You are on page 1of 19

Archives of Suicide Research, 18:295–312, 2014

Copyright # International Academy for Suicide Research


ISSN: 1381-1118 print=1543-6136 online
DOI: 10.1080/13811118.2013.824825

Psychological Models of
Suicide
Shira Barzilay and Alan Apter

Suicidal behavior is highly complex and multifaceted. Consequent to the pioneering


work of Durkheim and Freud, theoreticians have attempted to explain the biological,
social, and psychological nature of suicide. The present work presents an overview and
critical discussion of the most influential theoretical models of the psychological
mechanisms underlying the development of suicidal behavior. All have been tested
to varying degrees and have important implications for the development of therapeutic
and preventive interventions. Broader and more in-depth approaches are still needed
to further our understanding of suicidal phenomena.

Keywords NSSI, self-destructive behavior, self-harm, self-mutilation, suicide

There is nothing more practical than a good contextual=background factors interact


theory with proximal triggers to predict the emerg-
K. Lewin (1951, p. 169) ence of suicidal ideation and behavior.
The aim of the present work was to
review and critically discuss the predominant
psychological theories of suicidal phenom-
INTRODUCTION ena which we believe have made important
contributions to the field. We focused
Despite the substantial research on suicidal mainly on contemporary models based on
behavior and its correlates, there have been empirical research that address the psycho-
few attempts to fit the established data into logical mechanisms underlying the develop-
a coherent overarching theory. Suicidal ment of suicidal behavior. All have been
behaviors are not isolated acts but a spec- tested and found to have varying degrees
trum of activities related to a diversity of of empirical support (see Table 1). Because
risk factors. Thus, to form a useful theory the classical psychodynamic theories have
of suicide, researchers need to go beyond received substantial attention over the years,
associations of suicide behaviors and focus they are summarized only briefly.
on causal mechanisms (Brent, 2011; Nock,
2009). As noted by Prinstein (2008), a Classical Psychodynamic Theories
sound theoretical model of suicide has to
meet several criteria: It must be consistent Freud’s first observations on
with the empirically documented risk fac- self-destruction led to psychoanalytic the-
tors and be able to account for them; it ories and studies that have influenced the
must comprehensively address the interplay exploration of suicide. In his seminal work,
between intra-individual dynamic systems Beyond the Pleasure Principle, Freud (1920)
and inter-individual risk factors; and it must conceptualized the internal forces that pro-
offer insight into the processes whereby mote self-destruction. He postulated that

295
Psychological Models of Suicide

TABLE 1. Empirical Psychological Models of Suicidal Behavior

Author Main assumptions

Baumeister (1990) Unbearable state of distorted self-awareness causes cognitive


deconstruction and a search for a means to escape.
Beck, Brown, Berchick et al. (1990) Suicide schema of biases in attention, information processing, and
memory impairs the individual’s ability to recall reasons for living or
being hopeful about life.
Brent & Mann (2006) Familial transmission of the impulsive-aggressive trait places individuals
at higher risk of suicidal behavior.
Joiner (2005) Feelings of non-belongingness and burdensomeness lead to a desire for
death. The realization of this desire is determined by one’s acquired
capability for self-harm.
O’Connor (2011) Suicidal behavior results from the interplay among background factors,
motivational factors, and volitional (i.e., behavioral enactment)
factors.
Plutchik, van Praag, & Conte (1989) Certain risk factors determine the level of impulsive aggression that
leads to violent act, while the presence of other factors determines if
aggression is directed inward or outward.
Rudd (2006) Cognitive, behavioral, physiological, and affective characteristics of the
suicide mode predict vulnerability to triggers and the severity and
duration of suicidal crises.
Schotte & Clum (1987) A deficit in interpersonal problem-solving increases the risk of suicidal
behavior as a reaction to stress.
Shneidman (1993) Unmet psychological needs cause torturing mental pain that leads to
suicide as the only option.
Williams (1997) Suicidal ideation develops from feelings of entrapment due to defeat in
stressful situations.

the life and death drives were opposing Elaborating on Freud’s death instinct,
basic instincts. The life drive was directed Menninger (1938) claimed that every sui-
at reducing the tension associated with sur- cide is an inverted homicide, or ‘‘murder
vival needs; the death drive was directed at in the 180th degree.’’ He conceptualized a
eliminating the tension of life itself. Freud suicidal triad consisting of the wish to kill
argued that the universal goal of all organ- (murder), the wish to be killed (guilt), and
isms is to return to the quiescence of the the wish to die (depression). The wish to
inorganic world and that repetition compul- kill is originally oriented to an external
sion forms the basis of all self-defeating object and later introjected into the ego,
behaviors. He believed that the energy to kill leading to feelings of guilt for wishing
oneself derived from an earlier repressed loved ones dead. As one’s ego is destroyed
desire to destroy another. Suicide repre- by self-hate and guilt, a depressed, hopeless
sented an internalization of this object and wish to die evolves, and the wish to
a turning of the external death wish inward, be killed as punishment for thoughts of
against a fragment of one’s own ego. destroying others. Menninger (1938) linked

296 VOLUME 18  NUMBER 4  2014


S. Barzilay and A. Apter

suicide and self-harm to castrating or Vitanliano, 1989; Rustein & Goldberg,


mutilating fantasies directed toward one’s 1973; Tatman, Greene, & Karr, 1993).
parents and siblings. However in general the major short-
Klein (1935, 1946) further emphasized coming of psychodynamic theories is that
the role of the death instinct, asserting that they are mostly unsuitable for empirical
primitive envy in early life represents a testing. Other criticisms of psychodynamic
severe form of innate aggression. In the approaches center on the notion of self-
view of Klein’s theory, suicide is caused destructiveness as an integral biological
by unbearable guilt over aggressive fanta- need of the human personality. Baumeister
sies towards internalized objects. The guilt and Scher (1988) studied the concept of
causes feelings of badness and destructive- primary self-destruction, defined as the
ness. Suicide is therefore an attempt to pre- intention of harming or defeating the self
vent one’s own destructiveness. This that may reflect a desire to be punished
psychodynamic theory was the first to or a desire to harm a disliked self. They
emphasize the role of object-relations in found no empirical evidence for primary
the suicidal process. self-destruction in humans. This suggests
Early psychoanalytical approaches to that only distorted psychological conditions
understanding the suicidal mind were could result in suicide.
intuitively inclusive and meaningful. They
created a historical change of how suicide
Psychological Pain Theories
was perceived, from moral, legal, philo-
sophical, or spiritual problem to a clinical
concern (Ellis, 2001). Their innovative view Shneidman’s Theory of Psychache. Shneidman
of suicide as a result of psychological dis- (1993) defined ten common factors (‘‘com-
tress or disorder set the ground for all munalities’’) of suicide: seeking a solution,
further investigations. Some ideas of these cessation of consciousness, intolerable
‘‘grand theories’’ have remained influential. psychological pain, frustrated psychological
For example, the primary role of impulsive- needs, hopelessness and helplessness,
aggressive in empirical psychological mod- ambivalence towards life and death, con-
els of suicide could be seen as rooted in striction of viable alternatives, flight from
the Freud and Menninger perspectives. life itself, communication of intent, and
Furthermore, the interpersonal models of dysfunctional lifelong coping patterns. He
suicide seem to share common elements suggested that suicide is caused by psychache
with object relations theories linking suici- (Shneidman, 1993, p. 51), an intense and
dal wishes to feeling of guilt. intolerable emotional pain that is different
There has been some effort to show from depression and hopelessness. The
empirical evidence for psychodynamic for- tortured individual seeks relief for his=her
mulations of suicide (Fowler, Hilsenroth, pain until there is no solution but death.
Groat et al., 2012; Maltsberger, 2004). He postulated two types of needs: primary
Orbach (1996, 2003) demonstrated the or biological and secondary or psychologi-
relationship between early negative attach- cal. The psychological needs that are essen-
ment and development of suicidal behavior tial for life include, among others, love and
as a result of negative attitudes toward the belonging, sense of control, positive self-
body, which was conceptualized as ‘‘the image, and meaningful relationships. It is
suicidal body.’’ Other studies supported the frustration of these needs by failures,
the conceptualization of suicide as rejections, and loss that leads to the devel-
aggression turned inward. (Apter, Plutchik, opment of psychache. The view of the sui-
& van Praag, 1993; Maiuro, O’Sullivan, & cidal act as being instrumental in fulfilling a

ARCHIVES OF SUICIDE RESEARCH 297


Psychological Models of Suicide

specific need is rooted in psychodynamic psychache is sufficient was not consistently


formulations. In psychodynamic theories supported. The majority of people who dis-
self-destruction may also be intended to play psychological pain do not experience
achieve a goal or fulfill a need such as suicidal ideation or exhibit suicidal beha-
joining a loved one (in fantasy), enhancing vior. Therefore there are questions remain-
self-worth, or gaining love. ing about how psychache interacts with
Shneidman’s concepts form the basis other risk factors to bring about suicide
of many contemporary suicide models, and what pathways lead from psychache
and they have directly or indirectly shaped to suicide.
many of the terms used in the field (Jobes
& Nelson, 2006). Shneidman’s conceptuali-
zation of suicide as a problem-solving Suicide as an Escape from the Self
behavior (Shneidman, 1993) influenced
other psychological theories of suicide, Baumeister (1990) argued that the
such as the escape theory (Baumeister, most common motive reported by people
1990) and the cry of pain model (Williams, for engaging in suicidal behavior is to
1997). Initial support for psychache as a escape from an aversive situation and to
factor in suicide is strong. Psychache has obtain respite from an unbearable state of
been shown to be a significant predictor mind. He described six steps leading to
of suicide ideation and attempts (Flynn & suicidality. First, there is a discrepancy
Holden, 2007; Holden, Mehta, Cunningham between expected standards and perceived
et al., 2001; Mills, Green, & Reddon, 2005; reality. Too-high expectations or setbacks
Patterson & Holden, 2012; Pereira, Kroner, frustrate goals and lead to personal failure.
Holden et al., 2010; Troister & Holden, Second, the individual interprets the failure
2010a; Troister & Holden, 2010b). Research as a function of his own characteristics,
has also supported Shneidman’s assertion qualities, or skills, leading to self-blame.
that the relationship of other factors with Third, an aversive state of distorted
suicide is mediated by psychache, and that self-awareness leads to unforgiving com-
psychache and suicide ideation co-vary over parison of the self with unachieved stan-
time (DeLisle & Holden, 2009; Troister & dards. Fourth, this self-awareness evokes
Holden, 2012). painful negative emotions. Fifth, the indi-
However, research supporting Shneid- vidual attempts to escape into a relatively
man’s theory is limited. First, a generally numb state of cognitive deconstruction,
accepted understanding and clear definition characterized by his=her focusing on con-
of what is constituted by psychological pain crete sensations and movements and tar-
does not exist. It is not clear what cogni- geting only immediate goals. Finally
tions and emotions characterize psychache. cognitive deconstruction results in reduced
Second, there are extremely strong correla- behavioral inhibition with emergence of
tions among psychache, depression, and suicide and other life-threatening beha-
hopelessness (approximately r ¼ .80 in viors. This step separates suicidal ideation
Troister & Holden, 2012). Therefore it from actual suicide, with the behavioral dis-
can be argued that these constructs are inhibition overcoming the fear of causing
not independent. Third, to further establish oneself pain through death.
psychache as a causal factor of suicidal Baumeister’s theory largely rejects the
outcomes, there is a need for research with concept that suicide is a sort of self-
larger sample sizes, high-risk suicidal groups execution and postulates that the appeal of
and longitudinal designs. Finally, from suicide is loss of consciousness and cessation
the theoretical perspective, the claim that of psychological pain. Another unique

298 VOLUME 18  NUMBER 4  2014


S. Barzilay and A. Apter

assumption is that negative suicidal Williams (1997) posited that suicide is a


emotions are experienced as an acute state product of feelings of defeat in response
rather than a prolonged one. Baumeister to humiliation or rejection which trigger
draws upon Baechler’s early model of perceptions of entrapment, combined with
suicide-as-escape (Baechler, 1980) and it is a failure to find alternative ways to solve
consistent with Shneidman’s conceptualiza- the problem. This model draws upon the
tion of suicide as an escape from mental concept of arrested flight reported in the ani-
pain. In addition, his concept of mal behavior literature and which has been
self-destructive behaviors that are rooted in suggested to account for depression in
cognitive distortion shares elements with humans (Gilbert & Allan, 1998). Williams
Shneidman’s ideas about faulty logic and and Pollock (2000, 2001) suggested that
cognitive dichotomies in the mind of the when individuals perceive their attempts
suicidal person (Shneidman, 1980). at solving problems to be unsuccessful,
A variety of indirect evidence provides they feel powerless to escape from the
support for the escape theory (Dean & Range, situation. The sense that the future holds
1999; Hunter & O’Connor, 2003; O’Connor little opportunity for reprieve leads to
& O’Connor, 2003; Tassava & Ruderman, hopelessness. However, rescue factors
1999). In particular, different observations can moderate the relationship between
tend to corroborate the view that a major entrapment and suicide and thereby reduce
disappointment in one’s self plays a major role suicide risk.
in precipitating suicidal behavior. Baumeister Overall, Williams’s model views suicidal
and his colleagues (DeWall, Baumeister, & behavior as a cry of pain rather than a cry for
Vohs, 2008; DeWall, Twenge, Koole et al., help in response to an intolerable emotional
2011; Twenge, Catanese, & Baumeister, or situational state. The model is important
2002, 2003) as well as other investigators because it integrates psychobiological and
(Chatard & Selimbegović, 2011) also found evolutionary factors, it emphasizes the
evidence for these notions. However, direct potential interactions between emotions
evidence for the theory is rather sparse and and cognitions in the road to suicide, and
mostly tested in non-clinical populations. highlights the role of entrapment and hope-
The theory lacks specific assumptions and lessness in the development of suicidal
support regarding when suicidal thoughts ideation and behavior (For an extensive
emerge in the six-stage model. It also does discussion of this model, see Johnson,
not deal with other motivations for escape Gooding, & Tarrier, 2008.) The model has
and impaired escape strategies (such as faulty been supported by studies that demon-
problem solving) or with cognitive and strated the importance of defeat and entrap-
emotional predispositions that underlie the ment (Gilbert & Allan, 1998; O’Connor,
transition from one step to the next and 2003; Rasmussen, Fraser, Gotz et al., 2010;
culminate in suicide. Nonetheless, the escape Rooke & Birchwood, 1998; Taylor, Wood
theory is a prominent theory of suicidal et al., 2010). Many studies have related poor
behavior (O’Connor, 2003), and influenced problem solving to suicide (see Pollock &
other theoreticians to offer elaborations of Williams, 1998, for a review). Williams and
the theory, including Williams (1997) and Pollock described it as over-general autobio-
O’Connor (2011). graphical memory, where a person faced
with a problem fails to access his past in
Arrested Flight Model (Cry of Pain) order to generate possible solutions. The
role of autobiographical memory in suicidal
Extending Baumeister’s definition of behavior was supported in several studies
suicide as a desire to escape from the self, (Arie, Apter, Orbach et al., 2008; Evans,

ARCHIVES OF SUICIDE RESEARCH 299


Psychological Models of Suicide

Williams, O’Loughlin et al., 1992; Pollock & associated with psychiatric disturbance,
Williams, 2001; Sinclair, Crane, Hawton et al., and cognitive processes associated with sui-
2007; Williams, Barnhofer, Crane et al., cidal acts. Heightened vulnerability induced
2005). Although suicide has been related to by one’s disposition includes five main fac-
some specific components of Williams’ tors that have been supported by empirical
theory, these components were not tested literature (impulsivity and aggression, prob-
simultaneously in one sample. In addition, lem solving deficits, an over-general mem-
the theoretical model is limited by the ory style, a trait-like maladaptive cognitive
difficulty in separating the constructs of style, and other personality variables, parti-
hopelessness, depression, defeat, entrap- cularly perfectionism and neuroticism).
ment, and suicidal ideation. For example, Cognitive theory suggested that vulner-
several studies support the notion that defeat ability factors interact with life stressors
and entrapment constitute a single construct and increase the likelihood of schemas
(Johnson, Tarrier, & Gooding, 2008; associated with psychiatric disturbance.
Taylor, Wood, Gooding et al., 2009, Taylor, These schemas facilitate biased information
Gooding et al., 2010). Although research processing that underlies suicide. However,
points to a strong relationship between suicidal individuals are also characterized
suicidal behavior, hopelessness, and lack of by suicide schemas that are specific to sui-
social support or rescue, it is not clear how cidal behavior. In this cognitive model
they relate to each other, and no single there are two types of suicide schemas.
pathway can be inferred from the model Non-impulsive attempts are those charac-
(Johnson, Gooding, & Tarrier, 2008). terized by chronic hopelessness, and impul-
sive attempts are those characterized by
perceptions of unbearability (Fawcett,
COGNITIVE THEORIES Busch, Jacobs et al., 1997).
The theories of Beck have had a sig-
The Comprehensive Cognitive Model nificant influence on the current under-
standing of suicidal behavior. Many of
Beck, Brown, Berchick et al. (1990) the constructs included in the model have
emphasized the cognitive aspect of suicid- an empirical basis (for extensive review
ality. They suggested that hopelessness see Wenzel, Brown, & Beck, 2009). Studies
plays a major role in suicide by disrupting that assessed suicide-specific attention bias
all components of the classic cognitive triad among suicide attempters have used
of beliefs about self, others, and the future. cross-sectional designs (Becker, Strohbach,
In a more recent study, Wenzel, Brown, & & Rinck, 1999; Keilp, Gorlyn, Oquendo
Beck (2009) introduced the concept of et al., 2008; Malloy-Diniz, Neves, Abrantes
maladaptive schemas which consist of biases et al., 2009; Williams & Broadbent, 1986).
in attention, information processing, and There was only one recent prospective
memory. Suicide-relevant attention biases design that showed that attention bias pre-
result in selective processing of suicide- cedes suicide attempt (Cha, Najmi, Park
relevant stimuli. Memory biases impair the et al., 2011). These findings suggested that
ability of the suicidal individual to recall suicide-specific attention bias accelerates
reasons for living or being hopeful about the likelihood of suicide attempt. However,
his=her life. Wenzel and Beck (2008) for- there is still a need to test other aspects of
mulated a comprehensive model of suicide this theory and to empirically support the
wherein the interaction among three main mechanisms in which the factors are inter-
constructs lead to suicidal act: dispositional acted and mediated to increase suicidal
vulnerability factors, cognitive processes risk.

300 VOLUME 18  NUMBER 4  2014


S. Barzilay and A. Apter

Fluid Vulnerability Theory slightest provocation will suffer recurrent


crises, with each event increasing their
To explain the process of suicide risk, vulnerability for future crises. Indirect evi-
Rudd (2006) proposed the fluid vulner- dence was found for some dispositional
ability theory. Beck (1996) used the term traits. Cognitive vulnerabilities such as rumi-
mode to describe the cognitive processes nation (Surrence, Miranda, Marroquı́n et al.,
whereby individuals interpret external and 2009) and cognitive inflexibility (Miranda,
internal stimuli. Modes are structural net- Gallagher, Bauchner et al., 2012) were found
works of cognitive, affective, motivational, to predict suicidal ideation. Other character-
physiological, and behavioral schemas that istics have distinguished multiple attempters
are activated simultaneously by relevant from single attempters (Miranda, Scott,
internal and external events. Repeated acti- Hicks et al., 2008). It was suggested that this
vation of a specific mode lowers the model is very helpful in the assessment of
threshold for its subsequent activation, suicidal patients by taking into account not
causing a unique vulnerability. Drawing only acute risk factors (e.g., current suicidal
on Beck’s work, Rudd, Rajab, and Dahm ideation) but also taking to account chronic
(2001) suggested a suicidal mode, defined as risk. However, the potential precipitants of
combination of a suicidal belief system, suicide are unclear, as is the manner in which
physiological-affective symptoms, and personality and genetic factors interact to
associated behaviors and motivations. The determine baseline levels of vulnerability.
fluid vulnerability theory is based on the To our knowledge, there has been no direct
assumption that suicidal episodes are examination of the theory including specific
time-limited and the factors that trigger assumptions for the cognitive vulnerabilities
the episode and contribute to its severity that increase suicidal risk.
and duration are fluid in nature. Rudd
believed that every individual has a baseline
vulnerability to suicide, which is determined DIATHESIS-STRESS THEORIES
by a combination of cognitive susceptibility
(e.g., attention bias, over-general memory), The diathesis-stress models postulate that
biological susceptibility (e.g., physiological suicidal behavior occurs as a consequence
and affective symptoms) and behavioral of the interaction between predisposing
susceptibility (e.g., deficient skills in inter- vulnerability factors and a triggering stress
personal or regulation domains). Variations factor. Different theoreticians suggest dif-
in vulnerability account for the emergence ferent contexts for the vulnerability to sui-
and chronicity of suicidality. The acute risk cide and the stress involved in suicidal
is heightened in the presence of aggravating behavior.
risk factors. Core cognitive themes that acti-
vate the suicide mode are a belief of being Social Problem-Solving Vulnerability
unloved, a belief that one is a burden to
others, feelings of helplessness, and inability Schotte and Clum (1982, 1987) pro-
to tolerate distress, among others. The posed that cognitive rigidity in problem
constellation of themes that comprise a per- solving causes a vulnerability to emergent
son’s suicide mode may serve as predictors hopelessness and suicidal ideation under
of that person’s vulnerability to a suicidal naturally occurring conditions of high life
crisis, the probable triggers and duration stress. Individuals with a low capacity for
of the crisis, and the potential for future flexible divergent thinking are unable to
crises. Individuals with a sensitive suicidal identify alternative solutions to their
belief system that can be activated by the problems or tend to anticipate negative

ARCHIVES OF SUICIDE RESEARCH 301


Psychological Models of Suicide

consequences for any proposed solutions. ideation as an outcome, rather than suicide
Therefore, when they experience life stress, attempts, and use of non-clinical samples.
they tend to become more hopeless, heigh- Overall, empirical research has not con-
tening their risk for suicidal behavior. firmed the mediational role of problem
This model shares ideas with other solving deficits (e.g., Schotte & Clum,
psychological theories of suicide as the sol- 1982), and therefore Wenzel and Beck
ution for problems due to a lack of other (2008) proposed a revised conceptualiza-
options. It expands previous thinking by tion for the problem solving deficits related
highlighting interpersonal deficits, thereby to suicide. It was suggested to view
suggesting potential intervention strategies problem-solving deficits as a dispositional
focused on interpersonal problem solving. characteristic that is associated with a host
Empirical research found that relative to of adverse outcomes, including psychiatric
non-suicidal individuals, suicidal individuals disturbance and suicidal crises.
generate fewer solutions to problems
(Pollock & Williams, 2004), are less likely
to use the alternatives they generate Clinical-Biological Model of Suicidal
(Schotte & Clum, 1987), estimate a greater Behavior
likelihood of negative consequences asso-
ciated with proposed solutions (Schotte & The clinical model of Mann, Apter,
Clum, 1987), and are more likely to use Bertolote et al. (1999) and Mann,
denial or avoidance strategies in dealing Waternaux, Haas et al. (2005) suggests that
with their problems (Orbach, Bar-Joseph, psychiatric illness serves as a stressor but
& Dror, 1990). However, the problem- leads to suicide only when combined with
solving deficits observed in suicidal indivi- vulnerability. The theory is based on the
duals might be state-dependent rather than notion of impulsive aggression, or the tendency
a trait; that is, they may be a consequence to respond to provocation or frustration
of negative life stress or depression. In addi- with hostility or aggression, as a trait factor
tions, there is no one clear definition to and common correlate of suicidal behavior
‘‘problem solving’’. When problem solving (Bridge, Goldstein, & Brent, 2006). Impul-
was defined as the ability to generate mul- sive aggression is increased by low seroto-
tiple solutions to problems, it was associa- nergic activity and=or substance abuse,
ted with suicide ideation but not with potentiating the relationship between psy-
hopelessness (Schotte & Clum, 1982) and chopathology and suicidal actions. Brent
interacted with stress to predict suicide and Mann (2006) proposed that the vulner-
ideation over time (Priester & Clum, ability to suicidal behavior is often familial
1993). However, when problem solving with a likely genetic component, and impul-
was defined as confidence in one’s ability sive aggression is a key to understanding
to solve problems, it was strongly associated familial transmission. Specifically, parental
with hopelessness and moderately associa- mood disorder and=or impulsive-aggressive
ted with suicide ideation (Dixon, Heppner, traits are transmitted to the child via genetics
& Anderson, 1991; Rudd, Rajab, & Dahm, or a suboptimal family environment (e.g.,
1994). Reinecke, DuBois, and Schultz abuse and neglect). As a consequence, mood
(2001) reported that depression and hope- disorder or impulsive aggression develops
lessness mediated the link between problem in the child and may ultimately lead to
solving and suicidal ideation. Thus, it is suicidal behavior in the presence of life
important to specify the precise type of stressors.
problem solving deficit under examination. The main contribution of this model
Other limitations include focus on suicide is its elaboration of background causal

302 VOLUME 18  NUMBER 4  2014


S. Barzilay and A. Apter

pathways to the development of Two-Stage Model of Outward and Inward


stress-activated susceptibility to suicidal Directed Aggression
behavior. It is well delineated and sup-
ported by empirical findings (Mann, The two-stage model of suicide and
Arango, Avenevoli et al., 2009; Melhem, violence (Apter, Plutchik, & van Praag,
Brent, Ziegler et al., 2007). Many studies 1993; Plutchik, 1995; Plutchik, van Praag,
reported a link between impulsivity and & Conte 1989) is based on the assumption
suicide (Brodsky, Malone, Ellis et al., that suicide and violence are expressions of
1997; Kingsbury, Hawton, Steinhardt et al., the same underlying aggressive impulse,
1999; Mann, Waternaux, Haas et al., 1999; and it is the presence or absence of other
Nock & Kessler, 2006) including evidence variables that determine what the direction
that impulsive-aggressive suicide is more the aggression will take. Plutchik, van
common among young suicide attempters Praag, and Conte (1989) listed possible
and suicide completers (Brent & Mann, triggers (stressors) that generate aggressive
2005; 2006). And the genetic influence impulses, including threats, challenges,
seems to be mediated many by serotonergic insults, loss of control, and perceived threat
mechanisms (Mann, 2013). However, sev- to one’s social rank. In the first stage, the
eral studies have failed to find a significant cutoff level of impulsive aggression that will
association between impulsivity and suicidal be expressed in overt behavior or action is
behavior (Horesh, 2001; Keilp et al., 2006; determined. Impulsive aggression may be
Oquendo, Waternaux, Brodsky et al., amplified or attenuated by other factors,
2000). Recent studies indicate that although such as social support, attitudes toward
impulsivity is considered a risk factor for violence, and access to a weapon. In the
suicidal behavior, death by suicide is not second stage, the object toward which the
necessarily an impulsive act. Witte, Merrill, aggression is directed is identified. The risk
Stellrecht et al. (2008) reported that less than of aggression towards the self (suicide) is
one-fourth suicide attempters did so with- increased in the presence of depression,
out planning. It was also found that patients total life problems, and recent psychopath-
who made impulsive suicide attempts scored ology symptoms. Impulsive trait and prob-
no higher on a self-report measure of impul- lems with the law direct the aggressive
sivity than patients who had planned their impulse toward others (violence).
attempts (Baca-Garcia, Diaz-Sastre, Garcia Multiple epidemiologic, clinical, retro-
Resa et al., 2005). Klonsky and May (2010) spective, prospective, and family studies
explained these mixed findings by suggest- have identified a strong link between
ing that studies have utilized broad measures aggression and suicide (For a recent review
of impulsivity that insufficiently differentiate see Gvion & Apter, 2011). Direct examina-
aspect of impulsivity, while only a specific tions of the two-stage model provided
trait of diminished ability to think through some support (Apter, Plutchik, & van
the consequences of one’s behavior before Praag, 1993; Plutchik, 1995; Plutchik, van
acting confers risk for suicidal behavior Praag, & Conte, 1989). However, evidence
above and beyond suicidal ideation. Thus, for relationships between psychopatholo-
it is possible that impulsivity in and of itself gies and outward and inward aggression
is a peripheral construct in understanding is inconsistent (Spielberg, Reheiser, &
suicidal behavior and there is need to ident- Sydeman, 1995; Whiteside & Abramovitz,
ify more refined mechanism with more 2004). Recent studies on suicide emphasized
explanatory power to account for suicidal the need to look at more specific aspects of
behavior (for a recent review see Gvion & aggression when studying suicide attempts
Apter, 2011). (Giegling, Olgiati, Hartmann et al., 2009)

ARCHIVES OF SUICIDE RESEARCH 303


Psychological Models of Suicide

such as gender differences between trait by people with a history of self-harm and
anger, anger expression, and suicide attempts other risk behaviors. An important advan-
among adolescents and young adults tage of the IPTS is that it is the first to dis-
(Goldston, Reboussin, & Daniel, 2006). tinguish individuals who think about suicide
From the theoretical perspective, this model without acting on those thoughts from indi-
attempts to explain the impulsive-aggressive viduals who translate suicidal ideation into
type of suicidal behavior which seems to action. Joiner and his colleagues conducted
occur more in younger people and has systematic empirical studies to examine spe-
received the most attention. However, other cific components and interactions proposed
personality constellations may underlie suici- by the IPTS. Studies have found a signifi-
dal behavior, such as narcissist-perfectionist cant effect of thwarted belongingness and
type and hopelessness-depressive type perceived burdensomeness on suicidal idea-
(Apter, 2010; Apter, King, Bleich et al., tion (Conner, Britton, Sworts et al., 2007;
2008). These may well be associated with Jahn & Cukrowicz, 2011; Jahn, Cukrowicz,
different biosocial diatheses. Linton et al., 2011; Joiner, Schmidt,
Schmidt et al., 2002; Joiner, Van Orden,
Witte et al., 2009; Van Orden, Lynam, Hol-
Interpersonal Theory of Suicide (IPTS) lar et al., 2006; Van Orden, Witte, Gordon
et al., 2008; Van Orden, Witte, James et al.,
The interpersonal theory of suicide 2008). However, contrary to IPTS hypoth-
(Joiner, 2005) suggests that individuals will eses, a meta-analysis of 192 studies on social
engage in serious suicidal behavior if they rejection concluded that social rejection
have both the desire to die and the capa- neither causes immediate distress nor
bility to act on that desire (Joiner, 2005; lowers self-esteem (Blackhart, Nelson,
Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz et al., Knowles et al., 2009).
2010). The desire to die results from two In support of the hypothesis of
distinct interpersonal psychological states, acquired capability for suicidal behavior, it
namely perceived burdensomeness, or a feeling was found that a history of non-suicidal
of being a burden to others, and thwarted self-injury and suicide attempts was related
belongingness, or a feeling of alienation. to less fear about engaging in suicidal
These feelings lead to the belief that one’s behavior (Muehlenkamp & Gutierrez,
death is worthwhile to others. When the 2007; Stanley, Gameroff, Michalsen et al.,
two states converge, suicidal ideation is 2001). Direct tests supported the link
enhanced. However, this is not enough between self-harm behaviors, acquired
for lethal suicide, as the desire to die may capability, and eventually with suicide
be overcome by a sufficiently powerful attempts (Bender, Gordon, Bresin et al.,
instinct of self-preservation. This qualifi- 2011; Joiner, Van Orden, Witte et al.,
cation is in line with the behavioral disinhi- 2009; Selby, Smith, Bulik et al., 2010; Smith,
biting component of Baumeister’s theory, Ribeiro, Mikolajewski et al., 2012; Van
and shares some elements with Orbach’s Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz et al., 2010; Van
(1996) ‘‘suicidal body,’’ According to Joiner Orden, Witte, Gordon et al., 2008). How-
(2005), individuals acquire a fearlessness of ever, findings regarding increased pain
pain, injury, and death through a process of tolerance by non-suicidal self-injury showed
repeatedly experiencing pain, often by inconsistent results (Franklin, Hessel, &
self-injury. He calls this an acquired capability Prinstein, 2011; Hooley, Ho, Slater et al.,
for suicide. 2010; Nock, Joiner, Gordon et al., 2006).
The interpersonal theory offers a Therefore, although IPTS has received
possible explanation for suicide attempts increased support in recent years, there are

304 VOLUME 18  NUMBER 4  2014


S. Barzilay and A. Apter

some theoretical and empirical limitations. translated into actual suicidal behaviors.
It remains unclear why acquired capabilities The transition is determined by behavioral
do not always translate into completed sui- enaction factors, as identified in the theory
cide. In addition, the range of experiences of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991), cry of
that contribute to self-injury habituation is pain model (Williams, 1997), and the inter-
unclear, as well as their relative power personal theory of suicide (Joiner, 2005).
(Van Orden et al., 2010). The theory also Examples include access to means of
fails to account for the central role of suicide, capability to attempt suicide,
psychopathologies, such as depression, in imitation, and impulsivity.
completed suicide. Research has also been This model is important because it
limited by cross-sectional study designs builds on existing evidence and theoretical
and in testing the simultaneous effect of knowledge. It integrates aspects of differ-
all three factors of the IPTS and the ent psychological theories and provides a
interrelations among them. theoretical map of how distal risk may be
translated into suicidal behavior via proxi-
Integrated Motivational-Volitional Model mal psychological risk processes. Recent
of Suicide Behavior (IMV) tests of the IMV model provided support
for some of its assumptions (O’Connor,
In an attempt to incorporate the major O’Carroll, Ryan et al., 2012; O’Connor,
components of previous theoretical Rasmussen, & Hawton, 2009, 2012).
models and empirical evidence, O’Connor Although findings are promising, the
(2011) suggested the three-phase integrated results are limited in their generalizability
motivational-volitional model of suicidal and there is a need to further test the
behavior. The model emphasizes the inter- IMV model in other samples and settings.
play between factors associated with the Moreover, there is some conceptual con-
development of suicidal ideation and the fusion. For example, the constructs of
translation of these thoughts into suicidal defeat and entrapment, may overlap. In
behavior. The premotivational phase consists addition, only several assumptions of the
of background factors (e.g., environmental model were examined and more refined
deprivation, vulnerabilities) and triggering hypotheses suggested by the model and
life events that provide the biosocial con- the specificity of the moderators of each
text for suicide, and the motivational phase step remains to be empirically tested.
consists of factors associated with the for-
mation of suicidal thoughts and intention SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
to end one’s life. The theory posits, as
adapted from the model of Williams Psychological theories of suicidal behavior
(1997), that suicidal ideation derives from have increased in number and complexity
feelings of entrapment that are triggered over the last decades and have potential
by experiences of defeat and humiliation. important implications for research and
Feelings of entrapment are exacerbated by the development of methods of suicide
specific state moderators, such as poor prevention. The early psychoanalytic
coping strategies, poor problem solving, models had a great influence in generating
and attribution biases. In the presence of interest and hypothesis about the field.
motivational moderators such as interper- They have not, however, generated much
sonal states and impaired subjective goals, empirical research with practical implica-
norms, and future positive thinking, they tions for treatment and prevention apart
lead to suicidal ideation. In the third from the pioneering efforts of Orbach
volitional phase, the suicidal thoughts are (1996). Nonetheless, there has been a recent

ARCHIVES OF SUICIDE RESEARCH 305


Psychological Models of Suicide

resurgence of interest in the empirical is a considerable conceptual communality


investigations of psychoanalytic theories across the various models. It seems that
and some evidence was found for the independent theoreticians use different
psychodynamic formulations of suicide terms for similar or overlapping ideas. It
(Fowler, Hilsenroth, Groat et al., 2012; can be argued that psychache, hopelessness,
Maltsberger, 2004). Development of con- defeat, entrapment etc. are not independent
temporary psychological models based on from each other. Therefore important
empirical research has been stimulated by advances in the field could be achieved by
the influential work of Shneidman and by integrating the disparate as well as the
exponents of the cognitive and behaviorally common perspectives and to refine them
oriented approaches. These psychological into a comprehensive unified approach.
models have provided valuable insights into Such an attempt has been recently proposed
the suicidal mind. Some models focused on by O’Connor (2011). A comprehensive
vulnerability factors that are related to sui- model should address the interplay among
cide, including impulsive-aggressive tenden- multiple contextual factors on different
cies, maladaptive cognitive styles, attention levels of analysis (e.g., physiologic, cogni-
bias, over-general memory, problem solving tive, social), state-related motivation for
deficits, and acquired capability for self- suicide both in the intra-psychic and inter-
harm. Other models focused on the situa- personal domains, and factors involved in
tional perceived stress leading an individual translation of the death wish into actions.
towards a suicidal act. Such theories center In addition to risk factors, protective factors
on mental pain, hopelessness, entrapment, need to be addressed as well (Brent, 2011).
and interpersonal distress. Few theories From the empirical perspective, most
have attempted to integrate both diathesis research has been limited by giving support
and stress related factors. for only some of the respective model’s
However, most of the models proposed components but not all. Furthermore, many
so far are limited in several ways. From the of the models that were reviewed rely on
theoretical perspective, the main short- indirect evidence and direct tests of the mod-
coming of most models is that they have a els are extremely rare. When direct evidence
narrow focus and are not sufficiently com- was offered, most studies used cross-
prehensive to account for all the correlated sectional designs and were limited to specific
factors identified in empirical studies of populations and settings. Therefore there is a
suicide behavior. It is not yet clear as to need for further research with larger sample
how the components interact with and are sizes in different populations (both com-
mediated by established risk factors to munity and high risk samples) and longitudi-
elevate or lower risk for suicidal behavior nal designs. Data analytic approaches that
in all its complexity. For example, most of will allow for evaluating more refined mech-
the existing conceptual frameworks such anism while testing the interrelations among
as those of Baumeister and Williams are pre- all the theory components simultaneously
dominantly non-developmental and fail to will be a major contribution to the existing
address background factors and causal path- knowledge. Better understanding of the
ways that increase vulnerability. Second, in causal mechanisms will lead to improved
many of the models there is no generally intervention strategies.
accepted understanding and clear definition
of the main constructs. For example, AUTHOR NOTE
there are different conceptualizations for
psychological pain, impulsivity, and Shira Barzilay and Alan Apter, Feinberg
problem-solving. At the same time, there Child Study Center, Schneider Children’s

306 VOLUME 18  NUMBER 4  2014


S. Barzilay and A. Apter

Medical Center of Israel, Petach Tikva, Becker, E. S., Strohbach, D., & Rinck, M. (1999). A
and Sackler School of Medicine, Tel Aviv specific attentional bias in suicide attempters.
University, Tel Aviv, Israel. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 730–735.
Correspondence concerning this article Bender, T. W., Gordon, K. H., Bresin, K., & Joiner,
T. E., Jr. (2011). Impulsivity and suicidality: The
should be addressed to Shira Barzilay,
mediating role of painful and provocative experi-
Feinberg Child Study Center, Schneider ences. Journal of Affective Disorders, 129, 301–307.
Children’s Medical Center of Israel, Petach Blackhart, G. C., Nelson, B. C., Knowles, M. L., &
Tikva 49202, Israel. E-mail: shirabarzilay@ Baumeister, R. F. (2009). Rejection elicits
gmail.com emotional reactions but neither causes immediate
distress nor lowers self-esteem: A meta-analytic
review of 192 studies on social exclusion. Person-
REFERENCES ality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 269–309.
Brent, D. A. (2011). Preventing youth suicide: Time
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. to ask how. Journal of American Academy of Child and
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 738–740.
50, 179–211. Brent, D. A., & Mann, J. J. (2005). Family genetic
Apter, A. (2010). Clinical aspects of suicidal behavior studies, suicide, and suicidal behavior. American
relevant to genetics. European Psychiatry, 25, 257–259. Journal of Medical Genetics: Part C, Seminars in Medical
Apter, A., King, R. A., Bleich, A., Fluck, A., Kotler, Genetics, 133C, 13–24.
M., & Kron, S. (2008). Fatal and non-fatal suicidal Brent, D. A., & Mann, J. J. (2006). Familial pathways
behavior in Israeli adolescent males. Archives of Sui- to suicidal behavior—understanding and prevent-
cide Research, 12, 20–29. ing suicide among adolescents. The New England
Apter, A., Plutchik, R., & van Praag, H. M. (1993). Journal of Medicine, 355, 2719–2721.
Anxiety, impulsivity and depressed mood in Bridge, J. A., Goldstein, T. R., & Brent, D. A. (2006).
relation to suicidal and violent behavior. Acta Psy- Adolescent suicide and suicidal behavior. J Child
chiatrica Scandinavica, 87, 1–5. Psychol Psychiatry, 47(3–4), 372–394.
Arie, M., Apter, A., Orbach, I., Yefet, Y., & Zalsman, Brodsky, B. S., Malone, K. M., Ellis, S. P., Dulit, R.
G. (2008). Autobiographical memory, interpersonal A., & Mann, J. J. (1997). Characteristics of border-
problem solving, and suicidal behavior in adolescent line personality disorder associated with suicidal
inpatients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 49, 22–29. behavior. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 154,
Baca-Garcia, E., Diaz-Sastre, C., Garcia Resa, E., 1715–1719.
Blasco, H., Braquehais Conesa, D., Oquendo, M. Cha, C. B., Najmi, S., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., &
A., . . . de Leon, J. (2005). Suicide attempts and Nock, M. K. (2010). Attentional bias toward
impulsivity. European Archives of Psychiatry and suicide-related stimuli predicts suicidal behavior.
Clinical Neuroscience, 255, 152–156. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 616–622.
Baechler, J. (1980). A strategic theory. Suicide and Chatard, A., & Selimbegovic, L. (2011). When
Life-Threatening Behavior, 10, 70–99. self-destructive thoughts flash through the mind:
Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Failure to meet standards affects the accessibility
Psychological Review, 97, 90–113. of suicide-related thoughts. Journal of Personality
Baumeister, R. F., & Scher, S. J. (1988). Self-defeating and Social Psychology, 100, 587–605.
behavior patterns among normal individuals: Conner, K. R., Britton, P. C., Sworts, L. M., &
Review and analysis of common self-destructive Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2007). Suicide attempts among
tendencies. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 3–22. individuals with opiate dependence: The critical
Beck, A. T. (1996). Beyond belief: A theory of modes, role of belonging. Addictive Behaviors, 32,
personality, and psychopathology. Frontiers of cogni- 1395–1404.
tive therapy (pp. 1–25). New York, NY: Guilford. Dean, P. J., & Range, L. M. (1999). Testing the
Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Berchick, R. J., Stewart, B. escape theory of suicide in an outpatient clinical
L., & Steer, R. A. (1990). Relationship between population. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23,
hopelessness and ultimate suicide: A replication 561–572.
with psychiatric outpatients. The American Journal DeLisle, M. M., & Holden, R. R. (2009). Differen-
of Psychiatry, 147, 190–195. tiating between depression, hopelessness, and

ARCHIVES OF SUICIDE RESEARCH 307


Psychological Models of Suicide

psychache in university undergraduates. Measure- Goldston, D. B., Reboussin, B. A., & Daniel, S. S.
ment and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, (2006). Predictors of suicide attempts: State and
42, 46–63. trait components. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. 115, 842–849.
(2008). Satiated with belongingness? Effects of Gvion, Y., & Apter, A. (2011). Aggression, impulsiv-
acceptance, rejection, and task framing on ity, and suicide behavior: A review of the litera-
self-regulatory performance. Journal of Personality ture. Archives of Suicide Research, 15, 93–112.
and Social Psychology, 95, 1367–1382. Holden, R. R., Mehta, K., Cunningham, E. J., &
DeWall, C. N., Twenge, J. M., Koole, S. L., Baumeister, McLeod, L. D. (2001). Development and prelimi-
R. F., Marquez, A., & Reid, M. W. (2011). Automatic nary validation of a scale of psychache. Canadian
emotion regulation after social exclusion: Tuning to Journal of Behavioural Science, 33, 224–232.
positivity. Emotion, 11, 623–626. Hooley, J. M., Ho, D. T., Slater, J., & Lockshin, A.
Dixon, W. A., Heppner, P. P., & Anderson, W. P. (2010). Pain perception and nonsuicidal self-
(1991). Problem-solving appraisal, stress, hope- injury: A laboratory investigation. Personality Disor-
lessness, and suicide ideation in a college popu- ders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 1, 170–179.
lation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 51–56. Horesh, N. (2001). Self-report vs. computerized
Ellis, T. (2001). Psychotherapy with suicidal patients. measures of impulsivity as a correlate of suicidal
In D. Lester (Ed.), Suicide prevention: Resources for the behavior. Crisis, 22, 27–31.
millennium (pp. 129–151). Philadelphia, PA: Hunter, E. C., & O’Connor, R. C. (2003). Hopeless-
Brunner-Routledge. ness and future thinking in parasuicide: The role
Evans, J., Williams, J. M., O’Loughlin, S., & Howells, of perfectionism. The British Journal of Clinical Psy-
K. (1992). Autobiographical memory and chology, 42, 355–365.
problem-solving strategies of parasuicide patients. Jahn, D. R., & Cukrowicz, K. C. (2011). The impact
Psychological Medicine, 22, 399–405. of the nature of relationships on perceived bur-
Fawcett, J., Busch, K. A., Jacobs, D., Kravitz, H. M., densomeness and suicide ideation in a community
& Fogg, L. (1997). Suicide: A four-pathway clini- sample of older adults. Suicide & Life-Threatening
cal-biochemical model. Annals of the New York Behavior, 41, 635–649.
Academy of Sciences, 836, 288–301. Jahn, D. R., Cukrowicz, K. C., Linton, K., & Prabhu,
Flynn, J. J., & Holden, R. R. (2007). Predictors of sui- F. (2011). The mediating effect of perceived bur-
cidality in a sample of suicide attempters. Canadian densomeness on the relation between depressive
Psychology, 48, 317. symptoms and suicide ideation in a community
Fowler, J. C., Hilsenroth, M. J., Groat, M., Biel, S., sample of older adults. Aging & Mental Health,
Biedermann, C., & Ackerman, S. (2012). Risk fac- 15, 214–220.
tors for medically serious suicide attempts: Evi- Jobes, D. A., & Nelson, K. N. (2006). Shneidman’s
dence for a psychodynamic formulation of contributions to the understanding of suicidal
suicidal crisis. Journal of American Psychoanalytic thinking. Cognition and suicide: Theory, research, and
Association, 60, 555–76. therapy (pp. 29–49). Washington, DC: American
Franklin, J. C., Hessel, E. T., & Prinstein, M. J. Psychological Association.
(2011). Clarifying the role of pain tolerance in sui- Johnson, J., Gooding, P., & Tarrier, N. (2008). Sui-
cidal capability. Psychiatry Research, 189, 362–367. cide risk in schizophrenia: Explanatory models
Freud, S. (1920). Beyond the pleasure principle. Leipzig, and clinical implications, The Schematic Appraisal
Vienna, Zurich: International Psychoanalytic Model of Suicide (SAMS). Psychology and Psycho-
Publisher. therapy, 81, 55–77.
Giegling, I., Olgiati, P., Hartmann, A. M., Calati, R., Johnson, J., Tarrier, N., & Gooding, P. (2008). An
Moller, H. J., Rujescu, D., & Serretti, A. (2009). investigation of aspects of the cry of pain model
Personality and attempted suicide. Analysis of of suicide risk: The role of defeat in impairing mem-
anger, aggression and impulsivity. Journal of Psychi- ory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 968–975.
atric Research, 43, 1262–1271. Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2005). Why people die by suicide.
Gilbert, P., & Allan, S. (1998). The role of defeat and Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
entrapment (arrested flight) in depression: An Joiner, T. E., Jr., Schmidt, N. B., Schmidt, K. L.,
exploration of an evolutionary view. Psychological Laurent, J., Catanzaro, S. J., Perez, M., & Pettit,
Medicine, 28, 585–598. J. W. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity as a specific and

308 VOLUME 18  NUMBER 4  2014


S. Barzilay and A. Apter

unique marker of anxious symptoms in youth JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association,
psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Abnormal Child 294, 2064–2074.
Psychology, 30, 167–175. Mann, J. J., Arango, V. A., Avenevoli, S., Brent, D.
Joiner, T. E., Jr., Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., A., Champagne, F. A., Clayton, P., . . . Wenzel,
Selby, E. A., Ribeiro, J. D., Lewis, R., & Rudd, A. (2009). Candidate endophenotypes for genetic
M. D. (2009). Main predictions of the interperso- studies of suicidal behavior. Biological Psychiatry,
nal–psychological theory of suicidal behavior: 65, 556–563.
Empirical tests in two samples of young adults. Mann, J. J., Waternaux, C., Haas, G. L., & Malone,
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 634–646. K. M. (1999). Toward a clinical model of suicidal
Keilp, J. G., Gorlyn, M., Oquendo, M. A., Brodsky, behavior in psychiatric patients. The American Jour-
B., Ellis, S. P., Stanley, B., & John Mann, J. nal of Psychiatry, 156, 181–189.
(2006). Aggressiveness, not impulsiveness or hos- Melhem, N. M., Brent, D. A., Ziegler, M., Iyengar, S.,
tility, distinguishes suicide attempters with major Kolko, D., Oquendo, M., . . . Mann, J. J. (2007). Fam-
depression. Psychological Medicine, 36, 1779–1788. ilial pathways to early onset suicidal behavior: Familial
Keilp, J. G., Gorlyn, M., Oquendo, M. A., Burke, A. K., and individual antecedents of suicidal behavior. The
& Mann, J. J. (2008). Attention deficit in depressed American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1364–1370.
suicide attempters. Psychiatry Research, 159, 7–17. Menninger, K. A. (1938). Man against himself. Oxford,
Kingsbury, S., Hawton, K., Steinhardt, K., & James, England: Harcourt, Brace.
A. (1999). Do adolescents who take overdoses Mills, J. F., Green, K., & Reddon, J. R. (2005). An
have specific psychological characteristics? A com- evaluation of the Psychache Scale on an offender
parative study with psychiatric and community population. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 35,
controls. Journal of the American Academy of Child 570–580.
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1125–1131. Miranda, R., Gallagher, M., Bauchner, B., Vaysman,
Klein, M. (1935). A contribution to the psychogen- R., & Marroquin, B. (2012). Cognitive inflexibility
esis of manic-depressive states. International Journal as a prospective predictor of suicidal ideation
of Psychoanalysis, 16, 145–174. among young adults with a suicide attempt history.
Klein, M. (1946). Notes on some schizoid mechan- Depression and Anxiety, 29, 180–186.
isms. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 27, 99–110. Miranda, R., Scott, M., Hicks, R., Wilcox, H. C., Harris
Klonsky, E. D., & May, A. (2010). Rethinking impul- Munfakh, J. L., & Shaffer, D. (2008). Suicide
sivity in suicide. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, attempt characteristics, diagnoses, and future
40, 612–619. attempts: Comparing multiple attempters to single
Lewin, K. (1951). In Dorwin Cartwright (Ed.), Field attempters and ideators. Journal of the American
theory in social science: Selected theoretical papers. Oxford, Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 32–40.
UK: Harpers. Muehlenkamp, J. J., & Gutierrez, P. M. (2007). Risk
Maiuro, R. D., O’Sullivan, M. C., & Vitaliano, P. P. for suicide attempts among adolescents who
(1989). Anger, hostility and depression in assaul- engage in non-suicidal self-injury. Archives of Suicide
tive vs. suicide-attempting males. Journal of Clinical Research, 11, 69–82.
Psychology, 45, 531–541. Nock, M. K. (2009). Suicidal behavior among adoles-
Malloy-Diniz, L. F., Neves, F. S., Abrantes, S. S., cents: Correlates, confounds, and (the search for)
Fuentes, D., & Correa, H. (2009). Suicide behavior causal mechanisms. Journal of the American Academy
and neuropsychological assessment of type I of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 237–239.
bipolar patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 112, Nock, M. K. (2010). Self-injury. Annual Review of
231–236. Clinical Psychology, 6, 339–363.
Maltsberger, J. t. (2004). The descent into suicide. Nock, M. K., Joiner, T. E., Jr., Gordon, K. H.,
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 85, 653–668. Lloyd-Richardson, E., & Prinstein, M. J. (2006).
Mann, J. J. (2013). The serotonergic system in mood Non-suicidal self-injury among adolescents: Diag-
disorders and suicidal behaviour. Philosophical nostic correlates and relation to suicide attempts.
Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Bio- Psychiatry Research, 144, 65–72.
logical Sciences, 368, 20120537. Nock, M. K., & Kessler, R. C. (2006). Prevalence of
Mann, J. J., Apter, A., Bertolote, J., Beautrais, A., and risk factors for suicide attempts versus suicide
Currier, D., Haas, A., . . . Hendin, H. (2005). Sui- gestures: Analysis of the National Comorbidity Sur-
cide prevention strategies: A systematic review. vey. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 616–623.

ARCHIVES OF SUICIDE RESEARCH 309


Psychological Models of Suicide

Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2004). A functional Plutchik, R., van Praag, H. M., & Conte, H. R.
approach to the assessment of self-mutilative (1989). Correlates of suicide and violence risk,
behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, III: A two-stage model of countervailing forces.
72, 885–890. Psychiatry Research, 28, 215–225.
O’Connor, R. C. (2003). Suicidal behavior as a cry of Pollock, L. R., & Williams, J. M. G. (1998). Problem
pain: Test of a psychological model. Archives of Sui- solving and suicidal behavior. Suicide and
cide Research, 7, 297–308. Life-Threatening Behavior, 28, 375–387.
O’Connor, R. C. (2011). The integrated motivational- Pollock, L. R., & Williams, J. M. G. (2001). Effective
volitional model of suicidal behavior. Crisis, 32, problem solving in suicide attempters depends on
295–298. specific autobiographical recall. Suicide and
O’Connor, R. C., O’Carroll, R. E., Ryan, C., & Life-Threatening Behavior, 31, 386–396.
Smyth, R. (2012). Self-regulation of unattainable Pollock, L. R., & Williams, J. M. (2004). Problem-
goals in suicide attempters: A two year prospective solving in suicide attempters. Psychological medicine,
study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 142, 248–255. 34, 163–167.
O’Connor, R. C., & O’Connor, D. B. (2003). Predict- Priester, M. J., & Clum, G. A. (1993). Perceived
ing hopelessness and psychological distress: The problem-solving ability as a predictor of depression,
role of perfectionism and coping. Journal of Counsel- hopelessness, and suicide ideation in a college
ing Psychology, 50, 362–372. population. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 79–85.
O’Connor, R. C., Rasmussen, S., & Hawton, K. Prinstein, M. J. (2008). Introduction to the special
(2009). Predicting deliberate self-harm in adoles- section on suicide and nonsuicidal self-injury: A
cents: A six month prospective study. Suicide and review of unique challenges and important direc-
Life-Threatening Behavior, 39, 364–375. tions for self-injury science. Journal of Consulting
O’Connor, R. C., Rasmussen, S., & Hawton, K. and Clinical Psychology, 76, 1–8.
(2012). Distinguishing adolescents who think about Rasmussen, S. A., Fraser, L., Gotz, M., MacHale, S.,
self-harm from those who engage in self-harm. The Mackie, R., Masterton, G., & O’Connor, R. C.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 200, 330–335. (2010). Elaborating the cry of pain model of sui-
Oquendo, M. A., Waternaux, C., Brodsky, B., Parsons, cidality: Testing a psychological model in a sample
B., Haas, G. L., Malone, K. M., & Mann, J. J. of first-time and repeat self-harm patients. The
(2000). Suicidal behavior in bipolar mood disorder: British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 15–30.
Clinical characteristics of attempters and nonat- Reinecke, M. A., DuBois, D. L., & Schultz, T. M.
tempters. Journal of Affective Disorders, 59, 107–117. (2001). Social problem solving, mood, and suicid-
Orbach, I. (1996). The role of the body experience in ality among inpatient adolescents. Cognitive Therapy
self-destruction. Clinical Child Psychology and Psy- and Research, 25, 743–756.
chiatry, 1, 607–619. Rooke, O., & Birchwood, M. (1998). Loss, humili-
Orbach, I. (2003). Suicide and the suicidal body: ation, and entrapment as appraisals of schizo-
2002 Dublin Award Address. [Addresses]. Suicide phrenic illness: A prospective study of depressed
& Life-Threatening Behavior, 33, 1–8. and non-depressed patients. British Journal of Clini-
Orbach, I., Bar-Joseph, H., & Dror, N. (1990). Styles cal Psychology, 37, 259–268.
of problem solving in suicidal individuals. Suicide Rudd, M. D. (2006). Fluid vulnerability theory: A cog-
& Life-Threatening Behavior, 20(1), 56–64. nitive approach to understanding the process of
Patterson, A. A., & Holden, R. R. (2012). Psychache acute and chronic suicide risk. Cognition and suicide:
and suicide ideation among men who are homeless: Theory, research, and therapy (pp. 355–368). Washing-
A test of Shneidman’s model. Suicide & Life- ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
Threatening Behavior, 42(2), 147–156. Rudd, M. D., Joiner, T., & Rajab, M. H. (2001). Treat-
Pereira, E. J., Kroner, D. G., Holden, R. R., & ing suicidal behavior: An effective, time-limited approach.
Flamenbaum, R. (2010). Testing Shneidman’s New York, NY: Guilford.
model of suicidality in incarcerated offenders Rudd, M. D., Rajab, M. H., & Dahm, P. F. (1994).
and in undergraduates. Personality and Individual Problem-solving appraisal in suicide ideators and
Differences, 49, 912–917. attempters. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
Plutchik, R. (1995). Outward and inward directed 64, 136–149.
aggressiveness: The interaction between violence Rustein, E. H., & Goldberg, L. (1973). The effects of
and suicidality. Pharmacopsychiatry, 28, Suppl 2, 47–57. aggressive stimulation on suicidal patients: An

310 VOLUME 18  NUMBER 4  2014


S. Barzilay and A. Apter

experimental study of the psychoanalytic theory of entrapment in schizophrenia: The relationship


suicide. In I. Rubinstein (Ed.), Psychoanalysis and with suicidal ideation and positive psychotic
contemporary sciences vol. 2. (pp. 157–174). New symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 178, 244–248.
York, NY: Macmillan. Taylor, P. J., Wood, A. M., Gooding, P., Johnson, J.,
Schotte, D. E., & Clum, G. A. (1982). Suicide ideation & Tarrier, N. (2009). Are defeat and entrapment
in a college population: A test of a model. Journal of best defined as a single construct? Personality and
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 690–696. Individual Differences, 47, 795–797.
Schotte, D. E., & Clum, G. A. (1987). Problem-solving Taylor, P. J., Wood, A. M., Gooding, P., & Tarrier,
skills in suicidal psychiatric patients. Journal of N. (2010). Appraisals and suicidality: The mediat-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 49–54. ing role of defeat and entrapment. Archives of Suicide
Selby, E. A., Smith, A. R., Bulik, C. M., Olmsted, M. Research, 14, 236–247.
P., Thornton, L., McFarlane, T. L., . . . Joiner, T. Troister, T., & Holden, R. R. (2010a). Comparing
E., Jr. (2010). Habitual starvation and provocative psychache, depression, and hopelessness in their
behaviors: Two potential routes to extreme suici- associations with suicidality: A test of Shneidman’s
dal behavior in anorexia nervosa. Behaviour Research theory of suicide. Personality and Individual Differ-
and Therapy, 48, 634–645. ences, 49, 689–693.
Shneidman, E. S. (1980). A possible classification of Troister, T., & Holden, R. R. (2010b). Factorial differ-
suicidal acts based on Murray’s need system. Suicide entiation among depression, hopelessness, and psychache in
& Life-Threatening Behavior, 10, 175–181. predicting suicidality. Presented at the American
Shneidman, E. S. (1993). Suicide as psychache. The Psychological Association Annual Convention,
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 181, 145–147. San Diego, CA.
Sinclair, J. M., Crane, C., Hawton, K., & Williams, J. M. Troister, T., & Holden, R. R. (2012). A two-year pro-
(2007). The role of autobiographical memory speci- spective study of psychache and its relationship to
ficity in deliberate self-harm: Correlates and conse- suicidality among high-risk undergraduates. Journal
quences. Journal of Affective Disorders, 102, 11–18. of Clinical Psychology, 68, 1019–1027.
Smith, A. R., Ribeiro, J. D., Mikolajewski, A., Taylor, Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F.
J., Joiner, T. E., & Iacono, W. G. (2012). An (2002). Social exclusion causes self-defeating beha-
examination of environmental and genetic contri- vior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
butions to the determinants of suicidal behavior 606–615.
among male twins. Psychiatry Research, 197, 60–65. Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F.
Spielberger, C. D., Reheiser, E. C., & Sydeman, S. J. (2003). Social exclusion and the deconstructed
(1995). Measuring the experience, expression, and state: Time perception, meaninglessness, lethargy,
control of anger. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric lack of emotion, and self-awareness. Journal of
Nursing, 18, 207–232. Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 409–423.
Stanley, B., Gameroff, M. J., Michalsen, V., & Mann, Van Orden, K. A., Lynam, Meredith E., Hollar,
J. J. (2001). Are suicide attempters who self- Daniel, & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2006). Perceived
mutilate a unique population? The American Journal Burdensomeness as an Indicator of Suicidal Symp-
of Psychiatry, 158, 427–432. toms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 457–467.
Surrence, K., Miranda, R., Marroquin, B. M., & Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Cukrowicz, K. C.,
Chan, S. (2009). Brooding and reflective rumi- Bratihwatie, S. r., Selby, E. A., & Joiner, T. E.,
nation among suicide attempters: Cognitive vul- Jr. (2010). The interpersonal theory of suicide.
nerability to suicidal ideation. Behaviour Research Psychological Review, 117, 575–600.
and Therapy, 47, 803–808. Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Gordon, K. H.,
Tassava, S. H., & Ruderman, A. J. (1999). Appli- Bender, T. W., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2008). Suicidal
cation of escape theory to binge eating and suicid- desire and the capability for suicide: Tests of the
ality in college women. Journal of Social and Clinical interpersonal-psychological theory of suicidal
Psychology, 18, 450–466. behavior among adults. Journal of Consulting Clinical
Tatman, S. M., Greene, A. L., & Karr, L. C. (1993). Use Psychology, 76, 72–83.
of the suicide probability scale (SPS) with adoles- Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., James, L. M., Castro,
cents. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behaviors, 23, 188–203. Y., Gordon, K. H., Braithwaite, S. R., . . . Joiner,
Taylor, P. J., Gooding, P., Wood, A. M., Johnson, J., T. E., Jr. (2008). Suicidal ideation in college stu-
Pratt, D., & Tarrier, N. (2010). Defeat and dents varies across semesters: The mediating role

ARCHIVES OF SUICIDE RESEARCH 311


Psychological Models of Suicide

of belongingness. Suicide and Life-Threatening Williams, J. M. G., & Broadbent, K. (1986). Distrac-
Behavior, 38, 427–435. tion by emotional stimuli: Use of a Stroop task
Wenzel, A., & Beck, A. T. (2008). A cognitive model with suicide attempters. British Journal of Clinical
of suicidal behavior: Theory and treatment. Applied Psychology, 25, 101–110.
and Preventive Psychology, 12, 189–201. Williams, J. M. G., & Pollock, L. (2000). Psychology
Wenzel, A., Brown, G. K., & Beck, A. T. (2009). of suicide behaviour. In K. Hawton & K. van
Cognitive therapy for suicidal patients: Scientific and Heeringen (Eds.), The international handbook of suicide
clinical applications. Washington, DC: American and suicidal behaviour (pp. 79–93). Chichester, UK:
Psychological Association. John Wiley & Sons.
Whiteside, S. P., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2004). Obsessive- Williams, J. M. G., & Pollock, L. (2001). Psychological
compulsive symptoms and the expression of anger. aspects of the suicidal process. In van Heeringen
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 259–268. (Ed.), Understanding suicdial behaviour: The suicidal pro-
Williams, J. M. G. (1997). Cry of pain: Understanding cess approach to research, treatment and prevention (pp. 76–
suicide and self-harm. London, UK: Penguin Books. 94). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.
Williams, J. M., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., & Beck, A. Witte, T. K., Merrill, K. A., Stellrecht, N. E., Bernert,
T. (2005). Problem solving deteriorates following R. A., Hollar, D. L., Schatschneider, C., & Joiner,
mood challenge in formerly depressed patients T. E., Jr. (2008). ‘‘Impulsive’’ youth suicide
with a history of suicidal ideation. Journal of Abnor- attempters are not necessarily all that impulsive.
mal Psychology, 114, 421–431. Journal of Affective Disorders, 107, 107–116.

312 VOLUME 18  NUMBER 4  2014


Copyright of Archives of Suicide Research is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like