You are on page 1of 7

Proceedings of 10th International Coral Reef Symposium, 431-437 (2006)

Developmental changes in coral larval buoyancy and vertical


swimming behavior: Implications for
dispersal and connectivity

Alina M. SZMANT* and Melissa G MEADOWS

Center for Marine Science, University of North Carolina at Wilmington, 5600 Marvin K. Moss Ln, Wilmington NC
28409 USA
*Corresponding author: A.M. Szmant
FAX: 910-962-2410, e-mail: szmanta@uncw.edu

Abstract Many hermatypic corals are bro adcast


spawners. Little is known about the dispersal potential of Introduction
their planktonic larvae. Settlement can begin 2-3 d ays A current major question in marine eco logy is to
post-fertilization, but com petency ma y last for a month or understand the connectivity among widely dispersed
more. Hyd rogra phic mod els can be use d to estimate larval populations, and how this connectivity varies with
dispe rsal, but they require information about the distance among localities. The dispersal abilities of
swimming beha vior of the larvae during transport, planktonic larvae of sessile benthic marine invertebrates
including buoyancy and vertical swimming behavior. are important to their eventual reproductive and
Coral eggs are positively buoyant when released, but recruitment success, as well as to connectivity among
embryos and larvae with time become less buoyant and populations. If larvae are retained close to the parental
acquire the ability to swim downw ards. The time-course source (either for behavioral or environmental reasons),
of such d evelo pme ntal changes is ne eded to coup le with chances of successful local recruitment may be higher
hydro graphic data to mo del dispersal curves. Such da ta but connectivity with more distant populations will be
are presented here for the major Caribbean hermatype, low. Ho wever, extensive self-seeding will eventually
Montastraea faveo lata. Larval buoyancy was measured lead to low genetic diversity. If, alternatively, most of
as ascent rate in a volumetric pipette, and measured every the larvae are carried away by currents, they may be
6 hours for 3.2 5 hours po st-fertilization. V ertical transported away from suitable habitat and/or the larvae
swimming behavior was measured as vertical distribution may be in the dangerous plankton for longer periods,
of the larvae within 2 L graduated cylinders, placed on an which would decrease their chances o f survivorship to
orbital shaker table, measured every 6 hours from 40 settleme nt. Potential recruitment to any particular
hours to 8.5 days p ost-fertilizatio n. Ne wly fertilized eggs loca lity will depend on upstream sources of larvae as
(non-swimming) had an ascent rate of 1.82 mm s-1, and well as the co nditions at that site that help retain loca lly
this decreased linearly to 1.04 mm s-1 over the following derived larvae (Hughes et al 199 9, 20 00). W hen
2.3 days, by which tim e many larvae were swimming. observed recruitment rates are low , it may be because
Over 80 % of the larv ae rem ained within the top 4 cm of larval supply is too small to over-com e the mortality
the 40 cm-tall cylinder over the first 2.5 days p ost- rates operating on that life stage, or because a site is in a
fertilization. Diel vertical migration became app arent system where there is poor larval retention or few
after ca. 3 daysdownwards in dark, upwards in light), and upstream sources of larvae. Thus understanding the
became more pronounced over time. However, even 8 dispersal patterns of larvae is important to understanding
days post-fertilization, less than 20 % of the larvae were whether a population is supply-side recruitment limited.
at the bottom of the cylinder at the end of the dark period. The dispersal potential of coral larvae remains poorly
These results show that while some larvae of this species studied because of the small size of the larvae and the
can settle within a few days, most do not develop strong difficulty of qua ntitatively sam pling them in the
bottom seeking behavior until much later, and could be plankton. One approach to estimating dispersal potential
dispe rsed far from their site of o rigin. is to examine the duration of the competency period.
Some coral species are brooders that release mature
K eywords coral larvae, planulae, swimming behavior, planula larvae which are able to settle within hours of
dispersal release (Ha rrison and W allace, 1 990 ). By contrast,

431
larvae of broad cast spawning corals have by necessity waters.
longer planktonic periods, and thus supposedly the A modeling approach was used to predict five
potential for broader dispersal. But brooded larvae are different patterns of larval dispersal for coral larvae
larger, and have a com plem ent of algal symb ionts originating from the Texas F lower Gardens in the G ulf
(zooxanthellae) that can nourish them during a long of Mexico (Lugo-Fernandez et al. 200 1). These authors
planktonic period (Harrison and Wallace 199 0). conclude that most of the spawn would remain within 40
Richmond (1988) found that larvae of the brooder, km of the spawning source. Several o f the model
Pocillopora dam icorn is, remained competent for much patterns have the larvae m oving long d istances over
longer than those of the bro adcast spawner, Acropora periods of 30 to 60 days. These predictions did not
tenuis, but based on the long larval competency durations take into acco unt the ability of larva e to rem ain
found by Richmond, both would be expected to have a com peten t in the plankton for such long p eriod s.
high dispersa l ability. The larval co mpe tency duration is known for only a
Another approach that has been used to estima te few species, and ranges from 20 d for Acropo ra tenuis
dispersal potential for corals was to compare coral to 100 d for Pocillopora dam icornis (Richmond 19 88),
recruitment onto settlement plates suspended in the water and from 26 d for Cyphastrea serailia to 56 d for
column at various distances from an isolated source reef, Goniastrea austr alensis and 78 d for Acanthastrea
Helix Reef, on the Great Barrier Reef (Sammarco and lordh owensis (W ilson and Harrison 1998). Szmant
Andrews 1988, 1989). This study found more coral spat (unpublished) has found a competency period of ca. 30
on settlement plates de ployed 300 m from the reef than days for Montastraea faveolata indicating that it has the
either further away or on the reef proper. These authors ability to disperse over significant distances. Oliver et al
concluded that coral reefs are self-seeding, and that coral (1992) found that modeled coral larval dispersal patterns
larvae have limited dispersal potential, but their study did not co rrelate w ell with ob served larva l abundance
suffered from the serious limitation that the plates were and recruitment patterns, and ascribed this to the fine-
not collected until 7 months after deployment. Given the scale three-dimensional flows which occur around
high and variable post-settlement mortality experienced topo graphically com plex structures such as cora l reefs
by coral spat (e.g. Babcock and M undy 1996; Szma nt (e.g. Wo lanski 1988; Wolanski and Hamner 1988;
and Miller, submitted) the Helix experiment was not W olanski et al. 1989). Therefore, approaches are
properly designed to distinguish the amount of settlement needed that take into account finer scale circulation
per se without the confounding effect of po st-settlement features around coral reefs, as well as differences in the
mortality. There is an extensive literature on the development and competency behavio rs among coral
importance of grazing and substrate quality to the species.
suitability of reef substrate for coral settlement (recently In this study, we exam ined the time-course of changes
summarized in Babcock and Mundy 1996), and substrates in buoyancy and the deve lopment of swimming
suspended in the water column far from the reef were behaviors of the larvae o f Mo ntastra ea faveolata. This
likely heavily fouled and unlikely candidates for coral species is one of the major reef-builders in the
settleme nt. Furthermore, the hydrography surrounding Caribbean, dominates the adult coral community on
plates suspended in the water column is not likely the many Caribbean reefs as massive colonies meters high,
same as that over a reef. A fast current would prevent but is infrequently found in recruit surveys (e.g.
larvae from being retained for long enough to explore Rylaarsdam 1983; Chiapp one and Sullivan 1995; Miller
substra te and attach. If a reef were present in the same et al 2002). M. faveolata is hermaphroditic and
area, water currents would be slowed, and to pog rapically broadcasts large numbers of gametes into the water
generated eddies would tend to retain larvae. column in a mass spawning event that is highly
Several studies have used aerial tracking of coral larval predictable (Szmant 1986, 19 91; M iller and Szmant, in
slicks in the d ays after spawning . In fact, “pink stripes” review). After gamete bundle release (eggs plus sperm)
several km long were first reported in the open ocean large slicks form on the ocean surface, which depending
surrounding Bermuda and were identified as coral slicks on weather and local hydrographic cond itions, can last
by Butler (1980). Australian investigators have found for a day o r more. The emb ryos take a minim um of 3 to
evidence for bo th local retention and self-seeding (larvae 5 days after fertilization to de velop into competent
trapped in back-reef lagoons) and wide dispersal of coral planula larvae (Szmant et al. 1997; Szmant and Miller
larvae from their natal reef depending on local weather submitted). The competency period can last up to 30
conditions, topography, and hydrography (W illis and days (Szm ant and Miller subm itted), but most larvae will
Oliver 1988, 1990; Wolanski et al. 1989). Oliver and likely attempt to settle within the first two weeks if they
W illis (1987) repo rted slicks 5 km long m ade up of both are in the appropriate environment. The eggs, embryos
dead and live larvae, and found that concentrations of live and planulae will be transported as passive particles by
larvae were higher in these slicks than in surrounding reef ocean currents during this early development, until the

432
larvae lose their positive buoyancy and develop the ability within the cylinders. Tests with dye and buoyant beads
to position them selves in the water colum n. If time- showed that this mixing only penetrated to the bottom of
courses of such larval transport characteristics were the top 2 00 m l section within the cylinder. At night, the
available, they could be used together with hydro graphic cylinders were covered with a card boa rd enclosure to
data in physica l-biological co upled mod els to predict the prevent artificial light from influencing larval behavior.
dispersal patterns and distances of any given spawn event Approximately every 6 hours for up to 207 hours, the
from any given reef. numbers of larvae within each 200 ml section (= 4.2 cm
vertical depth) of the graduated cylinder were counted.
M ethods Due to the high number of larvae residing in the top
Gamete collection and fertilization layer of the water column, it was difficult to obtain an
Mo ntastrea faveo lata gamete bundles were collected accurate count for that dep th interval, and thus those
at Key Largo Dry Rocks (Key Largo, FL) by divers with larvae were not counted. Percentages of the total
large cone-shaped, cloth mesh nets deployed over number of larvae for all depths excluding those in the top
individual coral heads. Spawning took place from 11:30- layer were then calculated for each section of the water
11:50 pm on August 18, 2003. Collector cups at the cod- column for each measurement time. Data are plotted
end of the nets were retrieved by the divers and returned against time of day and distinguished by light conditions
to the research vessel, where the bundles from individual (day / night) at the time of measurement.
colonies were combined. Following the break-up of the
gamete bund les, the mixture of gametes from several Results
dozen colonies was allowed to fertilize for ca. 1 hour, Buoyancy
after which the sperm were washed from the eggs by The ascent rate of newly fertilized M ontastraea
repeated rinsing with filtered seawater. Unfertilized eggs faveo lata eggs was 1.8 2 mm /sec. Ascent rate
began to break up after ca. 12 hours and were removed consistently dropped over time as the larvae became
from the culture w ith repeated washes. Eggs (300 to 350 older, up to an age of 56 hours (linear re gressio n, r 2 =
um in diameter), embryos and planula larvae (ca. 500 to 0.96; Figure 1). At 56 hours, the average ascent rate had
700 um in length) were removed from this culture at dropped to 1.04 mm/sec and approximately 20% of the
frequent intervals beginning as soo n as possible after larvae had become neutrally buoyant or developed the
fertilization to measure their buoyancy, and later to be ability to swim vertically within the pipet. By 78 hours
used for the vertical swimming behavior study described of age, 100% of the planulae were swimming or neutrally
below. buoyant (Table 1).

Buoyancy Table 1. Percent of larvae that had beco me ne utrally


The buoyancy of eggs, embryos or larvae was buoyant or had begun swimming over the time-course of
measured every 6 hours from 0 to 7 8 hours p ost- measurem ent.
fertilization by measuring their ascent rate. The apparatus
used to measure ascent rate consisted o f a 25 ml graduated
Embryo or % %
pipet mounted to a ring stand . The pipet was filled with
Larval Age Neutrally Swimming
seawater, and a pipettor on top was used to slowly draw
(hrs) Buoyant
embryos or larvae just into the tip of the pipet. From
there, they rose through the water column within the pipet 0 - 45 0 0
at their own accord. Larvae were timed with a stop-watch
as they rose over a d istance of 55 mm, and ascent rates 56 20 0
were calculated. This was repeated for 15 individual
larvae, and an average ascent rate was calculated. 63.5 -- 67
Percentages of larvae that were neutrally buoyant and/or
73 -- 73
had developed swimming behavior were also calculated
where applicable. 78 -- 100

Vertical swimming behavior


Approximately 1000 early planula larvae were placed
into each of four 2-L graduated cylinders containing 2 L
of filtered sea water at 42 hours after fertilization. The
experimental cylinders were 51 cm tall. The cylinders
were placed on orbital shaker tables to provide water
movement and turbulence important to gas exchange

433
Fig. 1. Ascent rate of larvae in mm/sec as they ro se
through a graduated pipet. As larvae grew older and used Fig. 2. The number of larvae in the bottom section of
up lipid reserves, buoyancy, expressed here as ascent rate, the water column of the experimental cylinders were
was reduced until 56 hours of age when some larvae counted at three times of day while being exposed to diel
became neutrally buoya nt or develo ped the ability to cycles of light and darkness. The larvae exhibited a diel
swim. pattern in their exp loration of the cylinder bottom. On
a given day, more larvae were in the bottom section in
the early morning, at the end of the dark period, and
fewer were on the bo ttom in the middle of the day after
Vertical swimming behavior exposure to several hou rs of light.
During the first few measurement intervals (42 hrs to
78 hrs), most of the M. faveolata larvae remained in the vertical position will also play a role in determining
top 200 ml section of the graduated cylinder, and were too larval dispersal patterns between spawning and
concentrated to count. W ith time, larvae be gan to settleme nt.
descend deepe r into the cylinder and to swim around the Based on measurements of bu oyancy, Montastraea
botto m of the cylinder. Larvae were foun d to exhibit a faveo lata larvae can be treated in hyd rogra phic m ode ls
diel swimming pattern which was weak at first but became as passive, positively buoyant particles until 56-78 hours
more pronounced by 108 hours after fertilization (Figure after spawning. Up to this time, the majority of the coral
2). They concentrated up near the surface of the water embryos and larvae are likely to remain near the surface
column during the daylight hours and moved down to of the water column. Hence their transport will be
explore the bottom substrate at night. Towards the end of determined by surface currents. This positively buoyant
the expe riment, elongated planulae were quite mobile and stage corresponds with the pre-competency period
could be observed swimming up and down past each other (Szmant and Miller, submitted), and thus, the re is
in the cylinder virtually no chance of larval settlement during this time
The percentage of larvae observed in the bottom due both to the inability of the larvae to overcome
section of the wa ter column in the early morning, after buoyancy to reac h the bottom , as well as a physiological
maximal time in the dark, increased o ver time . T he inability to settle.
percentage of larvae in the bottom section peaked at 60% After 78 hours, M. fa veolata larvae can no longer be
after 127 hours post-fertilization (Figure 3). considered passive particles. While these larvae are
small, have only a weak swimming ability, and cannot
Discussion swim against horizontal currents, they do have the ability
The time-course of buoyancy and vertical migration to change their vertical position in the water column.
patterns documented here are important parameters that Depend ing on how well larvae are able to swim up and
will influence the path these coral p lanulae will take down, this may have a significant influence on transport.
during their planktonic development period, and The influence of vertical swimming ability on
ultimately where they are likely to settle. Buoyancy as horizontal transport has been described for fish larvae
well as vertical swimming abilities will determine the (Kingsford and Choat 1986, Weinstein et al 1980, Dame
position of larvae in the water colum n. The longer a larva and Allen 1996, Bradbu ry and Snelgrove 200 1).
remains as a passive particle ‘stuck’ in the surface layer,
the further it is likely to disperse from its natal reef.
Because current speed and direction vary with depth,

434
Fig. 3. M orning dep th profiles for larvae in graduated cylinders. Over time, as mo re larva e dev elop the ability to swim
downwards, an increasing number of larvae are found exploring the substrate at the bottom of the water column. This
peaked at 127 hours and then dropped off slightly.

Although coral larvae are much weaker swimm ers, A recent paper has reported that planulae of two
vertical swimming has the potential for having a species of coral reach peak comp etency between 60 and
significant effect on the transport of these larvae as it has 66 HP F, and the authors claim their data to mean that
been shown for fish larvae. corals have lim ited dispersa l abilities and mostly local
The deve lopm ent of vertical swimming b ehavior is recruitment (Miller and Mundy 200 3). H owever, only
also crucial to the ability of larvae to search for su itable 10% survivorship was reported, and of that 10% , only
substra te and settle. After more than 200 HPF, fewer than 32.3% were settled at the end of the experim ent (10 days
20 % of the experimental larvae had developed this after fertilization). When the authors conclude that the
downward seeking behavior, as measured at the end of the majo rity of coral larvae settle early, they neglected to take
dark period. This suggests that the remaining 80 % of the into account the remaining 68 % of their larvae that were
planulae would remain in the plankton for longer than 8 still swimming after 10 days. Thus, a more parsimonious
days before attem pting to settle. Depending on interpretation of their data is that while a small percent of
hydro graphic conditions, most larvae may be able to be coral larvae become competent after a few days, most are
transported over considerable distances before even unab le to settle for a much longer duration. The potential
developing the downward swimming behavior necessary for dispersal increases with the time it takes a larva to
for the exploration o f substrate for settlem ent. become competent. Most previous studies claiming

435
limited dispersal po tential for corals have relied simply on thus the num ber o f larvae a vailable to settle will decrease
the soonest competency is reached, and have failed to over time. T he net effect of sup erimp osing a mortality
consider the fate of the remaining larvae that d id not settle function on the time-course of development of settlement
in their experiments. behavior by larvae of M . faveolata will be that a higher
Many previous estimates of larval dispersal potential percent of the initial spawn will survive to settle within a
have neglec ted to take into account the finer physiological 5 day transport radius from the natal reef, and that fewer
and behavioral dynamics of coral larvae that may larvae will survive to settle at great distances. However,
influence their transport. Competency perio d duration is given the large numbers of larvae produced by a major
an imp ortant part of this potential but is known for only mass spawn of this species (estimated at 106 eggs per m 2
a few species. Long comp etency period s will conve y a of live tissue; with a 10 % cove r by this species on a
large potential for dispersal for species with that typical Caribbean reef, a small 100 m x 100 m patch reef
characteristic. Published competency durations range will produce 10 9 eggs) this does not preclude a significant
from 20 d for Acropo ra tenuis to 100 d for Pocillopora number of larvae surviving to settle at distances of 10 s to
dam icorn is (Richmond 1988), and from 26 d for 100s of km from their source. The lack of genetic
Cyphastrea serailia to 56 d for Go niastre a au stralen sis structure for this species among widely separated
and 78 d for Acanthastrea lordho wensis (W ilson and localities in the Caribb ean suppo rts this conclusion
Harrison 1998). Szmant and M iller (in review) have (population genetic study of Severance 2002).
found a com peten cy period of ca. 30 days for
Montastraea faveolata indicating that it has the ability to Acknow ledgements
disperse over significant distances. Lugo-Fernandez et al The idea for this project came out of d iscussions with
(2001) made an assumption in their modeling approach colleagues Dr. B arry Ruddick and Chris Taggart from
that coral larvae could remain in the plankton for Dalhousie University. This work was conducted under
prolonged periods and predicted quite extensive dispersal perm it FKNM S 2002-066, with funding provided by the
patterns in the Gulf of Mexico. H owever, even when a NOAA Restoration Program. This work could not have
long period of competency is known, it does not suffice to been accomp lished without the logistic support from the
use this information to pred ict dispersal. This was FKNM S, especially Bill Valley and Lt. Comm . Dave
demonstrated by Oliver et al. (19 92) when mod els based Score. Field and lab assistance were provided by a large
on com peten cy failed to accurately p ortray recruitment number of colleagues, students, and volunteers, espe cially
patterns. Also, the short-comings of the Helix Reef Dr. Margaret M iller and her staff (Charles Fasano, Dana
experiment( Sammarco and Andrews 1988; 1989) W illiams, Mark V ermeij, Iliana Baums), Ben Mason, D r.
dem onstrate the importance of hydrography in larval M onica Medina, Dr. Mary Alice Coffroth, and TNC
dispersal experiments and models. These studies reveal volunteers Loretta Lawrence and B rad R osov.
a need for mo re advanced m ode ling if accurate dispersal
patterns are to be predicted. Duration of the passive high References
buoyancy period and the interval to onset of vertical Babcock R, Mundy C. (199 6) Coral recruitment:
swimming behaviors differ not only between species but Consequences of settlement choice for early growth
also within a cohort of coral larvae, and they have a large and survivo rship in two scleractinians. J Exp Mar
potential for affecting transport. We suggest that this Biol Ecol 206: 179-201
information must be includ ed with detailed hydrogra phic Bradb ury IR, Snelgrove PVR (2001) Contrasting larval
information in ord er to pred ict dispersal patterns. transport in demersal fish and benthic invertebra tes:
In the present study, we show that there is a range of the roles o f behaviour and advective processes in
time over which planulae of Mo ntastra ea faveolata determining spatial p attern. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 58:
develop the downward swimming behavior pre-requisite 811-823.
for the search for substrate and settlement. Settlement Butler JN (198 0) Pink stripe on the ocean. Nat Hist (N
assays done in shallow petri dishes especially do not take Y) 89: 62-63.
this into account. Even after five days, only 60 % of the Chiappone M, Sullivan KM (1996) Distribution,
larvae, excluding those that were still in the very top of abundance and sp ecies comp osition of juvenile
the cylinder, were exhibiting the dark-period downward scleractinian corals in the Flo rida R eef T ract. B ull
swimming beha vior. A model based on these data would Mar Sci 58: 555-569
suggest that less than 60 % of the M. faveolata larvae Dame RF, Allen DM (1996) B etween estuaries and the
derived from a particular spawn event will settle on reefs sea. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 200: 169-185
that are within 3 to 5 days of transport from the spawn Harrison PL, Wallace CC (1990) Reproduction, dispersal
site, while the remaining 40 % have the potential for and recru itment of scleractinian corals. In:
being carried further aw ay. Of course, mortality in the "Ecosystems of the Wor ld", Vo l. 25: Coral Reefs.
plankton will diminish the larval supply over time, and Elsevier, New York. pp. 133-208.

436
Hughes TP, Baird AH, Dinsdale EA, Moltscha niwskyj Szmant AM (1986) Reproductive ecology of Caribbean
NA, Pratchett MS, Tanner JE, Willis BL (1999) reef corals. Coral Reefs 5: 43-54
Patterns of recruitment and abundance of corals along Szmant AM (1991) Sexual reproduction by the Caribbean
the Great Barrier Reef. Nature 397: 59-63 reef corals Mo ntastrea annularis and M. cavern osa.
Hughes TP , Baird AH, D insdale EA , Motschaniwskyj Mar Ecol Prog Ser 74: 13-25
NA, Pratchett M S, Tanner JE , Willis BL (2000) Szmant AM , Miller MW ( 2005) Settlement preferences
Supply-side ecology works both ways: The link and post-settlement mortality of laboratory cultured
between benthic adults, fecundity, and larval recruits. and settled larvae of the Carib bean herm atypic corals
Ecology 81: 2241-2249 Mo ntastra ea faveolata and Acropora palmata in the
Kingsford MJ, Choat JH (1986) Influence of surface Florida Keys, USA. Proc 10 th Internat Coral Reef
slicks on the distributio n and onshore movements of Symp, Okinawa, June 2004
small fish. Mar Biol 91: 161-171 Szmant AM , Weil E, Miller MW, Colón DE (1997)
Lugo-Fernandez A, Deslarzes KJP, Price JM, Boland GS, Hybridization within the species complex of
Morin MV (2001) Inferring probable dispersal of scleractinian coral Mo ntastra ea annu laris. Marine
Flower Garden Banks coral larvae (Gulf of Mexico) Biology 129: 561-572
using observed and simulated drifter trajectories. W einstein M P, W eiss SL, Hodson RG, Gerry LR (1980)
Cont Shelf Res 21: 47-67 Retention of three taxa of postlarval fishes in an
Miller K, Mundy C (2003) Rap id settlement in broad cast intensively flushed tidal estuary, Cape Fear R iver,
spawning corals: implications for larval dispersa l. North Carolina. Fish Bull U.S. 78: 419-436
Coral Reefs: 22: 99-106 W illis BL, Oliver JK (1988) Inter-reef dispersal of coral
Miller MW , Weil E, Szmant AM (200 0) Coral larvae following the annual mass spawning on the
recruitment and juvenile mortality as structuring Great Barrier Reef. Proc Sixth Int Coral Reef Symp,
factors for reef benthic communities in Biscayne Townsville, Australia. Volume 2: 853-859
National Park, USA. Coral Reefs 19: 115-123 W illis BL, Oliver JK (19 90) Direct tracking of coral
Miller M W , Szmant AM. (in review). Lessons Learned larvae: Implications for dispersal studies of
from Experimental Key-Species Restoration. Chapter planktonic larvae in topographically complex
18 in “Ecological shifts along the Florida reef tract: environments. Field Inferences in the Ecology of
the past as a key to the future”, Precht WF, Miller SL Marine Invertebrate Larvae: Proc of a Symp pp. 145-
(editors) Springer-Verlag. 162 , Ophelia 32, no. 1-2
Oliver JK, W illis BL (1987) Coral-spawn slicks in the Wilson JR, Harrison PL (1998) Settlement-competency
Great Barrier Reef: Preliminary observations. periods of larvae of three species of scleractinian
Marine Biology 94: 521-529 corals. Ma rine B iology 131 : 339 -345 .
Oliver JK, King BA , Willis BL, Babcock B C, W olanski W olanski E (1988) Island wakes shallow waters. J
E (1992) Dispersal of coral larvae from a lagoonal Geophys Res 93: 1335-1336
reef. 2. Comparisons between model predictions and W olanski E, Hamner W M (1988) Topog raphically
observed conc entrations. Co nt Shelf Res 12: 873- controlled fronts in the ocea n and their biological
889. influence. Science 241: 177-181
Richmond RH (19 88) Competency and dispersal W olanski E, Burrage D, K ing B (1989) Trapping and
potential of planula larvae o f a spawning versus a dispersion of coral eggs around Bowden Reef, Great
brooding coral. Proc S ixth Int Coral Reef Symp, Barrier Reef, following mass coral spawning. Cont
Townsville Australia 2: 827-831 Shelf Res 9: 479-496
Rylaarsdam KW (1983) Life histories and abundance
patterns of colonial corals on Jamaican reefs. Mar
Ecol Prog Ser 13: 249 - 260
Sammarco P, Andrews JC (1988) Localized dispersal
and recruitment in Great Barrier Reef corals: The
Helix experiment. Science 239: 1422-1424
Sammarco P, Andrews JC (1989) The Helix exp eriment:
Differential localized dispersal and recruitment
patterns in Great Barrier Reef corals. Limnol
Oceanogr 34: 896-912
Severance EG (2002) Con nectivity, genetic diversity and
complex evolution of reef-building Scleractinia. PhD
Dissertation, University of South Florida, Tampa FL
USA

437

You might also like