an experiential state that is intrinsically rewarding and convergent) and examine the relationship of the DFS–2 to the
defined by total immersion and enjoyment (Csikszentmihalyi, Five-factor model (FFM) of personality traits (Costa & McCrae,
1990). Csikszentmihalyi (1975) coined the term flow to describe 1992).
observations of optimal human experience across multiple tasks
and activities. Studies at least partially support a key assumption AUTOTELIC PERSONALITY
that a balance of challenge and skill level gives rise to flow at Investigations of flow have led to the concept of an autotelic
the state level (Engeser & Rheinberg, 2008; Keller, Ringelham, personality, roughly defined as the tendency to engage in an ac-
& Blomann, 2011). The flow experience has been variously de- tivity “for its own sake” (i.e., intrinsic motivation; Csikszentmi-
scribed, but common descriptions include a merging of action halyi, 1975, 1990). Autotelic personality can be operationalized
and awareness, lack of self-consciousness, intense concentra- in the context of the ESM in two ways: frequency of experienc-
tion, clear goals, unambiguous feedback, a sense of control, ing flow (e.g., Asakawa, 2004) and quality of the flow experience
a distorted sense of time, and a desire to repeat the experi- (e.g., Carli, Fave, & Massimini, 1988). In this investigation, we
ence (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975, 1990). The relative balance of view autotelic personality as a constellation of traits that repre-
challenge and skill level is often used to index flow using an sent an increased propensity to experience flow across different
experience sampling method (ESM) wherein individuals report situations. The investigation of autotelic personality provides a
their experience in real time. Due in part to the limitations of fresh take on flow research and a fruitful avenue for future re-
retrospective self-report, complications of having participants search endeavors, as most studies to date have focused on flow
self-assess for abstruse experiences (Fiske, 1971), and the abil- propensity within the context of a chosen “high-flow” activ-
ity of the ESM to identify the contingencies of behavior (e.g., ity (e.g., music, sports, arts, etc.). Consistent with trait theory,
Scollon, Kim-Prieto, & Diener, 2003), the ESM has become we conceptualize autotelic personality as the tip of a spectrum
the standard in flow assessment. However, the difficulty of ob- propensity for experiencing flow across a wide range of ac-
taining ESM data has prompted the use of single-time mea- tivities and not simply specific situational contexts—in short,
surements of flow propensity with varying levels of content cross-situational consistency. An established measure of flow
validity. A recently derived measure of flow propensity, the propensity would aid researchers in exploring the correlates
Dispositional Flow Scale–2 (DFS–2; Jackson & Eklund, 2002) of autotelic personality and establishing the construct within a
possesses strong content validity by including nine subscales, larger nomological network (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955) while
each of which assesses a major component of flow. Previous allowing them to avoid the tangled logistics of ESM research.
studies examining the psychometric properties of the DFS–2
have focused mostly on the internal validity. In this study, we DISPOSITIONAL MEASURES OF FLOW
take a closer look at the external validity (criterion-related and To date, few dispositional measures of flow propensity have
been put forth in the literature. Csikszentmihalyi and Larson
(1984) were the first in developing the Flow Questionnaire
Received October 1, 2012; Revised January 22, 2014.
(FQ), a measure used for assessing individual differences in
Editor’s Note: This manuscript was accepted under the Editorship of
Gregory Meyer.
flow propensity via semistructured interviews. The FQ assesses
Address correspondence to Scott R. Ross, Department of Psychology, several major components of flow, but it lacks content validity
DePauw University, 7 Larabee Street, Greencastle, IN 46135; Email: srross@ in capturing the entire phenomological flow experience. The
depauw.edu Flow State Scale (FSS; Jackson & Marsh, 1996) is a state
465
466 JOHNSON, KEISER, SKARIN, ROSS

measure of flow with highly similar item content to the DFS–2 ness, as well as Extraversion and Agreeableness accounted for
in which individuals respond to their experience in the activity at least 38% of the variance in DFS–2 global scores and over
in which they have just participated. The DFS–2 (Jackson & 50% in a multivariate factor reflecting the DFS–2 components
Eklund, 2002) is a dispositional (or “trait”) measure of flow that aimed at general life activities.
assesses how respondents generally feel when taking part in a
given activity. Both measures possess much stronger content
THIS STUDY
and construct validity than the FQ as they assess all nine
of Csikszentmihalyi’s (1975, 1990) major flow components. This study advances the literature by examining the DFS–2’s
Despite the comprehensiveness of both the FSS and the DFS–2, potential as a measure of autotelic personality. Convergent
few studies have examined the ability of these instruments to validity was assessed via the FQ in dichotomous and Likert-
function as indicators of autotelic personality. The DFS–2’s scaled formats. Criterion-related validity was assessed using the
strong psychometric properties of internal consistency, content ESM as an aggregate behavioral criterion of flow propensity.
validity (i.e., construct representativeness), and factorial In addition, we examined all flow indexes in relation to the
validity, in addition to its dispositional focus, make it a prime FFM domains to provide further criterion-related validity. To
candidate for a single-time measure of flow propensity. Al- our knowledge, no previous study has attempted to predict flow
though the DFS–2 instructions refer to dispositional tendencies experiences (e.g., ESM sampling) using a dispositional flow
within the context of a particular domain (e.g., music, sports, measure. Until recently, previous studies had not examined
arts, etc.), they could easily be tailored so that the items assess flow within the context of the FFM. Employing the FFM in
general dispositional tendencies (i.e., life). Previous studies flow research should help to elucidate the construct of flow in
have shown that the “flow condition” can cut across any partic- an established nomological network. We hypothesize that the
ular activity (Massimini & Carli, 1988). By targeting responses DFS–2 will be related to the FQ and predict frequency of ESM
to general life activities, the DFS–2 can assess cross-situational flow experiences. The DFS–2 should also be strongly associated
consistency and provide a putative measure of flow propensity. with lower Neuroticism and higher Conscientiousness and
exhibit at least modest relationships with other FFM domains
CRITERION-RELATED VALIDITY OF THE DFS–2 (Extraversion +, Openness +). Finally, it is hypothesized that
Despite valuable studies supporting the internal validity of the the DFS–2 will demonstrate stronger criterion-related validity
DFS–2 (Jackson & Eklund, 2002; Jackson, Martin, & Eklund, than the FQ, because of the DFS–2’s higher content validity
2008), the criterion-related validity of the DFS–2 remains open, and construct representativeness.
with convergent validity restricted to examinations of interre-
lationships among DFS–2 subscales (see Wang, Liu, & Khoo, METHOD
2009). Both behavioral criteria (e.g., ability to predict frequency Participants
of flow experiences) and characterological criteria (e.g., rela-
tionships with known personality constructs) are needed for Fifty-two undergraduate students (M = 19.2 years, SD =
establishing the DFS–2 as a measure of autotelic personality. 1.12) at a selective Midwestern liberal arts college in the United
The ESM provides the best avenue for behavioral criterion States were recruited for participation. Participants were ran-
validation of the DFS–2. If the DFS–2 reflects greater flow domly selected from courses for participation and were con-
propensity, then higher scores should be associated with more tacted via e-mail. One participant failed to complete all mea-
time spent in flow. The well-established domains of the FFM of sures and was excluded from analyses. Participants received $30
personality provide appropriate characterological criteria and an or extra credit in psychology courses as payment for completion
established nomological network in which to identify the trait of the study.
constituents of autotelic personality. An autotelic personality
theoretically represents a specific constellation of personality Procedure
traits; if the DFS–2 assesses autotelic personality in terms of Participants attended a mandatory training session explain-
flow propensity, then DFS–2 scores should bear specific, pre- ing the process of the ESM, the methods of the study, and
dictable relationships with FFM domains. Of these, Neuroti- the procedure for filling out the Experience Sampling Forms
cism, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Openness should (ESFs). Students were randomly assigned to fill out the single-
be particularly relevant to autotelic personality. Individuals low administration self-report questionnaires before or after com-
in Neuroticism should be free of distraction and have reduced pleting the ESM portion of the study.
self-consciousness. Those high in Conscientiousness should be Participants received six signals per day for 7 days, yield-
more cognizant of their goals and more capable of directing their ing 42 possible responses per participant, for a total of 1,856
activities toward achieving those goals in an efficient manner, samplings. Signals were sent via text message to cell phones
with increased concentration, control, and motivation. Aspects between the hours of 10 a.m. and 10 p.m. using Windows Live
of Extraversion, including energy and activity, might propel in- Messenger. This 12-hr time period was divided into six 2-hr
dividuals into tasks that they find enjoyable, increasing task intervals; each signal was sent randomly within one of these
engagement and desire to repeat the experience. Openness to intervals, with no signal arriving within 1 hr of the previous
Experience could underlie greater absorption or immersion in signal. Participants were instructed to fill out an ESF as soon as
activities. Recently, Ullén et al. (2012) found partial support for possible after receiving the signal, with their entries based on
these predictions, where Neuroticism and Conscientiousness ac- the activity they were engaged in at the moment they were sig-
counted for 22% of the variance in a newly developed Swedish naled. Instructions were given that an ESF should not be filled
measure of flow in everyday life activities. Additionally, Ross out more than 30 min after receiving a signal.
and Keiser (2014) found that Revised NEO Personality Inven- Two different operationalizations of flow were utilized, the
tory (NEO PI–R) domains of Neuroticism and Conscientious- first being derived from Asakawa’s (2004) method. For the first
DISPOSITIONAL FLOW SCALE–2 467

operationalization (FlowPercent), experiences of challenge and TABLE 1.—Correlations among autotelic personality measures and ESM flow
skill had to exceed the group average and be within one raw indexes.
score point of each other; individuals’ total such experiences Variable FlowPercent InTheZone FQDRtot FQStot DFS–2
were divided by their total number of responses and multiplied
by 100 to determine the percentage of time they were in the flow FlowPercent — .33∗ .12 .24 .34∗
experience, or FlowPercent. A secondary ESM flow indicator InTheZone — .22 .01 .00
called InTheZone was also included. The operationalization of FQDRtot — — .74∗∗ .31∗
FQStot — — .60∗∗
InTheZone was the mean score for the ESF item measuring the
extent to which participants felt they were “in the zone” when Note. ESM = experience sampling method; InTheZone = average of responses to the
they were signaled. In the zone is an American colloquialism ESM item “In the zone”; FQDRtot = total score from dichotomous response items of the
used by athletes to denote the flow phenomenon (Weinberg & Flow Questionnaire (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1984); FQStot = summed scores from
scales added to Flow Questionnaire items; DFS–2 = Dispositional Flow Scale–2 (Jackson
Gould, 2006). An InTheZone experience is commonly under- & Eklund, 2002).

stood to include feelings of intense concentration, effortless p < .05 level (2-tailed). ∗∗ p < .01 level (2-tailed).
action, and absorption in the moment. This second definition
allows a within-method measure of flow and validity check to
two ways. First, dichotomous responses were summed to form
which we can compare our challenge-skill derived flow index.
the FQ Dichotomous Response Total (FQDRtot). Second, we
Measures extended the range of responses to improve the psychometric
properties by adding a 7-point Likert scale with anchors of very
Experience Sampling Form. The ESF was completed by rarely and extremely often. Responses were summed to create
participants each time they received a signal. The measure is the FQ Scale Total (FQStot). These variables were used to assess
composed of items designed to assess affect (e.g., “happy,” reciprocal convergent validity for the FQ and DFS–2.
“friendly”), activation or potency (e.g., “strong,” “active,” “fo-
cused”), and motivation (e.g., “wish to be somewhere else,” NEO Personality Inventory–Revised. The NEO PI–R
“motivation”), as well as items concerning the participant’s cur- (Costa & McCrae, 1992) is a 240-item questionnaire used to
rent environs and activities (see Asakawa, 2004; Massimini & assess the five FFM personality domains (i.e., Neuroticism, Ex-
Carli, 1988). Additionally, the ESF contains two items assessing traversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness).
perceived challenge and skill for the current activity in which Items are scored on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from strongly
the participant is engaged; these items were used to assess the agree to strongly disagree. Sample items include “I am not a
presence of flow. All nonsetting items are Likert scaled, ranging worrier” (Neuroticism) and “I have a very active imagination”
from 1 (very little) to 9 (very much). Investigators have used the (Openness). Domain-level alpha coefficients ranged from .86
ESM to assess the subjective experiences of flow across various for Conscientiousness to .93 for Neuroticism.
samples (e.g., Japanese college students, Asakawa, 2004; Italian
high school students, Carli et al., 1988; working adults, Csik-
szentmihalyi & LeFevre, 1989; and U.S. high school students, RESULTS
Shernoff, Csikszentmihalyi, Shneider, & Shernoff, 2003). Autotelic Personality Measures and Flow Indexes
Dispositional Flow Scale–2. The DFS–2 (Jackson & Ek- Zero-order correlations were calculated to determine rela-
lund, 2002, 2004) is a dispositional measure of flow that as- tionships among all flow indexes. The DFS–2 was significantly
sesses how respondents generally feel when taking part in a related to the primary ESM flow index, FlowPercent, although
given activity. It was constructed based on Csikszentmihalyi’s it was unrelated to the secondary flow index, InTheZone (see
(1975, 1990) nine proposed components of flow. Each compo- Table 1). We also examined the relationships between specific
nent is assessed with four items on a Likert scale, ranging from DFS–2 subscales and other flow indexes. FQStot was strongly
1 (never) to 5 (always). Coefficient alpha values for the nine related to DFS–2 total scores and most DFS–2 subscales. The
subscales ranged from .80 for Autotelic Experience to .91 for DFS–2 subscales Clear Goals and Autotelic Experience trended
Clear Goals. DFS–2 total scores had an internal consistency of toward significance in this analysis (p < .06), with only DFS–2
.91. Instructions for the DFS–2 were modified to target “any ac- Unambiguous Feedback being clearly unrelated (p > .50). The
tivity in life,” rather than participants’ experiences in a specific DFS–2 subscales Transformation of Time, Autotelic Experi-
activity. This modification allowed for the opportunity to test ence, and Loss of Self Consciousness all correlated significantly
the DFS–2 as a measure of autotelic personality. with FlowPercent (rs ranging from .33–.41, all p < .05). In The
Zone did not correlate significantly with any DFS–2 subscale.
Flow Questionnaire. The FQ (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson,
1984) consists of two separate questionnaires that were com- Autotelic Personality and FFM Personality
bined to create a four-item response questionnaire used to assess Zero-order correlations and multiple regression analyses were
previous flow experiences. Consistent with Asakawa (2010), used to examine ESM flow indexes vis-à-vis FFM domains.
four quotations describing firsthand accounts of the flow expe- With the exception of InTheZone scores with Neuroticism (see
rience are given, and the participant indicates whether or not he Table 2), no relationships between FFM domains and ESM
or she has had such an experience (e.g., “My mind isn’t wander- flow indexes were detected (p > .05). DFS–2 scores exhibited
ing. I am not thinking of something else. I am totally involved in strong relationships with Neuroticism and Conscientiousness.
what I am doing. My body feels good. I don’t seem to hear any- FQStot scores correlated significantly with Neuroticism and
thing. The world seems to be cut off from me. I am less aware approached significance with Conscientiousness (p = .068).
of myself and my problems.”). Responses were calculated in A multiple regression analysis using NEO PI–R domains to
468 JOHNSON, KEISER, SKARIN, ROSS

TABLE 2.—Correlations of autotelic personality measures and ESM flow indexes


with Five-factor model domains.

Variable FlowPercent InTheZone FQDRtot FQStot DFS–2

Neuroticism −.12 −.34∗ −.11 −.40∗∗ −.64∗∗


Extraversion .20 .09 .19 .09 .32∗
Openness .06 .05 .19 .09 .15
Agreeableness −.10 .18 .22 .26 −.14
Conscientiousness .26 −.18 .19 .29 .64∗∗

Note. ESM = Experience sampling method; InTheZone = average of responses to the


ESM item “in the zone”; FQDRtot = total score from dichotomous response items of the
Flow Questionnaire (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1984); FQStot = summed scores from
scales added to Flow Questionnaire items; DFS–2 = modified Dispositional Flow Scale–2 FIGURE 1.—Path model of Five-factor model personality, autotelic personality,
(Jackson & Eklund, 2002).

and flow experience. DFS–2 = Dispositional Flow Scale–2 (Jackson & Eklund,
p < .05 level (2-tailed). ∗∗ p < .01 level (2-tailed).
2002); FlowPercent = percentage of time spent in flow, as defined by the
percent of experience sampling method responses involving challenge and skill
endorsed above the sample mean for those items and within one point of each
predict DFS–2 total scores predicted 52% of the variance other. Listed parameter estimates are unstandardized/completely standardized.
∗∗∗ p < .005.
in DFS–2 scores (p < .001) with Neuroticism (β = −.47,
p < .005) emerging as a significant predictor and Conscien-
tiousness approaching significance (β = .30, p < .10). Overall,
the DFS–2 was strongly related to FFM personality. DISCUSSION
The findings reported here provide strong, initial support for
the use of the DFS–2 as a measure of autotelic personality. The
DFS–2 predicted a moderate amount of the variation in time
Path Model spent in flow (FlowPercent), as defined by challenge-skill bal-
To examine the relationships among basic personality (FFM), ance in the ESM. In addition, the strong relationship between
DFS–2 global flow, and ESM flow indexes, a path model was the Likert-scaled version of the FQ and DFS–2 total scores pro-
developed. Means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s alphas vides reciprocal convergent validity for the modified FQ and the
for path model variables are listed in Table 3. Zero-order DFS–2. However, the original dichotomous response version of
correlations in this sample suggested including the following the FQ generally failed to correlate significantly with time spent
covariances: Neuroticism with Conscientiousness (r = −.66, in flow and the FFM. Researchers should take caution when
p < .001), Extraversion with Openness (r = .36, p < .05), and using the FQ and only use a Likert-scaled format in assessing
Extraversion with Conscientiousness (r = .44, p < .01). These flow when using this measure. Many component subscales of
were added to the model. The a priori path model fit well, χ 2(3, the DFS–2 also predicted ESM indexes and were related to the
N = 51) = 3.342, ns, comparative fit index (CFI) = .990, root Likert-scaled FQ index. Additionally, our results seem to indi-
mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA) = .047. None cate little to no direct relationship between flow propensity as
of the effects of the FFM domains on FlowPercent were sig- measured by behavioral ESM indexes and FFM personality. The
nificant, nor were the effects of Extraversion and Openness on simplest explanation is a lack of statistical power to detect small
DFS–2 total scores; consequently, these parameters were suc- effects between the ESM and FFM. Additionally, differences in
cessively fixed to zero. Model fit did not degrade significantly, method likely contributed to small effects for aggregate ESM
χ 2 diff(6, N = 51) = 2.912, ns, CFI > .99, RMSEA < .001. indexes with a single-administration trait measure of the FFM.
Because they were unrelated to both FlowPercent and DFS–2 Conversely, we found moderate to strong relationships between
total scores, Extraversion and Openness were dropped from the the DFS–2 and FFM personality, with overlapping variance ex-
model. The simpler model fit well, χ 2(2, N = 51) = 2.087, ceeding 50%. Although this estimate is likely inflated by model
ns, CFI = .998, RMSEA = .029. The model is depicted in overfitting, it does suggest considerable overlap between a pu-
Figure 1. tative measure of general flow propensity (e.g., the DFS–2, tar-
geting general life activities) and FFM traits, a finding that is
consistent with recent investigations of flow and the FFM (see
TABLE 3.—Means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s alpha values for path Ross & Keiser, 2014; Ullén et al., 2012). Further, it is generally
model variables. consistent with recently published findings that highlight over-
lap between personality traits and the global DFS–2 index of
Variable M SD Cronbach’s alpha flow at 38% to over 50%, depending on the multivariate model
employed (see Ross & Keiser, 2014).
Neuroticism 92.0 19.7 .90 NEO Neuroticism and Conscientiousness were especially po-
Extraversion 117.6 20.2 .91
Openness 115.2 20.7 .92 tent predictors of global flow, consistent with recent findings by
Conscientiousness 117.7 17.5 .84 Ross and Keiser (2014) and Ullén et al. (2012). Individuals who
DFS–2 125.9 13.7 .91 score highly on the DFS–2 should be less anxious and self-
FlowPercent 8.9% 7.4% conscious, with clearer goal focus and persistence than lower
Note. DFS–2 = Dispositional Flow Scale–2 (Jackson & Eklund, 2002). The original
scoring individuals. In addition, a modest to moderate effect was
items were modified so that they referred to general experience in any activity in life rather found for Extraversion and the DFS–2, which might reflect the
than within the context of a specific activity. increased reward sensitivity of extraverts (Keiser & Ross, 2011)
DISPOSITIONAL FLOW SCALE–2 469

and those in flow (de Manzano, Theorell, Harmat, & Ullén, beyond this balance (i.e., propensity to experience the other ma-
2010). Although the 95% confidence interval for this correla- jor components of flow). Despite these limitations, the DFS–2
tion is 0.05 to 0.55 in this sample, we have reason to believe predicted the ESM flow index as well as the “in the zone” mean
that this finding is not sample-specific. Although not found by from the ESM itself. These issues are particularly pervasive in
Ullén et al. (2012) using a different global index of flow in life ESM studies that typically, although not always, include small
activities, the effect of extraversion (r = .35) was found by Ross sample sizes in the range of 40 to 60 participants (see Verduyn,
and Keiser (2014) in a much larger sample using the NEO PI–R Delvaux, Van Coillie, Tuerlinckx, & Van Mechelen, 2009).
and the DFS–2 adapted in the same way as for this study. Our findings advance the literature by providing convergent
Our findings lend themselves to a path model of flow con- and criterion-related validity for the DFS–2 as a measure of
structs (see Figure 1). At one end lies personality constructs, autotelic personality. We showed that a potential measure of
and at the other end lies flow experiences as measured by the autotelic personality succeeds in predicting the classic indicator
ESM. Our index of basic personality, the NEO PI–R reflecting of flow states in situ. Future investigations should include larger
the FFM, is the most proximal measure to basic personality di- sample sizes that would possess the sensitivity and power neces-
mensions. On the other end, the ESM flow index is the most sary to discern the subtle relationships among basic personality,
proximal measure to flow experiences as states. In between, we autotelic personality, and the flow experience.
find the DFS–2 as a stable measure of flow propensity. If this
model were accurate, we would expect to find stronger relations REFERENCES
to flow indexes as we move from the trait-personality end toward Asakawa, K. (2004). Flow experience and autotelic personality in Japanese
the state-flow end. Such a model fits our data quite well. The college students: How do they experience challenges in daily life? Journal of
DFS–2 appears to have entirely accounted for the relationships Happiness Studies, 5, 123–154. doi:10.1023/B:JOHS.0000035915.97836.89
that Neuroticism and Conscientiousness have with ESM flow Asakawa, K. (2010). Flow experience, culture, and well-being: How do autotelic
propensity, although we might have lacked the power to detect Japanese college students feel, behave, and think in their daily lives? Journal
any covariance that the DFS–2 did not cover. Despite these find- of Happiness Studies, 11, 205–223. doi:10.1007/s10902-008-9132-3
Campbell, D., & Fiske, D. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation
ings, the sample size in this study does not allow for sufficient
by the multitrait–multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105.
power to clearly answer this question. A major limitation of the doi:10.1037/h0046016
path modeling is that the likelihood of rejecting our proposed Carli, M., Fave, A., & Massimini, F. (1988). The quality of experience in the flow
model was low. In setting alpha at either the .05 or .10 levels channels: Comparison of Italian and U.S. students. In I. Csikszentmihalyi &
for chi-square test of model fit, it gives us a 6% or 11% power, M. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of
respectively, to reject the model. Using an RMSEA index of .10 flow in consciousness (pp. 364–383). New York, NY: Cambridge University
gives us a 22% power to reject the model. Consequently, because Press.
of sample size, this model might provide more heuristic or ex- Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory
ploratory value for future researchers rather than act as a test of (NEO PI–R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO–FFI) professional man-
the direct effects of Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, or both in ual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests.
terms of DFS–2 scores. Similarly, sample-specific inflations of
Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281–302. doi:10.1037/h0040957
certain correlations (e.g., between NEO Neuroticism and Con- Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Play and intrinsic rewards. Journal of Humanistic
scientiousness at .66; most studies find correlations between Psychology, 15, 41–63. doi:10.1177/002216787501500306
these NEO domains in the .20–.30 range, as in Ross, Canada, Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New
& Rausch, 2002) suggest the need for replication using larger York, NY: Harper & Row.
samples. For example, although only Neuroticism and Consci- Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being adolescent: Conflict and
entiousness accounted for significant variance in DFS–2 global growth in the teenage years. New York, NY: Basic Books.
scores, Ross and Keiser (2014) found that Extraversion and Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work
Agreeableness were also important. Null findings for Openness and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 815–822.
suggest that it might have less to do with characterizing autotelic doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.5.815
de Manzano, O., Theorell, T., Harmat, L., & Ullén, F. (2010). The psychophys-
personality as measured by flow propensity than originally hy-
iology of flow during piano playing. Emotion, 10, 301–311.
pothesized. Engeser, S., & Rheinberg, F. (2008). Flow, performance and moderators of
The correlation between ESM index of FlowPercent might challenge-skill balance. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 158–172.
indicate a theoretical cap on the strength of relationship that Fiske, D. (1971). Measuring the concepts of personality. Oxford, UK: Aldine.
cross-method flow propensity measures could exhibit with Fleeson, W., & Gallagher, P. (2009). The implications of Big Five standing
FlowPercent. The trait flow measures, however, use a one-time for the distribution of trait manifestation in behavior: Fifteen experience-
self-report method. This cross-method, within-method distinc- sampling studies and a meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
tion is important, as the within-method comparison would pos- Psychology, 97, 1097–1114. doi:10.1037/a0016786
sess the greatest potential for demonstrating a high correla- Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: The
tion because of common methods variance (Campbell & Fiske, Flow State Scale–2 and Dispositional Flow Scale–2. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 24, 33–150. Retrieved from EBSCOhost.
1959). The effect size in predicting ESM flow states from the
Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2004). The Flow Scales manual. Morgantown,
DFS–2 is consistent with previous ESM studies predicting spe- WV: Fitness Information Technology.
cific behaviors from traits (Scollon et al., 2003) and might be as Jackson, S., & Marsh, H. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to
high as can be expected when only a limited slice of a particu- measure optimal experience: The Flow State Scale. Journal of Sport and
lar trait’s manifestations is analyzed (see Fleeson & Gallagher, Exercise Psychology, 18, 17–35. Retrieved from EBSCOhost.
2009). Although challenge-skill balance might be important to Jackson, S. A., Martin, A. J., & Eklund, R. C. (2008). Long and short measures of
flow, it is only one part of the experience (Engeser & Rheinberg, flow: The construct validity of the FSS–2, DFS–2, and new brief counterparts.
2008). Autotelic personality should have other manifestations Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30, 561–587.
470 JOHNSON, KEISER, SKARIN, ROSS

Keiser, H. N., & Ross, S. R. (2011). Carver and White’s BIS/FFS/BAS scales Scollon, C. N., Kim-Prieto, C., & Diener, E. (2003). Experience sampling:
and domains and facets of the Five factor model of personality. Person- Promises and pitfalls, strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Happiness Stud-
ality and Individual Differences, 51, 39–44. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.03. ies, 4, 5–34. doi:10.1023/A:1023605205115
007 Shernoff, D., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Shneider, B., & Shernoff, E. (2003).
Keller, J., Ringelham, S., & Blomann, F. (2011). Does skills-demands com- Student engagement in high school classrooms from the perspec-
patibility result in intrinsic motivation? Experimental test of a basic notion tive of flow theory. School Psychology Quarterly, 18, 158–176.
proposed in the theory of flow experiences. Journal of Positive Psychology, doi:10.1521/scpq.18.2.158.21860
6, 408–417. Ullén, F., de Manzano, Ö., Almeida, R., Magnusson, P. E., Pedersen, N. L.,
Massimini, F., & Carli, M. (1988). The systematic assessment of flow in Nakamura, J., . . . Madison, G. (2012). Proneness for psychological flow in
daily experience. In Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow everyday life: Associations with personality and intelligence. Personality and
in consciousness (pp. 266–287). New York, NY: Cambridge University Individual Differences, 52, 167–172. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.003
Press. Verduyn, P., Delvaux, E., Van Coillie, H., Tuerlinckx, F., & Van Mechelen,
Ross, S. R., Canada, K. E., & Rausch, M. K. (2002). Self-handicapping I. (2009). Predicting the duration of emotional experience: Two experience
and the Five factor model of personality: Mediation between neuroti- sampling studies. Emotion, 9, 83–91.
cism and conscientiousness. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, Wang, C., Liu, W., & Khoo, A. (2009). The psychometric properties of Dispo-
1173–1184. sitional Flow Scale–2 in Internet gaming. Current Psychology: Research and
Ross, S. R., & Keiser, H. N. (2014). Autotelic personality through a Five-factor Reviews, 28, 194–201. doi:10.1007/s12144-009-9058-x
lens: Individual differences in flow propensity. Personality and Individual Weinberg, R., & Gould, D. (2006). Foundations of sport and exercise psychology
Differences, 59, 3–8. (4th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Copyright of Journal of Personality Assessment is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and
its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like