You are on page 1of 49

Journal Pre-proof

Transcranial alternating current stimulation of α but not β frequency sharpens multiple


visual functions

Hisato Nakazono, Katsuya Ogata, Akinori Takeda, Emi Yamada, Takahiro Kimura,
Shozo Tobimatsu

PII: S1935-861X(19)30431-0
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2019.10.022
Reference: BRS 1597

To appear in: Brain Stimulation

Received Date: 15 July 2019


Revised Date: 23 September 2019
Accepted Date: 31 October 2019

Please cite this article as: Nakazono H, Ogata K, Takeda A, Yamada E, Kimura T, Tobimatsu S,
Transcranial alternating current stimulation of α but not β frequency sharpens multiple visual functions,
Brain Stimulation, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2019.10.022.

This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition
of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of
record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published
in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that,
during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal
disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

© 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.


Nakazono et al. - 1

3 Transcranial alternating current stimulation of α but not β

4 frequency sharpens multiple visual functions

8 Hisato Nakazono1, 2, Katsuya Ogata1, 3, Akinori Takeda1, Emi Yamada1, Takahiro Kimura4,

9 Shozo Tobimatsu1

10

11

1
12 Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Neurological Institute, Faculty of Medicine,

13 Graduate School of Medical Sciences, Kyushu University, Fukuoka 812-8582, Japan

2
14 Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Medical Science, Fukuoka International

15 University of Health and Welfare, Fukuoka 814-0001, Japan

3
16 Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, School of Health Sciences at Fukuoka,

17 International University of Health and Welfare, Fukuoka 831-8501, Japan

4
18 Institute of Liberal Arts and Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-1192, Japan

19
Nakazono et al. - 2

20 Address Correspondence to Hisato Nakazono, PhD:

21 Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Medical Science, Fukuoka International

22 University of Health and Welfare, Fukuoka 814-0001, Japan

23 E-mail: nakazono@med.kyushu-u.ac.jp

24 Phone: +81-92-832-1200

25 Fax: +81-92-832-1167

26
Nakazono et al. - 3

27 Abstract

28 Background: Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) can entrain and enhance

29 cortical oscillatory activity in a frequency-dependent manner. In our previous study

30 (Nakazono et al.2016), 20 Hz (β) tACS significantly increased excitability of primary

31 motor cortex compared with 10 Hz (α) tACS. α oscillations are a prominent feature of the

32 primary visual cortex (V1) in a resting electroencephalogram. Hence, we investigated

33 whether α and β tACS can differentially influence multiple visual functions.

34 Methods: Firstly, we evaluated the after-effects of α and β tACS on pattern-reversal (PR)

35 and focal-flash (FF) visual evoked potentials (VEPs). Secondly, we determined the

36 relationship between resting α oscillations and PR-VEPs modulated by tACS. Thirdly, the

37 behavioral effects of tACS were assessed by contrast sensitivity.

38 Results: α tACS modulated the amplitudes of PR-VEPs, compared with β tACS, but did

39 not modulate the FF-VEPs. Time-frequency analysis revealed that α tACS facilitated

40 event-related α phase synchronizations without increasing power, which consequently

41 increased the PR-VEP amplitudes. There was a significant positive correlation between

42 PR-VEP amplitudes and resting α oscillations. These findings suggested that α tACS

43 modulated α oscillations, and affected visual functions of contrast and spatial frequency.

44 Indeed, α tACS also improved subjects’ contrast sensitivity at the behavioral level.

45 Conversely, β tACS increased posterior α activity, but did not change VEP amplitudes.
Nakazono et al. - 4

46 Conclusions: α tACS can influence different neuronal populations from those influenced

47 by β tACS. Thus, our results provide evidence that α tACS sharpens multiple visual

48 functions by modulating α oscillations in V1.

49
Nakazono et al. - 5

50 Keywords: α and β tACS, α oscillations, contrast sensitivity, visual cortex, visual evoked

51 potentials

52

53

54 Highlights

55 We studied whether α and β tACS can differentially influence the occipital cortex.

56 Effects of α and β tACS on VEPs, resting EEG and contrast sensitivity were assessed.

57 α tACS but not β tACS was effective for sharpening multiple visual functions.

58 Effects of tACS on visual cortex can be a frequency-dependent manner.

59

60 Abbreviations:

61 tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation; VEP, visual evoked potential; PR-VEP,

62 pattern-reversal visual evoked potential; FF-VEP, focal-flash visual evoked potential; EEG,

63 electroencephalography; ERP, event-related potential; V1, primary visual cortex; V2,

64 secondary visual cortex; ERSP, event-related spectral perturbation; ITC, inter-trial

65 coherence; cpd, cycle per degree; LMM, linear mixed-effects modeling


Nakazono et al. - 6

66 Introduction

67 Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) is a non-invasive technique

68 known to modulate brain oscillations. Interestingly, tACS affects brain functions or

69 behavior in a frequency-dependent manner [1–5]. The effective stimulation frequency of

70 tACS is regarded as matching the targeted brain oscillations [4,6]. Indeed,

71 electroencephalographic (EEG) studies have revealed that tACS at α frequency (~10 Hz)

72 over the visual cortex enhances resting α activity in the parieto-occipital area during [7] and

73 after stimulation [8–11]. Posterior α oscillations are a prominent feature in resting EEG,

74 and are thought to have an association with a broad range of visual neurocognitive

75 functions. However, the role of these oscillations in visual system function remains unclear

76 [12]. Recently, tACS has been applied to investigate a causal link between α oscillations

77 and visual processing [7,13–15]. Nevertheless, frequency-dependent effects of α tACS

78 were not observed in some studies [16,17]. Therefore, the effects of α tACS on the visual

79 cortex may be complex and depend on task demands.

80 In this study, we recorded visual evoked potentials (VEPs) to determine the effects

81 of α tACS on the visual system. The human visual system processes different visual

82 information via multiple, parallel pathways or channels, and VEPs can be used to assess

83 such pathways [18]. For example, in pattern-reversal VEPs (PR-VEPs), the small

84 checkerboard pattern stimulates the fovea and activates mostly contrast channels [19],
Nakazono et al. - 7

85 whereas flash stimulation is processed by luminance channels [20]. Several earlier studies

86 have postulated that VEPs are related to α oscillations [21–23]. In such a model, the phases

87 of the background α oscillations are partially aligned with the visual stimulus and, in turn,

88 the oscillations contribute to the generation of VEP components. However, this relationship

89 remains controversial [24–26]. Therefore, we hypothesized that if there is a causal

90 relationship between background α oscillations and visual system processes, then α tACS

91 should modulate not only resting α activity but also visual system functions such as VEPs

92 and visual task performance. To test this hypothesis, we first investigated whether α (10

93 Hz) tACS modulated PR- and focal-flash (FF-) VEP amplitudes after stimulation, as

94 compared with β (20 Hz) tACS, in Experiment 1. Next, we assessed the event-related α

95 phase concentration or power dynamics in VEPs modulated by α and β tACS using

96 time-frequency analysis. We also investigated resting α power modulations after tACS in

97 Experiment 2. Finally, we measured the behavioral effects of α and β tACS on contrast

98 sensitivity in Experiment 3 because PR-VEP amplitudes depend on pattern spatial

99 frequency [27]. Since there may be different spatial frequency preferences in the separate

100 visual cortical areas (e.g., primary (V1) and secondary (V2) visual cortices) [28], we used

101 three different spatial frequencies for the contrast sensitivity assessments.

102

103 Material and methods


Nakazono et al. - 8

104 Participants

105 A total of 33 healthy subjects (17 females; mean ± SD, 26.8 ± 7.8 years old) took

106 part. None of the subjects had any history of neurological, psychiatric or other medical

107 problems. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. The right eye was

108 dominant in 30 subjects. All participants gave written informed consent in accordance with

109 the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Kyushu

110 University. Thirteen subjects (7 females; 26.1 ± 7.4 years old) joined in Experiment 1. The

111 sample size was chosen based on previous studies of tACS targeted to the visual cortex [8–

112 11,13]; this number of subjects showed sufficient statistical power (1 − β = 0.9).

113 Subsequently, sample sizes of N ≥ 15 were determined to be necessary for Experiments 2

114 and 3, to achieve statistical power of 0.8, on the basis of the effect size in Experiment 1.

115 Therefore, 17 subjects (7 females; 28.4 ± 8.3 years old; 5 had also participated in

116 Experiment 1) and 15 subjects (8 females; 29.5 ± 9.1 years old; 7 had also participated in

117 Experiments 1 and/or 2) took part in Experiments 2 and 3, respectively.

118

119 Experimental designs

120 This research was designed as a randomized, crossover and single-blinded study.

121 Sessions with the different tACS frequencies (i.e., Experiment 1: α and β tACS,

122 Experiments 2 and 3: α and β tACS and sham) were separated by at least 2 days.
Nakazono et al. - 9

123 In Experiment 1, PR- and FF-VEPs were recorded before, and every 10 min after,

124 tACS, for up to 30 min (Fig. 1A). The order of VEP types was randomized across subjects.

125 The sham condition was not used in Experiment 1 because we were primarily concerned

126 with the difference in VEP alteration between FF- and PR-VEPs following α and β tACS.

127 In Experiment 2, resting EEG and VEP recordings were completed before, and up to 10

128 min after, stimulation (Fig. 1B). In the VEP measurement, PR-VEPs were recorded because

129 α tACS was found to be effective only on PR-VEPs in Experiment 1. Resting EEG was

130 always recorded before PR-VEP recordings to minimize the influence of VEP stimuli on

131 the resting α oscillations. In Experiment 3, contrast sensitivity was measured at baseline

132 and up to 10 min after tACS (Fig. 1C). Each session started with a training run to

133 familiarize the subject with the contrast sensitivity task. The sham condition was employed

134 in Experiments 2 and 3, to confirm the tACS effects.

135

136 tACS

137 Participants sat on a comfortable chair in a quiet room with dim lighting during

138 experiments. tACS was delivered by a battery-driven current stimulator (DC

139 Stimulator-Plus, NeuroConn GmbH, Ilmenau, Germany) through two self-adhesive

140 electrodes (PALS electrodes, Axelgaard Manufacturing Co., Ltd., Fallbrook, CA). tACS

141 was applied at 1 mA (peak-to-peak) for 20 min (the impedance was kept below 5 kΩ), with
Nakazono et al. - 10

142 5 s ramping up and down. The target tACS electrode (3.5 × 3.5 cm) was placed at Oz’ (2.5

143 cm above the inion), and the reference electrode (7 × 5 cm) was positioned at Cz

144 (International 10–20 system). The tACS electrodes were affixed using a support bandage.

145 These electrode positions were chosen to affect the occipital cortex [29,30]. For sham

146 stimulation, α tACS was applied for only 30 s at the beginning of the 20 min period. After

147 stimulation, all subjects were asked to report whether they experienced any tACS-induced

148 sensations.

149

150 Recordings of VEPs and resting EEG

151 VEPs were recorded from the electrode at Oz’ with the reference electrode at FCz

152 (forehead) [31,32]. For PR-VEP recordings, a checkerboard pattern with 15-min checks

153 was used. For FF-VEP recordings, a uniform white field was presented for 200 ms and then

154 replaced by a black field for 800 ms. For both types of VEPs, the stimuli subtended 6° of

155 visual angle from the vertical and horizontal midline in the lower visual field (Fig. 1A) [32].

156 The resting EEG was recorded from Oz’ and Pz (International 10–20 system) [9,33] with

157 the reference electrode at FCz. To measure background brain oscillations, subjects’ resting

158 EEG responses were recorded for 60 s. Participants were instructed to fixate on a red point

159 at the center of the screen during VEP and resting EEG recordings (Fig. 1). Details of VEP

160 and EEG recordings are provided in the supplementary methods.


Nakazono et al. - 11

161

162 Measurement of contrast sensitivity

163 Contrast sensitivity was measured with single Gabor patches (visual angle, 10°;

164 vertical orientation) in binocular vision (Fig. 1C). Contrast sensitivity was measured at

165 three different spatial frequencies (0.5, 3 and 9 cycle per degree (cpd)), and the test

166 consisted of 30 trials (10 trials per spatial frequency, in random order). For further details,

167 see supplementary methods.

168

169 Data Analyses

170 Data analyses were performed using functions implemented in the MNE-Python

171 software [34] and custom-programmed tools in Python.

172

173 Analysis of VEP amplitudes

174 The artifact-free VEP data from Oz’ were averaged offline. We measured the major

175 components of FF- and PR-VEPs: peak-to-peak amplitudes of PR-VEPs (N75, P100 and

176 N145) and FF-VEPs (N1, P1 and N2) (Fig. 2A) [32]. The two VEPs recordings from before

177 tACS were averaged, and used as the baseline (pre-tACS). The VEP amplitudes in trials

178 after tACS (post-tACS) were normalized relative to those of the pre-tACS.

179
Nakazono et al. - 12

180 Time-frequency analysis for VEP data

181 In the time-frequency analysis, the event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) and

182 inter-trial coherence (ITC) were calculated. The epochs of 2000 ms (with 1000 ms before

183 pattern-reversal) were extracted from the PR-VEP data recorded at electrode Oz’ in

184 Experiment 2. Trial-by-trial time-frequency analysis was performed using Morlet wavelet

185 transforms in frequencies between 4 and 40 Hz, in steps of 1 Hz, with wavelet cycles

186 increased from 2 to 20. ERSP represents mean power changes relative to the prestimulus

187 period (-300 to 0 ms) across epochs, and was calculated as the log-ratio [log10

188 (poststimulus period/prestimulus period)]. ERSP measures event-related changes in spectral

189 power over time [35]. Meanwhile, the ITC is a measure of the phase synchronization of the

190 EEG activity to the time-locking events [36,37]. The ITC values range from 0 to 1, with

191 ITC values close to 1 reflecting almost perfect phase coincidence across epochs.

192 To evaluate the ERSP and ITC related to VEP responses, we defined the analysis

193 time windows from 50 to 200 ms after pattern-reversal (Supplementary Fig. S2). Moreover,

194 frequency bands of interest were selected in the α (10 ± 1 Hz) and β (20 ± 1 Hz) ranges,

195 taking into account the tACS frequencies. The ERSP and ITC values of the analysis time

196 windows in the α or β ranges were averaged for each subject. To measure the after-effects

197 of tACS, two VEP recordings before tACS were pooled as the pre-tACS, and then

198 subtracted from the post-tACS for each subject.


Nakazono et al. - 13

199

200 Analysis of resting EEG

201 Resting EEG data from the electrodes at Oz’ and Pz were each split into 2 s epochs.

202 A fast Fourier transform was applied to each epoch, to calculate power spectra of 0.5–30 Hz,

203 then log-transformed spectra were averaged [38]. To evaluate power changes induced by α

204 and β tACS, the mean power in the α (10 ± 1 Hz) or β (20 ± 1 Hz) range was calculated. To

205 measure the after-effects of tACS, the pre-tACS power values were subtracted from the

206 post-tACS data (Δlog10 power = log10 power (post) − log10 power (pre)). Moreover, we

207 measured α and β peak frequencies in resting EEG data of pre-tACS (Supplementary Table

208 S1, Supplementary Fig. S3).

209

210 Analysis of contrast sensitivity

211 We excluded trials without a button press, then calculated the average contrast

212 sensitivity for each spatial frequency and each subject separately as a baseline (pre-tACS).

213 The individual contrast sensitivity values (i.e., 10 trials of each spatial frequency) in

214 post-tACS were normalized to the pre-tACS baseline value in each subject.

215

216 Statistical analyses

217 The tACS after-effects on each measurement were evaluated using a linear
Nakazono et al. - 14

218 mixed-effects modeling (LMM) with subjects as a random effect, and time (Experiment 1:

219 post 0–30 min; Experiment 2 and Experiment 3: post 0–10 min) and tACS (α and β tACS

220 or sham) as the fixed effects for all experiments. When a main effect of tACS was observed,

221 we used pairwise comparisons for a post-hoc analysis, and p values were adjusted using

222 Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. To evaluate the relationship between VEP

223 amplitudes and resting α power in Experiment 2, the after-effect at time points post 0-10

224 min on each measurement were averaged together as a total post-tACS, and then Pearson’s

225 correlation coefficients were calculated. To test the stability for pre-tACS in each

226 evaluation, LMM was performed with tACS as a factor. The data are presented as means ±

227 standard error of the mean. The significance level for all tests was set at p < 0.05. Statistical

228 analysis was carried out with SPSS (version 17.0 for Windows, IBM, Armonk, NY).

229

230 Results

231 Regarding the stability of pre-tACS measurements in each experiment

232 (Experiment 1: amplitudes of PR- and FF-VEPs; Experiment 2: N75-P100 amplitude of

233 PR-VEPs, and α or β range values (ERSP, ITC and resting EEG power); Experiment 3:

234 contrast sensitivities). There were no significant differences between tACS sessions in any

235 of the measurements (p ≥ 0.098).

236
Nakazono et al. - 15

237 Experiment 1

238 α tACS enhanced PR-VEPs but not FF-VEPs

239 α tACS increased PR-VEP N75-P100 amplitudes compared with β tACS (Fig. 2B

240 upper left). LMM revealed a significant effect of tACS, but no significant effects of time or

241 tACS × time interaction were found (Table 1). The PR-VEP P100-N145 amplitudes did not

242 differ significantly between the α and β tACS conditions (Table 1, Fig. 2B upper right). In

243 addition, there were no significant effects of time, tACS or their interaction on FF-VEP

244 N1-P1 amplitudes and P1-N2 amplitudes (Table 1, Fig. 2B lower).

245

246 Experiment 2

247 α tACS modulated event-related α phase synchronization without power change

248 We replicated the finding from Experiment 1 that α tACS increased N75-P100

249 amplitudes. In brief, LMM revealed a significant effect of tACS, but there was no

250 significant effect of time or time × tACS interaction (Table 1). Post-hoc analysis revealed

251 that N75-P100 amplitudes were significantly enhanced after α tACS, compared with sham,

252 and that there was a tendency toward higher amplitudes for α tACS compared with β tACS

253 (see Fig. 3 and figure legend).

254 Based on the time-frequency analysis, Figure 4A shows the changes from

255 pre-tACS in averaged ERSP in the α (10 ± 1 Hz) and β (20 ± 1 Hz) ranges. For the α ERSP,
Nakazono et al. - 16

256 LMM revealed a significant effect of tACS without significant effects of time or time ×

257 tACS interaction (Table 1). Post-hoc analysis revealed that β tACS decreased the α ERSP

258 compared with α tACS, but not compared with the sham condition (Fig. 4A left). In the β

259 ERSP, there were no significant effects of tACS, time, or time × tACS interaction (Table 1,

260 Fig. 4A right).

261 Figure 4B shows the after-effects of tACS on α and β ITC. LMM revealed a

262 significant effect of tACS without significant effects of time or time × tACS interaction

263 (Table 1). Post-hoc analysis showed that α tACS enhanced α ITC compared with that of β

264 tACS and sham (Fig. 4B left). In β ITC, there was a significant effect of tACS, without a

265 significant effect of time or time × tACS interaction (Table 1). Pairwise comparisons

266 revealed that β ITC was increased for α tACS compared with β tACS, but did not differ

267 from sham (Fig. 4B right).

268

269 α and β tACS augmented resting α activity

270 Figure 5A shows the grand-averaged power spectra across all subjects at Pz. α

271 tACS increased the resting α power, while β tACS increased both of resting α and β power.

272 In the α power, LMM revealed significant effects of tACS and time, but no significant time

273 × tACS interaction was found (Table 1). Both α and β tACS increased α power compared

274 with sham (Fig. 5B left). In β power, there was a significant effect of tACS without a
Nakazono et al. - 17

275 significant effect of time or time × tACS interaction (Table 1). β tACS increased β power

276 compared with both α tACS and sham (Fig. 5B right). Examination of the after-effects on α

277 and β power at electrode Oz’, showed that the power modulation by tACS was less marked

278 than that at electrode Pz (Supplementary Fig. S4).

279 tACS-induced changes in resting α power at Oz’ or Pz were positively correlated

280 with changes in PR-VEP amplitudes in the α tACS condition only (Fig. 6). We also

281 confirmed that the mean peak frequencies in the α and β ranges at electrodes Oz’ and Pz

282 were very close to the tACS frequencies (Supplementary Table S1). When subjects’ peak

283 frequencies in the α and β ranges were compared between Oz’ and Pz, both peak

284 frequencies at Pz were higher than those at Oz’ (Supplementary Fig. S3).

285

286 Experiment 3

287 α tACS improved contrast sensitivity

288 For each of the three spatial frequencies (i.e., 0.5-9 cpd), LMM revealed

289 significant effects of tACS, but no significant effects of time or time × tACS interaction

290 were found (Table 1). In comparison with the sham stimulation, α tACS improved the

291 contrast sensitivities at all three spatial frequencies (p ≤ 0.032, corrected), whereas β tACS

292 only increased the contrast sensitivity at 3 cpd (p = 0.018, corrected) (Fig. 7).

293
Nakazono et al. - 18

294 No effects of tACS-induced flickering sensation

295 In each of the three experiments, about half of the subjects reported a slight

296 flickering sensation during β tACS (see more details in Supplementary results). To

297 investigate the possible influence of tACS-induced flickering perception, we classified our

298 subjects into two groups: a group of those with the sensation (flickering group) and a group

299 of those without it (non-flickering group). We compared the total post-tACS between the

300 two groups, but found no significant differences (p ≥ 0.52, unpaired t-test) (Supplementary

301 Fig. S5). Therefore, our findings cannot be explained simply by flickering sensations.

302

303 Discussion

304 Our aim was to elucidate the effects of α tACS on the visual system. In

305 Experiment 1, α tACS increased the amplitudes of only the early components of PR-VEPs,

306 and this effect lasted for at least 30 min after stimulation. In Experiment 2, α tACS

307 increased resting α oscillatory activity, and a significant positive correlation between

308 PR-VEP amplitudes and resting α oscillatory activity was observed. α tACS also increased

309 the α phase synchronization in time-frequency analysis. Experiment 2 also demonstrated

310 that β tACS enhanced resting α activity, but did not affect VEP amplitudes. In Experiment

311 3, α tACS improved contrast sensitivity. Taken together, α tACS is more effective for

312 modulating PR-VEPs and contrast sensitivity than is β tACS. Possible neural mechanisms
Nakazono et al. - 19

313 and physiological implications are discussed below.

314 α tACS modulates early PR-VEP components and contrast sensitivity

315 In general, VEPs represent the activation of neuronal populations specific to the

316 nature of the visual stimulation. PR-VEP stimuli with an optimal check size preferentially

317 stimulate the fovea, and activate the contrast and spatial frequency channels of V1 [19].

318 Flash stimuli are processed by luminance channels [20]. Therefore, we assume that α tACS

319 affects specific channels of V1, and consequently increases the N75-P100 amplitudes of

320 PR-VEPs. Magnetoencephalographic studies have revealed that the N75 and P100

321 components have sources around the calcarine fissure of striate cortex (V1) whereas the

322 N145 source is localized to striate or extrastriate cortex [18]. Additionally, in a study by

323 Plant et al. (1983) early components of PR-VEP amplitudes were modulated by the spatial

324 frequency of visual stimuli, whereas those of late components were not [39]. In an animal

325 study, the optimal spatial frequency of cells in V1 ranged from 0.5 to 8 cpd, and cells in V2

326 responded to lower spatial frequencies than did cells in V1 [40]. Indeed, our finding

327 revealed that α tACS improved contrast sensitivity for the spatial frequencies between

328 0.5-9 cpd. Collectively, these findings support our assumption that α tACS activates the

329 contrast and spatial frequency channels of V1.

330

331 The interaction between posterior α oscillations and VEP amplitudes


Nakazono et al. - 20

332 For many years, researchers have debated whether VEPs are modulated by

333 ongoing α activity. There are two hypothetical models that explain the relationship between

334 VEPs and ongoing α activity: the evoked model and the phase-reset model. The former

335 assumes that VEPs are independent of background oscillatory activity [24–26]. The latter

336 supposes that ongoing α rhythm is partially phase reset by a sensory input, and part of the

337 VEP is generated by a reorganization of ongoing α oscillations [22,23,41]. So far, the

338 debate has not been conclusively settled.

339 Our time-frequency analysis revealed that α tACS increased the α ITC but not the

340 α ERSP, along with enhancing PR-VEP amplitudes. In the phase-reset model, it is predicted

341 that the event-related phase reorganization occurs independent of the event-related power

342 modulation [35,42,43]. In other words, the phase resetting of background α oscillations can

343 lead to increased α phase concentration (i.e., α ITC) without event-related α power (i.e.,

344 α ERSP) modulation. Thus, our α tACS after-effects agree well with predictions of the

345 phase-reset model. Furthermore, α tACS increased resting α activity, and there was a

346 positive correlation between PR-VEP amplitudes and resting α activity modulation, which

347 also supports the phase-reset model [41]. Conversely, β tACS was found to increase resting

348 α activity, whereas it did not change VEP amplitudes or α ITC. These β tACS effects

349 suggest that background α enhancement does not necessarily result in VEP modulation,

350 which is in line with the evoked model. Interestingly, β tACS decreased the α ERSP
Nakazono et al. - 21

351 compared with α tACS, but not compared with the sham condition. This effect suggests that

352 α and β tACS both increase resting α activity but induce different poststimulus α dynamics.

353 Thus, our findings indicate that the phase-reset model cannot completely explain our results,

354 suggesting that the two different models could coexist in parieto-occipital cortex. Indeed,

355 several studies have proposed the coexistence of these two models, according to which,

356 both partial phase-reset and partial evoked models contribute to the dynamics to VEP

357 generation [35,44].

358 Although α oscillations have been extensively investigated, their functional role is

359 not fully understood. One influential hypothesis is that α oscillations reflect the functional

360 inhibition of task-irrelevant brain regions [45,46]. In fact, previous studies revealed that

361 strong posterior α activity has a negative impact on visual behavioral performance [47,48].

362 However, other studies have demonstrated positive correlations of α activity with

363 behavioral performance [49–51], and it has been suggested that the functional role of α

364 oscillations may vary along the cortical hierarchy [52,53]. Thus, we speculate that α and β

365 tACS could modulate different neuronal networks or sources that are partially involved in

366 the generation of α oscillations, and consequently would induce different effects on

367 PR-VEP amplitudes.

368

369 tACS for spike timing-dependent plasticity


Nakazono et al. - 22

370 It has been suggested that tACS entrains ongoing oscillations in a

371 frequency-dependent manner [4]. Many studies have shown that α tACS over the occipital

372 cortex enhances α power in the parieto-occipital area during or after stimulation [7–11]. In

373 this study, we replicated the finding that α tACS increased the resting α power, and further

374 demonstrated that β tACS enhanced not only resting β power but also α power in the

375 parieto-occipital cortex. Meanwhile, all tACS conditions, including sham, increased resting

376 α power over time (post 0 vs. post 10) in Experiment 2. Increased resting α activity in a

377 sham condition was also reported in a previous tACS study as natural α increase [11], and it

378 may explain the effect of time revealed by LMM in Experiment 2. Recently, network

379 modeling studies revealed that when the stimulation frequency of tACS was a harmonic of

380 the endogenous oscillations, the endogenous rhythmic activity was increased as a

381 subharmonic resonance [54,55]. These resonance or entrainment effects are in good

382 accordance with our results. However, the entrainment evidence regarding tACS has been

383 supported during (i.e., online to) stimulation [7,56], and the mechanisms of after-effects

384 (offline effects) are unclear. A recent study showed that the entrainment effects of tACS did

385 not last after stimulation [10], and spike timing–dependent plasticity (STDP) was proposed

386 as a plausible candidate to explain after-effects [8,10,57]. According to the STDP

387 hypothesis, α tACS can selectively strengthen the synaptic weights of neuronal circuits

388 (recurrent loops) with a reverberation frequency at 10 Hz, and lead to the increased α
Nakazono et al. - 23

389 oscillatory activity, after stimulation, as a synaptic plasticity effect [8,10]. In this model,

390 online entrainment of tACS can contribute frequency-specific synaptic strengthening by

391 STDP [57]. Therefore, we speculate that β tACS can affect the neuronal circuits that

392 reverberate at 10 Hz and at 20 Hz, related to the above-mentioned subharmonic resonance

393 mechanisms, and result in enhancement of power in both the α and β ranges after

394 stimulation.

395 It is interesting to note that the modulation of resting α and β oscillations after

396 tACS was more pronounced at Pz than at Oz’ (Fig. 5 and Supplementary Fig. S4). In the

397 resting state, posterior α oscillations have a widespread origin in the parieto-occipital areas

398 [58] and may reflect a complex product of both thalamo-cortical and cortico-cortical

399 interactions in the visual cortex [12]. Indeed, subjects’ α or β peak frequencies in resting

400 EEG in pre-tACS differed somewhat between the Oz’ and Pz electrodes (Supplementary

401 Fig. S3). This may imply that posterior α and β oscillations reflect summation of several

402 local network rhythms. However, it is beyond the scope of our study to discriminate the

403 cortical sources modulated by tACS in the present experiments. This point should be

404 clarified in the future studies.

405

406 Limitations

407 Although tACS was applied at a constant amplitude (i.e., 1 mA), about half of
Nakazono et al. - 24

408 subjects noticed a slight flickering sensation during β tACS. Indeed, a previous study

409 revealed that the tACS-induced retinal flickering threshold was lowest for β tACS [59]. To

410 avoid the perception of flicker, different intensities are necessary for α and β tACS.

411 However, current intensity is an important factor influencing tACS effects [55,60], and thus

412 we used the same intensity at α and β tACS in this study. Moreover, although we

413 investigated the influences of tACS-induced flickering, these perceptions could not explain

414 the after-effects of α and β tACS on PR-VEPs and resting α power modulation.

415

416 Conclusions

417 Our findings revealed that α tACS over occipital cortex increased PR-VEP

418 amplitude and resting α oscillatory activity. However, β tACS induced the enhancement of

419 resting α power without VEP amplitude modulation. These results suggest that the

420 generators of VEPs and posterior α oscillations partially share the same neuronal networks,

421 and that tACS can affect such networks in a frequency-dependent manner.

422

423 Acknowledgments:

424 We are grateful to Dr. Junji Kishimoto for the constructive comments and support for

425 statistical analysis. We thank Claire Barnes, PhD, from Edanz Group

426 (www.edanzediting.com/ac) for editing a draft of this manuscript.


Nakazono et al. - 25

427

428 Author contributions

429 H.N., K.O. and S.T. designed the study; H.N. and K.O. collected the data; T.K., A.T. and

430 E.Y. supervised data collection; H.N., A.T. and K.O. analyzed the data; H.N., K.O. and S.T.

431 wrote the paper; T.K. commented on the paper.

432

433 Funding

434 This work was supported in part by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science a

435 Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (JSPS KAKENHI 15J03164 to HN) and Grant-in-Aid

436 for Early-Career Scientists (JSPS KAKENHI 18K17720 to HN) and Grant-in-Aid for

437 Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (MEXT KAKENHI 15H05875 to ST).

438

439 Competing Interests:

440 The authors declare no conflict of interest.


Nakazono et al. - 26

441 References

442 [1] Nakazono H, Ogata K, Kuroda T, Tobimatsu S. Phase and Frequency-Dependent

443 Effects of Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation on Motor Cortical

444 Excitability. PLoS One 2016;11:e0162521. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0162521.

445 [2] Kanai R, Paulus W, Walsh V. Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS)

446 modulates cortical excitability as assessed by TMS-induced phosphene thresholds.

447 Clin Neurophysiol 2010;121:1551–4. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2010.03.022.

448 [3] Voss U, Holzmann R, Hobson A, Paulus W, Koppehele-Gossel J, Klimke A, et al.

449 Induction of self awareness in dreams through frontal low current stimulation of

450 gamma activity. Nat Neurosci 2014;17:810–2. doi:10.1038/nn.3719.

451 [4] Herrmann CS, Rach S, Neuling T, Strüber D. Transcranial alternating current

452 stimulation: a review of the underlying mechanisms and modulation of cognitive

453 processes. Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7:279. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00279.

454 [5] Feurra M, Bianco G, Santarnecchi E, Del Testa M, Rossi A, Rossi S.

455 Frequency-dependent tuning of the human motor system induced by transcranial

456 oscillatory potentials. J Neurosci 2011;31:12165–70.

457 doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0978-11.2011.

458 [6] Thut G, Schyns PG, Gross J. Entrainment of perceptually relevant brain oscillations

459 by non-invasive rhythmic stimulation of the human brain. Front Psychol 2011;2:170.
Nakazono et al. - 27

460 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00170.

461 [7] Helfrich RF, Schneider TR, Rach S, Trautmann-Lengsfeld SA, Engel AK, Herrmann

462 CS. Entrainment of brain oscillations by transcranial alternating current stimulation.

463 Curr Biol 2014;24:333–9. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.041.

464 [8] Zaehle T, Rach S, Herrmann CS. Transcranial alternating current stimulation

465 enhances individual alpha activity in human EEG. PLoS One 2010;5:e13766.

466 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013766.

467 [9] Neuling T, Rach S, Herrmann CS. Orchestrating neuronal networks: sustained

468 after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation depend upon brain states.

469 Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7:161. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00161.

470 [10] Vossen A, Gross J, Thut G. Alpha Power Increase After Transcranial Alternating

471 Current Stimulation at Alpha Frequency (α-tACS) Reflects Plastic Changes Rather

472 Than Entrainment. Brain Stimul 2015;8:499–508. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.004.

473 [11] Kasten FH, Dowsett J, Herrmann CS. Sustained aftereffect of α-tACS lasts up to 70

474 minutes after stimulation. Front Hum Neurosci 2016;10:245.

475 doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00245.

476 [12] Clayton MS, Yeung N, Kadosh RC. The many characters of visual alpha oscillations.

477 Eur J Neurosci 2017;12:3218–21. doi:10.1111/ejn.13747.

478 [13] Müller NG, Vellage A-K, Heinze H-J, Zaehle T. Entrainment of Human Alpha
Nakazono et al. - 28

479 Oscillations Selectively Enhances Visual Conjunction Search. PLoS One

480 2015;10:e0143533. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0143533.

481 [14] Kar K, Krekelberg B. Transcranial alternating current stimulation attenuates visual

482 motion adaptation. J Neurosci 2014;34:7334–40.

483 doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5248-13.2014.

484 [15] Cecere R, Rees G, Romei V. Individual Differences in Alpha Frequency Drive

485 Crossmodal Illusory Perception. Curr Biol 2015;25:231–5.

486 doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.034.

487 [16] Brignani D, Ruzzoli M, Mauri P, Miniussi C. Is Transcranial Alternating Current

488 Stimulation Effective in Modulating Brain Oscillations? PLoS One 2013;8:e56589.

489 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056589.

490 [17] Clayton MS, Yeung N, Cohen Kadosh R. The Effects of 10 Hz Transcranial

491 Alternating Current Stimulation on Audiovisual Task Switching. Front Neurosci

492 2018;12:67. doi:10.3389/fnins.2018.00067.

493 [18] Tobimatsu S, Celesia GG. Studies of human visual pathophysiology with visual

494 evoked potentials. Clin Neurophysiol 2006;117:1414–33.

495 doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.004.

496 [19] Holder GE, Celesia GG, Miyake Y, Tobimatsu S, Weleber RG. International

497 Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology: recommendations for visual system testing.


Nakazono et al. - 29

498 Clin Neurophysiol 2010;121:1393–409. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.010.

499 [20] Spekreijse H, Estevez O, Reits D. Visual evoked potentials and the physiological

500 analysis of visual processes in man. In: Desmedt J, editor. Vis. Evoked Potentials

501 Man New Dev., Oxford: Clarendon Press; 1977, p. 16–89.

502 [21] Brandt ME, Jansen BH, Carbonari JP. Pre-stimulus spectral EEG patterns and the

503 visual evoked response. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1991;80:16–20.

504 doi:10.1016/0168-5597(91)90037-X.

505 [22] Makeig S, Westerfield M, Jung TP, Enghoff S, Townsend J, Courchesne E, et al.

506 Dynamic brain sources of visual evoked responses. Science 2002;295:690–4.

507 doi:10.1126/science.1066168.

508 [23] Hanslmayr S, Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Gruber W, Doppelmayr M, Freunberger R, et

509 al. Alpha phase reset contributes to the generation of ERPs. Cereb Cortex

510 2007;17:1–8. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj129.

511 [24] Shah AS, Bressler SL, Knuth KH, Ding M, Mehta AD, Ulbert I, et al. Neural

512 Dynamics and the Fundamental Mechanisms of Event-related Brain Potentials.

513 Cereb Cortex 2004;14:476–83. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh009.

514 [25] Mazaheri A, Jensen O. Posterior alpha activity is not phase-reset by visual stimuli.

515 Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103:2948–52. doi:10.1073/pnas.0505785103.

516 [26] Risner ML, Aura CJ, Black JE, Gawne TJ. The Visual Evoked Potential is
Nakazono et al. - 30

517 independent of surface alpha rhythm phase. Neuroimage 2009;45:463–9.

518 doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.031.

519 [27] Plant GT. Transient visually evoked potentials to sinusoidal gratings in optic neuritis.

520 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1983;46:1125–33. doi:10.1136/jnnp.46.12.1125.

521 [28] Skottun BC. On the use of spatial frequency to isolate contributions from the

522 magnocellular and parvocellular systems and the dorsal and ventral cortical streams.

523 Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015;56:266–75. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.07.002.

524 [29] Neuling T, Wagner S, Wolters CH, Zaehle T, Herrmann CS. Finite-Element Model

525 Predicts Current Density Distribution for Clinical Applications of tDCS and tACS.

526 Front Psychiatry 2012;3:83. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00083.

527 [30] Ruhnau P, Neuling T, Fuscá M, Herrmann CS, Demarchi G, Weisz N. Eyes wide

528 shut: Transcranial alternating current stimulation drives alpha rhythm in a state

529 dependent manner. Sci Rep 2016;6:27138. doi:10.1038/srep27138.

530 [31] Kimura T, Ogata K, Tobimatsu S. Repetitive paired-pulse transcranial magnetic

531 stimulation over the visual cortex alters visual recovery function. Brain Stimul

532 2013;6:298–305. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2012.05.005.

533 [32] Kimura T, Ogata K, Nakazono H, Tobimatsu S. Repetitive Paired-pulse Transcranial

534 Magnetic Stimulation Over the Visual Cortex Selectively Inhibits Focal Flash VEPs.

535 Brain Stimul 2014;7:275–80. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2013.12.010.


Nakazono et al. - 31

536 [33] Dowsett J, Herrmann CS. Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation with

537 Sawtooth Waves: Simultaneous Stimulation and EEG Recording. Front Hum

538 Neurosci 2016;10:135. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00135.

539 [34] Gramfort A, Luessi M, Larson E, Engemann DA, Strohmeier D, Brodbeck C, et al.

540 MEG and EEG data analysis with MNE-Python. Front Neurosci 2013;7:267.

541 doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00267.

542 [35] Makeig S, Debener S, Onton J, Delorme A. Mining event-related brain dynamics.

543 Trends Cogn Sci 2004;8:204–10. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.03.008.

544 [36] Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O, Delpuech C, Pernier J. Stimulus specificity of

545 phase-locked and non-phase-locked 40 Hz visual responses in human. J Neurosci

546 1996;16:4240–9. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04240.1996.

547 [37] Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial

548 EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods

549 2004;134:9–21. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009.

550 [38] Smulders FTY, ten Oever S, Donkers FCL, Quaedflieg CWEM, van de Ven V.

551 Single-trial log transformation is optimal in frequency analysis of resting EEG alpha.

552 Eur J Neurosci 2018;48:2585–98. doi:10.1111/ejn.13854.

553 [39] Plant GT, Zimmern RL, Durden K. Transient visually evoked potentials to the

554 pattern reversal and onset of sinusoidal gratings. Electroencephalogr Clin


Nakazono et al. - 32

555 Neurophysiol 1983;56:147–58. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(83)90069-X.

556 [40] Foster K, Gaska J, Nagler M, Pollen D. Spatial and temporal frequency selectivity of

557 neurones in visual cortical areas V1 and V2 of the macaque monkey. J Physiol

558 1985;365:331–63.

559 [41] Becker R, Ritter P, Villringer A. Influence of ongoing alpha rhythm on the visual

560 evoked potential. Neuroimage 2008;39:707–16.

561 doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.016.

562 [42] Klimesch W, Schack B, Schabus M, Doppelmayr M, Gruber W, Sauseng P.

563 Phase-locked alpha and theta oscillations generate the P1-N1 complex and are

564 related to memory performance. Cogn Brain Res 2004;19:302–16.

565 doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.11.016.

566 [43] Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Gruber WR, Hanslmayr S, Freunberger R, Doppelmayr M.

567 Are event-related potential components generated by phase resetting of brain

568 oscillations? A critical discussion. Neuroscience 2007;146:1435–44.

569 doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.03.014.

570 [44] Min BK, Busch NA, Debener S, Kranczioch C, Hanslmayr S, Engel AK, et al. The

571 best of both worlds: Phase-reset of human EEG alpha activity and additive power

572 contribute to ERP generation. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;65:58–68.

573 doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.03.002.
Nakazono et al. - 33

574 [45] Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha oscillations: The

575 inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev 2007;53:63–88.

576 doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003.

577 [46] Jensen O, Mazaheri A. Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity:

578 gating by inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci 2010;4:186.

579 doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186.

580 [47] Hanslmayr S, Aslan A, Staudigl T, Klimesch W, Herrmann CS, Bäuml KH.

581 Prestimulus oscillations predict visual perception performance between and within

582 subjects. Neuroimage 2007;37:1465–73. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.011.

583 [48] van Dijk H, Schoffelen J-M, Oostenveld R, Jensen O. Prestimulus oscillatory

584 activity in the alpha band predicts visual discrimination ability. J Neurosci

585 2008;28:1816–23. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1853-07.2008.

586 [49] Palva S, Palva JM. New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations. Trends Neurosci

587 2007;30:150–8. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001.

588 [50] Mo J, Schroeder CE, Ding M. Attentional modulation of alpha oscillations in

589 macaque inferotemporal cortex. J Neurosci 2011;31:878–82.

590 doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5295-10.2011.

591 [51] Mayer A, Schwiedrzik CM, Wibral M, Singer W, Melloni L. Expecting to see a

592 letter: Alpha oscillations as carriers of top-down sensory predictions. Cereb Cortex
Nakazono et al. - 34

593 2016;26:3146–60. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv146.

594 [52] Palva S, Palva JM. Functional roles of alpha-band phase synchronization in local and

595 large-scale cortical networks. Front Psychol 2011;2:204.

596 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00204.

597 [53] Sadaghiani S, Kleinschmidt A. Brain Networks and / -Oscillations : Structural and

598 Functional Foundations of Cognitive Control. Trends Cogn Sci 2016;20:1–13.

599 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.09.004.

600 [54] Alagapan S, Schmidt SL, Lefebvre J, Hadar E, Shin HW, Frӧhlich F. Modulation of

601 Cortical Oscillations by Low-Frequency Direct Cortical Stimulation Is

602 State-Dependent. PLoS Biol 2016;14:e1002424. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002424.

603 [55] Herrmann CS, Murray MM, Ionta S, Hutt A, Lefebvre J. Shaping Intrinsic Neural

604 Oscillations with Periodic Stimulation. J Neurosci 2016;36:5328–37.

605 doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0236-16.2016.

606 [56] Mehta AR, Brittain J-S, Brown P. The selective influence of rhythmic cortical versus

607 cerebellar transcranial stimulation on human physiological tremor. J Neurosci

608 2014;34:7501–8. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0510-14.2014.

609 [57] Veniero D, Vossen A, Gross J, Thut G. Lasting EEG/MEG Aftereffects of Rhythmic

610 Transcranial Brain Stimulation: Level of Control Over Oscillatory Network Activity.

611 Front Cell Neurosci 2015;9:477. doi:10.3389/fncel.2015.00477.


Nakazono et al. - 35

612 [58] Haegens S, Cousijn H, Wallis G, Harrison PJ, Nobre AC. Inter- and intra-individual

613 variability in alpha peak frequency. Neuroimage 2014;92:46–55.

614 doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.049.

615 [59] Turi Z, Ambrus GG, Janacsek K, Emmert K, Hahn L, Paulus W, et al. Both the

616 cutaneous sensation and phosphene perception are modulated in a frequency-specific

617 manner during transcranial alternating current stimulation. Restor Neurol Neurosci

618 2013;31:275–85. doi:10.3233/RNN-120297.

619 [60] Moliadze V, Atalay D, Antal A, Paulus W. Close to threshold transcranial electrical

620 stimulation preferentially activates inhibitory networks before switching to excitation

621 with higher intensities. Brain Stimul 2012;5:505–11. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2011.11.004.

622
Nakazono et al. - 36

623 Figure legends

624

625 Figure 1.

626 Experimental design. (A) Time course and stimuli in Experiment 1. The visual stimuli for

627 the pattern-reversal VEPs (PR-VEPs) and focal-flash VEPs (FF-VEPs) were presented in

628 the lower hemi-field, and subtended 6° in radius. Both types of VEPs were recorded before

629 (pre-tACS) and after (post-tACS) tACS. VEPs were measured for up to 30 min after tACS.

630 (B) Time course and stimuli in Experiment 2. (C) Time course and stimuli in Experiment 3.

631 Measurements were performed before, immediately after, and 10 min after tACS in

632 Experiments 2 and 3.

633

634 Figure 2.

635 The after-effects of tACS on FF-and PR-VEPs in Experiment 1. (A) The waveforms of PR-

636 VEPs (upper) and FF-VEPs (lower) in a representative subject pre-tACS. Waveforms of

637 PR-VEPs consisted of N75, P100 and N145 components, while those of FF-VEPs consisted

638 of N1, P1 and N2 components. (B) Time courses of the PR-VEP amplitudes (upper) and

639 FF-VEP amplitudes (lower) following α or β tACS. VEP amplitudes in post-tACS were

640 normalized to those in pre-tACS. α tACS increased only N75-P100 amplitudes compared

641 with β tACS (upper left). Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. ** p < 0.001.
Nakazono et al. - 37

642 Figure 3.

643 The after-effects of tACS on PR-VEP amplitudes in Experiment 2. Time course of the

644 normalized N75-P100 amplitudes after α tACS, β tACS and sham is shown. α tACS

645 enhanced N75-P100 amplitudes of PR-VEP compared with β tACS or sham (α tACS vs. β

646 tACS: p = 0.085; α tACS vs. sham: p = 0.017; β tACS vs. sham: p = 1.0, corrected).

647 * p < 0.05.

648

649 Figure 4.

650 The after-effects of tACS on event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) (A) and inter-trial

651 coherence (ITC) (B) in the α (10 ± 1 Hz) and β (20 ± 1 Hz) ranges. ∆ ERSP and ∆ ITC

652 were calculated by subtracting ERSP or ITC of the pre-tACS from those of the post-tACS.

653 (A) β tACS decreased the α ERSP compared with α tACS (α tACS vs. β tACS: p = 0.017;

654 α tACS vs. sham: p = 0.462; β tACS vs. sham: p = 0.496, corrected), but there were no

655 significant differences between tACS and sham in the α and β ERSP (α range (left); 10 ± 1

656 Hz, β range (right); 20 ± 1 Hz). (B) α tACS increased the α ITC compared with β tACS

657 and sham (α tACS vs. β tACS: p < 0.001; α tACS vs. sham: p = 0.048; β tACS vs. sham: p

658 = 0.664, corrected). Meanwhile, α tACS increased the β ITC compared with β tACS,

659 although there were no significant differences between tACS and sham (α tACS vs. β

660 tACS: p = 0.011; α tACS vs. sham: p = 0.342; β tACS vs. sham: p = 0.513, corrected).
Nakazono et al. - 38

661 * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.001.

662

663 Figure 5.

664 The after-effects of tACS on resting power at Pz. (A) Grand-averaged resting EEG activity

665 for tACS sessions (left; α tACS, middle; β tACS, right; sham) in pre- and post-tACS.

666 Shaded areas indicate the standard error of the mean. Red dashed lines indicate the α (10 ±

667 1 Hz) and β (20 ± 1 Hz) ranges. (B) The after-effects of tACS on resting EEG (α range

668 (left); 10 ± 1 Hz, β range (right); 20 ± 1 Hz). ∆ log10 power was calculated by subtracting

669 log10 power of the pre-tACS from those of the post-tACS (Δlog10 power = log10 power

670 (post) − log10 power (pre)). α and β tACS increased resting α activity compared with sham

671 (α tACS vs. β tACS: p = 1.0; α tACS vs. sham: p = 0.029; β tACS vs. sham: p = 0.002,

672 corrected), while β tACS also increased β power compared with α tACS or sham (10 Hz

673 tACS vs. 20 Hz tACS: p = 0.025; 10 Hz tACS vs. sham: p = 1.0; 20 Hz tACS vs. sham: p =

674 0.044, corrected). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.

675

676 Figure 6.

677 Correlations between the resting α power at the Oz’ (left) and Pz (right) electrodes and

678 N75-P100 amplitudes of PR-VEPs. The normalized data on resting α power and N75-P100

679 amplitudes for the two post-tACS times (i.e., post 0, 10 min) were collapsed for each
Nakazono et al. - 39

680 subject and each electrode, to calculate the ∆ resting α power and N75-P100 ratio,

681 respectively. Each circle represents the after-effects of resting α power at Oz’ or Pz vs.

682 N75-P100 amplitudes in the α tACS, β tACS or sham conditions. In both electrodes, a

683 positive correlation was found in the α tACS condition but no such relationship was

684 obtained in the β tACS and sham conditions (α tACS, Pz: r = 0.523, p = 0.031; Oz’: r =

685 0.533, p = 0.028; other tACS conditions, p ≥ 0.236, Pearson correlation). Lines are the

686 regression lines for each correlation. * p < 0.05.

687

688 Figure 7.

689 The after-effects of tACS on contrast sensitivity. These figures show the mean contrast

690 sensitivity values at post-tACS normalized by the ones at pre-tACS (0.5 cpd: left; 3 cpd:

691 middle; 9 cpd: right). α tACS improved the contrast sensitivity for 0.5–9 cpd stimuli,

692 compared with sham, whereas β tACS only improved contrast sensitivity for the 3 cpd

693 stimulus. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.001.


Table 1. Results of a linear mixed-effects model for the three experiments

tACS Time Interaction


F [df, error] p F [df, error] p F [df, error] p

Experiment 1 (n=13)
PR-VEP (N75-P100) 12.878 [1,84] <0.001 0.077 [3,84] 0.972 0.227 [3,84] 0.877
(P100-N145) 0.388 [1,84] 0.535 0.323 [3,84] 0.809 0.213 [3,84] 0.887
FF-VEP (N1-P1) 1.976 [1,84] 0.163 0.303 [3,84] 0.823 0.867 [3,84] 0.462
(P1-N2) 0.096 [1,84] 0.758 0.344 [3,84] 0.794 0.386 [3,84] 0.764
Experiment 2 (n=17)
PR-VEP (N75-P100) 4.487 [2,80] 0.014 0.407 [1,80] 0.525 0.664 [2,80] 0.518
α ERSP 4.033 [2,80] 0.021 0.108 [1,80] 0.743 0.743 [2,80] 0.483
β ERSP 0.366 [2,80] 0.694 0.806 [1,80] 0.372 1.128 [2,80] 0.329
α ITC 7.073 [2,80] <0.001 0.212 [1,80] 0.647 0.193 [2,80] 0.825
β ITC 4.445 [2,80] 0.015 0.086 [1,80] 0.77 0.322 [2,80] 0.726
resting α power 6.901 [2,80] 0.002 6.478 [1,80] 0.013 0.101 [2,80] 0.904
resting β power 4.535 [2,80] 0.014 0.002 [1,80] 0.969 0.041 [2,80] 0.959
Experiment 3 (n=15)
0.5 cpd 3.544 [2,873.019] 0.029 0.15 [1,873.013] 0.699 1.545 [2,873.026] 0.214
3 cpd 7.931 [2,875.013] <0.001 0.988 [1,875.011] 0.32 0.111 [2,875.016] 0.895
9 cpd 5.544 [2,857.069] 0.004 0.678 [1,856.992] 0.411 1.538 [2,856.999] 0.215

The bold font indicates that the differences reached statistical significance.
PR-VEP: pattern-reversal VEP; FF-VEP: focal-flash VEP; ERSP: event-related spectral
perturbation; ITC: inter-trial coherence
Highlights

We studied whether α and β tACS can differentially influence the occipital cortex.

Effects of α and β tACS on VEPs, resting EEG and contrast sensitivity were

assessed.

α tACS but not β tACS was effective for sharpening multiple visual functions.

Effects of tACS on visual cortex can be a frequency-dependent manner.

You might also like